Elwood & Adams [1] exposed Carcinus maenas to electric shocks. They reported that 6/20 unshocked crabs did not move but all (20/20) shocked crabs did move. No control crabs showed ‘extreme responses’ [1, p. 2] but 4/20 shocked crabs did. Most crabs walked (14 control and 16 shocked). When comparing only walking crabs, haemolymph lactate was higher in shocked crabs. At the end of the discussion, they state ‘Although these physiological responses are expected should an animal experience pain, they do not prove the feeling of pain in decapods…’ [1, p. 3]. They nevertheless conclude that their results, interpreted in the context of previous research, ‘…fulfils the criteria expected of a pain experience’ [1, p. 1]. We contend that their conclusion that crabs experienced pain (as summarized in the title and abstract) is unfounded, because neither is stress a necessary prerequisite for pain nor is it always coupled with pain.
We are concerned by the authors relating stress to pain. Lactate may indicate physiological stress in some crustaceans, but physiological stress is not pain. Many stimuli used to study stress, confinement of fish, for example, are not associated with stimulation of nociceptors. There is a wealth of literature demonstrating that many different stimuli elicit physiological stress, including benign experimenter interventions [2]. Physiological stress is neither specifically nor selectively related to pain and cannot be inferred to be a useful index of pain. Stress is a confounding factor in ‘pain’ experiments [3] and efforts often are made to show stress does not occur (e.g. [4]). Hence, elevated lactate in shocked crabs does not necessarily depend on pain.
Similarly, pain is not necessarily stress, as the former, at the very minimum, requires activation of nociceptive pathways, followed by conscious higher level neural processing [5], neither of which was demonstrated.
In discussing behaviours that they argue reflect pain, the authors cite learning to avoid aversive stimuli and ‘activities that go beyond mere reflex response’ [1, p. 1]; neither are adequate criteria for pain [5]. Likewise, behaviours like shell abandonment or grooming described in prior publications by the same laboratory are insufficient to demonstrate pain. The authors do not provide a persuasive argument for the continued use of these criteria for pain.
There also are methodological shortcomings that confound their interpretations. Walking was not quantified; animals were subjectively partitioned into subgroups; the tissue producing lactate was not determined and muscle activity was not quantified.
The authors state that shocks ‘did not appear to cause substantial muscle contraction, which might have accounted for high lactate, because the crabs walked normally’ [1, p. 3]. Crustacean muscle is characterized by large motor units, the majority innervated with only two large axons with low thresholds that are much more easily stimulated with external electrodes than small sensory axons [6]. Thus, an alternative and more parsimonious explanation for their lactate results is that the shocks induced some muscle activity.
A variety of stressors can elevate lactate in crabs; the most potent is activity, which can elevate lactate an order of magnitude above the values the authors reported for shocked crabs [7]. Moreover, the ‘elevated’ [1, p. 2] lactate levels reported are within the normal range for this species, suggesting that they were not unduly stressed [8].
The authors did not report heart or breathing rates, making it difficult to assess if their results were associated with shocks, or indirect physiological effects. Another alternative explanation of their results is that their shocks stopped the heart and respiratory movements [9], thus increasing anaerobic metabolites (lactate) before sampling.
In conclusion, we argue that the reported data fail to show that haemolymph lactate is an appropriate measure of ‘pain-like states’ [1, p. 2] in crabs or provide evidence to support the conclusion that crabs exhibit ‘pain-like states’. We agree with Dawkins that one should address animal welfare pragmatically using stress-related indicators without reference to conscious experiences [10]. However, legally, the ability to feel pain often is used as a criterion to distinguish among species that deserve moral attention from those that do not [11]. Thus, we are concerned that policy-makers may make inappropriate decisions about crab welfare based on this over-interpreted study.
Footnotes
The accompanying reply can be viewed at http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2016.0126.
Authors' contributions
All authors contributed to drafting, editing and gave final approval.
Competing interests
We have no competing interests.
Funding
E.D.S. is funded by NSERC.
References
- 1.Elwood RW, Adams L. 2015. Electric shock causes physiological stress responses in shore crabs, consistent with prediction of pain. Biol. Lett. 11, 20150800 ( 10.1098/rsbl.2015.0800) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Egan AE, Ulrich-Lai YM. 2015. Activation of physiological stress response. Physiol. Behav. 150, 43–52. ( 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.03.007) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Ashley PJ, Ringrose S, Edwards KL, Wallington E, McCrohan CR, Snedden LU. 2009. Effect of noxious stimulation upon antipredator response and dominance status in rainbow trout. Anim. Behav. 77, 403–410. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.10.015) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Jones SG, Kamunde C, Lemke K, Stevens ED. 2014. The dose–response relation for the antinociceptive effect of morphine in a fish, rainbow trout. J. Vet. Pharmacol. Ther. 35, 563–570. ( 10.1111/j.1365-2885.2011.01363.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Rose JD, Arlinghaus R, Cooke SJ, Diggles BK, Sawynok W, Stevens ED, Wynne CDL. 2014. Can fish really feel pain? Fish Fish. 15, 97–133. ( 10.1111/faf.12010) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Hoyle G. 1962. Neuromuscular physiology. Adv. Comp. Physiol. Biochem. 1, 177–216. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Neill D, Thomson J. 2012. The stress induced by the Crustastun process in two commercially important decapods crustaceans. See http://eprints.gla.ac.uk/81433.
- 8.Taylor EW, Butler PJ. 1978. Aquatic and aerial respiration in the shore crab, Carcinus maenas (L.), acclimated to 15°C. J. Comp. Physiol. 127, 315–323. ( 10.1007/BF00738415) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Wilkens JL, Wilkens LA, McMahon BR. 1974. Central control of cardiac and scaphognathite pacemakers in the crab, Cancer magister. J. Comp. Physiol. 90, 89–104. ( 10.1007/BF00698370) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Dawkins MS. 2012. Why animals matter: animal consciousness, animal welfare and human well-being. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Arlinghaus R, Schwab A, Cooke SJ, Cowx IG. 2009. Contrasting pragmatic and suffering-centred approaches to fish welfare in recreational angling. J. Fish Biol. 75, 2448–2463. ( 10.1111/j.1095-8649.2009.02466.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]