Abstract
Purpose
Automated scotopic, mesopic, and photopic perimetry are likely to be important paradigms in the assessment of emerging treatments of retinal diseases, yet our knowledge of the photoreceptor mechanisms detecting targets under these conditions remains largely dependent on simian data. We therefore aimed to establish the photoreceptor/postreceptoral mechanisms detecting perimetric targets in humans under photopic, mesopic, and scotopic conditions and to make recommendations for suitable clinical testing strategies for selective perimetry.
Methods
Perimetric sensitivities within 30° of fixation were determined for eight wavelengths (410, 440, 480, 520, 560, 600, 640, and 680 nm) under scotopic, mesopic (1.3 cd.m−2) and photopic (10 cd.m−2) conditions. Data were fitted with vector combinations of rod, S-cone, nonopponent M+L-cone mechanism, and opponent M- versus L-cone mechanism templates.
Results
Scotopicperimetric sensitivity was determined by rods peripherally and by a combination of rods and cones at, and immediately around, fixation. Mesopic perimetric sensitivity was mediated by M+L-cones and S-cones centrally and by M+L-cones and rods more peripherally. Photopic perimetric sensitivity was determined by an opponent M- versus L-cone, a nonopponent M+L-cone, and an S-cone mechanism centrally and by a combination of an S-cone and an M+L-cone mechanism peripherally.
Conclusions
Under scotopic conditions, a 480-nm stimulus provides adequate isolation (≥28 dB) of the rod mechanism. Several mechanisms contribute to mesopic sensitivity: this redundancy in detection may cause both insensitivity to broadband white targets and ambiguity in determining which mechanism is being probed with short-wavelength stimuli. M- and L-cone–derived mechanisms are well isolated at 10 cd.m−2: these may be selectively probed by a stimulus at 640 nm (≥ 20 dB isolation).
Translation Relevance
In human observers, multiple mechanisms contribute to the detection of Goldmann size III and size V perimetric targets under scotopic, mesopic, and photopic conditions. The relative contribution of these mechanisms appears to differ from those found previously for macaques. Our results furthermore suggest that caution must be exercised when using microperimetric techniques, which are typically conducted under mesopic conditions and which are likely to be important in the assessment of emerging treatments for retinal disease. This is because mesopic background conditions maximize the redundancy of target detection. Furthermore, our results demonstrate that spectral manipulation of the stimulus alone cannot be used to reliably separate rod from cone responses under these conditions.
Keywords: rod perimetry, cone perimetry, selective perimetry, mesopic perimetry, photopic perimetry
Introduction
Clinical perimetry probes photopic or mesopic threshold sensitivity and typically employs broadband white adapting backgrounds of 10 or 1.3 cd.m−2, respectively.1 While most modern computerized perimeters use 10 cd.m−2 broadband white backgrounds, several newer instruments, in particular those that combine scanning laser ophthalmoscopy with perimetry (so-called “microperimeters”), use 1.3 cd.m−2 backgrounds.2 Microperimeters provide a means of studying the structure–function relationship in retinal disease2 and are likely to prove pivotal in studies of emerging treatments, such as gene3 and stem cell therapies. Similarly, selective perimetric techniques, which seek to distinguish rod from cone responses under scotopic and photopic conditions,4 are likely to be important in studies of emerging treatments. Such techniques are ideal because they offer the potential of identifying isolated or preferential loss of photoreceptor classes. Furthermore, they are paradigms that reduce the “redundancy” of target detection: it has been proposed that this feature explains the observation that selective perimetry provides early detection of ophthalmic and neuro-ophthalmic disease.5 By extension, it is reasonable to suppose that such tests might therefore better demonstrate the possible ameliorative effects of emerging therapies by detecting them earlier (i.e., when they would not be evident using standard perimetric paradigms).
Although automated perimeters have been in use for more than three decades, our understanding of the mechanisms determining sensitivity to Goldmann size III-V targets under clinical conditions is primarily derived from trials using three macaca mulatta.6 In macaques, photopic and mesopic perimetric sensitivity is mediated by a combination of opponent M- versus L-cone, nonopponent M+L-cone, and S-cone mechanisms, with the relative contribution depending on the size and wavelength of the target.7 Analogous information pertaining to human subjects is unavailable: this is principally because of the onerous nature of acquiring such data.8 The early perimetric literature in humans subjects suggests that the rods and cones are approximately equally sensitive to broadband white targets presented on broadband white backgrounds at 1.3 cd.m−2.7 However, more recent studies using two-color stimulus techniques (one short- and one long-wavelength target), which in effect represent a minimalist approach to determining spectral sensitivity, suggest that the cones may predominantly determine mesopic sensitivity for size III targets (Crossland MD, et al. IOVS. 2012;53:ARVO E-Abstract 4822). There is evidence from dark adaptation experiments that scotopic targets are detected by rods peripherally and by a combination of rods and cones at fixation.4 Results from the two-color technique suggest that paracentral targets may also be detected by a combination of rods and cones (Crossland MD, et al. IOVS. 2012;53:ARVO E-Abstract 4822).
There is thus uncertainty regarding the physiological substrate of perimetric target detection in humans under photopic, mesopic, and to a lesser extent scotopic, lighting conditions. We therefore aimed to measure topographical scotopic, mesopic, and photopic spectral sensitivity functions in order to identify the photoreceptor and postreceptoral mechanisms mediating perimetric sensitivity under these conditions. Moreover, we aimed to use our results to predict photoreceptor mechanism isolations for different background and stimulus combinations as a means of aiding the interpretation and design of selective perimetric tests.
Materials and Methods
Three fit and healthy color-normal (as assessed by the Ishihara Plate Test and Farnsworth Munsell 100-Hue) adult male subjects with no history of ocular disease or systemic disease known to affect vision and aged 32 to 41 years were recruited to perform this investigation, which conformed to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. All experiments were conducted using a modified Humphrey 640 perimeter (Humphrey Instruments, San Leandro, CA). Tests were conducted under scotopic, mesopic, and photopic conditions (standard Humphrey white background, luminance 1.3 and 10 cd.m−2; approximate retinal illuminance 1.3 and 2.1 log Td respectively for mesopic and photopic conditions).9 Subjects were preadapted for 5, 20, and 40 minutes for photopic, mesopic, and scotopic conditions, respectively. Sensitivity was determined using a “full threshold” paradigm10 for size III and V targets (0.4° and 1.7° diameter, respectively) under photopic and mesopic conditions and for size V targets under scotopic conditions. Stimulus duration was 200 msec and the typical interstimulus interval was 1 to 3 seconds (Patella VM, personal communication, 2015). The stimulus color was controlled by interposing filters in the pathway of the stimulus projection mechanism: filters with half-bandwidths of 10 nm with peak transmissions at 410, 440, 480, 520, 560, 600, 640, and 680 nm were used. Additionally, a 2 log unit neutral density filter was used to extend the instrument's dynamic range. In order to derive topographical spectral sensitivity curves for subjects, we measured sensitivity at 17 locations: [0°, 0°], [±3°, ±3°], [±9°, ±9°], [±15°, ±15°], [±21°, ±21°] (azimuth, elevation) in one eye using each of the stimulus wavelengths. These locations were chosen as they correspond to locations sampled in the 30-2 field test and although completion of the test protocol required highly motivated observers to perform the more than 17,000 stimulus judgements required, the task was less onerous than that described in simians.6 The order of testing with respect to wavelength was randomized and two field tests were performed for each subject for each stimulus and background condition. The system was calibrated with a Pritchard 670 telespectroradiometer (Photo Research Inc., Chatsworth, CA). Data for each subject were corrected for absorption by the crystalline lens via a procedure developed by van Norren and Vos11 and described in detail previously.12 Corrections for macular pigment density at the point of fixation were made via a psychophysical method. Briefly, sensitivity estimates for the 440-nm stimulus were compared with those for the 520-nm stimulus at the point of fixation and at [±9°, ±9°], making the assumption that the same mechanism was governing threshold at each location (i.e., rods for scotopic conditions, cones for photopic conditions). The depression in sensitivity to the 440-nm stimulus relative to the 520-nm stimulus at the point of fixation was assumed to occur secondary to absorption by the macular pigment: this value was then used to make individual adjustments to the macular pigment optical density template of Stockman and colleagues.13
Data at each perimetric location were averaged6 and fitted with combinations of rod or S-cone, a nonopponent M+L-cone, and an opponent M- versus L-cone spectral sensitivity function. In order to generate templates for fitting, Lamb's photopigment template equation (Equation 1)14 was used:
where a = 70; b = 28.5; c = −14.1; A = 0.880; B = 0.924; C = 1.104; D = 0.655, with λmax set to 419, 531, and 561 nm for the S-, M-, and L-cone photopigments, respectively, and to 496 nm for rhodopsin.15,16 Photopigment spectra were then converted to photoreceptor sensitivities using the equation:
where OD is the optical density of the photopigment: this was set to 0.4 for the M- and L-cones and to 0.3 for the S-cones and rods (no adjustment for individual variations in optical density were made; see Figure A1). Generated cone spectra were then fitted to spectral sensitivity data using vector combinations17,18 of an S-cone sensitivity curve, an opponent M- versus L-cone sensitivity curve,19 a combined M+L-cone nonopponent sensitivity curve, and a rod sensitivity curve:
where k1, k2, k3, k4, k5, and s1 are scaling constants, LCS is L-cone sensitivity, MCS is M-cone sensitivity, SCS is S-cone sensitivity, and RS is rod sensitivity.6
Templates were fitted using the Levenberg-Marquardt algorithm with instrumental weighting as implemented in QtiPlot version 0.9.920: corrected R2 values were used to determine the best-fitting model. At each stimulus location the relative light absorption for equiquantal targets was calculated in order to illustrate the topographical variation in relative sensitivity of detecting mechanisms. The difference in calculated sensitivity for targets at specified wavelengths was also calculated as “isolation” (if, for example, the rods are 28 dB more sensitive to a stimulus than the M+L-cone mechanism, then the isolation of the rods is 28 dB). Mechanism isolations were calculated for each of the stimulus wavelengths employed at each location in order to facilitate comparisons between previously described methods and to aid in the design of possible future selective perimetric tests.
Results
Intertest sensitivities at each location for each condition were consistent to less than or equal to 3 dB for each subject. The scotopic visual field displayed a deep central depression (>10 dB) at short wavelengths consistent with the known rod-free zone (Figure A2).21 Peripheral sensitivity was rod-dependent, while cones contributed to central and paracentral sensitivity (Fig. 1). The visual field under mesopic conditions displayed a more modest central depression at short wavelengths, consistent with absorption of short wavelengths by macular pigment/detection by the S-cone mechanism. For targets greater than or equal to 520 nm, the mesopic field had a central peak (Fig. A3). Spectral sensitivity estimates for both size III and V targets suggest that sensitivity at and around fixation was determined by a combination of S-cones and M+L-cones, while for more peripheral locations sensitivity was determined by rods and M+L-cones (Figs. 2A, 2B). The photopic visual field demonstrated a central depression at short wavelengths; this was of similar magnitude to that observed under mesopic conditions and is again consistent with absorption of short wavelengths by macular pigment/detection by the S-cone mechanism. A central peak was observed for targets greater than or equal to 520 nm (Fig. A4). For the size III and size V stimulus conditions (Figs. 3A, 3B), targets were detected by a combination of S-cones, a nonopponent M+L-cone, and an opponent M- versus L-cone mechanism at fixation and by the S-cones and nonopponent M+L-cone mechanism beyond fixation (except at [−3°, +3°] for Goldmann size V targets; Fig. 3B). As outlined above, we also calculated mechanism isolations for different stimulus wavelengths for each background condition to aid in the design and interpretation of selective perimetric tests (see Tables 1–3).
Table 1.
Table 3.
The average macular pigment optical density for centrally presented targets at 480 nm for our subjects was 0.2 (range, 0.1–0.3) under scotopic conditions and 0.7 (range, 0.5–1.0) under photopic conditions. The difference in estimates reflects the fact that macular pigment density declines with eccentricity (density estimates under rod-isolating conditions exclude the central fovea and are therefore anticipated to be lower than under photopic conditions). The average scaling constant for the crystalline lens optical density was 1.15 (range, 1.08–1.20).11
Discussion
Our study seeks to elucidate perimetric spectral sensitivity under scotopic, mesopic, and photopic conditions and to further identify the receptoral/postreceptoral mechanisms governing target detection. General limitations of our study include the fact that we assumed “normal” peak sensitivities and optical densities for the photopigments of our observers. Furthermore, there remains uncertainty regarding the way in which signals from different pathways are combined by the visual system and whilst the vector combination17,18 we used may better reflect this combination than other models, it may still be imperfect.
As anticipated, scotopic peripheral visual function is rod dominated (≥ 28 ± 2 dB of isolation from M+L-cones at ≤ 480 nm). At fixation, and to a lesser extent at [±3°, ±3°], participation of the M+L-cone mechanism was evident (rod isolation 11 ± 1 dB centrally and ≥ 28 ± 2 dB peripherally at ≤480 nm; see Fig. 1, Table 1). As only two mechanisms are active, the established practice of two-color scotopic perimetry4 to distinguish rod from cone, or mixed rod/cone, detection appears to be superficially sound; however, it is not uncommon for S-cone intrusion to occur in retinitis pigmentosa.22 This would be incorrectly interpreted as mixed-rod/cone detection unless more extensive spectral sensitivity measurements were made.
Under mesopic conditions, size III and V targets appeared to be detected by S-cones and the M+L-cone mechanism at, and close to, fixation while at more peripheral locations detection was mediated by rods and a nonopponent M+L-mechanism. Mesopic testing therefore appears to maximize the redundancy of target detection; this has two important sequelae. First, identification of those mechanisms governing perimetric sensitivity is complex, as short wavelength targets may be detected by either S-cones or rods. Thus, testing with short-wavelength targets under mesopic conditions may not provide unambiguous information unless such testing forms part of a more detailed spectral sensitivity assessment. Second, this redundancy in target detection is anticipated to result in insensitivity in identifying functional deficits secondary to pathology, and may indeed account for the early observation that mesopic sensitivity for achromatic targets is resilient to isolated losses of cone or rod function.7 Accordingly, we suggest that testing under scotopic and photopic conditions is preferable if broadband white stimuli are to be used. Furthermore, our results suggest that if mesopic testing is employed, long wavelength targets (≥ 640 nm, which provide ≥ 17 ± 3 dB of M+L-cone isolation; see Table 2) are preferable to broadband white stimuli.
Table 2.
Under photopic conditions, sensitivity at the point of fixation for Goldmann size III and V targets was best modeled by a combination of S-cones, a nonopponent M+L-cone mechanism, and an opponent M- versus L-cone mechanism. These data are consistent with previous observations for centrally presented targets23 and with simian data.6 Spectral sensitivities at more peripheral locations were best described by a vector combination of S-cones and a nonopponent M+L-cone mechanism; this finding is in contrast to the psychophysical findings of Harwerth et al.6 in the macaque, who found evidence for participation of the M- versus L-cone opponent mechanism at most stimulus locations using the same background conditions as ours. Several factors may account for this discrepancy: first, our results may reflect a physiological difference between the human visual system and that of macaca mulatta. For example, it is known that the retinal illuminance for the same light source is higher in the macaque than the human24 (this fact would favor color-opponent mechanisms) and there may be differences in the gains, spatial, and temporal summation properties of the opponent and nonopponent mechanisms between species. Second, our data were modeled using a vector combination of mechanisms17,18 and quantal data,25 while Harwerth et al.6 used energy data for analysis and assumed that the spectral sensitivity function represented an upper envelope of the component functions. It is also important to note that even when Harwerth et al.'s6 data and analysis supported detection via the opponent M- versus L-cone mechanism, the “isolation” of this mechanism from the nonopponent mechanism was poor. Finally, it is also important to note that our findings are in keeping with physiological recordings from macaque retinal ganglion cells using size III stimuli, which demonstrate that M-cells are preferentially activated by such stimuli in conventional (white-on-white) perimetry.26 Our findings would also suggest limitations to the general applicability of Harwerth et al.'s6 data to the human visual system.8
Our photopic data suggest that neither central or peripheral sensitivity for 200-msec targets presented on a 10-cd.m−2 background are well-described by the CIE V(λ) function; these observations are in agreement with previous studies of peripheral sensitivity27 and with estimates of central sensitivity under similar conditions.28 This phenomenon is partially explained by considering the postreceptoral pathways involved in target detection. The brightness-matching paradigm used to derive the V(λ) function uses a flickering stimulus (25 Hz in the most recent update of the CIE function).25 This is likely to favor the parasol ganglion cell pathway and other pathways with robust responses to flickering stimuli such as the smooth monostratified cells,29 while the stimulus used in the Humphrey perimeter is more likely to enable other pathways, such as the S-cone pathway and the midget cell pathway30 (which have poorer temporal sensitivity), to have some participation in target detection. Furthermore, increased peripheral sensitivity to short wavelength targets reflects the distribution of S-cones.31
Although the use of short wavelength targets presented on 10 cd.m−2 broadband white backgrounds for the isolation of the S-cone mechanism has been described,32 the degree of isolation achieved (≥9 ± 1 dB for a 440 nm size V stimulus) is less than that previously reported for the more commonly employed yellow backgrounds at 100 cd.m−2 (i.e., the background condition typically used in so-called short wavelength automated perimetry).33 A broadband white 10 cd.m−2 background condition is therefore not recommended for isolation of the S-cone mechanism for clinical testing. Stimuli at greater than or equal to 640 nm, as used in some previous investigations,34 would be anticipated to provide greater than or equal to 20 ± 2 dB of M+L-cone isolation (assuming that the minimum isolation is at least on par with that for size III targets under mesopic conditions; see Table 3) and are therefore appropriate for clinical testing. However, for reasons elaborated above, the two-color technique may provide erroneous information about rod intrusion under photopic conditions: full identification of the active mechanisms may therefore require testing with more stimulus wavelengths.22
Acknowledgments
Supported by grants from the Bayer Global Ophthalmology Awards Program (MPS), the RANZCO Eye Foundation (Novartis/RANZCO Scholarship: MPS) and the NIHR Biomedical Research Centres of Oxford University Hospitals (REM) & Moorfields Eye Hospital NHS Foundation Trust (REM, ATM).
Appendix
References
- 1. Henson DB. Visual Fields. 2nd ed. Oxford: Butterworth-Heinemann; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 2. Acton JH,, Greenstein VC. Fundus-driven perimetry (microperimetry) compared to conventional static automated perimetry: similarities, differences, and clinical applications. Can J Ophthalmol. 2013; 48: 358–363. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. MacLaren RE, Groppe M, Barnard AR, et al. . Retinal gene therapy in patients with choroideremia: initial findings from a phase 1/2 clinical trial. Lancet. 2014; 383: 1129–1137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Jacobson SG, Voigt WJ, Parel JM, et al. . Automated light- and dark-adapted perimetry for evaluating retinitis pigmentosa. Ophthalmology. 1986; 93: 1604–1611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Johnson CA. Selective versus nonselective losses in glaucoma. J Glaucoma. 1994; 3 (suppl 1): S32–S44. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Harwerth RS,, Smith EL,, III,, DeSantis L. Mechanisms mediating visual detection in static perimetry. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1993; 34: 3011–3023. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Sloan LL. The Tubinger perimeter of Harms and Aulhorn. Recommended procedures and supplementary equipment. Arch Ophthalmol. 1971; 86: 612–622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Johnson CA,, Marshall D,, Jr. Aging effects for opponent mechanisms in the central visual field. Optom Vis Sci. 1995; 72: 75–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Wyszecki GW,, Stiles WS. Color science. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 1982. [Google Scholar]
- 10. Simunovic MP,, Regan BC,, Mollon JD. Is color vision deficiency an advantage under scotopic conditions? Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2001; 42: 3357–3364. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Norren DV,, Vos JJ. Spectral transmission of the human ocular media. Vision Res. 1974; 14: 1237–1244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Simunovic MP,, Cullerne A,, Colley A,, Wilson TD. How well does color perimetry isolate responses from individual cone mechanisms? J Glaucoma. 2004; 13: 22–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Stockman A,, Sharpe LT,, Fach C. The spectral sensitivity of the human short-wavelength sensitive cones derived from thresholds and color matches. Vision Res. 1999; 39: 2901–2927. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Lamb TD. Photoreceptor spectral sensitivities: common shape in the long-wavelength region. Vision Res. 1995; 35: 3083–3091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Dartnall HJ,, Bowmaker JK,, Mollon JD. Human visual pigments: microspectrophotometric results from the eyes of seven persons. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1983; 220: 115–130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Thomas PB,, Mollon JD. Modelling the Rayleigh match. Vis Neurosci. 2004; 21: 477–482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Ingling CR,, Jr, Huong-Peng-Tsou B. Orthogonal combination of the three visual channels. Vision Res. 1977; 17: 1075–1082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Kremers J,, Meierkord S. Rod-cone-interactions in deuteranopic observers: models and dynamics. Vision Res. 1999; 39: 3372–3385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Thornton JE,, Pugh EN,, Jr. Red/Green color opponency at detection threshold. Science. 1983; 219: 191–193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Nocedal J,, Wright SJ. Numerical Optimization. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 21. Ahnelt PK. The photoreceptor mosaic. Eye (Lond). 1998; 12 (pt 3b): 531–540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Massof RW,, Johnson MA,, Finkelstein D. Peripheral absolute threshold spectral sensitivity in retinitis pigmentosa. Br J Ophthalmol. 1981; 65: 112–121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Pokorny J,, Smith VC,, Verriest G,, Pinckers AJLG. Congenital and Acquired Color Vision Defects. New York: Grune & Stratton; 1979. [Google Scholar]
- 24. Lee BB,, Pokorny J,, Smith VC,, Martin PR,, Valberg A. Luminance and chromatic modulation sensitivity of macaque ganglion cells and human observers. J Opt Soc Am A. 1990; 7: 2223–2236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Sharpe LT,, Stockman A,, Jagla W,, Jagle H. A luminous efficiency function, V*(lambda), for daylight adaptation. J Vis. 2005; 5 (11): 948–968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Swanson WH,, Sun H,, Lee BB,, Cao D. Responses of primate retinal ganglion cells to perimetric stimuli. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2011; 52: 764–771. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Weale RA. Spectral sensitivity and wave-length discrimination of the peripheral retina. J Physiol. 1953; 119: 170–190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Verriest G,, Uvijls A. Spectral increment thresholds on a white background in different age groups of normal subjects and in acquired ocular diseases. Doc Ophthalmol. 1977; 43: 217–248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Crook JD, Peterson BB, Packer OS, et al. . The smooth monostratified ganglion cell: evidence for spatial diversity in the Y-cell pathway to the lateral geniculate nucleus and superior colliculus in the macaque monkey. J Neurosci. 2008; 28: 12654–12671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Solomon SG,, Lennie P. The machinery of colour vision. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007; 8: 276–286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Calkins DJ. Seeing with S cones. Prog Retin Eye Res. 2001; 20: 255–287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Jacobson SG,, Marmor MF,, Kemp CM,, Knighton RW. SWS, (blue) cone hypersensitivity in a newly identified retinal degeneration. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1990; 31: 827–838. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Sample PA,, Johnson CA,, Haegerstrom-Portnoy G,, Adams AJ. Optimum parameters for short-wavelength automated perimetry. J Glaucoma. 1996; 5: 375–383. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Zele AJ,, Dang TM,, O'Loughlin RK,, Guymer RH,, Harper A,, Vingrys AJ. Adaptation mechanisms, eccentricity profiles, and clinical implementation of red-on-white perimetry. Optom Vis Sci. 2008; 85: 309–317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]