Skip to main content
Obesity Science & Practice logoLink to Obesity Science & Practice
. 2016 Apr 28;2(2):128–135. doi: 10.1002/osp4.40

Impact of perceived weight stigma among underserved women on doctor–patient relationships

J M Ferrante 1,2,, K Seaman 3, A Bator 1, P Ohman‐Strickland 1,2,4, D Gundersen 1,2, L Clemow 1, R Puhl 5
PMCID: PMC4902272  NIHMSID: NIHMS787919  PMID: 27293804

Summary

Objective

The aim of this study was to evaluate how perception of weight stigma among underserved women with obesity impacts doctor–patient relationships.

Methods

This study consisted of an interviewer‐administered survey of 149 women with obesity (body mass index (BMI) > 30 kg m−2) immediately after their physician visit at four Federally Qualified Health Centers. Perceptions of weight stigma and physician empathy were measured using the Stigma Situations in Health Care instrument and Consultation and Relational Empathy (CARE) measure, respectively. Associations of CARE and Stigma scores with BMI and patient characteristics were analysed using Mantel–Haenszel chi‐squared test and ordinal logistic regression.

Results

The mean CARE score was 42.1 (standard deviation 8.4; range 11.0–50.0), and mean stigma score was 4.6 (standard deviation 7.6; range 0–43.0). Each increase in BMI category was associated with almost twofold increased odds of higher perception of stigma (odds ratio, 1.90, 95% confidence interval 1.30–2.78, P = 0.001). BMI was not associated with CARE. However, for each increase in stigma category, the odds of lower CARE score doubled (odds ratio, 0.52, 95% confidence interval 0.36–0.75, P = 0.0005).

Conclusions

While BMI was not associated with perception of physician empathy, higher frequency of weight stigmatizing situations was negatively associated with perception of physician empathy. Reducing weight stigma in primary care could improve doctor–patient relationships and quality of care in patients with obesity.

Keywords: Minorities, obesity, patient–provider communication, weight stigma

Introduction

High rates of obesity remain a public health priority in the USA, particularly among minority women and those who are poor or less educated 1. These populations have worse health outcomes. A contributing factor to poorer health outcomes in women with obesity may be the avoidance of health care and healthy behaviours due to experiences of weight stigma in healthcare settings. Weight stigma refers to denigration towards individuals who are viewed to be overweight or obese and can result in negative stereotyping, unfair treatment and discrimination 2. The prevalence of weight‐based prejudice and stigmatization is high, approaching 69% in healthcare settings 3, 4. Weight bias results in shorter physician visits, fewer medical treatments and fewer preventive health screenings in patients with obesity 5, 6. Additionally, experiencing weight stigma leads to psychological distress and unhealthy behaviours, such as binge‐eating, avoidance of exercise and unsuccessful weight loss attempts 7, 8, 9, 10. Furthermore, internalization of perceived weight stigma increases the odds of obesity, remaining obese and gaining weight over time, further reinforcing obesity 11, 12, 13.

Perceptions of weight stigma in health care among patients with obesity may impair doctor–patient relationships and reduce quality of care and health outcomes 6. Better doctor–patient relationships have been positively associated with patient safety, adherence to recommended medical treatments and preventive services, self‐reported health and well‐being and better health outcomes across a range of diseases 14. While studies have shown perceptions of weight stigma increases with body mass index (BMI), studies examining association of BMI with patient satisfaction have shown mixed results, with some showing higher BMI to be associated with poorer relationships 15, 16, while others showing no difference or greater patient satisfaction in those with higher BMI 17, 18, 19.

A basic and crucial component of the doctor–patient relationship is physician empathy. Empathy can be defined as ‘an ability to (i) understand the patient's situation, perspective and feelings (and their attached meanings); (ii) communicate that understanding and check its accuracy; and (iii) act on that understanding with the patient in a helpful (therapeutic) way’ 20. Empathy has been shown to correlate with patient enablement 21, 22, 23, patient satisfaction 24, 25 and improved health outcomes in a wide range of clinical settings 23, 26, 27, 28, 29, 30, 31. To date, research is lacking that evaluates the association of BMI and patients' experiences of weight stigma in health care with their perception of physician empathy.

This study examined whether BMI is associated with perception of weight stigma in health care among women in underserved settings and how perceived weight stigma impacts ratings of physician empathy. The study's hypothesis was that patients with higher BMI experience more weight stigma in health care and that they would perceive less empathy from their physicians during the clinical encounter.

Materials and methods

This cross‐sectional study consisted of an interviewer‐administered survey of a convenience sample of 154 women with obesity (BMI > 30 kg m−2), at four New Jersey Federally Qualified Health Centers (FQHCs). Five different research team members recruited and interviewed patients. FQHCs were chosen because minority and poor/less educated women have the highest rates of obesity and may be disproportionately exposed to weight stigma 32, 33, 34. Women were recruited on site at each centre and interviewed immediately after their physician visit. They were eligible if they were aged 21–70 years, had obesity, were English speaking and were established patients in the practice for over 12 months, with at least one other appointment at the centre within the last 12 months. Written informed consent was obtained prior to administering the survey.

Women's perception of weight stigma in the practice was measured using a modification of Myers and Rosen's Stigmatizing Situations Inventory (SSI) 35. Because of the lack of validated measures that specifically assess patient experiences of weight stigma in the context of health care, we selected questions related to healthcare experiences of patients from broader measures that have been previously published and used to study weight stigma 35, 36. The response scale of Puhl and Brownell's modification of the SSI was changed to reflect the number of stigmatizing experiences in the particular practice site during the past 12 months. The resultant Stigma Situations in Health Care (SSHC) instrument contained 20 items regarding different weight stigmatizing situations in health care (e.g. ‘A doctor blaming unrelated physical problems on your weight.’); see Table 1. Women were asked how often they encountered each situation, with points given corresponding to each response: never (0 points), that day (1 point), one other time in the past 12 months (1 point), more than once (2 points) or multiple times (3 points) in the past 12 months (Cronbach's alpha was 0.92).

Table 1.

Stigma situations in healthcare items

How often has this happened to you in this practice during the past 12 months? At least once N (%)
1. A doctor blaming unrelated physical problems on your weight. 11 (7.38)
2. A doctor saying weight is a health problem when you are in good health. 26 (17.69)
3. A doctor makes cruel remarks, ridicules you or calls you names. 1 (0.67)
4. A doctor recommending a diet even if you did not intend to discuss weight. 27 (18.24)
5. Not being able to find medical equipment, such as blood pressure cuffs or gowns that fit you. 8 (5.37)
6. A doctor telling you to lose weight but not providing weight loss treatment options or advice on how to get help for weight loss. 17 (11.41)
7. Being stared at by medical staff when you go to the doctor's office. 16 (10.74)
8. Having medical staff make negative comments about weight to others. 4 (2.68)
9. Having health care professionals suggest diets to you without you asking for advice. 22 (14.77)
10. Overhearing medical staff make rude comments to you. 5 (3.36)
11. When you are weighed on a scale, the scale is not large enough for your size. 2 (1.34)
12. When you are weighed on a scale, the medical staff makes negative comments about your weight. 1 (0.67)
13. Having nurses make negative remarks, ridicule you or call you names. 0 (0)
14. Having office staff, for example a front desk receptionist, make negative remarks to you. 2 (1.34)
15. Not being able to fit in chairs in the waiting room. 0 (0)
16. A doctor refusing to do an exam on you because of your weight. 1 (0.67)
17. Having doctors or other health professionals assume you overeat or binge‐eat because you are overweight. 7 (4.70)
18. Having doctors or other health professionals assume you have emotional problems because you are overweight. 10 (6.71)
19. Being treated as less competent by health care providers because of your weight. 7 (4.70)
20. Being treated as lazy by health care providers because of your weight. 7 (4.70)
Overall Stigma score,*mean (SD); median (IQR) 4.6 (7.6); 2.0 (5.0)
*

Overall Stigma score was calculated by summing the scores of each item. Each item score was calculated based on points given for frequency of each experience: never (0 points), that day (1 point), one other time (1 point), more than once (2 points) and multiple times in past 12 months (3 points).

IQR, interquartile range; SD, standard deviation.

Patients' perception of their physician's empathy was assessed using the Consultation and Relational Empathy (CARE) measure 37. This instrument has been extensively validated and found reliable in different populations, healthcare settings and countries 38, 39, 40, 41, 42, 43, 44. The CARE measure consists of 10 items with answers on a 5‐point rating scale (1 = poor to 5 = excellent; Cronbach's alpha was 0.96); see Table 2. The survey also included questions from the Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System (BRFSS) survey regarding participants' demographic information and ratings of their physical and mental health 45. All participants received a $25 gift card at completion of the survey. The Robert Wood Johnson Medical School Institutional Review Board approved this study.

Table 2.

CARE measure items

How was your doctor at… Mean (SD)*
Making you feel at ease? 4.35 (0.85)
Letting you tell your story? 4.26 (0.94)
Really listening? 4.16 (1.04)
Being interested in you as a whole person? 4.12 (1.06)
Fully understanding your concerns? 4.29 (0.98)
Sharing care and compassion? 4.28 (0.99)
Being positive? 4.31 (0.88)
Explaining things clearly? 4.24 (0.96)
Helping you take control? 4.10 (1.07)
Making a plan of action with you? 4.08 (1.00)
Overall CARE score,** mean (SD); median (IQR) 42.2 (8.4); 45.0 (14.0)
*

Score for each item ranged from 1 = poor to 5 = excellent.

**

Overall score is sum from scores of each item.

CARE,Consultation and Relational Empathy; IQR, interquartile range; SD, standard deviation.

The dependent variables were Stigma score and CARE score. The scores were calculated by summing the points for each item from each instrument. Because the distributions of scores were highly skewed, we categorized the scores of CARE (<35, 36–44, 45–49 and 50) and Stigma (0, 1–4 and >5) based on their quartile and tertile distributions, respectively. The main independent variable analysed was BMI, calculated based on self‐reported weight and height. Because participants' weights were measured prior to their physician visit, we expected that their responses would be accurate. BMI was categorized based on standardized definitions of obese categories: obese class I (30–34.9 kg m−2), obese class II (35–39.9 kg m−2) and obese class III (40 kg m−2) 46. Potential confounders were age, race, marital status, education level, employment status, physical health, mental health and practice site.

The patient population and Stigma and CARE scores were characterized using descriptive statistics. Five subjects with reported BMI less than 30 were excluded in the analysis, as the target population of the study was women with obesity; therefore, the final sample size was 149. Bivariate associations of Stigma and CARE scores with patient characteristics were examined using Mantel–Haenszel chi‐squared test for ordinal data. Ordinal logistic regression using the proportional odds model was performed because of non‐normal distribution and ordinality of data 47. The association of BMI with Stigma and CARE scores was examined, as well as the association of Stigma to CARE scores, adjusting for BMI, physical health, mental health and practice site. Moderation analysis was conducted, with perceived Stigma, a potential moderator of the relationship between BMI and CARE by testing the interaction of BMI with Stigma. Likewise, CARE was tested as a potential moderator of the relationship between BMI and Stigma. All analyses were performed using SAS 9.3 software (SAS Institute Inc, Cary, NC, USA). All reported p‐values and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were two‐sided with significance level of 0.05.

Results

Table 3 summarizes demographic characteristics of the 149 participants. The average age was 47.5 years (standard deviation (SD) 10.5), and the mean BMI was 39.4 kg m−2 (SD 7.0). Most were African–American (77.2%), never married (47.7%), with high school or less education (65.6%) and unemployed (64.4%). The mean CARE score was 42.1 (SD 8.4), and median CARE score was 45 (interquartile range 14; range 11–50). The mean Stigma score was 4.6 (SD 7.6), and median Stigma score was 2.0 (interquartile range 5.0; range 0–43). The most frequently cited stigmatizing situations were: having a doctor or other healthcare professionals suggest diets when the patient did not intend to discuss weight; having a doctor saying that the patient's weight is a health problem despite the patient being in good health; having a doctor telling the patient to lose weight, but not providing treatment options or advice on how to get help; being stared at by medical staff; and having a doctor blame unrelated health problems on weight (Table 1).

Table 3.

Patient characteristics (N = 149)*

Patient Characteristic Mean (SD) or N (%)
Age (years) 47.5 (SD 10.5)
<40 39 (26.2%)
41–50 43 (27.5%)
51‐‐60 57 (38.3%)
>60 11 (7.4%)
Race/ethnicity
White, non‐Hispanic 12 (8.0)
Black, non‐Hispanic 115 (77.2)
Hispanic 19 (12.8)
Other 3 (2.0)
Marital Status
Married 35 (23.5 %)
Never married 71 (47.7%)
Divorced, widowed, separated 43 (28.9%)
Education level
High school or less 97 (65.1 %)
Some college or more 52 (34.9 %)
Employment
Employed 53 (35.6 %)
Not employed 96 (64.4 %)
Body mass index (kg m−2) 39.4 (SD 7.0)
30–34.9 43 (28.9 %)
35–39.9 51 (34.2 %)
>40 55 (36.9 %)
Physical health not good (days in past month) 8.7 (SD 10.6)
0 48 (32.2%)
1–7 49 (32.9%)
8–21 28 (18.8%)
22–30 24 (16.1%)
Mental health not good (days in past month) 8.9 (SD 11.7)
0 57 (38.3%)
1–7 40 (26.8%)
8–21 20 (13.4%)
22–30 31 (20.8%)
*

Total may not add to 149 because of missing data.

Table 4 describes results from the bivariate analysis. BMI was significantly associated with Stigma score (p = 0.001), but not with the CARE score (p = 0.469). The only other patient characteristic that was significantly associated with Stigma score was physical health (p = 0.029). None of the patient characteristics was associated with CARE score; however, Stigma score was significantly associated with CARE score (p = 0.0005). The proportional odds models showed that each increase in BMI category was associated with almost twofold increased odds of higher perception of Stigma (odds ratio (OR), 1.90, 95% confidence interval (CI) 1.30–2.78, p = 0.001). Additionally, each increase in CARE category was associated with 40% lower odds of higher Stigma (OR, 0.62, 95% CI 0.47–0.81). Results did not change after controlling for physical health, mental health and practice site. Furthermore, for each increase in Stigma category, the odds of higher CARE score decreased by half (OR, 0.52, 95% CI 0.36‐0.75, p = 0.0005); in other words, for each increase in Stigma category, the odds of lower CARE scored doubled. This association between Stigma and CARE score did not change after adjusting for BMI, physical health, mental health and practice site. No significant interactions were found between BMI and CARE in the Stigma models, nor BMI and Stigma in the CARE models.

Table 4.

Bivariate association of patient characteristics with Stigma and CARE scores

Patient characteristic Stigma score mean (SD) CARE score mean (SD) Stigma score association (p‐value)* CARE score association (p‐value)*
Age (years) 0.43 0.643
≤40 3.6 (7.1) 43.2 (7.5)
41–50 6.4 (9.2) 41.3 (7.9)
51–60 4.2 (6.8) 42.9 (9.3)
>60 3.6 (6.4) 39.5 (7.4)
Race 0.98 0.928
Black 4.8 (8.0) 42.1 (8.5)
Non‐black 3.8 (6.0) 42.4 (8.3)
Marital status 0.288 0.545
Married 4.5 (5.6) 43.9 (7.4)
Never married 4.4 (7.7) 41.3 (8.9)
Divorced, widowed, separated 4.9 (8.8) 42.2 (8.2)
Educational level 0.098 0.821
High school or less 5.2 (8.4) 41.9 (8.5)
Some college or more 3.4 (5.6) 42.6 (8.3)
Employment status 0.101 0.310
Employed 3.1 (5.1) 43.8 (6.8)
Not employed 5.4 (8.6) 41.3 (9.1)
Body mass index (kg m−2) 0.001 0.469
30–34.9 4.0 (9.5) 42.8 (9.1)
35–39.9 3.5 (5.3) 41.7 (8.3)
>40 6.0 (7.6) 42.1 (8.1)
Physical health not good (days in past month) 0.029 0.17
0 2.8 (4.7) 44.0 (6.9)
1–7 3.9 (6.8) 43.4 (7.8)
8–21 5.5 (6.4) 38.1 (8.1)
22–30 8.3 (12.5) 40.9 (11.0)
Mental health not good (days in past month) 0.141 0.20
0 2.8 (4.5) 44.1 (7.3)
1–7 4.0 (5.0) 41.1 (9.1)
8–21 6.0 (11.0) 41.0 (7.5)
22–30 7.7 (10.9) 40.9 (9.8)
Practice site 0.147 0.48
1 7.9 (10.1) 42.3 (7.1)
2 3.2 (6.1) 45.1 (6.3)
3 3.0 (4.1) 39.3 (9.1)
4 5.2 (9.3) 40.7 (10.8)
*

P‐values were calculated using Mantel–Haenszel chi‐squared test for ordinal data.

CARE, Consultation and Relational Empathy measure; SD, standard deviation; Stigma, Stigma Situations in Health Care instrument.

Discussion

This study confirms previous studies demonstrating perceived weight discrimination increases with BMI 34. While BMI was not associated with perception of physician empathy, higher number of experiences of weight stigma in health care was negatively associated with perception of physician empathy. This study may have elucidated the reason behind the conflicting results from previous studies evaluating the association of BMI with lower rapport building by physicians and lower patient satisfaction with the doctor–patient relationship 15, 16, 17, 18, 19. BMI, itself, may not be contributing to poorer ratings of doctor–patient relationships; rather, the perception of weight stigma in health care may be a contributing factor in doctor–patient relationships.

The most frequently cited stigmatizing situations were related to doctors equating weight with bad health and discussing weight and diets when the women did not want to discuss weight. Guidelines from the National Institutes of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases acknowledge that patients do not want providers to assume that their presenting health problems are due to weight and suggest that providers should ask about main health concerns first, followed by respectfully asking if the patient would be willing to talk about weight 48. Additionally, when women did want to discuss weight, they were not provided with specific advice or treatments. This concurs with other research describing the desire for specific weight loss strategies and individualized plans by physicians who were empathetic, sensitive and compassionate 49. In this study, women associated receipt of generic weight loss advice with physicians lacking attention, concern and support.

This study is the first to specifically examine the association of women's perception of weight stigma in health care with their ratings of physician empathy. While Gudzune et al. found that persons who perceived negative judgement about their weight from their physicians were less likely to report high trust in their physicians, they did not specifically assess perceptions of physician empathy 50. The cross‐sectional design of this study, however, precludes any assumptions about causality. BMI and lower perception of physician empathy may also be independently influencing perceived stigma. The moderation analyses showed no interactions between BMI and CARE in the Stigma models, nor BMI and Stigma in the CARE models.

The Stigma Situations in Health Care instrument may not have captured all stigmatizing experiences, so the results may underestimate the prevalence of weight stigma. In this study, the Stigma score was skewed, with many respondents scoring 0, suggesting that these results may be conservative. If there were more women in the sample with higher perceived stigma, greater associations may have been found. This sample of women, requiring them to be established patients with at least two clinic visits within 12 months, selected for a sample of women who were already very engaged in the practice. This sample could represent individuals who were less likely to have had stigmatizing experiences in health care. Women with higher perceived stigma may be more likely to doctor shop or avoid seeking care altogether 50, 51. The convenience sample would have missed these women who potentially might have higher perceived stigma. The convenience sample may have also introduced other potential biases because of patient age, health status, time of year of recruitment or other unmeasured factors. Additionally, by collapsing the Stigma and CARE scores into categories, the power to find significant associations may have been reduced. Other studies examining physician empathy using the CARE measure, found similarly high means and skewed results, with many perfect scores of 50. 21, 30, 38, 43 Most measures assessing weight bias in health evaluate stigma as expressed by health providers towards patients, rather than patient experiences 52. The only two studies that included patient perspectives of weight stigma in health care did not have pre‐defined cut‐off points 35, 36. Sensitivity analysis of the Stigma instrument testing other cut‐points was conducted, and results did not change. While this study suggests that the Stigma measure has good internal reliability (Cronbach's alpha 0.92) in this sample of patients, there is a need to develop and test measures to examine stigma in healthcare settings in broader and more diverse populations.

There are other limitations to consider when interpreting these results. First, this study population was comprised of mostly African–American, English‐speaking women seeking care at four FQHCs in New Jersey. Results may not apply to other populations, those who are non‐English speaking or women who avoid health care because of lack of access or dissatisfaction with the healthcare system. However, the conclusions may extend to English‐speaking underserved women with obesity seeking care in other FQHCs. Second, different cultural perceptions of body weight and stigma may potentially affect these results. African–American women with obesity tend to describe excess weight as healthy and attractive and have higher levels of body satisfaction, body image and self‐esteem than White women 49, 53, 54, 55, 56. It is unknown whether African–American women experience or internalize weight stigma differently than White women, especially in healthcare settings. While experiences of weight discrimination are particularly prevalent among African–American women (24%), being African–American has not been shown to be independently associated with experiences of weight discrimination 34. Future research is needed with a larger more diverse sample to enable comparisons between women of different races.

In addition, there were five different interviewers who recruited and interviewed subjects. Subjects may have been more or less likely to report stigmatizing experiences to different interviewers. Potential biases were minimized by having standardized training, recruitment and interview protocols. A final limitation is the likelihood of some clustering of women sharing the same physician, thereby causing the true variance of the estimates to be larger than what was calculated and biasing the findings towards significance. Unfortunately, information on the participant's physicians was not obtained, so this could not be accounted for in the analyses. However, when practice site was added as a variable in the models, the results did not change. Although beyond the scope of this study, it will be important for future work to collect information on providers to evaluate whether provider‐related factors (such as gender, age, race and provider's BMI) are associated with patient perceptions of weight stigma and its impact on patient–provider relationships.

Conclusion

This study demonstrated that underserved women with higher BMI were more likely to experience weight stigmatizing situations in primary care and perceived weight stigma in health care was negatively associated with perception of physician empathy. Because of the lack of validated measures assessing patient experiences of weight stigma in healthcare settings, this study offers a new contribution of a potential Stigma Situations in Health Care instrument that could be informative as part of research efforts to understand perceptions of health care among populations with obesity. While it was beyond the scope of this study to conduct a survey validation or more comprehensive analysis of psychometrics on this measure, these are important issues for future research.

Because prior studies have documented poorer health outcomes and health habits in patients experiencing weight stigma, as well as those experiencing lower physician empathy and poorer patient–provider relationships 6, 7, 8, 9, 14, 23, 26, 29, 30, it is important to decrease weight stigma in healthcare settings. Promising interventions targeted at health professionals to decrease weight stigma include increasing self‐awareness of biases, providing education on the uncontrollable reasons for obesity and using anti‐stigma films 57, 58, 59, 60, 61, 62. In addition, interventions teaching acceptance and mindfulness in patients with obesity may decrease their perceptions of obesity‐related stigma, psychological distress and body dissatisfaction and improve healthy eating behaviours 63, 64. These findings suggest that interventions to reduce weight stigma may be warranted in primary care settings to improve doctor–patient relationships and quality of care in patients with obesity.

Conflicts of interest statement

No conflict of interest was declared.

Funding

This research was supported by the National Cancer Institute of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number R21CA165939 (JMF). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. The National Cancer Institute had no involvement in the study design; collection, analysis and interpretation of data; writing of the report; nor the decision to submit the article for publication.

Acknowledgements

We thank the following individuals for their assistance in recruiting patients and conducting the research: M. Nell Quest, Connie Pascal, Shaniqua McGraw, Gabrielle Davis. This study was conducted in conjunction with the New Jersey Primary Care Research Network.

Ferrante, J. M. , Seaman, K. , Bator, A. , Ohman‐Strickland, P. , Gundersen, D. , Clemow, L. , and Puhl, R. (2016) Impact of perceived weight stigma among underserved women on doctor–patient relationships. Obesity Science & Practice, 2: 128–135. doi: 10.1002/osp4.40.

References

  • 1. Wang Y, Beydoun MA. The obesity epidemic in the United States–gender, age, socioeconomic, racial/ethnic, and geographic characteristics: a systematic review and meta‐regression analysis. Epidemiol Rev 2007; 29: 6–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Tomiyama AJ. Weight stigma is stressful. A review of evidence for the cyclic obesity/weight‐based stigma model. Appetite 2014; 82: 8–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Andreyeva T, Puhl RM, Brownell KD. Changes in perceived weight discrimination among Americans, 1995–1996 through 2004–2006. Obesity 2008; 16: 1129–1134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Ferrante JM, Piasecki AK, Ohman‐Strickland PA, et al. Family physicians' practices and attitudes regarding care of extremely obese patients. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2009; 17: 1710–1716PMCID: PMC2953252. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Forhan M, Salas XR. Inequities in healthcare: a review of bias and discrimination in obesity treatment. Can J Diabetes 2013; 37: 205–209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Phelan SM, Burgess DJ, Yeazel MW, Hellerstedt WL, Griffin JM, van Ryn M. Impact of weight bias and stigma on quality of care and outcomes for patients with obesity. Obes Rev 2015; 16: 319–326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Carels RA, Wott CB, Young KM, Gumble A, Koball A, Oehlhof MW. Implicit, explicit, and internalized weight bias and psychosocial maladjustment among treatment‐seeking adults. Eat Behav 2010; 11: 180–185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Jackson SE, Beeken RJ, Wardle J. Obesity, perceived weight discrimination, and psychological well‐being in older adults in England. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2015; 23: 1105–1111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Wott CB, Carels RA. Overt weight stigma, psychological distress and weight loss treatment outcomes. J Health Psychol 2010; 15: 608–614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Gudzune KA, Bennett WL, Cooper LA, Bleich SN. Perceived judgment about weight can negatively influence weight loss: a cross‐sectional study of overweight and obese patients. Prev Med 2014; 62: 103–107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Sutin AR, Terracciano A. Perceived weight discrimination and obesity. PLoS One 2013; 8: e70048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Hansson LM, Rasmussen F. Association between perceived health care stigmatization and BMI change. Obes Facts 2014; 7: 211–220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Jackson SE, Beeken RJ, Wardle J. Perceived weight discrimination and changes in weight, waist circumference, and weight status. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2014; 22: 2485–2488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Doyle C, Lennox L, Bell D. A systematic review of evidence on the links between patient experience and clinical safety and effectiveness. BMJ Open 2013; 3: e001570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Gudzune KA, Beach MC, Roter DL, Cooper LA. Physicians build less rapport with obese patients. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2013; 21: 2146–2152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Huizinga MM, Cooper LA, Bleich SN, Clark JM, Beach MC. Physician respect for patients with obesity. J Gen Intern Med 2009; 24: 1236–1239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Bottone FG, Musich S, Wang SS, Hommer CE, Yeh CS, Hawkins K. Obese older adults report high satisfaction and positive experiences with care. BMC Health Serv Res 2014; 14: 220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Fong RL, Bertakis KD, Franks P. Association between obesity and patient satisfaction. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2006; 14: 1402–1411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Gudzune KA, Huizinga MM, Cooper LA. Impact of patient obesity on the patient–provider relationship. Patient Educ Couns 2011; 85: e322–e325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Mercer SW, Reynolds WJ. Empathy and quality of care. Br J Gen Pract 2002; 52(Supp l): S9–S12. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Mercer SW, Jani BD, Maxwell M, Wong SY, Watt GC. Patient enablement requires physician empathy: a cross‐sectional study of general practice consultations in areas of high and low socioeconomic deprivation in Scotland. BMC Fam Pract 2012; 13: 6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Mercer SW, Neumann M, Wirtz M, Fitzpatrick B, Vojt G. General practitioner empathy, patient enablement, and patient‐reported outcomes in primary care in an area of high socio‐economic deprivation in Scotland–a pilot prospective study using structural equation modeling. Patient Educ Couns 2008; 73: 240–245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Price S, Mercer SW, MacPherson H. Practitioner empathy, patient enablement and health outcomes: a prospective study of acupuncture patients. Patient Educ Couns 2006; 63: 239–245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Menendez ME, Chen NC, Mudgal CS, Jupiter JB, Ring D. Physician empathy as a driver of hand surgery patient satisfaction. J Hand Surg Am 2015; 40: 1860–1865 e1862. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Parrish RC 2nd, Menendez ME, Mudgal CS, Jupiter JB, Chen NC, Ring D. Patient satisfaction and its relation to perceived visit duration with a hand surgeon. J Hand Surg Am 2015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Attar HS, Chandramani S. Impact of physician empathy on migraine disability and migraineur compliance. Ann Indian Acad Neurol 2012; 15(Suppl 1): S89–S94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Bikker AP, Mercer SW, Reilly D. A pilot prospective study on the consultation and relational empathy, patient enablement, and health changes over 12 months in patients going to the Glasgow Homoeopathic Hospital. J Altern Complement Med 2005; 11: 591–600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. MacPherson H, Mercer SW, Scullion T, Thomas KJ. Empathy, enablement, and outcome: an exploratory study on acupuncture patients' perceptions. J Altern Complement Med 2003; 9: 869–876. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Neumann M, Wirtz M, Bollschweiler E, et al. Determinants and patient‐reported long‐term outcomes of physician empathy in oncology: a structural equation modelling approach. Patient Educ Couns 2007; 69: 63–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Rakel D, Barrett B, Zhang Z, et al. Perception of empathy in the therapeutic encounter: effects on the common cold. Patient Educ Couns 2011; 85: 390–397. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Steinhausen S, Ommen O, Antoine SL, Koehler T, Pfaff H, Neugebauer E. Short‐ and long‐term subjective medical treatment outcome of trauma surgery patients: the importance of physician empathy. Patient Prefer Adher 2014; 8: 1239–1253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Flegal KM, Carroll MD, Kit BK, Ogden CL. Prevalence of obesity and trends in the distribution of body mass index among US adults, 1999‐2010. JAMA 2012; 307: 491–497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Ogden CL, Lamb MM, Carroll MD, Flegal KM. Obesity and Socioeconomic Status in Adults: United States 1988–1994 and 2005–2008. National Center for Health Statistics: Hyattsville, MD, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Puhl RM, Andreyeva T, Brownell KD. Perceptions of weight discrimination: prevalence and comparison to race and gender discrimination in America. Int J Obes (Lond) 2008; 32: 992–1000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Myers A, Rosen JC. Obesity stigmatization and coping: relation to mental health symptoms, body image, and self‐esteem. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 1999; 23: 221–230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Puhl RM, Brownell KD. Confronting and coping with weight stigma: an investigation of overweight and obese adults. Obesity 2006; 14: 1802–1815. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Mercer SW, Maxwell M, Heaney D, et al. The consultation and relational empathy (CARE) measure: development and preliminary validation and reliability of an empathy‐based consultation process measure. Fam Pract 2004; 21: 699–705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Mercer SW, McConnachie A, Maxwell M, Heaney D, Watt GC. Relevance and practical use of the Consultation and Relational Empathy (CARE) Measure in general practice. Fam Pract 2005; 22: 328–334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Fung CS, Hua A, Tam L, Mercer SW. Reliability and validity of the Chinese version of the CARE measure in a primary care setting in Hong Kong. Fam Pract 2009; 26: 398–406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Mercer SW, Fung CS, Chan FW, Wong FY, Wong SY, Murphy D. The Chinese‐version of the CARE measure reliably differentiates between doctors in primary care: a cross‐sectional study in Hong Kong. BMC Fam Pract 2011; 12: 43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Kersten P, White PJ, Tennant A. The consultation and relational empathy measure: an investigation of its scaling structure. Disabil Rehabil 2012; 34: 503–509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Aomatsu M, Abe H, Abe K, et al. Validity and reliability of the Japanese version of the CARE measure in a general medicine outpatient setting. Fam Pract 2014; 31: 118–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Bikker AP, Fitzpatrick B, Murphy D, Mercer SW. Measuring empathic, person‐centred communication in primary care nurses: validity and reliability of the Consultation and Relational Empathy (CARE) Measure. BMC Fam Pract 2015; 16: 149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Hanzevacki M, Jakovina T, Bajic Z, Tomac A, Mercer S. Reliability and validity of the Croatian version of Consultation and Relational Empathy (CARE) measure in primary care setting. Croat Med J 2015; 56: 50–56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Center for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) . Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System Survey Questionnaire. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention: Atlanta, Georgia, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Jensen MD, Ryan DH, Apovian CM, et al. 2013 AHA/ACC/TOS Guideline for the Management of Overweight and Obesity in Adults: a report of the American College of Cardiology/American Heart Association Task Force on Practice Guidelines and The Obesity Society. J Am Coll Cardiol 2014; 63: 2985–3023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Gameroff MJ. Using the proportional odds model for health‐related outcomes: why, when, and how with various SAS procedures; paper 205–30 In: SAS Institute Inc (ed.). Proceedings of the Thirtieth Annual SAS® Users Group International Conference. SAS Institute Inc: Cary, NC, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Weight‐control Information Network . Talking with Patients about Weight Loss: Tips for Primary Care Providers. National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services: Bethesda, MD, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Chugh M, Friedman AM, Clemow LP, Ferrante JM. Women weigh in: obese African American and White women's perspectives on physicians' roles in weight management. J Am Board Fam Med 2013; 26: 421–428PMCID: 3791510. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Gudzune KA, Bennett WL, Cooper LA, Bleich SN. Patients who feel judged about their weight have lower trust in their primary care providers. Patient Educ Couns 2014; 97: 128–131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Gudzune KA, Bleich SN, Richards TM, Weiner JP, Hodges K, Clark JM. Doctor shopping by overweight and obese patients is associated with increased healthcare utilization. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2013; 21: 1328–1334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. DePierre JA, Puhl RM. Experiences of weight stigmatization: a review of self‐report assessment measures. Obes Facts 2012; 5: 897–918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Anderson LA, Eyler AA, Galuska DA, Brown DR, Brownson RC. Relationship of satisfaction with body size and trying to lose weight in a national survey of overweight and obese women aged 40 and older, United States. Prev Med 2002; 35: 390–396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Thomas AM, Moseley G, Stallings R, Nichols‐English G, Wagner PJ. Perceptions of obesity: Black and White differences. J Cult Divers 2008; 15: 174–180. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Thomas S, Ness RB, Thurston RC, Matthews K, Chang CC, Hess R. Racial differences in perception of healthy body weight in midlife women: results from the Do Stage Transitions Result in Detectable Effects study. Menopause 2013; 20: 269–273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Pickett S, Peters RM. Beliefs about personal weight among African American women. Clin Nurs Res 2015. doi:10.1177/1054773815606693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Poustchi Y, Saks NS, Piasecki AK, Hahn KA, Ferrante JM. Brief intervention effective in reducing weight bias in medical students. Fam Med 2013; 45: 345–348. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Swift JA, Tischler V, Markham S, et al. Are anti‐stigma films a useful strategy for reducing weight bias among trainee healthcare professionals? Results of a pilot randomized control trial. Obes Facts 2013; 6: 91–102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. McVey GL, Walker KS, Beyers J, Harrison HL, Simkins SW, Russell‐Mayhew S. Integrating weight bias awareness and mental health promotion into obesity prevention delivery: a public health pilot study. Prev Chronic Dis 2013; 10: E46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Falker AJ, Sledge JA. Utilizing a bariatric sensitivity educational module to decrease bariatric stigmatization by healthcare professionals. Bariatr Nurs Surg Patient Care 2011; 6: 73–78. [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Gujral H, Tea C, Sheridan M. Evaluation of nurse's attitudes toward adult patients of size. Surg Obes Relat Dis 2011; 7: 536–540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Lee M, Ata RN, Brannick MT. Malleability of weight‐biased attitudes and beliefs: a meta‐analysis of weight bias reduction interventions. Body Image 2014; 11: 251–259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Lillis J, Hayes SC, Bunting K, Masuda A. Teaching acceptance and mindfulness to improve the lives of the obese: a preliminary test of a theoretical model. Ann Behav Med 2009; 37: 58–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Weineland S, Hayes SC, Dahl J. Psychological flexibility and the gains of acceptance‐based treatment for post‐bariatric surgery: six‐month follow‐up and a test of the underlying model. Clin Obes 2012; 2: 15–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Obesity Science & Practice are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES