Abstract
Resource partitioning is well known along food and habitat for reducing competition among sympatric species, yet a study on temporal partitioning as a viable basis for reducing resource competition is not empirically investigated. Here, I attempt to identify the mechanism of temporal partitioning by intra- and interspecific diving analyses of three sympatric cormorant species at different freshwater wetlands around the Delhi region. Diving results indicated that cormorants opted for a shallow diving; consequently, they did not face any physiological stress. Moreover, diving durations were linked with seasons, foraging time and foraging habitats. Intraspecific comparison suggested that cormorants spent a longer time underwater in early hours of the day. Therefore, time spent for dive was higher in the forenoon than late afternoon, and the interspecific analysis also yielded a similar result. When Phalacrocorax niger and Phalacrocorax fuscicollis shared the same foraging habitat, they tended to differ in their foraging time (forenoon/afternoon). However, when P. niger and Phalacrocorax carbo shared the same foraging time, they tended to use different foraging habitats (lentic/lotic) leading to a mechanism of resource partitioning. Thus, sympatric cormorants effectively use time as a resource to exploit the food resources and successful coexistence.
Keywords: behaviour, cormorants, diving time, resource partition, sympatric, temporal
1. Background
In heterogeneous environments, niche differentiation functions as a mechanism of coexistence among competitors [1], usually along habitats, food resources, time axis or a combination of them. Niche differentiation facilitates ecological diversification through coexistence by avoidance of direct confrontation (interference competition) or reduction of resource overlap (resource competition) [2]. Sympatric species evolve different activity patterns to reduce interspecific resource and interference competition [1,3–5]. Temporal partitioning is a viable mechanism for reducing resource competition and sometimes trade-offs in foraging ecology as well [6–8]. However, time appeared to be a farless common mode of resource partition among sympatric species when compared with habitat [9] and food type [3,9]. Although ecological interactions over time have been observed in a number of communities viz., reptiles [10,11], birds [12] and mammals [6], the mechanism of time as an ecological resource is still poorly understood [3,12,13].
It has been well documented that birds could identify the subtle differences in the prey environment [14], even within prey species; for example, they are able to distinguish prey characteristic features [15–18] for effective use of resources. In freshwater wetlands of the Delhi region, three sympatric cormorant species viz., little cormorant (Phalacrocorax niger) (hereafter P. niger), Indian cormorant (Phalacrocorax fuscicollis) (hereafter P. fuscicollis) and large cormorant (Phalacrocorax carbo) (hereafter P. carbo) [19–21] forage together which are ideal models for a comparative study of diving behaviour and flexibility in foraging choices [2,22]. Foraging consists of dive time (hereafter = TD) and surface/pause time (hereafter = TP), and each dive follows a period on the surface for respiration [23,24]. It is well established that water depth [16,25], age [26], sex [27], underwater predation pressure [28] and kleptoparasitism [16] influences diving performance. However, intra- and interspecific species interactions along with environmental factors that affect diving behaviour receive little attention [5,11].
Here, I assess the diving behaviour of three cormorant species at different freshwater wetlands in the Delhi region, to identify interactions with environmental factors viz., breeding seasons, foraging time and foraging habitats, to explore adjustments that they make to use resource effectively for successful coexistence.
2. Material and methods
2.1. Study area
Field studies were conducted from January 2004 to December 2010 at different freshwater wetlands scattered within a radius of 180 km around Delhi (figure 1). Broadly, the area falls under the semi-arid Punjab plains 4A and upper Gangetic plains 7A of the biogeographical classification [29]. Wetlands were classified as lentic or lotic, and both the habitats were located inside protected areas (sanctuaries and parks) as well as in non-protected areas. Lotic wetlands viz., Okhla Barrage Bird Sanctuary, Wazirabad Barrage and Sonia Vihar wetlands were connected directly to the river Yamuna [22].
2.2. Diving observations
Cormorants were observed with binoculars (7′ × 50′′) and a telescope (15×). Focal animal sampling [30] was adopted, and observations were made from land at a distance of approximately 25–300 m as prescribed [31]. Each foraging bout was estimated from the time an individual started foraging until it finished completely, and a typical post-feeding behaviour [32] was used to ascertain the completion of foraging. A foraging bout was regarded as disturbed when a focal bird abruptly terminated foraging due to a disturbance event, caused by human activity or some other agent. Care was taken not to lose track of the focal bird. When there was doubt about the identity of a focal bird, especially, when two foraging birds crossed paths, or if a bird disappeared permanently from the view, or if the diving bout sequence was less than five dives, then that record was summarily discarded. Only serious dives were taken for analysis, and very short dives, sometimes little more than head dips were omitted for analysis as it was considered to be commuting dives [25]. A typical dive cycle [24,33,34] comprised: (i) pause time during which a bird replenishes oxygen at surface, (ii) descending time—a bird moves from water surface to foraging area, (iii) foraging time—a bird spends for foraging at the bottom, and (iv) ascending time—a bird returns to surface. Observations on TP and TD were collected, and TD represented the union of descending, foraging and ascending time. The time for which a focal bird remained on surface TP and underwater TD was estimated to the nearest second by using a digital electronic stopwatch. Observations were made on relatively calm days, and field data collection was avoided on rainy or heavy windy days. Observations were not altered on any account, for any other reasons [25].
Time, date, wetland name, focal species details and notes of the weather were noted on data recording sheets. Months of July–September, October–November and December–February were considered as breeding seasons for P. niger, P. fuscicollis and P. carbo, respectively [19,20]. Therefore, the diving behaviour observed within those months was categorized as breeding dives, and that in the remaining months was considered as non-breeding dives for each species, accordingly. Although seasonal differences of diving time were an intrinsic biological character, for the convenience of analysis, it was included along with other extrinsic environmental parameters. Further, dives were segregated as lentic or lotic depending on the nature of wetland in which those observations were made. Wetlands of Okhla Barrage Bird Sanctuary, Wazirabad Barrage and Sonia Vihar were classified as lotic wetlands, and the remaining as lentic wetlands (figure 1).
2.3. Data analysis
The data were entered into a worksheet, and analysis was performed using Minitab 17.1.0. A Kolmogorov–Smirnov normality test was employed to check the normality of the data. GLM-ANOVA was employed to test the differences with respect to TD, TP and bout length among the three species of cormorants. To establish a relationship between TD and TP for each species separately, linear regressions were employed in which TP was made a dependent variable and TD as an independent variable, and limitations pertaining to regression were checked [18]. The dives within bouts are mutually related in a way that bouts from different dive cycles are not; for example, within a species, smaller or younger birds would typically dive for shorter times. Therefore, this non-independence of bouts was taken into account in the analysis following the method of Lea et al. [35]. To test the dive of cormorants as either reactive or anticipatory, mean TP for the bout of dives concerned was first subtracted from each TP, to give what can be called ‘residual pause times’ [35]. TD values were then submitted to multiple regression using dive bouts, residual preceding pause time (TP-Pre) and succeeding pause time (TP-Suc).
The intraspecific variability of TD was tested in relation to environmental variables as described later. The coefficient of variation (CV) of TD and TP was high; therefore, the data were subjected to further scrutiny among the species to explore the influence of other environmental factors. Both intraspecific and interspecific analyses were performed. For intraspecific analysis, GLM-ANOVA and post hoc tests were done. As the dependent variables were categorical representing species, nominal logistic regression models were employed to detect the interspecific interactions and influence of environmental variables. Therefore, the dependent categorical values of species (coded as: P. niger: 0; P. fuscicollis: 1; P. carbo: 2) regressed upon the environmental variables viz., foraging habitat (lentic/lotic), foraging time (forenoon: before 12.00/afternoon: after 12.00) and the seasons (breeding/non-breeding) of the three sympatric species of cormorants. All the results are expressed as mean ± s.d.
3. Results
Diving observations of 1012 dive cycles which comprised 63 diving bouts from different individuals of three cormorant species were collected from 15 freshwater wetlands in the Delhi region (table 1).
Table 1.
parameters | P. niger | P. fuscicollis | P. carbo |
---|---|---|---|
dive time (s)a | 14.72 ± 6.65 (538) | 19.82 ± 8.54 (204) | 18.26 ± 7.13 (270) |
dive time CV (%) | 45 | 43 | 39 |
pause time (s; n.s.) | 6.36 ± 3.82 (538) | 6.31 ± 3.2 (204) | 6.09 ± 2.78 (270) |
pause time CV (%) | 60 | 51 | 46 |
dives per bout (n.s.) | 16.3 ± 8.57 (33) | 15.69 ± 7.62 (13) | 15.89 ± 3.81 (17) |
length of the bout (s; n.s.) | 356.1 ± 204.3 (33) | 410.2 ± 168.2 (13) | 386.7 ± 113.5 (17) |
TD/TP | 2.74 | 3.52 | 3.28 |
GLM-ANOVA, p < 0.001.
3.1. Variations in TD and TP
Phalacrocorax niger, smallest in terms of body-size, had the shortest mean TD among three species (14.72 ± 6.65; n = 538). Significant differences in TD (GLM-ANOVA, F2,1009 = 45.951; p < 0.001) were observed when compared among three species of cormorants (table 1). As per the post hoc Scheffe test, TD of P. niger significantly differed from that of both P. fuscicollis (p < 0.001) and P. carbo (p < 0.001). However, the difference between P. fuscicollis and P. carbo was not significant (p < 0.065).
Further, TP was non-significant (n.s.) among three species of cormorants (GLM-ANOVA, F2,1009 = 6.47; p < 0.580, n.s.), and estimated as 6 s in all three species (table 1) which indicates their role of respiration and physiological limits. When TP was regressed upon TD, values of the slopes were extremely low for all three species suggesting that these species opted for shallow diving (figure 2). The regression equations of three species were estimated as follows: P. niger: TP = 0.0926 TD + 4.9909 (r2 = 0.0258, p < 0.001), P. fuscicollis: TP = 0.116 TD + 4.015 (r2 = 0.0959, p < 0.001) and P. carbo: TP = 0.0997 TD + 4.2702 (r2 = 0.0653, p < 0.001) (figure 2). Although TD includes various activities viz., ascending, searching for prey, vigilance from predator and descending, CV of TD was lower than that of TP (range 45–60%), mainly due to the handling time of prey plus respiration at the surface. However, there was a significant positive relationship between TD and TP in all the three species of cormorants after controlling for the effect of non-independence of diving bouts (table 2). Further, dives were reactive in nature in all the three species of cormorants. In P. fuscicollis, quite a substantial difference was noted, which happened to fall short of significance (p < 0.068) in favour of anticipatory breathing (table 2).
Table 2.
t-test |
ANOVA |
||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
species | set | predictors | coefficient ± s.e. | t-value | p-value | model R2 (%) | d.f. | F-value | p-value |
P. niger | |||||||||
1 | TP | 0.368 ± 0.0647 | 5.68 | 0.001 | 40.7 | 1, 504 | 32.25 | 0.001 | |
2 | TP-Suc | 0.368 ± 0.0647 | 5.69 | 0.001 | 40.8 | 1, 504 | 32.34 | 0.001 | |
3 | TP-Pre | −0.001 ± 0.0723 | −0.01 | 0.985 | 36.2 | 1, 468 | 0.000 | 0.985 | |
P. fuscicollis | |||||||||
1 | TP | 0.250 ± 0.162 | 1.54 | 0.126 | 42.0 | 1, 190 | 2.360 | 0.126 | |
2 | TP-Suc | 0.255 ± 0.163 | 1.57 | 0.118 | 42.0 | 1, 190 | 2.460 | 0.118 | |
3 | TP-Pre | 0.396 ± 0.216 | 1.83 | 0.068 | 42.4 | 1, 178 | 3.360 | 0.068 | |
P. carbo | |||||||||
1 | TP | 0.569 ± 0.135 | 4.21 | 0.001 | 39.4 | 1, 252 | 17.74 | 0.001 | |
2 | TP-Suc | 0.566 ± 0.135 | 4.19 | 0.001 | 35.3 | 1, 252 | 17.57 | 0.001 | |
3 | TP-Pre | −0.029 ± 0.146 | −0.20 | 0.844 | 35.9 | 1, 235 | 0.400 | 0.844 |
The TD and TP ratio of P. niger were lowest among the three species. As expected, a positive gradient along body-size with a ratio of 2 : 1 for P. niger and 3 : 1 for P. carbo was estimated (table 1). Although buoyancy force depends on body-size, it did not make any difference in mean foraging bout length (GLM-ANOVA, F2,62 = 0.480; p < 0.621, n.s.). Much of the differences in diving activity followed a pattern along body-size gradation representing their physiological limits.
3.2. Intraspecific diving interactions
Intraspecific variations of TD values were examined in relation to selected environmental variables viz., foraging habitat, time of the day and breeding seasons. For each species, possible effects of confounding variables were removed by using only a subset of the data. For instance, testing if the TD values differ between lentic and lotic habitats, only data of Okhla Barrage Bird Sanctuary and Jagatpur were used because they were close to each other. It was ensured that selected datasets were as close as possible, in terms of the time of the day as well as the month, so that the effects of these variables do not confound the analysis. Except for time of the day, results do not conclusively show that TD values differed in relation to environmental variables taken. In several datasets of P. niger, P. fuscicollis and P. carbo, mean TD values of the time slot prior to 11.00 were higher than late afternoon or evening (table 3), indicating that birds spent longer time underwater in early hours of the day.
Table 3.
mean dive time under different time slots |
|||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
species | site no | I (less than 11.00) | II (11.00–14.00) | III (14.00–16.00) | IV (greater than 16.00) | GLM-ANOVA | post hoc Scheffe test |
P. niger | 7 | 16.1 ± 6.3 (11) | 7.4 ± 1.7 (7) | 9.2 ± 3.3 (93) | 11.8 ± 5.03 (9) | F = 13.141 | I versus II** |
p = 0.001 | versus III** | ||||||
P. fuscicollis | 1, 2, 11 | 17.8 ± 7.01 (23) | 23.8 ± 5.20 (17) | 20.1 ± 7.04 (33) | — | F = 3.985 | I versus II* |
p = 0.02 | |||||||
P. carbo | 7, 9, 10, 12 | 22.8 ± 6.8 (49) | 10.6 ± 2.05 (18) | 13.9 ± 5.3 (42) | 18.3 ± 10.3 (13) | F = 22.876 | I versus II** |
p = 0.001 | versus III** |
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.001.
3.3. Interspecific diving interactions
The environmental variables viz., breeding seasons, habitats and foraging time, had an independent effect on TD; micro- and macro-level effects were identified among three cormorant species.
At macro-level, TD varied significantly between breeding and non-breeding seasons (P. niger and P. fuscicollis (p < 0.001); P. niger and P. carbo (p < 0.001). However, no such variations were noted in TP of cormorants (table 4). At the micro-level, a clear segregation and interactions over habitat selection (lentic/lotic) and foraging time (forenoon/afternoon) were recorded.
Table 4.
logit (1) |
logit (2) |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
P. fuscicollis/P. niger |
P. carbo/P. niger |
|||||
predictors | co-efficient | s.d. | p-value | co-efficient | s.d. | p-value |
constant | −1.275 | 0.338 | 0.001 | −1.682 | 0.331 | 0.001 |
breeding seasons (breeding 1, non-breeding 2) | −0.689 | 0.212 | 0.001 | 0.712 | 0.168 | 0.001 |
TD (dive time) | 0.092 | 0.124 | 0.000 | 0.058 | 0.012 | 0.001 |
TP (pause time) | −0.019 | 0.028 | 0.483 | −0.039 | 0.028 | 0.159 |
foraging time (forenoon-1, afternoon-2) | −0.497 | 0.112 | 0.001 | −0.139 | 0.103 | 0.177 |
habitats (lentic-1, lotic-2) | 0.586 | 0.302 | 0.052 | 1.861 | 0.235 | 0.001 |
A significant diurnal variation in foraging time (forenoon/afternoon) was observed between P. niger and P. fuscicollis (p < 0.001); subsequently, no significant difference was noted in the selection of foraging habitats (p < 0.052) (table 4). In other words, the chance of a sighting of both species (P. niger and P. fuscicollis) at the same site was possible; however, it differed in their foraging time between forenoon and afternoon. Interestingly, the results were exactly opposite when the comparison was made between P. carbo and P. niger. No significant difference in foraging time was observed between them (p < 0.117). However, a significant difference in the selection of foraging habitats (p < 0.001) was observed (table 4). Both species foraged at the same time, but differed in their foraging habitats. When P. carbo selected lotic habitats, the smaller P. niger restricted to lentic water-bodies (table 4).
4. Discussion
4.1. Diving behaviour
Diving behaviour of cormorants combines the key physiological and ecological factors [24]. TP explains the role of respiratory physiology (ability to obtain oxygen at the surface) and TD explains ecological factors that affect the diving behaviour. Anaerobic dive was reported in deeper wetlands, and cormorants were recorded to dive to great depths of approximately 99 m [36,37] in marine habitats. As the wetlands in the study areas are shallow (less than 10 m) and monsoonal in nature [22], birds would prefer energetically profitable dives. Further, TP was more or less uniform of 6 s in all the species (figure 2); therefore, diving results support the predictions of optimal breathing models [33–35] and aerobic diving. If animals operate within their oxygen capacity, then I would not expect any correlation between TD and TP [35].
An attempt was made to look for a point of inflection in the TD and TP regression curve, as in other marine studies, [26,36] to check whether birds crossed the aerobic diving limit. No such inflection was noticed (figure 2), suggesting that cormorant species did not perform any deeper dives leading to physiological stress. If the cormorants had incurred a diving cost (say oxygen debt), then it would have reflected in higher slope values of TD/TP regressions. It re-confirmed that TP was not proportionally longer with TD, and cormorants were not undergoing any oxygen depletion.
Here, only a small spectrum of diving ability of cormorants was accounted, and if the habitat was deep enough, then TD would have extended a longer time in underwater. It was noted that air-breathing divers changed their dive tactics, time allocation depending on the aim of the dive and surfaced without depleting their estimated stores of oxygen [11]. The results of this study showed enough empirical support with earlier ideas [24,35] viz., dives are not always terminated solely on the basis of oxygen or finding an acceptable prey or close to aerobic diving limit or perhaps beyond. The dives were reactive in all three species of cormorants, which clearly supports the assumption that TP should depend on the preceding dive time, satisfying the condition of reactive breathing. Although the results were not in favour of the anticipatory breathing mode, where dive time depends on the preceding surface time, in any of the three cormorants, a slight edge of anticipatory dive was observed in P. fuscicollis (table 2).
4.2. Time as ecological resource
It is well documented in birds that they tend to make finer adjustments to use resources, distinguish subtle differences in the foraging environment [14], compensate for foraging time [7], and distinguish prey dimensions [8,15,16] and colour [17]. Therefore, the adjustments that the bird makes for effective use of resources, which leads to coexistence, would be clearly understood through intra- and interspecific analysis [2,5]. Resource partitioning of cormorants with respect to food [9,16] and habitat [23,36,38,39] is known, and each cormorant species have their unique diving time [40] that differed significantly from each other [38]. Diving activity followed a pattern along body-size gradation representing their own typical physiological limits [2,16,35,41]. However, cormorants’ diving efficiency (TD and TP ratio) was high, indicating interspecific interactions and the influence of environmental factors [5,11,38] that affected the diving behaviour, other than the physiological differences.
Intraspecific analysis of TD suggested that foraging differences were noticed, in the case of P. niger and P. carbo, over the time of day. However, no satisfactory conclusion was found on habitats and seasons owing to the lack of confounding sub-datasets. Interspecific interactions over the season (macro-level) and the time of day (micro-level) influenced the diving behaviour of cormorants. The difference in TD between breeding and non-breeding was obvious as birds need to find food for themselves and their nestlings. Therefore, it is expected that birds would undertake an extra foraging effort during breeding time and sexual difference in foraging also possible [27].
At micro-level, the diving difference was observed with extrinsic environmental variables viz., foraging habitat (lentic/lotic) and foraging time of the day (forenoon/afternoon). Changes in the diving behaviour reflect differences in prey abundance, prey behaviours in foraging habitats [42] and predation pressure [28]. Habitats are known to exert a strong influence on the temporal distribution of prey, which ultimately influenced the distribution pattern of cormorants [43]. When P. niger and P. fuscicollis selected the same habitats, a segregation in foraging time was observed; however, the result was reverse in P. niger and P. carbo, which indicated a resource partitioning to minimize competition.
The probable ecological factors that drive this apparent time sharing, perhaps, could be the prevailing differences in the diurnal cycles of prey species, and preference of prey size selection among cormorants. It was experimentally proved that cormorants caught fishes that ranged from 30 to 140 mm, and the prey sizes taken by each species were significantly different [16]. Further, a significant difference in handling time was noted for the same size of fishes, which indirectly indicated a preferential selection of different prey species [22]. Unlike the European shag (Phalacrocorax aristotelis), all cormorant species typically brought fish to the water surface, one at a time [21], so it attracts issues of kleptoparasitism as well [16]. It was noted that there was a slight variation in foraging time between winter and summer with the cormorants being visual pursuit divers; so the influence of day light was obvious [22]. These factors, perhaps, either on their own or in combination of others, mediate the apparent time-sharing among cormorant species. Thereby, the three species of cormorants successfully coexist through partitioning of time to exploit the resources effectively.
Acknowledgements
I am grateful for the support and mentorship of Prof. C. R. Babu, Dr A. J. Urfi and Dr P. A. Azeez. I am happy to know, from the editor, that Prof. Stephen Lea and one anonymous referee graciously reviewed the manuscript; I am grateful to both of them, and the quality of this manuscript has improved considerably because of their constructive and critical comments. My heartfelt thanks to various forest officials who have extended their support and cooperation during the study. I thank Dr N. D. Dalai, Mr M. Parthiban, Miss D. Kalpana and Mr D. Rajneesh for their support in the laboratory and field.
Data accessibility
The supporting data are available on Dryad: http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.93k26.
Authors' contributions
M.M. conducted all work relating to this publication, with minor discursive input from others.
Competing interests
I declare I have no competing interests.
Funding
I acknowledge the financial support of University Grants Commission (A 091413 JRF-NET JUNE 2001) and SERB (SR/FT/LS-12/2011), New Delhi.
References
- 1.MacArthur R, Levins R. 1967. The limiting similarity, convergence, and divergence of coexisting species. Am. Nat. 101, 377–385. (doi:10.1086/282505) [Google Scholar]
- 2.Laiolo P, Seoane J, Illera JC, Bastianelli G, Carrascal ML, Obeso JR. 2015. The evolutionary convergence of avian lifestyles and their constrained coevolution with species’ ecological niche. Proc. R. Soc. B 282, 20151808 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2015.1808) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Schoener TW. 1974. Resource partitioning in ecological communities. Science 185, 27–39. (doi:10.1126/science.185.4145.27) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Richards SA. 2002. Temporal partitioning and aggression among foragers: modeling the effects of stochasticity and individual state. Behav. Ecol. 13, 427–438. (doi:10.1093/beheco/13.3.427) [Google Scholar]
- 5.Svanback R, Bolnick DI. 2007. Intraspecific competition drives increased resource use diversity within a natural population. Proc. R. Soc. B 274, 839–844. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2006.0198) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Ziv Y, Abramsky Z, Kotler BP, Subach A. 1993. Interference competition and temporal and habitat partitioning in two gerbil species. Oikos 66, 237–246. (doi:10.2307/3544810) [Google Scholar]
- 7.Urfi AJ, Goss-Custard JD, Durell SEA le V dit. 1996. The ability of oystercatchers Haematopus ostralegus to compensate for lost feeding time: field studies on individually marked birds. J. Appl. Ecol. 33, 873–883. (doi:10.2307/2404958) [Google Scholar]
- 8.Nagarajan R, Lea SEG, Goss-Custard JD. 2002. Mussel valve discrimination and strategies used in valve discrimination by the Oystercatcher, Haematopus ostralegus. Funct. Ecol. 16, 339–345. (doi:10.1046/j.1365-2435.2002.00635.x) [Google Scholar]
- 9.Lack D. 1945. The ecology of closely related species with special reference to cormorant (Phalacrocorax carbo) and shag (P. aristotelis). J. Anim. Ecol. 14, 12–16. (doi:10.2307/1395) [Google Scholar]
- 10.Pianka ER. 1969. Sympatry of desert lizards (Ctenotus) in Western Australia. Ecology 50, 1012–1030. (doi:10.2307/1936893) [Google Scholar]
- 11.Okuyama J, Tabata R, Nakajima K, Arai N, Kobayashi M, Kagawa S. 2014. Surfacers change their dive tactics depending on the aim of the dive: evidence from simultaneous measurements of breaths and energy expenditure. Proc. R. Soc. B 281, 20140040 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2014.0040) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Jaksić FM. 1982. Inadequacy of activity time as a niche difference: the case of diurnal and nocturnal raptors. Oecologia 52, 171–175. (doi:10.1007/BF00363832) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Kronfeld-Schor N, Dayan T.. 2003. Partitioning of time as an ecological resource. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 34, 153–181. (doi:10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.34.011802.132435) [Google Scholar]
- 14.Goss-Custard JD, Caldow RWG, Clarke RT, Durell SEA le V dit, Sutherland WJ. 1995. Deriving population parameters from individual variations in foraging behavior. I. Empirical game theory distribution model of oystercatchers, Haematopus ostralegus feeding on mussels Mytilus edulis. J. Anim. Ecol. 64, 265–276. (doi:10.2307/5761) [Google Scholar]
- 15.Nagarajan R, Lea SEG, Goss-Custard JD. 2002. Re-evaluation of patterns of mussel (Mytilus edulis) selection by European oystercatchers (Haematopus ostralegus). Can. J. Zool. 80, 846–853. (doi:10.1139/z02-057) [Google Scholar]
- 16.Mahendiran M, Urfi AJ. 2010. Foraging patterns and kleptoparasitism among three sympatric cormorants (Phalacrocorax spp.) from the Delhi region, North India. Hydrobiologia 638, 21–28. (doi:10.1007/s10750-009-0002-8) [Google Scholar]
- 17.Nagarajan R, Goss-Custard JD, Lea SEG. 2002. Oystercatchers use colour preference to achieve longer-term optimality. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 269, 523–528. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2001.1908) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Le Rossignol AP, Buckingham SG, Lea SEG, Nagarajan R.. 2011. Breaking down the mussel (Mytilus edulis) shell: which layers affect oystercatchers’ (Haematopus ostralegus) prey selection? J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 405, 87–92. (doi:10.1016/j.jembe.2011.05.021) [Google Scholar]
- 19.Ganguli U. 1975. A field guide to the birds of the Delhi area. Indian Council of Agricultural Research: New Delhi. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Ali S, Ripley SD. 1987. Compact book of the birds of India, Pakistan together with those of Bangladesh, Nepal, Bhutan and Sri Lanka. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Urfi AJ, Mahendiran M. 2005. A review of cormorants in India and a note on the cormorant colonies in the heronries of the Delhi zoo. Corm. Res. Gr. Bull. 6, 39–43. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Mahendiran M. 2010. A comparative field study on the ecology of cormorants in the Delhi region. PhD thesis, Delhi University, New Delhi.
- 23.Wilson RP, Wilson M-PT. 1988. Foraging behaviour in four sympatric cormorants. J. Anim. Ecol. 57, 943–955. (doi:10.2307/5103) [Google Scholar]
- 24.Houston AI. 2011. Assessing models of optimal diving. Trends Ecol. Evol. 26: 292–297. (doi:10.1016/j.tree.2011.03.003) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Ryan PG. 2007. Diving in shallow water: the foraging ecology of darters (Aves: Anhingidae). J. Avian Biol. 38, 507–514. (doi:10.1111/j.2007.0908-8857.04070.x) [Google Scholar]
- 26.Wanless S, Harris MP. 1991. Diving patterns of full-grown and juvenile rock shags. Condor 93, 44–48. (doi:10.2307/1368604) [Google Scholar]
- 27.Anderson CD, Roby DD, Collis K. 2004. Foraging patterns of male and female double-crested cormorants nesting in the Columbia River estuary. Can. J. Zool. 82, 541–554. (doi:10.1139/z04-019) [Google Scholar]
- 28.Frid A, Heithaus MR, Dill LM. 2007. Dangerous dive cycles and the proverbial ostrich. Oikos 116, 893–902. (doi:10.1111/j.2007.0030-1299.15766.x) [Google Scholar]
- 29.Rodgers AW, Panwar HS. 1988. Planning a protected area network in India 1. The Report, Wildlife Institute of India, Dehradun.
- 30.Altmann J. 1974. Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour 49, 227–266. (doi:10.1163/156853974X00534) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Hobson KA, Sealy SG. 1985. Diving rhythms and diurnal roosting times of pelagic cormorants. Wilson Bull. 97, 116–119. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Ross RK. 1976. Notes on the behavior of captive great cormorants. Wilson Bull. 88, 143–145. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Kramer DL. 1988. The behavioral ecology of air breathing by aquatic animals. Can. J. Zool. 66, 89–94. (doi:10.1139/z88-012) [Google Scholar]
- 34.Houston AI, Carbone C. 1992. The optimal allocation of time during the diving cycle. Behav. Ecol. 3, 255–265. (doi:10.1093/beheco/3.3.255) [Google Scholar]
- 35.Lea SEG, Daley C, Boddington PJC, Morison V. 1996. Diving patterns in shags and cormorants (Phalacrocorax): tests of an optimal breathing model. Ibis 138, 391–398. (doi:10.1111/j.1474-919X.1996.tb08056.x) [Google Scholar]
- 36.Wilson RP, Quintana F. 2004. Surface pauses in relation to dive duration in imperial cormorants; how much time for a breather? J. Exp. Biol. 207, 1789–1796. (doi:10.1242/jeb.00967) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Orta J. 1992. Family Phalacrocoracidae (cormorants). In Handbook of the birds of the world, volume 1: ostrich to ducks (eds del Hoyo J, Elliott A, Sargatal J), pp. 326–353. Barcelona, Spain: Lynx Edicions. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Cooper J. 1986. Diving patterns of cormorants Phalacrocoracidae. Ibis 128, 562–570. (doi:10.1111/j.1474-919X.1986.tb02707.x) [Google Scholar]
- 39.Gremillet D, Wilson RP, Storch S, Gary Y. 1999. Three-dimensional space utilization by a marine predator. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Series 183, 263–273. (doi:10.3354/meps183263) [Google Scholar]
- 40.Dewar JM. 1924. The bird as a diver: a contribution to the natural history of diving birds. UK: HF & G. Wetherby. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Halsey LG. 2003. Testing optimal foraging models for air-breathing divers. Anim. Behav. 65, 641–653. (doi:10.1006/anbe.2003.2090) [Google Scholar]
- 42.Childress R, Bennun L, Harper D. 2002. Population changes in sympatric great and long-tailed cormorants (Phalacrocorax carbo and P. africanus): the effects of niche overlap or environmental change? Hydrobiologia 488, 163–170. (doi:10.1023/A:023338816801) [Google Scholar]
- 43.Dorfman E, Kingsford MJ. 2001. Environmental determinants of distribution and foraging behaviour of cormorants (Phalacrocorax spp.) in temperate estuarine habitats. Mar. Biol. 138, 1–10. (doi:10.1007/s002270000437) [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The supporting data are available on Dryad: http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.93k26.