Skip to main content
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
. 2016 Jun 1;10:249. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00249

Why Broca's Area Damage Does Not Result in Classical Broca's Aphasia

Alfredo Ardila 1,*, Byron Bernal 2, Monica Rosselli 3
PMCID: PMC4887502  PMID: 27313519

Traditionally it has been assumed that language production is controlled by Broca's area, corresponding to Brodmann's area (BA) 44 (pars opercularis of the left hemisphere) (e.g., Head, 1926; Luria, 1947/1970; Goldstein, 1948; Hécaen, 1972; Damasio and Geschwind, 1984). Since about some 20 years ago, it has been considered that BA45 (pars triangularis) is also part of the Broca's area (Foundas et al., 1996). Some authors have referred to a more extended language production system; Hagoort (2005, 2006) proposed that there is a “Broca's complex,” including BA44, BA45, and also BA47. Lemaire et al. (2013) refer to an extended Broca's area; Kadis et al. (2016) to an expressive language network; Bernal et al. (2015) to a Broca's network; and Ardila et al. (2016) proposed a “Broca's complex” including not only left BA44 and BA45, but also BA46, BA47, partially BA6 (mainly its mesial supplementary motor area) and extending subcortically toward the basal ganglia and the thalamus.

During the last years, there has been a significant interest in re-analyzing the function of Broca's area in language (e.g., Hagoort, 2005; Thompson-Schill, 2005; Grodzinky and Amunts, 2006; Burns and Fahy, 2010). Different proposals have been presented including: binding the elements of the language (Hagoort, 2005), selecting information among competing sources (Thompson-Schill, 2005), generating/extracting action meanings (Fadiga et al., 2006); sequencing motor/expressive elements (Ardila and Bernal, 2007); cognitive control mechanism for the syntactic processing of sentences (Novick et al., 2005); construction of higher parts of the syntactic tree in speech production (Grodzinsky, 2000, 2006); and verbal working memory (Haverkort, 2005). Other authors have suggested that Broca's area subregions might be compatible with the system of prefrontal hierarchical control (Bookheimer, 2002). Koechlin and Jubault (2006) for instance, proposed that the more posterior subregions of Broca's area are preferentially engaged in language tasks based on phonological processing (in which discrete actions must be organized in time), whereas the more anterior regions including BA44, BA45, and BA47 are more precisely involved in tasks based on syntactic and semantic processing, presumably representing a higher organizational hierarchy.

Evidently, the cumulative research on the functions of Broca's area is intriguing but has not given a final answer to what is the fundamental function of Broca's area in language processing.

In the classical aphasia literature it is assumed that damage in the Broca's area is responsible for the clinical manifestations observed in Broca's aphasia (e.g., Head, 1926; Luria, 1947/1970; Goldstein, 1948; Hécaen, 1972; Damasio and Geschwind, 1984). Usually, it is assumed that Broca's aphasia includes two major impairments: apraxia of speech and agrammatism (e.g., Hécaen, 1972; Luria, 1976; Kertesz, 1979; Benson and Ardila, 1996). Only with the introduction of the CT scan did it become evident that the damage restricted to the Broca's area was not enough to produce the “classical” Broca's aphasia; extension to the insula, lower motor cortex, and subjacent subcortical and periventricular white matter is required (Alexander et al., 1990; Benson and Ardila, 1996). The mild language disturbance observed in cases of damage of Broca's areas was named “Broca's area aphasia” or “minor Broca's aphasia” or “Broca's aphasia type I (Benson and Ardila, 1996). This

type of aphasia is characterized by mildly non-fluent speech, relatively short sentences and mild agrammatism. Phonetic deviations, a few phonological paraphasias can be observed (Mohr et al., 1978) and some foreign accent can also be noticed (Ardila et al., 1988). Noteworthy, brain damage restricted to the Broca's area represents an extremely unusual clinical condition. Beginning with Broca, the overwhelming majority of Broca's aphasia patients present an extended brain damage, significantly exceeding the Broca's area (Broca, 1863; Mohr et al., 1978; Naeser and Hayward, 1978; Kertesz, 1979). Dronkers et al. (2007) using high resolution MR imaging studied the brains of the two initial cases of aphasia reported by Broca, Leborgne and Lelong, and found that both patients' lesions extended significantly into medial regions of the brain, in addition to the surface lesions described by Broca. They concluded that Broca's aphasia is associated to large lesions extending beyond the Broca's area.

From the above observations, it can be concluded that Broca's aphasia requires extensive brain lesions. Lesions restricted to Broca's area are associated with just in mild language production defects.

Using direct cortical surface recordings in neurosurgical patients it has been reported that during the cued production of words, a sequence of neural events proceeds from word memories in the temporal lobe to the articulatory movements in the motor cortical motor area. Broca's area plays a monitoring role with reciprocal interactions with temporal and frontal motor brain areas. These results corroborate that Broca's area coordinates the movement of information across extended brain circuits involved in speaking. Consequently, word production requires the participation of multiple cortical areas (Flinker et al., 2015).

Functional neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that language production not only activates Broca's area, but also a wide circuit including the surrounding areas (BA46, BA47, and the anterior insula), the supplementary motor areas, and extending subcortically (Figure 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Brain areas involved in language production. (Left panel) “Broca's system” (according to Ardila et al., 2016), including BA44, BA45, BA46, BA47, mesial BA6 (supplementary motor area; not seen) and extending subcortically toward the basal ganglia and the thalamus (not seen). (Right panel) fMRI activation during speaking in a normal adult subject; observed is a wide activation including not only BA44 but also the surrounding areas, extending to the supplementary motor area (courtesy of the Department of Radiology− Nicklaus Children's Hospital).

If considering BA44 (and BA45) represents only a fragment of the brain system involved in language production, it becomes understandable that restricted damage in this area results in just a partial (“minor”) Broca's aphasia. During speech production, Broca's area is not the only area that becomes active, but there is an extended brain system including not only cortical but also subcortical areas. Classical Broca's area represents just a step in the brain language production system. Noteworthy, damage in other areas of this language production system (BA46, BA47, supplementary motor area, and subcortical areas—basal ganglia and thalamus) can also result in language production deficits (Papathanasiou et al., 2012), though with some idiosyncratic characteristics.

In conclusion we propose that lesions confined to the canonical Broca's area do not result in classical Broca's aphasia due to the large functional connectivity of this area with adjacent frontal and subcortical areas.

Author contributions

AA, Primary writing; BB, fMRI analyses; MR, literature review; final writing.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

  1. Alexander M. P., Naeser M. A., Palumbo C. (1990). Broca's area aphasias: aphasia after lesions including the frontal operculum. Neurology 40, 353–362. 10.1212/WNL.40.2.353 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ardila A., Bernal B. (2007). What can be localized in the brain? Towards a “factor” theory on brain organization of cognition. Int. J. Neurosci. 117, 935–969. 10.1080/00207450600912222 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Ardila A., Bernal B., Rosselli M. (2016). How localized are language brain areas? A review of Brodmann areas involvement in language. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 31, 112–122. 10.1093/arclin/acv081 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Ardila A., Rosselli M., Ardila O. (1988). Foreign accent: an aphasic epiphenomenon? Aphasiology 2, 493–499. 10.1080/02687038808248955 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Benson D. F., Ardila A. (1996). Aphasia: A clinical perspective. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bernal B., Ardila A., Rosselli M. (2015). Broca's area network in language function: a pooling-data connectivity study. Front. Psychol. 6:687. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00687 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bookheimer S. (2002). Functional MRI of language: new approaches to understanding the cortical organization of semantic processing. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 25, 151–188. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142946 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Broca P. (1863). Localization des fonctions crbrales: siege du langage articulé. Bull. Soc. d'Anthropol. 4, 200-203. [Google Scholar]
  9. Burns M. S., Fahy J. (2010). Broca's area: rethinking classical concepts from a neuroscience perspective. Top. Stroke Rehabil. 17, 401–410. 10.1310/tsr1706-401 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Damasio A. R., Geschwind N. (1984). The neural basis of language. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 127–147. 10.1146/annurev.ne.07.030184.001015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Dronkers N. F., Plaisant O., Iba-Zizen M. T., Cabanis E. A. (2007). Paul Broca's historic cases: high resolution MR imaging of the brains of Leborgne and Lelong. Brain 130, 1432–1441. 10.1093/brain/awm042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Fadiga L., Craighero L., Roy A. (2006). Broca's region: a speech area? in Broca's Region, eds Grodzinky Y., Amunts K. (New York, NY: Oxford University Press; ), 137–152. 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195177640.003.0009 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Flinker A., Korzeniewska A., Shestyuk A. Y., Franaszczuk P. J., Dronkers N. F., Knight R. T., et al. (2015). Redefining the role of Broca's area in speech. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 2871–2875. 10.1073/pnas.1414491112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Foundas A. L., Leonard C. M., Gilmore R. L., Fennell E. B., Heilman K. M. (1996). Pars triangularis asymmetry and language dominance. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 719–722. 10.1073/pnas.93.2.719 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Goldstein K. (1948). Language and Language Disturbances. New York, NY: Grune and Stratton. [Google Scholar]
  16. Grodzinky Y., Amunts K. (eds.). (2006). Broca's Region. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195177640.001.0001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Grodzinsky Y. (2000). The neurology of syntax: language use without Broca's area. Behav. Brain Sci. 23, 1–21. 10.1017/S0140525X00002399 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Grodzinsky Y. (2006). The language faculty, Broca's region, and the mirror system. Cortex 42, 464–468. 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70378-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hagoort P. (2005). Broca's complex as the unification space for language, in Twenty-first Century Psycholinguistics: Four Cornerstones, ed Cutler A. (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; ), 157–172. [Google Scholar]
  20. Hagoort P. (2006). On Broca, brain and binding, in Broca's Region, eds Grodzinsky Y., Amunts K. (Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; ), 242–253. 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195177640.003.0015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  21. Haverkort M. (2005). Linguistic representation and language use in aphasia, in Twenty-first Century Psycholinguistics: Four Cornerstones. ed Cutler A. (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence ErlbaumAssociates Inc.), 57–68. [Google Scholar]
  22. Head H. (1926). Aphasia and Kindred Disorders of Speech. London: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  23. Hécaen H. (1972). Introduction a la Neuropsychologie. Paris: Larousse. [Google Scholar]
  24. Kadis D. S., Dimitrijevic A., Toro-Serey C. A., Smith M. L., Holland S. K. (2016). Characterizing information flux within the distributed pediatric expressive language network: a core region mapped through fMRI-constrained MEG effective connectivity analyses. Brain Connect. 6, 76–83. 10.1089/brain.2015.0374 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kertesz A. (1979). Aphasia and Associated Disorders: Taxonomy, Localization, and Recovery. Austin, TX: Holt Rinehart & Winston. [Google Scholar]
  26. Koechlin E., Jubault T. (2006). Broca's area and the hierarchical organization of human behavior. Neuron 50, 963–974. 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.05.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Lemaire J. J., Golby A., Wells W. M. III, Pujol S., Tie Y., Rigolo L., et al. (2013). Extended Broca's area in the functional connectome of language in adults: combined cortical and subcortical single-subject analysis using fMRI and DTI tractography. Brain Topogr. 26, 428–441. 10.1007/s10548-012-0257-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Luria A. R. (1947/1970). Traumatic Aphasia: Its Syndromes, Psychology and Treatment, Vol. 5. Amsterdam: Walter de Gruyter. [Google Scholar]
  29. Luria A. R. (1976). Basic Problems of Neurolinguistics, Vol. 73 Amsterdam: Walter de Gruyter; 10.1515/9783110800159 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. Mohr J. P., Pessin M. S., Finkelstein S., Funkenstein H. H., Duncan G. W., Davis K. R. (1978). Broca aphasia: pathologic and clinical. Neurology 28, 311–324. 10.1212/WNL.28.4.311 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Naeser M. A., Hayward R. W. (1978). Lesion localization in aphasia with cranial computed tomography and the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Exam. Neurology 28, 545–545. 10.1212/WNL.28.6.545 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Novick J. M., Trueswell J. C., Thompson S. L. (2005). Cognitive control and parsing: reexamining the role of Broca's area in sentence comprehension. Cogni. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 5, 263–281. 10.3758/CABN.5.3.263 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Papathanasiou I., Coppens P., Potagas C. (2012). Aphasia and Related Neurogenic Communication Disorders. Burlington, MA: Jones & Bartlett Publishers. [Google Scholar]
  34. Thompson-Schill S. L. (2005). Dissecting the language organ: a new look at the role of Broca's area in language processing, in Twenty-first Century Psycholinguistics: Four cornerstones, ed Cutler A. (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.), 173–190. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Human Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES