Abstract
Foreknowledge of target stimulus features improves visual search performance, due to “feature-based attention” (FBA). Recent studies have reported that “feature-based expectation” (FBE) also heightens decision sensitivity. Superficially, it sounds like the latter work has simply rediscovered (and relabelled) the effects of FBA. However, this is not the case. Here, we explain why.
Attention can prioritize the processing of stimulus features (e.g. red) or dimensions (e.g. colour). This “feature-based attention” (FBA) has been most intensively investigated using visual search paradigms. Consider a search task in which observers view several dot motion patches, and are asked to detect which one is moving coherently (Fig. 1a). Feature-based cues providing valid foreknowledge of the target motion direction (e.g. 45°) facilitate detection performance, relative to neutral or invalid cues [1, 2].
A distinct line of research has investigated how expectations about features influence behaviour and modulate brain activity [3]. Consider a discrimination task in which observers view two dot motion patches, and are asked to report whether the motion direction in one patch (e.g., right of fixation) is clockwise (+45°) or counterclockwise (−45°) of vertical (Fig. 1b). When cues signal the expected direction of dot motion (e.g. +45°), observers can combine this prior knowledge with visual feature information. This leads to an overall increase in accuracy.
This advantage for expected features on the discrimination task seems wholly consistent with FBA, just as facilitation in search tasks seems to follow naturally from expectations about the target feature. Superficially, it may thus appear that these two manipulations (which, in our example, both cue an expectation of +45° motion) simply index the same attentional process. Here, however, we argue that this is not the case. Rather, we draw a distinction between manipulations (i) that provide information about the relevance of perceptual signals for a decision (FBA), and (ii) that offer information about signal probability, but not relevance. We call the latter “feature-based expectation” (FBE). In recent years, the distinction between FBA and FBE has attracted growing interest but also provoked confusion. Here, we discuss a principled way of distinguishing these two sources of foreknowledge.
Consider these tasks from the perspective of a theoretical agent who makes the best possible decisions given uncertainty in sensory signals. This “ideal observer” will weight sources of information according to their relevance to the task at hand. Consider first the search task (Fig. 1a,c). In the absence of a motion direction cue, the natural strategy is to sum noisy motion energy across all possible directions, and compare the resulting maxima between patches. However, in the presence of a cue, one motion direction becomes more relevant than others, and thus can be given more weight in the decision. This should lead to more sensitive detection judgments, as described in past studies of FBA [1, 2, 4].
By contrast, an ideal observer discriminating motion direction at a single location should add up and compare noisy motion evidence in favour of the two categories (e.g. clockwise vs. counterclockwise of vertical). Here, both the expected (e.g. +45°) and unexpected (−45°) motion signals are equally relevant, and should be given equivalent weight, irrespective of whether an FBE cue is available or not (Fig. 1d). Thus, according to the ideal observer framework, knowledge of stimulus probabilities should not enhance discrimination sensitivity. Rather, performance increases because the ideal observer adjusts the decision criterion to respond consistently with the cue more often, leading to a response bias towards the expected category.
The predictions of this normative framework – that FBA will increase decision sensitivity, whereas FBE will increase bias but not sensitivity – are supported by a longstanding psychophysical literature that has computed sensitivity (d’) and bias (β) from relative proportions of correct and error trials. Interestingly, however, recent studies have cast doubt on the classical perspective, suggesting instead that FBE and FBA may both facilitate detection sensitivity. Here, we evaluate these claims in the light of the ideal observer framework outlined above.
In one recent study [5], observers detected a degraded visual object that could occur at one of four spatial locations. The authors report that valid expectations about the category membership (e.g. “car”) of the target object facilitated detection. Here, however, the cues offered observers the opportunity to weight visual information according to its relevance in the high-dimensional space of object classes (e.g. to weight “car” more heavily than “non-car” features). Thus, a facilitation in performance is expected under the ideal observer framework. A similar logic applies to demonstrations that degraded objects are identified more readily when preceded by a valid written name [6]. In the terms proposed above, these interesting findings may be attributable to FBA, rather than FBE.
In another recent study [7], observers judged the presence or absence of a vertical grating embedded in noise, whilst orthogonal FBE and FBA cues provided information about the probability (cue present vs. absence) and relevance of the visual signals respectively. Rather than using conventional approaches to estimating decision-theoretic statistics, the authors used psychophysical reverse correlation methods, a more sensitive approach that measures the influence that signal-like fluctuations in noise (‘noise energy’) in a psychophysical stimulus have on decisions [8]. Choices were better predicted by noise energy in the presence of valid expectation cues (Fig. 2a), and unlike the influence of FBA cues, this effect was strongest when sensory signals were weak (Fig. 2b). This finding was explained by a computational simulation in which FBE cues facilitate detection sensitivity via an early gain control mechanism, whereas FBA acts to reduce noise in the decision process. In another study [9], observers discriminated the tilt (clockwise vs. counterclockwise) of a noisy grating, and PRC was used to assess the influence of sensory signals on choices, under FBA and FBE cues. Here, FBE cues increased sensitivity to more diagnostic, “off-channel” features, whereas FBA had a more general multiplicative influence on decision sensitivity (Fig. 2c).
The latter studies suggest that human behaviour deviates from that of an ideal observer. FBE cues (that offer probabilistic information alone) can enhance decision sensitivity. However, they do so in a fashion that is distinct from classical manipulations of FBA. The precise computational mechanisms underlying FBE and FBA remain unclear, but one possibility is that FBE cues engage an adaptive gain control mechanism that allows limited resources to be allocated efficiently, i.e. to those sensory features that are most likely to occur [3]. Consistent with this view, gratings with expected orientation elicit lower-amplitude BOLD signals in early visual cortex, and they can be decoded more accurately from multivoxel activity, suggestive of sharper population tuning for expected orientation [10].
More generally, FBA has been studied using a range of experimental approaches. Some researchers have employed a variant of the search paradigm that asks observers to respond when a specific feature occurs in an ongoing stimulation stream, reporting a neural advantage on “match” trials. Others have manipulated the similarity of features in a primary and secondary decision task, and measured the benefits when they overlap, prompting the “feature-similarity gain hypothesis”, a prominent model of FBA [11]. Because FBA effects are strongest when targets defined by a common feature are repeated between or within trials, others have suggested that FBA may be related to bottom-up processes such as repetition priming [12]. Given this diversity of experimental approaches, it is perhaps unsurprising that the mechanisms underlying FBA remain controversial. We suggest that it will be crucial for future research to explicitly orthogonalise FBE and FBA – guided by the normative distinction we highlight here – and to measure their separate influence, at both the neural and the computational levels.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health, National Institutes of Health (Grant R01 MH097965 to T. E.) and the European Research Council (Starter Award 281628 to C.S.).
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Ho TC, et al. Perceptual consequences of feature-based attentional enhancement and suppression. Journal of vision. 2012;12:15. doi: 10.1167/12.8.15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Ling S, et al. How spatial and feature-based attention affect the gain and tuning of population responses. Vision Res. 2009;49:1194–1204. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.05.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Summerfield C, de Lange FP. Expectation in perceptual decision making: neural and computational mechanisms. Nature reviews. Neuroscience. 2014;15:745–756. doi: 10.1038/nrn3838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.White AL, et al. Stimulus competition mediates the joint effects of spatial and feature-based attention. Journal of vision. 2015;15:7. doi: 10.1167/15.14.7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Stein T, Peelen MV. Content-specific expectations enhance stimulus detectability by increasing perceptual sensitivity. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2015;144:1089–1104. doi: 10.1037/xge0000109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Eger E, et al. Mechanisms of top-down facilitation in perception of visual objects studied by FMRI. Cerebral cortex. 2007;17:2123–2133. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Wyart V, et al. Dissociable prior influences of signal probability and relevance on visual contrast sensitivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2012;109:3593–3598. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1120118109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Solomon JA. Noise reveals visual mechanisms of detection and discrimination. Journal of vision. 2002;2:105–120. doi: 10.1167/2.1.7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Cheadle S, et al. Feature expectation heightens visual sensitivity during fine orientation discrimination. Journal of vision. 2015;15:14. doi: 10.1167/15.14.14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kok P, et al. Less is more: expectation sharpens representations in the primary visual cortex. Neuron. 2012;75:265–270. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Maunsell JH, Treue S. Feature-based attention in visual cortex. Trends in neurosciences. 2006;29:317–322. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2006.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Theeuwes J. Feature-based attention: it is all bottom-up priming. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences. 2013;368:20130055. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]