Skip to main content
Biology Letters logoLink to Biology Letters
. 2016 Apr;12(4):20160126. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2016.0126

Stress was never said to be pain: response to Stevens et al. (2016)

Robert W Elwood 1,
PMCID: PMC4881359  PMID: 27048469

Despite the claim by Stevens et al. [1], Elwood & Adams [2] did not conclude ‘that crabs experienced pain’ [1, p. 1]. Indeed, in both the introduction and the conclusion, it was made clear that ‘absolute proof of pain in not possible in any animal’ [2, p. 3]. If it is impossible, then it should be very clear to any reader that no claim of proof is being made. Impossibility of proof, however, creates problems when studying animal pain. Therefore, criteria have been suggested [3,4] that should be fulfilled before the possibility of pain in an animal taxon is entertained. If criteria are not fulfilled, then the case for pain would be lessened. With this in mind, a number of criteria have been tested for crustaceans and, in general, the data fulfil ‘the criteria expected of pain’ [2, p. 1]. Fulfilling expected criteria is obviously not the same as claiming proof. The critique's suggestion of such a claim in the paper is spurious. Physiological stress has been put forward as one of many criteria of pain. It is not the same as pain, and the implications of the title and text of the critique are unfounded as, again, no such suggestion was made.

Many studies have shown stress responses in crustaceans but they are often confounded by increased activity when electric shock is given [5]. The paper [2] provides a way of largely overcoming this problem by comparing subgroups of shocked and non-shocked crabs that showed a similar level of behaviour. The shocked crabs nevertheless had greater physiological signs of stress than did controls. The paper [2] considered if that might be due to greater muscular activity but concluded that there were no overt signs of such activity. The critique suggests that heart and ‘breathing’ rates should have been monitored. Measuring either in crustaceans is difficult, and invasive techniques are required. For example, heart rate monitoring typically requires a hole to be drilled into the carapace, which would likely have affected stress levels.

The critique states that pain ‘at the very minimum requires activation of nociceptive pathways, followed by conscious higher level processing, neither of which was demonstrated’ [1, p. 1]. However, nociceptors have been demonstrated in many invertebrates [6,7] and local anaesthetics block receptor input [8,9]. The demand that ‘conscious higher level neural processing’ [1, p. 1] be demonstrated is more worrying. It is impossible to show this for any animal [10]; so, by implication, the critique dismisses the possibility of pain in any animal. I cannot agree that we should reject pain as a possibility until conscious processing is shown. To do so would be an invitation to withdraw protection of animals in a wide variety of situations and that would be highly retrogressive. However, a later paragraph appears to reverse that position by accepting that ‘stress-related indicators without reference to conscious experience’ [1, p. 2] should be used. Surprisingly, this latter statement agrees entirely with the approach of the original paper [2]. The critique thus offers a very muddled position. The protection given to animals by policy-makers is based on criteria of pain being tested and found to be fulfilled. As different taxa are tested for those criteria, we shall no doubt find some that return negative results. That crustaceans tend to show positive results has been a surprise to the present author. They have been shown not to respond by mere reflex [8], rather they show long-term motivational change [11], swift avoidance learning [12], giving up of valuable resources to avoid noxious stimuli [12,13], trade-offs with other motivational requirements indicating central processing [13], inhibition of responses by local anaesthetics [8] and stress responses [2]. That is, many studies to date are indisputably consistent with the idea of pain, but as stated above, being consistent with something is not the same as absolute proof. With animal protection, evidence-based decisions have been made without absolute proof and offer protection if pain is considered a strong possibility [4]. Some authors [14] (including some of the present critique), however, dismiss the possibility of pain in a wide variety of invertebrate and vertebrate taxa and thus influence policy-makers against protection for those animals.

Footnotes

The accompanying comment can be viewed at http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2015.1006.

References

  • 1.Stevens ED, et al. 2016. Stress is not pain. Comment on Elwood and Adams (2015) ‘Electric shock causes physiological stress responses in shore crabs, consistent with prediction of pain’. Biol. Lett. 12, 20151006 ( 10.1098/rsbl.2015.1006) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Elwood RW, Adams L. 2015. Electric shock causes physiological stress responses in shore crabs, consistent with prediction of pain. Biol. Lett. 11, 20150800 ( 10.1098/rsbl.2015.0800) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Bateson P. 1991. Assessment of pain in animals. Anim. Behav. 42, 827–839. ( 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80127-7) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Sneddon LU, Elwood RW, Adamo SA, Leach MC. 2014. Defining and assessing animal pain. Anim. Behav. 97, 202–212. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.09.007) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Fossat P, Bacque-Cazenave J, De Deurwaerdere P, Cattaert D, Delbecque J-P. 2015. Serotonin, but not dopamine, controls stress response and anxiety-like behavior in crayfish, Procambarus clarkii. J. Exp. Biol. 218, 2745–2752. ( 10.1242/jeb.120550) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Pastor J, Soria B, Belmonte C. 1996. Properties of the nociceptive neurons of the leech segmental ganglion. J. Neurophysiol. 75, 2268–2279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Tobin DM, Bargmann CI. 2004. Invertebrate nociception: behaviors, neurons and molecules. J. Neurobiol. 61, 161–174. ( 10.1002/neu.20082) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Barr S, Laming PR, Dick JTA, Elwood RW. 2008. Nociception or pain in a decapod crustacean? Anim. Behav. 75, 745–751. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.07.004) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Ross LG, Ross B. 2008. Anaesthetic and sedative techniques for aquatic animals, 3rd edn Oxford, UK: Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Dawkins MS. 2012. Why animals matter: Animal consciousness, animal welfare and human well-being. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Appel M, Elwood RW. 2009. Gender differences, responsiveness and memory of a potentially painful event in hermit crabs. Anim. Behav. 78, 1373–1379. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.09.008) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Magee B, Elwood RW. 2013. Shock avoidance by discrimination learning in the shore crab (Carcinus maenas) is consistent with a key criterion for pain. J. Exp. Biol. 216, 353–358. ( 10.1242/jeb.072041) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Elwood RW, Appel M. 2009. Pain in hermit crabs? Anim. Behav. 77, 1243–1246. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.01.028) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Rose JD, Arlinghaus R, Cooke SJ, Diggles BK, Sawynok W, Stevens ED, Wynne CDL. 2014. Can fish really feel pain? Fish Fisheries 15, 97–133. ( 10.1111/faf.12010) [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Biology Letters are provided here courtesy of The Royal Society

RESOURCES