Skip to main content
Frontiers in Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Neuroscience
. 2016 May 31;10:242. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00242

GluN2B-Containg NMDA Receptors on Adult-Born Granule Cells Contribute to the Antidepressant Action of Fluoxetine

Lindsay Tannenholz 1,2, René Hen 1,2,3,4,*, Mazen A Kheirbek 2,3,5,*
PMCID: PMC4885883  PMID: 27303260

Abstract

Ablation of adult neurogenesis in mice has revealed that young adult-born granule cells (abGCs) are required for some of the behavioral responses to antidepressants (ADs), yet the mechanism by which abGCs contribute to AD action remains unknown. During their maturation process, these immature neurons exhibit unique properties that could underlie their ability to influence behavioral output. In particular, abGCs in the DG exhibit a period of heightened plasticity 4–6 weeks after birth that is mediated by GluN2B-expressing NMDA receptors. The functional contribution of this critical window to AD responsiveness is unclear. Here, we determined the behavioral and neurogenic responses to the AD fluoxetine (FLX) in mice lacking GluN2B-containing NMDA receptors in abGCs. We found that these mice exhibited an attenuated response to FLX in a neurogenesis-dependent behavioral assay of FLX action, while neurogenesis-independent behaviors were unaffected by GluN2B deletion. In addition, deletion of GluN2B attenuated FLX-induced increases in dendritic complexity of abGCs suggesting that the blunted behavioral efficacy of FLX may be caused by impaired differentiation of young abGCs.

Keywords: neurogenesis, dentate gyrus granule cells, GluN2B, synaptic plasticity, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI), antidepressants

Introduction

Adult hippocampal neurogenesis is a process that results in the generation of new neurons in the dentate gyrus (DG) throughout life (Gross, 2000; Kempermann et al., 2015). After their birth, newborn cells progress through different developmental stages that are marked by unique gene expression patterns, morphological features, and electrophysiological properties (Zhao et al., 2008; Li et al., 2009; Drew et al., 2013, 2015; Piatti et al., 2013). Treatment with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) such as fluoxetine (FLX) can stimulate all stages of adult hippocampal neurogenesis. Increases in proliferation and survival have been observed leading to an overall increase in the number of adult-born granule cells (abGCs) integrating into the hippocampal circuit (Malberg et al., 2000; Santarelli et al., 2003; Encinas et al., 2006; Wang et al., 2008; David et al., 2009). Maturation of these abGCs is enhanced, as is a neurogenesis-dependent form of long-term potentiation (LTP) in the DG evoked by medial perforant path stimulation under intact GABAergic tone (ACSF-LTP; Wang et al., 2008). The timeline of neurogenesis effects match the drug's delayed onset of therapeutic action (Malberg et al., 2000; Santarelli et al., 2003; Wang et al., 2008). Importantly, when adult hippocampal neurogenesis is ablated, some of the behavioral effects of chronic FLX treatment are lost, revealing that abGCs not only respond to this treatment on a cellular level, but also contribute to the antidepressant (AD) action of the drug (Santarelli et al., 2003; Airan et al., 2007; Wang et al., 2008; David et al., 2009). While ablation methods have demonstrated the necessity of abGCs in certain AD-related behaviors, these techniques remove all abGCs from the circuit and do not allow for further dissection of chronic FLX's numerous effects on adult hippocampal neurogenesis. An alternative approach to studying this process would be to target and alter specific properties of young abGCs without removing these cells from the hippocampal circuit in order to determine which of these properties are critical for their contribution to the therapeutic action of FLX.

During the maturation process, abGCs exhibit distinct properties that could underlie their ability to influence behavioral output (Zhao et al., 2008; Li et al., 2009; Deng et al., 2010; Denny et al., 2012; Drew et al., 2013, 2015; Piatti et al., 2013; Danielson et al., 2016). In the period 4–6 weeks after birth, abGCs are more plastic than their mature counterparts—they exhibit a lower threshold for LTP induction and larger LTP amplitude (Ge et al., 2007). GluN2B antagonists block this enhanced plasticity, demonstrating that this unique electrophysiological property of abGCs is mediated by GluN2B-containing NMDA receptors (Snyder et al., 2001; Ge et al., 2007). This enhanced plasticity can be seen on the population level as well, underlying ACSF-LTP in the DG, which can be eliminated with ablation of neurogenesis or GluN2B antagonists (Snyder et al., 2001; Saxe et al., 2006). Conversely, increasing neurogenesis genetically or with chronic AD treatment increases levels of ACSF-LTP (Wang et al., 2008; Sahay et al., 2011a). Thus, a potential mechanism by which abGCs may contribute to AD behavioral efficacy is by increasing the number of highly plastic units in the DG circuit. Previously, we showed that deletion of GluN2B from abGCs in the DG abolished ACSF-LTP and impaired contextual fear discrimination, but had no impact on anxiety or AD-like behaviors (Kheirbek et al., 2012). Here, using the same approach, we tested whether GluN2B-containing NMDA receptors impact the AD action of FLX. We show that deletion of the GluN2B subunit significantly attenuates a neurogenesis-dependent behavioral response to FLX, and additionally may block FLX's ability to enhance young abGCs' maturation and subsequent integration into the hippocampal network.

Materials and methods

Mice

Experimental mice were homozygous for a loxP-flanked Grin2b allele (GluN2Bf/f; von Engelhardt et al., 2008), homozygous for the ROSA26-STOP-floxed enhanced yellow fluorescent protein (EYFP) transgene (ROSA26fstopEYFP/fstopEYFP; Srinivas et al., 2001), and hemizygous for a NestinCreERT2 transgene (NCreERT2; Dranovsky et al., 2011). Grin2b, the gene coding for GluN2B, consists of 12 coding exons along with three non-coding exons located in the 5′-untranslated region (Klein et al., 1998). GluN2Bf/f mice have loxP sites surrounding exon 9 (the 6th coding exon) of Grin2b. In the presence of Cre, exon 9 gets excised. Western blotting performed using an antibody that targets the C-terminal domain downstream of exon 9 has shown decreased GluN2B expression following Cre recombination, demonstrating that excision of exon 9 results in the complete absence of GluN2B, not merely a truncated protein product (von Engelhardt et al., 2008). Ectopic expression has been reported in the NestinCreERT2 line and the impact this may have on our behavioral phenotype will be addressed in the discussion (Sun et al., 2014). Eight- to twelve-week-old mice were injected with tamoxifen (TMX, 3 mg dissolved in a solution of corn oil and 10% ethanol) or vehicle (corn oil/10% ethanol solution) intraperitoneally (IP) once per day for 5 consecutive days. Previous work with this NCreERT2 line did not reveal any significant sustained toxic effects of CreERT2 translocation to the nucleus in progenitor or daughter cells in the DG (Dranovsky et al., 2011; Kheirbek et al., 2012). Additionally, it has been shown that a brief pulse of TMX does not affect hippocampus-dependent behavior 6 weeks after administration of the drug (Sahay et al., 2011a). All experiments were approved and conducted in accordance to the guidelines of the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at Columbia University and the New York State Psychiatric Institute.

Drugs

For behavior and neurogenesis measures, AD treatment began ~6 weeks after the last TMX injection. 5-bromo-2′-deoxyuridine (BrdU) was injected (150 mg/kg, IP dissolved in saline) once per day for 2 days just prior to the start of AD treatment. For dendritic morphology analysis, AD treatment began ~5 weeks after TMX administration and BrdU (75 mg/kg, IP dissolved in saline) was injected 4 times over 8 h the day before AD treatment began. For all mice, FLX (18 mg/kg/day in water) or vehicle (VEH, water) was delivered by oral gavage and in the drinking water (160 mg/mL) at a schedule of 5 consecutive days gavage followed by 2 consecutive days of water, then repeated for the duration of treatment.

Behavioral testing

All behavioral experiments were conducted in male mice 18–24 weeks of age.

Novelty-suppressed feeding

In the novelty suppressed feeding (NSF) test, chronic AD treatment decreases latency to feed in the center of a novel arena after overnight food deprivation (Samuels and Hen, 2011). In mice with ablated neurogenesis, chronic AD treatment is no longer effective at lowering the latency to feed; therefore, NSF represents a neurogenesis-dependent behavioral assay of AD response (Santarelli et al., 2003; David et al., 2009). Mice were food restricted for 22–24 h and testing began when an animal was placed in the corner of a brightly lit (~1250 lux) plastic box (50 cm long × 28 cm wide × 15 cm deep) covered with the same type of bedding used in the animal's homecage. A single pellet of food (regular chow) had been positioned on a platform in the center of the box prior to the start of the test. The latency to eat (defined as the mouse sitting on its haunches and biting the pellet with the use of forepaws) was timed. Immediately after the latency was recorded, the food pellet was removed from the arena. Animals that did not eat within 10 min were censored during the statistical analysis. At the end of the session, animals were placed in their home cage and the amount of food consumed in 5 min was measured (home cage consumption). Each mouse was weighed before food deprivation and before testing to assess the percentage of body weight lost. Percent body weight lost and home cage consumption served as relative measures of animal hunger.

Tail suspension test

The tail suspension test (TST) is a behavioral despair test during which the mobility of mice suspended by their tail is measured. A depressed-like state is characterized by greater immobility while ADs are able to decrease immobility (Cryan et al., 2005). Sessions lasted 5 min and were video recorded. Scoring was done by an observer blind to induction and treatment.

Elevated plus maze

Mice were placed in the center of an elevated plus maze (EPM) consisting of both open and closed arms and were given 5 min to explore. The test was video recorded and activity was measured using TopScan software. Their preference for open (anxiogenic) or enclosed (anxiolytic) spaces was used to assess anxiety (Hogg, 1996).

Immunohistochemistry

For all experiments, mice were perfused (4% paraformaldehyde), brains postfixed, cryoprotected, and sections (35 μm) of the entire DG were labeled for BrdU, glial fibrillary acidic protein (GFAP), neuronal nuclei (NeuN), doublecortin (DCX), Ki-67, or green fluorescent protein (GFP) [rat-anti-BrdU, 1:100 (Serotec); rabbit-anti-GFAP, 1:1500 (DAKO); mouse-anti-NeuN, 1:500 (Millipore); goat-anti-DCX, 1:500 (Santa Cruz); rabbit-anti-Ki67, 1:100 (Vector); chicken-anti-GFP, 1:500 (Abcam)], as previously described (Scobie et al., 2009). An experimenter blind to induction and treatment counted Ki67+, BrdU+, and DCX+ cells, as well as DCX+ cells exhibiting tertiary dendrites, in every sixth section throughout the DG. For colabeling of BrdU with NeuN or GFAP, confocal scans (FluoView1000; Olympus) at 40 × were taken of BrdU+ cells across the anteroposterior axis of the DG. For the morphological analysis of immature neurons, z-stack images of BrdU+/DCX+ cells were traced and imported into Adobe Illustrator CS5 where neurons were reconstructed using the tracing tool. Images of the reconstructed neurons were opened in Fiji (http://fiji.sc/Fiji) where dendritic length was measured using the freehand trace tool and Sholl analysis was conducted using the Sholl analysis plug-in with parameters previously described (Sahay et al., 2011a).

Statistical analysis

Statistical significance was assessed by ANOVA or, in the case of planned comparisons, by unpaired two-tailed Student's t-test using Statview Software. Results in figures are mean ± SEM. Since many mice did not eat before the 10-min cutoff in the NSF test, information about their latency to feed was incomplete and these observations were censored. The Cox proportional hazards model was used to analyze this data so that we could both correctly account for the censored data points and evaluate the effect of multiple variables on survival (Hosmer et al., 2008). SAS Software was used to perform this analysis. A complete statistical summary for all experiments is included in Tables 1, 2.

Table 1.

Statistical analysis of the behavioral data.

Behavioral Test N Measurement Statistical test Factor Statistic p-value Figures
Novelty suppressed feeding 15–18 mice/group Hazard ratio Cox proportional hazards model Induction χ(1)2 = 1.4807 0.2237 Figure 2
Treatment χ(1)2 = 21.7498 <0.0001
Interaction χ(1)2 = 5.3018 0.0213
Subgroup treatment effects CTRL mice Treatment χ(1)2 = 21.7498 <0.0001 Figure 2A
iGluN2BNes mice Treatment χ(1)2 = 5.1273 0.0236 Figure 2A
Homecage consumption 2-way ANOVA Induction F(1, 61) = 0.375 0.5424 Figure 2B
Treatment F(1, 61) = 1.305 0.2578
Interaction F(1, 61) = 0.115 0.7357
% Weight change 2-way ANOVA Induction F(1, 61) = 0.196 0.6596 Figure 2B
Treatment F(1, 61) = 0.014 0.9047
Interaction F(1, 61) = 0.023 0.8811
Elevated plus maze 15–18 mice/group Open arm time 2-way ANOVA Induction F(1, 61) = 0.098 0.7555 Figure 3A
Treatment F(1, 61) = 0.643 0.4258
Interaction F(1, 61) = 0.031 0.8610
Open arm entries 2-way ANOVA Induction F(1, 61) = 0.966 0.3295 Figure 3B
Treatment F(1, 61) = 0.575 0.4511
Interaction F(1, 61) = 0.082 0.7756
Open arm distance 2-way ANOVA Induction F(1, 61) = 0.358 0.5518 Figure 3C
Treatment F(1, 61) = 0.214 0.6457
Interaction F(1, 61) < 0.0001 0.9989
Total Distance 2-way ANOVA Induction F(1, 61) = 0.437 0.5112 Figure 3D
Treatment F(1, 61) = 2.947 0.0911
Interaction F(1, 61) = 0.605 0.4398
Tail Suspension Test 15–18 mice/group Immobility 2-way repeated measures ANOVA Induction F(1, 60) = 0.346 0.5585 Figure 3E
Treatment F(1, 60) = 1.982 0.1644
Interaction F(1, 60) = 0.001 0.9706
Minute F(4, 240) = 114.851 <0.0001
Minute × induction F(4, 240) = 0.386 0.8187
Minute × treatment F(4, 240) = 4.136 0.0029
Minute × induction × treatment F(4, 240) = 0.124 0.9737
Immobility (last 2 min) 2-way ANOVA Induction F(1, 60) = 0.023 0.8809 Figure 3F
Treatment F(1, 60) = 10.402 0.0020
Interaction F(1, 60) = 0.027 0.8692
Planned Comparison t-test CTRL mice Treatment t(29) = 2.389 0.0236
iGluN2BNes mice Treatment t(31) = 2.174 0.0375

p < 0.05 are highlighted in bold.

Table 2.

Statistical analysis of neurogenesis measures.

Immunos N Measurement Statistical test Factor Statistic p-value Figures
Ki-67 10–15 mice/group Proliferation 2-way ANOVA Induction F(1, 46) = 0.103 0.7492 Figure 4B
Treatment F(1, 46) = 0.250 0.6198
Interaction F(1, 46) = 0.127 0.7231
BrdU 14–16 mice/group Survival 2-way ANOVA Induction F(1, 54) = 7.897 0.0069 Figure 4C
Treatment F(1, 54) = 25.980 <0.0001
Interaction F(1, 54) = 0.002 0.9615
Planned Comparison t-test CTRL mice Treatment t(26) = −4.472 0.0001
iGluN2BNes mice Treatment t(28) = −3.143 0.0039
BrdU/NeuN/GFAP 3 mice/group 19–39 cells/mouse %BrdU+/NeuN+ 2-way ANOVA Induction F(1, 8) = 0.060 0.8133 Figure 4D
Treatment F(1, 8) = 7.332 0.0268
Interaction F(1, 8) = 0.439 0.5261
Planned Comparison t-test CTRLmice Treatment t(4) = −0.870 0.4334
iGluN2BNes mice Treatment t(4) = −2.770 0.0503
%BrdU+/GFAP+ 2-way ANOVA Induction F(1, 8) = 0.001 0.0775
Treatment F(1, 8) = 4.715 0.0617
Interaction F(1, 8) = 0.971 0.3534
Planned Comparison t-test CTRLmice Treatment t(4) = 1.226 0.2874
iGluN2BNes mice Treatment t(4) = 2.292 0.0837
DCX 8–16 mice/group Immature Neuron Number 2-way ANOVA Induction F(1, 41) = 0.125 0.7253 Figure 4F
Treatment F(1, 41) = 6.851 0.0124
Interaction F(1, 41) = 0.578 0.4514
Planned Comparison t-test CTRLmice Treatment t(20) = −2.909 0.0087
iGluN2BNes mice Treatment t(21) = −1.141 0.2667
6 mice/group Tertiary Dendrites 2-way ANOVA Induction F(1, 20) = 0.532 0.4742 Figure 4G
Treatment F(1, 20) = 6.356 0.0203
Interaction F(1, 20) = 0.511 0.4832
Planned Comparison t-test CTRLmice Treatment t(10) = −2.022 0.0708
iGluN2BNes mice Treatment t(10) = −1.506 0.1629
BrdU/DCX 3 mice/group 2–5 cells/ mouse Dendritic length 2-way ANOVA Induction F(1, 34) = 1.724 0.1980 Figure 4H
Treatment F(1, 34) = 2.222 0.1453
Interaction F(1, 34) = 0.995 0.3256
Planned Comparison t-test CTRLmice Treatment t(15) = −1.739 0.1025
iGluN2BNes mice Treatment t(19) = −0.358 0.7246
Sholl Analysis Repeated measures ANOVA—FLX treated mice Induction F(1, 18) = 2.152 0.1596 Figure 4I
Radius F(20, 360) = 24.446 <0.0001
Radius × Induction F(20, 360) = 1.174 0.2742
t-test radius 70 Induction t(18) = 3.760 0.0014
radius 80 Induction t(18) = 2.202 0.0410
Repeated measures ANOVA—VEH treated mice Induction F(1, 16) = 0.117 0.7363 Figure 4J
Radius F(20, 320) = 16.139 <0.0001
Radius × Induction F(20, 320) = 0.187 >0.9999

p < 0.05 are highlighted in bold.

Results

In order to examine the impact young abGCs' enhanced plasticity has on behavioral and neurogenic responses to chronic AD treatment, we specifically deleted the GluN2B subunit from those neurons. To both spatially and temporally control the deletion of GluN2B, we employed a double-transgenic mouse model in which mice express TMX-regulated Cre-recombinase under the control of the Nestin promoter (NCreERT2), while an encoding region immediately preceding the first transmembrane domain of GluN2B is flanked by loxP sites (GluN2Bf/f, Figure 1A). To label cells that underwent recombination, mice were further bred to an inducible EYFP reporter line (ROSA26fstopEYFP/fstopEYFP). TMX injection in adult mice leads to deletion of GluN2B and expression of YFP in neural precursor cells (NPCs) and their subsequent progeny (iGluN2BNes, Figure 1A). Littermates injected with vehicle served as our controls (CTRL, Figure 1A). We have previously characterized this mouse line and found that 6 weeks after TMX treatment, ~70% of immature, DCX+ granule cells in iGluN2BNes mice expressed EYFP and therefore were born from NPCs that have undergone recombination and presumably lack GluN2B (Kheirbek et al., 2012). This was confirmed electrophysiologically as slices from iGluN2BNes mice lack GluN2B-dependent ACSF-LTP (Kheirbek et al., 2012). Chronic FLX treatment began 6 weeks after TMX or vehicle administration in order to determine the effect of ADs in mice whose young abGC population lacks GluN2B (Figure 1B).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Experimental Design. (A) Genetic design for the deletion of GluN2B from abGCs. TMX administration into NCreERT2 × GluN2Bf/f × ROSA26fstopEYFP/fstopEYFP mice eliminates the GluN2B subunit from NPCs and all of their progeny, while EYFP labels cells that underwent recombination thus acting as a surrogate for GluN2B deletion. Representative 10X images show cells are restricted to the lower portion of the granule cell layer near the subgranular zone, consistent with the expression pattern of immature abGCs (scale bar = 100 μm). (B) Experimental timeline: Adult NCreERT2 × GluN2Bf/f × ROSA26fstopEYFP/fstopEYFP mice were injected with either TMX (iGluN2BNes) or vehicle (CTRL). FLX or VEH treatment began 6 weeks later. Behavioral testing began after 4 weeks of chronic AD treatment. For dendritic complexity analysis, FLX or VEH treatment began 5 weeks after induction and mice were sacrificed after 3 weeks of AD treatment.

Four weeks after beginning chronic FLX treatment, iGluN2BNes mice were tested in anxiety and AD-like behavioral assays. Mice were tested in the NSF test, in which AD efficacy has been shown to require adult hippocampal neurogenesis (Santarelli et al., 2003; David et al., 2009). Analysis of latency to feed in this test revealed a significant interaction between induction and treatment leading us to look at comparisons across induction group (Table 1; see also Section Materials and Methods for a more detailed description of the statistical analysis used to analyze this data). FLX robustly lowered the latency to feed in the novel arena in CTRL mice with mice on FLX ~11 times more likely to eat at a given time than VEH mice (Figure 2A). For our iGluN2BNes group, FLX still significantly lowered the latency to feed in the novel arena, but the effect was more modest with mice receiving the drug only ~ 2.5 times more likely to eat at a given time than VEH mice (Figure 2B). For convenience we have also presented these data in a bar graph format (see insets in Figure 2A and corresponding legend). The observed differences in latency to feed were not due to changes in appetite as all groups exhibited similar levels of home cage food consumption and weight loss during the overnight food deprivation (Figure 2B). This indicates that loss of GluN2B from abGCs causes a blunting of the FLX effect in a neurogenesis-dependent behavioral assay of AD response.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Deletion of GluN2B attenuates the decrease in latency to feed in the NSF test after chronic FLX treatment. (A) Cumulative survival plots comparing CTRL mice (left) or iGluN2BNes mice (right) treated with VEH or FLX. FLX significantly lowered the latency to feed in each group, with the greatest effect between treatment groups seen in CTRL mice. Insets show the average latency to feed in the novel arena, with those mice not feeding within the time limit of the test assigned a latency of 600 s. (B) No significant difference was observed in home cage consumption (left) or % weight change (right). HR, Hazard Ratio. *p < 0.05, #p < 0.0001.

We also determined whether FLX treatment or GluN2B deletion altered anxiety-related behavior in the EPM. All groups showed similar levels of open arm exploration as measured by time spent in the open arms, open arm entries, and distance traveled in the open arms (Figures 3A–C). Furthermore, total exploration of the maze was similar for all the animals (Figure 3D). Together this shows that neither AD treatment nor GluN2B deletion altered anxiety-like behavior in this test.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

GluN2B deletion did not impact anxiety-related behavior in the EPM, nor did it affect FLX's ability to lower immobility in the TST. In the EPM, no difference among groups was observed in open arm exploration as measured by (A) time spent in the open arms, (B) number of open arm entries, or (C) distance traveled in the open arms. (D) The total distance traveled in the maze did not differ among groups. (E) In the TST, VEH-treated mice display increasingly higher levels of immobility as the test progresses, while FLX reduced immobility over time. (F) GluN2B deletion did not impact FLX's ability to lower immobility during the last 2 min of the test. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.

Next, we tested the mice for FLX effects in the TST, a neurogenesis-independent behavioral assay of AD response (David et al., 2009). Mice on FLX were less immobile over time than VEH-treated mice (Figure 3E) and spent significantly less time immobile during the final 2 min of the test compared to VEH-treated mice with no significant interaction between induction and treatment (Figure 3F). This indicates that iGluN2BNes and CTRL mice respond similarly to FLX in a neurogenesis-independent assay of AD response.

We then assessed the neurogenic effects of FLX in iGluN2BNes and CTRL mice. The number of proliferating Ki-67+ cells did not significantly differ among groups, revealing that neither GluN2B deletion nor FLX treatment altered the number of new cells being produced at the time of sacrifice (Figure 4B). To assess the effect of FLX on survival, mice were injected with BrdU just prior to the start of AD treatment and surviving cells were counted 6 weeks later. Here, we found that the overall number of BrdU+ cells was lower after GluN2B deletion; however, FLX still increased survival in both CTRL and iGluN2BNes mice (Figures 4A,C). To determine the phenotype of BrdU+ cells, tissue sections were triple labeled for BrdU, NeuN (a neuronal marker), and GFAP (a glial marker). As expected, the majority of BrdU+ cells co-labeled with NeuN, though FLX treatment slightly increased the proportion of cells that were neuronal (Figure 4D). DCX, which is transiently expressed in newly generated neurons, was measured to quantify the number of immature neurons in the DG (Brown et al., 2003; Couillard-Despres et al., 2005). While FLX increased the number of DCX+ cells in CTRL mice, this effect was blunted in iGluN2BNes mice (Figures 4E,F). DCX+ cells undergo significant morphological changes as they mature. To determine if our manipulations altered this maturation process we counted the subset of DCX+ cells that had reached a more mature stage of development as exhibited by the presence of tertiary dendrites. FLX increased the number of DCX+ cells with tertiary dendrites in CTRL mice, but this effect was less pronounced in iGluN2BNes mice (Figure 4G). Finally, we measured the effect of FLX on dendritic structure by analyzing the dendrites of 3-week-old DCX+ cells. Once again, we found that FLX increased dendritic length and complexity in CTRL mice, but not in iGluN2BNes mice (Figures 4H–K).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

FLX increased dendritic complexity in CTRL but not iGluN2BNes mice. (A) Representative images of BrdU+ cells in the DG (scale bar = 100 μm) (B) No effect on proliferation was observed as measured by number of Ki-67+ cells. (C) FLX increased survival of abGCs in both CTRL and iGluN2BNes mice, though overall survival was lower in iGluN2BNes mice. (D) The majority of BrdU+ cells co-labeled with the neuronal marker, NeuN. FLX treatment increased the percentage of BrdU+ cells that were neuronal. (E) Representative images of DCX expression in the DG (scale bar = 100 μm). (F) The number of immature neurons and (G) immature neurons with tertiary dendrites increased following FLX treatment with the most robust effect seen in CTRL mice. (H) There was a trend for FLX to increase total dendritic length of 3-week-old DCX+ abGCs in CTRL mice, but not iGluN2BNes mice. (I) Sholl analysis revealed FLX increased dendritic complexity in CTRL mice, but not iGluN2BNes mice. (J) GluN2B deletion alone did not affect the dendritic complexity of 3-week-old DCX+ abGCs. (K) Representative tracings of 3-week-old abGCs (scale bar = 20 μm). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, #p < 0.0001.

To determine if abGC maturation correlated with the behavioral response to FLX, we compared the number of DCX+ cells with tertiary dendrites in FLX-treated mice to outcomes in our FLX-responsive behavioral tests. We found a trend for greater tertiary dendrite number to correlate with a lower latency to feed in the NSF test (Figure 5A) in FLX-treated mice. When looking at the TST, which has been suggested to be independent of levels of neurogenesis (David et al., 2009), higher tertiary dendrite numbers were not predictive of a larger FLX response (Figure 5B). Tertiary dendrites were not related to behavioral outcomes in VEH-treated mice in either test (Figures 5A,B).

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Trend for dendritic complexity in FLX-treated mice to correlate with behavioral response in the neurogenesis-dependent NSF test. (A) Higher numbers of immature neurons with tertiary dendrites tended to correlate with a lower latency to feed in NSF in FLX-treated mice (right) but not those mice that received VEH (left). (B) Similar correlations were not evident in the neurogenesis-independent TST. Tertiary dendrite counts were obtained for 6 mice/group (see Table 2). For the NSF correlations, any mouse that had been censored was excluded from the correlations. For the TST correlations, 1 VEH-treated iGluN2BNes mouse was not included because he had to be removed from the TST for hanging onto his tail.

Discussion

In this study, we have determined the consequence of altering young neurons' physiology on FLX efficacy without eliminating the young neurons themselves. By conditionally deleting the GluN2B subunit from abGCs of mice prior to the onset of AD administration, we revealed a blunting of the behavioral response to chronic FLX treatment. This suggests that eliminating abGCs' enhanced plasticity decreases their ability to influence DG output resulting in an AD response that is less robust than the response seen in CTRL mice. Control experiments revealed the specificity of this effect as GluN2B deletion did not impact the effect of FLX in a neurogenesis-independent behavioral assay of AD response (TST) or in an assay of anxiety that was insensitive to FLX in this strain of mice (EPM). While some studies in rodents do find that chronic AD treatment results in an anxiolytic phenotype in the EPM (Kurt et al., 2000; Bondi et al., 2008; David et al., 2009; Venzala et al., 2012; Samuels et al., 2015), this effect is not consistently seen (Durand et al., 1999; File et al., 1999; Griebel et al., 1999; Silva and Brandao, 2000; Borsini et al., 2002; Bondi et al., 2008; Oh et al., 2009; Venzala et al., 2012; Baek et al., 2015). This may be due to the manner in which different strains exhibit anxiety behavior in this test (Ducottet and Belzung, 2005) or the variability in testing conditions (Hogg, 1996).

The results presented here also revealed differences in the neurogenic effects of FLX in mice lacking GluN2B in abGCs. Interestingly, FLX effects on dendritic complexity were attenuated in iGluN2BNes mice, which also lack the full behavioral response to FLX. Our analysis indicated a trend for FLX-induced increases in dendritic complexity to be predictive of behavioral efficacy in the NSF test (Figure 5A). These results suggest that manipulating young neurons' plasticity attenuates FLX's ability to facilitate maturation thereby impacting the behavioral response. This supports the hypothesis that chronic FLX's efficacy may be dependent on accelerating the dendritic development of abGCs as they differentiate, which would serve to increase the number of young abGCs that are functionally integrated into the DG circuit (Wang et al., 2008).

The mechanism by which GluN2B deletion impairs the FLX-induced dendritic changes in young abGCs is not yet known, but one possibility is that it is through a brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF)-dependent mechanism. Chronic SSRIs have been shown to increase BDNF in the DG (Nibuya et al., 1996; Samuels et al., 2015) and this effect has been shown to be reduced in mice that do not respond to SSRIs (Samuels et al., 2014, 2015). Mice with reduced BDNF signaling show impaired behavioral and neurogenic responses to AD treatment (Saarelainen et al., 2003; Monteggia et al., 2004; Sairanen et al., 2005). In addition, mice with abGCs lacking the BDNF receptor, tropomyosin receptor kinase B (TrkB), show reduced dendritic maturation, impaired ACSF-LTP, and lack behavioral and neurogenic responses to chronic AD-treatment (Bergami et al., 2008; Donovan et al., 2008; Li et al., 2008). These effects on survival, dendritic complexity, LTP, and AD sensitivity are quite similar to the ones observed in iGluN2BNes mice at baseline or following chronic FLX treatment suggesting a link between our manipulation of GluN2B expression and BDNF signaling. BDNF signaling can upregulate the mRNA and protein expression of NMDA receptor subunits, increase trafficking of NMDA receptors to the cell membrane, and enhance the activity of NMDA receptors via phosphorylation (Lin et al., 1998; Slack et al., 2004; Caldeira et al., 2007). Through this enhancement of NMDA receptor activity, BDNF may potentiate activity-dependent development, while the elimination of GluN2B-containing NMDA receptors would likely limit the extent to which BDNF could regulate this process. An alternative possibility is that NMDA receptor signaling can positively modulate the BDNF signaling cascade (Zafra et al., 1991; Springer et al., 1994), which could then act in an autocrine manner to activate TrkB receptors on abGCs and impact dendritic complexity.

Our analysis of neurogenesis revealed two other surprising results. First, we found that chronic FLX treatment did not increase proliferation in either CTRL or iGluN2BNes mice. This is in contrast to the many studies that have observed a link between chronic AD treatment and increased proliferation (reviewed in Hanson et al., 2011). This is likely due to the mixed background of the mice used in the present study, which includes the C57BL/6 strain. C57 mice have high levels of proliferation at baseline (Kempermann et al., 1997a) and do not display an increase in proliferation in response to FLX in the absence of prior stress (David et al., 2009).

Second, we found that the survival of abGCs in iGluN2BNes mice was lower than in CTRL mice, which was not seen in our previous study (Kheirbek et al., 2012). The present study differed in that the mice used here were subject to oral gavage 5 days of every week, which leads to prolonged increases in corticosterone levels (Dalm et al., 2008). One possibility is that the mice in this study may have only become sensitive to the influence GluN2B deletion has on survival with the addition of this chronic stressor. Despite the overall lower survival seen in iGluN2BNes mice, FLX was still effective at increasing survival relative to VEH-treated animals. It is interesting that the stress from oral gavage was sufficient to reveal the action of FLX on survival, but not proliferation. However, survival and proliferation are regulated by distinct mechanisms, and as such it is possible to affect one and not the other. For example, environmental enrichment and hippocampus-dependent learning tasks increase cell survival without effecting proliferation (Kempermann et al., 1997b; Gould et al., 1999). Also, an increase in survival with no effect on proliferation was found in C57BL/6 mice that had received chronic FLX treatment via daily IP injections and were thus exposed to a comparable amount of stress as our mice (Couillard-Despres et al., 2009). Examination of the fate of surviving BrdU+ cells showed that the percentage of BrdU+ cells that co-labeled with NeuN increased with FLX treatment, mimicking effects on cell fate seen after enrichment (Dranovsky et al., 2011).

It should be noted that the SGZ is not the only neurogenic region in the adult mouse brain (Zhao et al., 2008). Using this particular genetic approach means that GluN2B will also be deleted form newborn neurons in the subventricular zone (Grubb et al., 2008). However, ablation of adult-born olfactory interneurons achieved using a NCreERT2 mouse line similar to the one used here did not significantly affect olfaction (Imayoshi et al., 2008). Innate olfactory responses and odor discrimination were intact as was the mouse's ability to acquire and retain odor-associated memory. Thus, it seems unlikely that the behavioral paradigms used in this study would be affected by the deletion of GluN2B from adult-born olfactory interneurons.

An additional caveat is that recombination also occurs ectopically outside of the neurogenic areas with our NCreERT2 mouse line (Sun et al., 2014). The most prominent ectopic recombination occurred in the cerebellum (where GluN2B mRNA is not expressed in adult mice; Monyer et al., 1994). The other areas with significant recombination were the CA1 and CA3 regions of hippocampus, thus some effects of GluN2B deletion may be due to loss of the subunit in these areas. However, our behavioral results in the NSF, EPM, and TST tests, phenocopies mice with targeted ablation of hippocampal neurogenesis, further linking the behavioral phenotype we observe to our manipulation of GluN2B in abGCs (Wu and Hen, 2014). In the future, retroviral approaches or local delivery of TMX will provide more targeted methods for localizing behavioral effects to abGCs.

It is particularly interesting that eliminating young neurons' enhanced plasticity does not completely block the FLX actions that are considered to be neurogenesis-dependent. This may be due to the fact that iGluN2BNes mice do not entirely lack FLX-induced increases in levels of neurogenesis, only the ability to add highly plastic units to the DG circuit. Alternatively, the remaining effects of ADs may be independent of neurogenesis. FLX has shown some residual behavioral effects following ablation similar to the attenuation in FLX efficacy we see in our iGluN2BNes mice (Wu and Hen, 2014). There is also accumulating evidence that mature granule cells in the DG are involved in the effects of ADs too (Samuels et al., 2015).

Possibly linking these two hypotheses, young abGCs have been suggested to modulate the activity of mature GCs within the DG. In the absence of neurogenesis, activity in the DG increases (Burghardt et al., 2012; Lacefield et al., 2012; Ikrar et al., 2013), whereas increasing levels of neurogenesis or stimulating young abGCs decreases DG activity (Ikrar et al., 2013; Drew et al., 2015). While the exact mechanism for this is unknown, it has been hypothesized that young abGCs may differentially modulate the local DG circuits (Sahay et al., 2011b; Lacefield et al., 2012). Our immunohistochemical analysis indicates that abGCs in iGluN2BNes mice still survive and mature into neurons, and thus may still be capable of influencing the hippocampal network in a manner that does not require enhanced plasticity. Future experiments will be required to determine whether these cells lacking GluN2B can impact local and downstream circuits like control cells.

While we observe an effect of GluN2B deletion in our study, it is possible that the same manipulation would not impact behavior in middle-aged or aged mice given that they display lower levels of neurogenesis than the young-adult mice used here (Hamilton et al., 2013). This consideration highlights the impact different experimental parameters can have on the study of neurogenesis. Differences in age, as well as strain, species, and ablation or enhancement techniques used likely underlie the conflicting reports in the literature in relation to adult hippocampal neurogenesis' function. For example, some studies show that reducing neurogenesis enhances anxiety (Revest et al., 2009; Snyder et al., 2011), while others find the opposite result with lower levels of neurogenesis associated with less anxiety (Fuss et al., 2010; Groves et al., 2013). Our behavioral results are in line with findings indicating that neurogenesis does not impact anxiety or antidepressant-like behavior at baseline, but does contribute to the behavioral response to antidepressants (Santarelli et al., 2003; Airan et al., 2007; Sahay and Hen, 2007; Surget et al., 2008; David et al., 2009; Sahay et al., 2011a).

Considering neurogenesis' impact on mood raises the question of how changes within the DG can influence downstream circuitry relevant for stress and anxiety. Recent findings suggest the hippocampus may be functionally segregated along the dorsoventral axis as a result of regional variation in anatomical connectivity (Fanselow and Dong, 2010; Kheirbek et al., 2013; Tannenholz et al., 2014). Targeting ablation of neurogenesis to the dorsal or ventral DG revealed that dorsal abGCs are required for learning a contextual fear discrimination task, whereas ventral abGCs were necessary for the anxiolytic/AD effects of FLX in the NSF test (Wu and Hen, 2014). While our manipulation targeted abGCs along the entire axis of the DG, it would be interesting to further dissect the role of this unique form of plasticity by specifically targeting ablation of GluN2B to either the dorsal or ventral DG.

Our results reveal that adult neurogenesis, and in particular the unique electrophysiological properties of young abGCs, contributes to the efficacy of AD treatment. These experiments, along with future studies aimed at further understanding the ways in which abGCs participate in ADs' mechanism of action, may help uncover novel avenues for therapeutic interventions.

Author contributions

LT, RH, and MK designed the experiments and wrote the paper. LT performed the experiments and analyzed the data.

Funding

LT was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (F31 MH100842). RH is supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (R37 MH068542 [MERIT]), the National Institute on Aging (R01 AG043688), the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (R01NS081203), NYSTEM (C029157), and the Hope for Depression Research Foundation (RGA 11-024). MK was supported by a NIH grant (K01MH099371), a NARSAD Young Investigator Award from the Brain & Behavior Research Foundation, and NYSTEM (C029157).

Conflict of interest statement

RH receives compensation as a consultant for Roche and Lundbeck. The other authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

  1. Airan R. D., Meltzer L. A., Roy M., Gong Y., Chen H., Deisseroth K. (2007). High-speed imaging reveals neurophysiological links to behavior in an animal model of depression. Science 317, 819–823. 10.1126/science.1144400 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Baek I. S., Park J. Y., Han P. L. (2015). Chronic antidepressant treatment in normal mice induces anxiety and impairs stress-coping ability. Exp. Neurobiol. 24, 156–168. 10.5607/en.2015.24.2.156 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bergami M., Rimondini R., Santi S., Blum R., Gotz M., Canossa M. (2008). Deletion of TrkB in adult progenitors alters newborn neuron integration into hippocampal circuits and increases anxiety-like behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 15570–15575. 10.1073/pnas.0803702105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bondi C. O., Rodriguez G., Gould G. G., Frazer A., Morilak D. A. (2008). Chronic unpredictable stress induces a cognitive deficit and anxiety-like behavior in rats that is prevented by chronic antidepressant drug treatment. Neuropsychopharmacology 33, 320–331. 10.1038/sj.npp.1301410 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Borsini F., Podhorna J., Marazziti D. (2002). Do animal models of anxiety predict anxiolytic-like effects of antidepressants? Psychopharmacology (Berl). 163, 121–141. 10.1007/s00213-002-1155-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brown J. P., Couillard-Despres S., Cooper-Kuhn C. M., Winkler J., Aigner L., Kuhn H. G. (2003). Transient expression of doublecortin during adult neurogenesis. J. Comp. Neurol. 467, 1–10. 10.1002/cne.10874 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Burghardt N. S., Park E. H., Hen R., Fenton A. A. (2012). Adult-born hippocampal neurons promote cognitive flexibility in mice. Hippocampus 22, 1795–1808. 10.1002/hipo.22013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Caldeira M. V., Melo C. V., Pereira D. B., Carvalho R. F., Carvalho A. L., Duarte C. B. (2007). BDNF regulates the expression and traffic of NMDA receptors in cultured hippocampal neurons. Mol. Cell. Neurosci. 35, 208–219. 10.1016/j.mcn.2007.02.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Couillard-Despres S., Winner B., Schaubeck S., Aigner R., Vroemen M., Weidner N., et al. (2005). Doublecortin expression levels in adult brain reflect neurogenesis. Eur. J. Neurosci. 21, 1–14. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03813.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Couillard-Despres S., Wuertinger C., Kandasamy M., Caioni M., Stadler K., Aigner R., et al. (2009). Ageing abolishes the effects of fluoxetine on neurogenesis. Mol. Psychiatry 14, 856–864. 10.1038/mp.2008.147 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cryan J. F., Mombereau C., Vassout A. (2005). The tail suspension test as a model for assessing antidepressant activity: review of pharmacological and genetic studies in mice. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29, 571–625. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.03.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Dalm S., Brinks V., van der Mark M. H., de Kloet E. R., Oitzl M. S. (2008). Non-invasive stress-free application of glucocorticoid ligands in mice. J. Neurosci. Methods 170, 77–84. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.12.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Danielson N. B., Kaifosh P., Zaremba J. D., Lovett-Barron M., Tsai J., Denny C. A., et al. (2016). Distinct contribution of adult-born hippocampal granule cells to context encoding. Neuron 90, 101–112. 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.02.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. David D. J., Samuels B. A., Rainer Q., Wang J.-W. W., Marsteller D., Mendez I., et al. (2009). Neurogenesis-dependent and -independent effects of fluoxetine in an animal model of anxiety/depression. Neuron 62, 479–493. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.04.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Deng W., Aimone J. B., Gage F. H. (2010). New neurons and new memories: how does adult hippocampal neurogenesis affect learning and memory? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 339–350. 10.1038/nrn2822 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Denny C. A., Burghardt N. S., Schachter D. M., Hen R., Drew M. R. (2012). 4- to 6-week-old adult-born hippocampal neurons influence novelty-evoked exploration and contextual fear conditioning. Hippocampus 22, 1188–1201. 10.1002/hipo.20964 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Donovan M. H., Yamaguchi M., Eisch A. J. (2008). Dynamic expression of TrkB receptor protein on proliferating and maturing cells in the adult mouse dentate gyrus. Hippocampus 18, 435–439. 10.1002/hipo.20410 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dranovsky A., Picchini A. M., Moadel T., Sisti A. C., Yamada A., Kimura S., et al. (2011). Experience dictates stem cell fate in the adult hippocampus. Neuron 70, 908–923. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Drew L. J., Fusi S., Hen R. (2013). Adult neurogenesis in the mammalian hippocampus: why the dentate gyrus? Learn. Mem. 20, 710–729. 10.1101/lm.026542.112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Drew L. J., Kheirbek M. A., Luna V. M., Denny C. A., Cloidt M. A., Wu M. V., et al. (2015). Activation of local inhibitory circuits in the dentate gyrus by adult-born neurons. Hippocampus. 10.1002/hipo.22557 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Ducottet C., Belzung C. (2005). Correlations between behaviours in the elevated plus-maze and sensitivity to unpredictable subchronic mild stress: evidence from inbred strains of mice. Behav. Brain Res. 156, 153–162. 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.05.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Durand M., Berton O., Aguerre S., Edno L., Combourieu I., Mormede P., et al. (1999). Effects of repeated fluoxetine on anxiety-related behaviours, central serotonergic systems, and the corticotropic axis axis in SHR and WKY rats. Neuropharmacology 38, 893–907. 10.1016/S0028-3908(99)00009-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Encinas J. M., Vaahtokari A., Enikolopov G. (2006). Fluoxetine targets early progenitor cells in the adult brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 8233–8238. 10.1073/pnas.0601992103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Fanselow M. S., Dong H. W. (2010). Are the dorsal and ventral hippocampus functionally distinct structures? Neuron 65, 7–19. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. File S. E., Ouagazzal A. M., Gonzalez L. E., Overstreet D. H. (1999). Chronic fluoxetine in tests of anxiety in rat lines selectively bred for differential 5-HT1A receptor function. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 62, 695–701. 10.1016/S0091-3057(98)00208-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Fuss J., Ben Abdallah N. M., Vogt M. A., Touma C., Pacifici P. G., Palme R., et al. (2010). Voluntary exercise induces anxiety-like behavior in adult C57BL/6J mice correlating with hippocampal neurogenesis. Hippocampus 20, 364–376. 10.1002/hipo.20634 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Ge S., Yang C. H., Hsu K. S., Ming G. L., Song H. (2007). A critical period for enhanced synaptic plasticity in newly generated neurons of the adult brain. Neuron 54, 559–566. 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.05.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Gould E., Beylin A., Tanapat P., Reeves A., Shors T. J. (1999). Learning enhances adult neurogenesis in the hippocampal formation. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 260–265. 10.1038/6365 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Griebel G., Cohen C., Perrault G., Sanger D. J. (1999). Behavioral effects of acute and chronic fluoxetine in Wistar-Kyoto rats. Physiol. Behav. 67, 315–320. 10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00298-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Gross C. G. (2000). Neurogenesis in the adult brain: death of a dogma. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 1, 67–73. 10.1038/35036235 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Groves J. O., Leslie I., Huang G. J., McHugh S. B., Taylor A., Mott R., et al. (2013). Ablating adult neurogenesis in the rat has no effect on spatial processing: evidence from a novel pharmacogenetic model. PLoS Genet. 9:e1003718. 10.1371/journal.pgen.1003718 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Grubb M. S., Nissant A., Murray K., Lledo P. M. (2008). Functional maturation of the first synapse in olfaction: development and adult neurogenesis. J. Neurosci. 28, 2919–2932. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5550-07.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hamilton L. K., Joppe S. E., Cochard L. M., Fernandes K. J. (2013). Aging and neurogenesis in the adult forebrain: what we have learned and where we should go from here. Eur. J. Neurosci. 37, 1978–1986. 10.1111/ejn.12207 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hanson N. D., Owens M. J., Nemeroff C. B. (2011). Depression, antidepressants, and neurogenesis: a critical reappraisal. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 2589–2602. 10.1038/npp.2011.220 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hogg S. (1996). A review of the validity and variability of the elevated plus-maze as an animal model of anxiety. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 54, 21–30. 10.1016/0091-3057(95)02126-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hosmer D. W., Lemeshow S., May S. (2008). Applied Survival Analysis: Regression Modeling of Time-To-Event Data. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Interscience. [Google Scholar]
  37. Ikrar T., Guo N., He K., Besnard A., Levinson S., Hill A., et al. (2013). Adult neurogenesis modifies excitability of the dentate gyrus. Front. Neural Circuits 7:204. 10.3389/fncir.2013.00204 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Imayoshi I., Sakamoto M., Ohtsuka T., Takao K., Miyakawa T., Yamaguchi M., et al. (2008). Roles of continuous neurogenesis in the structural and functional integrity of the adult forebrain. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 1153–1161. 10.1038/nn.2185 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kempermann G., Kuhn H. G., Gage F. H. (1997a). Genetic influence on neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of adult mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 94, 10409–10414. 10.1073/pnas.94.19.10409 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kempermann G., Kuhn H. G., Gage F. H. (1997b). More hippocampal neurons in adult mice living in an enriched environment. Nature 386, 493–495. 10.1038/386493a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kempermann G., Song H., Gage F. H. (2015). Neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Biol. 7:a018812. 10.1101/cshperspect.a018812 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kheirbek M. A., Drew L. J., Burghardt N. S., Costantini D. O., Tannenholz L., Ahmari S. E., et al. (2013). Differential control of learning and anxiety along the dorsoventral axis of the dentate gyrus. Neuron 77, 955–968. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.12.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kheirbek M. A., Tannenholz L., Hen R. (2012). NR2B-dependent plasticity of adult-born granule cells is necessary for context discrimination. J. Neurosci. 32, 8696–8702. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1692-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Klein M., Pieri I., Uhlmann F., Pfizenmaier K., Eisel U. (1998). Cloning and characterization of promoter and 5′-UTR of the NMDA receptor subunit epsilon 2: evidence for alternative splicing of 5′-non-coding exon. Gene 208, 259–269. 10.1016/S0378-1119(98)00005-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Kurt M., Arik A. C., Celik S. (2000). The effects of sertraline and fluoxetine on anxiety in the elevated plus-maze test in mice. J. Basic Clin. Physiol. Pharmacol. 11, 173–180. 10.1515/JBCPP.2000.11.2.173 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Lacefield C. O., Itskov V., Reardon T., Hen R., Gordon J. A. (2012). Effects of adult-generated granule cells on coordinated network activity in the dentate gyrus. Hippocampus 22, 106–116. 10.1002/hipo.20860 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Li Y., Luikart B. W., Birnbaum S., Chen J., Kwon C. H., Kernie S. G., et al. (2008). TrkB regulates hippocampal neurogenesis and governs sensitivity to antidepressive treatment. Neuron 59, 399–412. 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.06.023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Li Y., Mu Y., Gage F. H. (2009). Development of neural circuits in the adult hippocampus. Curr. Top. Dev. Biol. 87, 149–174. 10.1016/S0070-2153(09)01205-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Lin S. Y., Wu K., Levine E. S., Mount H. T., Suen P. C., Black I. B. (1998). BDNF acutely increases tyrosine phosphorylation of the NMDA receptor subunit 2B in cortical and hippocampal postsynaptic densities. Brain Res. Mol. Brain Res. 55, 20–27. 10.1016/S0169-328X(97)00349-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Malberg J. E., Eisch A. J., Nestler E. J., Duman R. S. (2000). Chronic antidepressant treatment increases neurogenesis in adult rat hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 20, 9104–9110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Monteggia L. M., Barrot M., Powell C. M., Berton O., Galanis V., Gemelli T., et al. (2004). Essential role of brain-derived neurotrophic factor in adult hippocampal function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 10827–10832. 10.1073/pnas.0402141101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Monyer H., Burnashev N., Laurie D. J., Sakmann B., Seeburg P. H. (1994). Developmental and regional expression in the rat brain and functional properties of four NMDA receptors. Neuron 12, 529–540. 10.1016/0896-6273(94)90210-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Nibuya M., Nestler E. J., Duman R. S. (1996). Chronic antidepressant administration increases the expression of cAMP response element binding protein (CREB) in rat hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 16, 2365–2372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Oh J. E., Zupan B., Gross S., Toth M. (2009). Paradoxical anxiogenic response of juvenile mice to fluoxetine. Neuropsychopharmacology 34, 2197–2207. 10.1038/npp.2009.47 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Piatti V. C., Ewell L. A., Leutgeb J. K. (2013). Neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus: carrying the message or dictating the tone. Front. Neurosci. 7:50. 10.3389/fnins.2013.00050 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Revest J. M., Dupret D., Koehl M., Funk-Reiter C., Grosjean N., Piazza P. V., et al. (2009). Adult hippocampal neurogenesis is involved in anxiety-related behaviors. Mol. Psychiatry 14, 959–967. 10.1038/mp.2009.15 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Saarelainen T., Hendolin P., Lucas G., Koponen E., Sairanen M., Macdonald E., et al. (2003). Activation of the TrkB neurotrophin receptor is induced by antidepressant drugs and is required for antidepressant-induced behavioral effects. J. Neurosci. 23, 349–357. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Sahay A., Hen R. (2007). Adult hippocampal neurogenesis in depression. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 1110–1115. 10.1038/nn1969 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Sahay A., Scobie K. N., Hill A. S., O'Carroll C. M., Kheirbek M. A., Burghardt N. S., et al. (2011a). Increasing adult hippocampal neurogenesis is sufficient to improve pattern separation. Nature 472, 466–470. 10.1038/nature09817 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Sahay A., Wilson D. A., Hen R. (2011b). Pattern separation: a common function for new neurons in hippocampus and olfactory bulb. Neuron 70, 582–588. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Sairanen M., Lucas G., Ernfors P., Castren M., Castren E. (2005). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor and antidepressant drugs have different but coordinated effects on neuronal turnover, proliferation, and survival in the adult dentate gyrus. J. Neurosci. 25, 1089–1094. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3741-04.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Samuels B. A., Anacker C., Hu A., Levinstein M. R., Pickenhagen A., Tsetsenis T., et al. (2015). 5-HT1A receptors on mature dentate gyrus granule cells are critical for the antidepressant response. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1606–1616. 10.1038/nn.4116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Samuels B. A., Leonardo E. D., Dranovsky A., Williams A., Wong E., Nesbitt A. M., et al. (2014). Global state measures of the dentate gyrus gene expression system predict antidepressant-sensitive behaviors. PLoS ONE 9:e85136. 10.1371/journal.pone.0085136 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Samuels B., Hen R. (2011). Novelty-suppressed feeding in the mouse, in Mood and Anxiety Related Phenotypes in Mice: Characterization Using Behavioral Tests, Vol. II, ed Gould T. D. (New York, NY: Humana Press; ), 107–121. [Google Scholar]
  65. Santarelli L., Saxe M., Gross C., Surget A., Battaglia F., Dulawa S., et al. (2003). Requirement of hippocampal neurogenesis for the behavioral effects of antidepressants. Science 301, 805–809. 10.1126/science.1083328 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Saxe M. D., Battaglia F., Wang J.-W. W., Malleret G., David D. J., Monckton J. E., et al. (2006). Ablation of hippocampal neurogenesis impairs contextual fear conditioning and synaptic plasticity in the dentate gyrus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 17501–17506. 10.1073/pnas.0607207103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Scobie K. N., Hall B. J., Wilke S. A., Klemenhagen K. C., Fujii-Kuriyama Y., Ghosh A., et al. (2009). Krüppel-like factor 9 is necessary for late-phase neuronal maturation in the developing dentate gyrus and during adult hippocampal neurogenesis. J. Neurosci. 29, 9875–9887. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2260-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Silva R. C., Brandao M. L. (2000). Acute and chronic effects of gepirone and fluoxetine in rats tested in the elevated plus-maze: an ethological analysis. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 65, 209–216. 10.1016/S0091-3057(99)00193-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Slack S. E., Pezet S., McMahon S. B., Thompson S. W., Malcangio M. (2004). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor induces NMDA receptor subunit one phosphorylation via ERK and PKC in the rat spinal cord. Eur. J. Neurosci. 20, 1769–1778. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03656.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Snyder J. S., Kee N., Wojtowicz J. M. (2001). Effects of adult neurogenesis on synaptic plasticity in the rat dentate gyrus. J. Neurophysiol. 85, 2423–2431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Snyder J. S., Soumier A., Brewer M., Pickel J., Cameron H. A. (2011). Adult hippocampal neurogenesis buffers stress responses and depressive behaviour. Nature 476, 458–461. 10.1038/nature10287 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Springer J. E., Gwag B. J., Sessler F. M. (1994). Neurotrophic factor mRNA expression in dentate gyrus is increased following in vivo stimulation of the angular bundle. Brain Res. Mol. Brain Res. 23, 135–143. 10.1016/0169-328X(94)90219-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Srinivas S., Watanabe T., Lin C. S., William C. M., Tanabe Y., Jessell T. M., et al. (2001). Cre reporter strains produced by targeted insertion of EYFP and ECFP into the ROSA26 locus. BMC Dev. Biol. 1:4. 10.1186/1471-213X-1-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Sun M. Y., Yetman M. J., Lee T. C., Chen Y., Jankowsky J. L. (2014). Specificity and efficiency of reporter expression in adult neural progenitors vary substantially among nestin-CreER(T2) lines. J. Comp. Neurol. 522, 1191–1208. 10.1002/cne.23497 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Surget A., Saxe M., Leman S., Ibarguen-Vargas Y., Chalon S., Griebel G., et al. (2008). Drug-dependent requirement of hippocampal neurogenesis in a model of depression and of antidepressant reversal. Biol. Psychiatry 64, 293–301. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.02.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Tannenholz L., Jimenez J. C., Kheirbek M. A. (2014). Local and regional heterogeneity underlying hippocampal modulation of cognition and mood. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:147. 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00147 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Venzala E., Garcia-Garcia A. L., Elizalde N., Delagrange P., Tordera R. M. (2012). Chronic social defeat stress model: behavioral features, antidepressant action, and interaction with biological risk factors. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 224, 313–325. 10.1007/s00213-012-2754-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. von Engelhardt J., Doganci B., Jensen V., Hvalby Ò., Göngrich C., Taylor A., et al. (2008). Contribution of hippocampal and extra-hippocampal NR2B-containing NMDA receptors to performance on spatial learning tasks. Neuron 60, 846–860. 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Wang J.-W. W., David D. J., Monckton J. E., Battaglia F., Hen R. (2008). Chronic fluoxetine stimulates maturation and synaptic plasticity of adult-born hippocampal granule cells. J. Neurosci. 28, 1374–1384. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3632-07.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Wu M. V., Hen R. (2014). Functional dissociation of adult-born neurons along the dorsoventral axis of the dentate gyrus. Hippocampus 24, 751–761. 10.1002/hipo.22265 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Zafra F., Castren E., Thoenen H., Lindholm D. (1991). Interplay between glutamate and gamma-aminobutyric acid transmitter systems in the physiological regulation of brain-derived neurotrophic factor and nerve growth factor synthesis in hippocampal neurons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 88, 10037–10041. 10.1073/pnas.88.22.10037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Zhao C., Deng W., Gage F. H. (2008). Mechanisms and functional implications of adult neurogenesis. Cell 132, 645–660. 10.1016/j.cell.2008.01.033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES