Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2016 May 24;5:e13238. doi: 10.7554/eLife.13238

Gap junction networks in mushroom bodies participate in visual learning and memory in Drosophila

Qingqing Liu 1,2,, Xing Yang 3,, Jingsong Tian 1,2, Zhongbao Gao 1,2, Meng Wang 1, Yan Li 1,*, Aike Guo 1,3,*
Editor: K VijayRaghavan4
PMCID: PMC4909397  PMID: 27218450

Abstract

Gap junctions are widely distributed in the brains across species and play essential roles in neural information processing. However, the role of gap junctions in insect cognition remains poorly understood. Using a flight simulator paradigm and genetic tools, we found that gap junctions are present in Drosophila Kenyon cells (KCs), the major neurons of the mushroom bodies (MBs), and showed that they play an important role in visual learning and memory. Using a dye coupling approach, we determined the distribution of gap junctions in KCs. Furthermore, we identified a single pair of MB output neurons (MBONs) that possess a gap junction connection to KCs, and provide strong evidence that this connection is also required for visual learning and memory. Together, our results reveal gap junction networks in KCs and the KC-MBON circuit, and bring new insight into the synaptic network underlying fly’s visual learning and memory.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13238.001

Research Organism: D. melanogaster

Introduction

In addition to chemical synapses, neurons communicate via gap junctions, also called electrical synapses. A gap junction consists of an intercellular channel that is formed by two hemichannels, each containing six homo- or heteromeric protein subunits. These subunits belong to the connexin or pannexin gene families in vertebrates and the innexin (inx) gene family in invertebrates. Gap junctions allow the circulation of small molecules, such as ions and cAMP (Hervé and Derangeon, 2013; Phelan and Starich, 2001). The function of gap junctions in noise reduction (DeVries et al., 2002; Veruki and Hartveit, 2002) and neuronal synchronization (Bloomfield and Völgyi, 2009; Pereda et al., 2013) is critical for visual and olfactory processing in mammals. In addition, the synchronization of neurons through the function of gap junctions was proposed to play a key role in cognitive processes, such as perception, attention, as well as learning and memory (Bissiere et al., 2011; Hormuzdi et al., 2004; Tamás et al., 2000). The complexity of the mammalian central nervous system makes it difficult to determine the function of gap junctions during cognitive processes. In comparison, insects have much simpler neural systems, yet still perform sophisticated cognitive functions, however, the role of gap junctions in cognition has rarely been investigated.

There are 8 types of inx genes expressed in Drosophila. In the brain, it has been reported that ogre (inx1), inx2 and inx3 were expressed in interstitial glia and lamina with similar expression patterns; the expression of inx5 and inx7 were detected in the cell body rind with very similar domains, and inx6 was detected uniquely in an outer layer of cell body rind; inx6 and inx7 were also discovered between the anterior paired lateral (APL) neuron and dorsal paired medial (DPM) neuron; shakB (inx8) was expressed in scattered neurons, giant fiber system and antenna lobe (Stebbings et al., 2002; Wu et al., 2011; Yaksi and Wilson, 2010). Gap junctions in giant fiber system are necessary for the escape behavior (Phelan et al., 1996). Gap junctions connect L1 with L2 neurons in the lamina, allowing for simultaneous activation of both pathways (Joesch et al., 2010). Gap junctions in olfactory glomeruli mediate lateral excitation which promotes olfaction sensitivity (Yaksi and Wilson, 2010). Especially, heterotypic gap junctions between APL and DPM neuron are critical for anesthesia-sensitive memory (Wu et al., 2011).

Mushroom bodies (MBs) are a pair of clearly distinguishable structures in the Drosophila brain, and play critical roles in cognition, such as olfactory coding (Lei et al., 2013; Masse et al., 2009), olfactory learning and memory (Davis, 2011; Wolf et al., 1998),context generalization (Liu et al., 1999), feature extraction (Peng et al., 2007), reversal learning (Ren et al., 2012; Wu et al., 2012), and value based decision making (Tang and Guo, 2001; Zhang et al., 2007). The precise role of MBs in visual learning and memory, however, remains under debate. It was thought previously that MBs are not necessary for visual operant conditioning in a flight simulator system (Wolf et al., 1998); however, recently it was reported that the MB γ lobe participates in visual learning and memory in color-based visual learning assays in groups of walking flies (Vogt et al., 2014). Kenyon cells (KCs), the major neurons of MBs, are the main components of the MB neural network, which is composed of dopamine neurons (DAs), MB output neurons (MBONs), and other cells (Aso et al., 2014a, b; Tanaka et al., 2008). While eight inx genes are expressed in Drosophila, the existence of functional gap junctions in KCs remains to be investigated. Here, we set out to study the presence and function of gap junctions in MBs, using the visual conditioning paradigm in a flight simulator as well as dye coupling and whole-cell recording approach.

Results

Non-chemical-synaptic transmission in MBs participates in visual learning and memory

To study the role of MBs in visual cognition in flying flies, we combined an optogenetic stimulator with a flight simulator system. A laser was focused to an area between the two compound eyes of the fly to activate halorhodopsin (eNpHR3.0) (Gradinaru et al., 2010), and all-trans retinal (ATR) was replaced with β-carotene to avoid photolysis (Figure 1A, Figure 1—figure supplement 1A–D). We found that visual learning and memory was impaired when α/β neurons of KCs, driven by 17d-GAL4, were hyperpolarized, and the memory was restored when GAL4 activity was blocked in MBs by MB247-GAL80 (Figure 1B, upper panel). These experiments were repeated with a second UAS-NpHR strain, yielding similar results (Figure 1—figure supplement 1E). Hyperpolarizing α’/β’ neurons with MB370B-GAL4, γ neurons with MB131B-GAL4, or α/β and γ neurons of KCs with MB247-GAL4 also abolished visual learning and memory (Figure 1B lower panel). These results indicate that KCs participate in visual learning and memory. However, when synaptic release was impaired via expression of shibirets1(shits1) (Kasuya et al., 2009; Praefcke and McMahon, 2004; van der Bliek and Meyerowitz, 1991) in KCs by 17d-GAL4, MB370B-GAL4 or MB247-GAL4, visual learning and memory was intact (Figure 1C). Tetanus toxin (TNT) was also used to inhibit the chemical-synaptic transmission. When TNT was expressed in α/β neurons (17d-GAL4) or all KCs (VT61721-GAL4), fly’s visual learning and memory remained intact (Figure 1—figure supplement 2). In addition to blocking the output of chemical synapses, hyperpolarization could also affect other mechanisms that influence visual learning and memory. To confirm the effect of hyperpolarization in KCs, Kir2.1, ectopic expression of an inward rectifier potassium channel that induces irreversible hyperpolarization was achieved in KCs by using the MB247–GeneSwitch GAL4 driver (MBSwitch) (Osterwalder et al., 2001). As shown in Figure 1D, visual learning and memory were again abolished. However, in agreement with previous report (Wolf et al., 1998), visual learning and memory remained intact in flies of which MBs were ablated by hydroxyurea (HU) (Figure 1—figure supplement 3). Taking together, these results indicated that visual learning and memory might be influenced in real time by non-chemical-synaptic transmission in MBs. We therefore speculated that electrical-synaptic transmission via gap junctions was a plausible candidate for such observation.

Figure 1. Visual learning and memory is abolished upon hyperpolarization of KCs.

(A) Setup of the optogenetic flight simulator system. A 593 nm laser was used to activate halorhodopsin. To avoid influencing the visual perception of the flies, a focuser was used to focus the laser into a Gaussian light spot and to direct it in between the two compound eyes (diameter, ~0.3 mm). (B) Visual learning and memory was impaired when UAS-eNpHR3.0-YFP was used to hyperpolarize KCs labeled with 17d-GAL4, MB370B-GAL4, MB131B-GAL4 and MB247-GAL4. The expression pattern of these GAL4 lines are illustrated above the bars. MB247-GAL80 restored memory impaired by17d-GAL4. The laser was off in the control groups (Control) and applied throughout the 24min assay (at 0.1 mW/mm2) in the test groups (Laser On). (C) Blocking the chemical synaptic output of KCs labeled with 17d-GAL4, MB370B-GAL4 and MB247-GAL4 did not affect visual learning and memory. The temperature was unchanged throughout the 24 min assay. The permissive temperature (PT) was 24 ± 1°C, and the restrictive temperature (RT) was 30 ± 0.5°C. (D) Hyperpolarizing KCs labeled with MB247-GeneSwitch impaired visual learning and memory. For 3 days prior to start of the experiment, the flies in the “RU486+” group were fed with RU486 (50 mM dissolved in EtOH, 1:100 mixed with food), and the flies in the “RU486–” group were fed with 1% EtOH in food. Scale bar, 100 μm. The expression pattern of each driver is illustrated. N=15 to 20 for each data point. All data represent the mean ± SEM. Asterisks in grey indicate the level of statistical significance of the performance indices compared against chance level. Asterisks in black indicate the level of statistical significance of the performance indices between groups. (*p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13238.002

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Establishing the optogenetic flight simulator system.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

Two difficulties have restricted the application of optogenetics to the study of Drosophila visual cognition. First, the excitation light can disturb visual perception; second, Drosophila need to take in all-trans retinal (ATR) in order to use optogenetics. However, ATR is light-sensitive and must be protected from light (Suh et al., 2007), while light deprivation leads to abnormal development of the visual system (Barth and Heisenberg, 1997). (A) To solve the first problem, we focused the laser light into a small Gaussian spot (593 nm, diameter 0.3 mm) and directed it between the two compound eyes of Drosophila. (B) Visual learning and memory under different laser intensities were tested in wild type CS and eNpHR3.0/+ flies. A laser strength of greater than 0.1 mW/mm2 impaired visual learning and memory. (C, D) To address the second problem, an optogenetic locomotor system was established. (C) Flies were individually placed into a 3.5 cm dish. The fly’s locomotor behavior was recorded by a camera with a visible light filter. Illumination was supplied by three infrared LEDs. In the test groups, three yellow LEDs supplied yellow light (590 nm, 0.06–0.1 mW/mm2). Temperature was controlled at 25 ± 1°C. Yellow arrow, recorded fly. Scale bar, 1 cm. (D) Locomotor behavior of cha-GAL4 > UAS-eNpHR3.0 and UAS-eNpHR3.0/+ flies raised in different conditions; this behavior reflects the activation of halorhodopsin. For each fly, the distance moved, time spent moving (activity) and speed were calculated. If the flies raised in a 12 hr L/D cycle were fed with ATR (changed every night), halorhodopsin would be disabled. The β-carotene has been reported to be transformed into ATR in Drosophila and is more stable in light (Wang et al., 2007; Yang and O'Tousa, 2007). Feeding flies β-carotene for 3 days after eclosion could partially rescue the halorhodopsin inactivation. Thus, β-carotene could be used in place of ATR for optogenetics. N = 18 to 20 for each data point. All data represent the mean ± SEM. (E) Hyperpolarization of KCs labeled with 17d-GAL4 with UAS-NpHR-YFP (raised with a 12 hr L/D cycle, fed with β-carotene food) also impaired visual learning and memory. Asterisks in grey and color indicate the level of statistical significance of the performance indices compared against chance level. Asterisks in black indicate the level of statistical significance of the performance indices between groups. (*p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001). No laser was used in the control group (Control). In the test group (Laser on), the laser was focused into a 0.3 mm diameter light spot (593 nm, 0.1 mW/mm2). N = 15 to 20 for each data point. All data represent the mean ± SEM.
Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Blocking the chemical synaptic output of KCs did not impair visual learning and memory.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

Visual learning and memory remained intact when chemical synaptic output of KCs labeled with 17d-GAL4 or VT61721-GAL4 was blocked all the lifetime (A) or only post-development (B) by tetanus-toxin expression. N = 10 to 16 for each data point. (C) The expression pattern of VT61721-GAL4. Scale bar, 100 μm.
Figure 1—figure supplement 3. HU-ablation of MBs did not impair visual learning and memory.

Figure 1—figure supplement 3.

(A) Flies of which MBs were ablated by HU demonstrated regular performance. (B, C) HU treatment ablated the MBs. The anti-Fasciclin II labelled brain without (B) or with (C) HU treatment. N = 12 for vehicle group, n = 11 for HU group. Scale bar, 100 μm.

Gap junctions in MBs participate in visual learning and memory

To test this hypothesis, we individually knocked down all eight inx genes individually in KCs using a RNAi approach (Wu et al., 2011). The efficiency of all RNAi was examined, and the resulting knockdown was at least 50% (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). The specificity and target sequences of RNAi are shown in Supplementary file 1. As shown in Figure 2A, knockdown of inx6 in 17d-GAL4-labeled neurons resulted in impairment of fly learning and memory. Flies with the knockdown of inx4, inx5 or inx6 were not able to achieve performance indices significantly different to zero, whereas the control groups did. The effect of gene downregulation was eliminated when the RNAi expression were blocked by MBGAL80. Visual learning and memory was also impaired in flies with the knockdown of inx4, inx5 or inx6 in MB247-GAL4-labeled KCs (Figure 2B). To exclude potential developmental effects, MBSwitch was used to induce RNAi expression in adult stage only. When inx4 was knocked down in adults, visual learning and memory was not affected, whereas adult knockdown of inx5 or inx6 exerted significant effects (Figure 2C). Similar results were obtained in experiments using independent RNAi strains (Figure 2D, JF5 target to inx5 and JF6 to inx6). Therefore, INX5 and INX6 are post-developmentally required in the α/β lobes of MBs for visual learning and memory whereas INX4 may contribute to MB development instead, thus affecting visual learning in an indirect manner.

Figure 2. Gap junctions in MBs are necessary for visual learning and memory.

(A) Knockdown of inx6 in KCs labeled with 17d-GAL4 impaired visual learning and memory. Knockdown of inx4 or inx5 eliminate the significance of the difference between the performance indices and zero. The effect of inx4, inx5 or inx6 knockdown was rescued by MBGAL80. (B, C) Knocking down inx4, inx5 or inx6 in KCs from an early development stage impaired visual learning and memory (B), while knockdown of inx4 in KCs after eclosion showed no effect (C). For 3 days before the experiment, the flies in the RU486+ group were fed with RU486 (50 mM stock dissolved in EtOH, 1:100 mixed with food), and the flies in the RU486– group were fed with 1% EtOH in food. (D) Knockdown of inx5 or inx6 in KCs labeled with 17d-GAL4 with another type of RNAi also impaired visual learning and memory. (E) Knockdown of inx5 or inx6 in MB370B split-GAL4-labeled α’/β’ neurons impaired visual learning and memory. (F) Knockdown of each of the inx genes in MB131B split-GAL4-labeled γ neurons had no effect on visual learning and memory. The expression pattern of each driver is illustrated. The experiments in (C, D) were performed as blind experiments. The following RNAi lines were used: inx1: v103816; inx2: v102194; inx3: v39094; inx4: v33277; inx5: v102814; inx6: v8638; inx7: v103256; inx8: v26801; JF5: TRiP. JF02877; and JF6: TRiP. JF02168. The specificity and target sequences of RNAi are shown in Supplementary file 1. N = 15 to 20 for each data point. All data represent the mean ± SEM. Asterisks in grey indicate the level of statistical significance of the performance indices compared against chance level. Asterisks in black indicate the level of statistical significance of the performance indices between groups. (*p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13238.006

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Effectiveness of inx RNAi lines.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

Relative amount of innexin mRNA in the manipulated flies (elav-GAL4 > UAS-inx RNAi) comparing to the control flies (elav-GAL4/+ and UAS-inx RNAi/+) is shown. The results were normalized to the relative amount of ribosomal protein 49 (Rp49). The following primers were used (forward and reverse, respectively):
Inx1: 5'-GCATCAACATTATCGTGCAGAT-3' and 5'-ACACATAGACCATGGGATCCA-3'
Inx2: 5'-ATTGTATTGTGGACGAGATCCC-3' and 5'-TTGTGGTACTTCACCTCGTCC-3'
Inx3: 5'-GATCCTCAAGTATTTCGTGAACAGT-3' and 5'-ACATCCGTACCGTAGGACATG-3'
Inx4: 5'-TGCTGCCCCTGAATATACTGA-3' and 5'-GAAGCAACTGAAGACGCATTC-3'
Inx5: 5'-GATGTCCGGTTTGAACCTTCT-3' and 5'-GTTTGGTCATGAAACGCAACT-3'
Inx6: 5'-CTGCAGATGCTCACCAGGTAT-3' and 5'-CTGATGAACACATTGAGCAGCT-3'
Inx7: 5'-ATCAAGCGGCATGCATACTAC-3' and 5'-GTTAGACCCACCATTCGAAGG-3'
Inx8: 5'-TACAATTTGCACAACTTGCCC-3' and 5'-AGTTGTCGGTATTTTCTTTTGCC-3'
Rp49: 5'-CCAAGGACTTCATCCGCCACC-3' and 5'-GCGGGTGCGCTTGTTCGATCC-3'

We further tested whether gap junctions in other KC neurons also participate in visual learning and memory. The split-GAL4s MB370B and MB131B were used to drive inx-RNAi expression in α’/β’ neurons or γ neurons, respectively. As shown in Figure 2E, knockdown of inx5 or inx6 in α’/β’ neurons abolished visual learning and memory, and knockdown of inx genes in γ neurons had no effect (Figure 2F). Together, these results suggest that the gap junctions composed of INX5/6 in α/β and α’/β’ neurons are involved in visual learning and memory in Drosophila.

Gap junctions couple KCs of the same subtype or different subtypes

To verify the existence of gap junctions in KCs, we utilized a dye coupling method. Either biocytin or Alexa Fluor 568 was loaded into one KC of each fly brain upon whole-cell recording. The dye-filled neurons were identified based on their position and morphological characteristics as previously reported (Aso et al., 2014a; Lee et al., 1999). Among the 143 successfully stained brains, 64 α/β neurons, 40 α’/β’ neurons and 39 γ neurons were loaded with dye. Dye-coupled neurons were observed in 14 brains (Figure 3A). In 10 out of the 14 brains, dye coupling was observed between the same subtype of KCs, including 8 α/β-α/β, 1 α’/β’-α’/β’ and 1 γ-γ (Figure 3A). One representative example is shown in Figure 3 and Video 1. There, biocytin was loaded into a GFP-expressing α/β posterior (α/β p) neuron (Tanaka et al., 2008) (driven by c205-GAL4). Across the calyx, the MB dendrites region, there was only one biocytin-filled fiber (Figure 3B); however, two biocytin-filled fibers were found in the peduncle and the lobe region (Figure 3C). One fiber overlapped with the GFP fluorescence, while the other fiber showed no signs of GFP expression (Figure 3D). Tracing the two fibers, we found that one of them belongs to an α/β p neuron, and the second belongs to an α/β core (α/β c) neuron (Figure 3D,E). Notably, there were three potential connection points between these two neurons, including the beginning of the lobes, the horizontal branch tip of the α/β p neuron, and the middle of the vertical branches (Figure 3D,E right panel, marked with “*”). In 4 of the 14 brains, dye coupling was observed between different KC subtypes (Figure 3A). In 2 out of the 4 brains, the dye was loaded into a single γ neuron but stained multiple γ neurons and an α’/β’ neuron (Figure 3—figure supplement 1 and Video 2). In the third brain, one α’/β’ soma was filled with dye and two α’/β’ somas, one α/β and one γ fiber were stained (Figure 3—figure supplement 2 and Video 3). In the last brain, the dye was loaded into a single α/β p neuron, and was also found in multiple α’/β’ neurons and some non-KC neurons (Figure 3—figure supplement 3 and Video 4). Together, these results suggest that gap junctions exist in both the calyx and the lobes that link KCs of the same or different subtypes as well as some non-KC neurons.

Video 1. Raw stack of the brain in Figure 3.

Download video file (7.6MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.13238.012

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13238.012

Video 2. Rotated stack of the brain in Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

Download video file (1.2MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.13238.013

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13238.013

Video 3. Raw stack of the brain in Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

Download video file (8.7MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.13238.014

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13238.014

Video 4. Raw stack of the brain in Figure 3—figure supplement 3.

Download video file (9.1MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.13238.015

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13238.015

Figure 3. Dye coupling reveals presence of gap junctions in KCs.

(A) Schematic representation of MB and summary of dye coupling results. (B–D) One example in which biocytin was loaded into one KC, labeling more than one KC. Magenta, fluorescence of UAS-mCD8::EGFP (EGFP)-labeled α/β p stratum (driven by c205-GAL4); green, biocytin-filled neurites. (B) Z stack of the calyx and somas. One biocytin-filled fiber was observed. Arrow, the biocytin-filled fiber of the patched neuron; dotted circle, the boundaries of the calyx. (C) Z stack of the peduncle and lobes. Arrowhead, second biocytin-filled fiber. PED, peduncle. (D) Z stack of the peduncle and lobes in MBs. “*”, points to the overlap between the two biocytin–filled fibers. Dotted lines show the boundaries of the α/β lobe and the α’/β’ lobe. (E) Reconstructed morphology of the MB. Left, one neurite (white) was coincident with the α/β p stratum and presumed to be an α/β p neurite; middle, the other neurite (green) was not coincident with the α/β p stratum and was presumed to be an α/β core (α/β c) neurite; Right, the 2 neurites. D, dorsal; M, medial; P, posterior. Scale bar, 30 μm.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13238.008

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Dye coupling between two γ neurons and one α’/β’ neuron.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

(A) Z stack of the soma layer of the MB. One dye-filled fiber is visible (arrowhead). (B) Z stack of the soma, calyx and peduncle (PED) of the MB. Two biocytin-filled long fibers (arrowhead) in peduncle are seen. (C) The dye–filled fibers shown in (A, B) was traced with TrackEM2. The two γ fibers are indicated in green and orange. The fibers that could belong to either of the two γ neurons or to other neurons are shown in purple. (D) The traced fibers, shown at the same angle of view as in (B). (E) Z stack of the lobes of the MB. Two γ fibers (arrowheads) and one α’ fiber (double arrowhead) were filled with dye. Magenta, the fluorescence of UAS-mCD8::EGFP (EGFP, driven by OK107-GAL4 and labeling the whole MB); green, the Alexa Fluor 568-filled neurites. Arrowhead, the dye–filled γ fibers; double arrowhead, the dye–filled α’ fiber; arrow, the loaded soma (removed with the electrode and represented by a small dotted circle). Large dotted circle, the boundary of the calyx; dotted lines, the boundaries of the α’/β’ lobe (white) and the γ lobe (blue). PED, peduncle. (F) Schematic diagram of the brain. “*”, intersection point of dye–filled fibers. Scale bar, 30 μm.
Figure 3—figure supplement 2. Dye coupling between α/β, α’/β’ and γ neurons.

Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

(A) Z stack of the soma and calyx layers of the MB. Two fluorescence-labeled somas were filled with dye (arrow). The one on the left was bright and was presumed to be the one that was loaded. This soma is also shown in (B) (arrow). (B) Z stack of the calyx and peduncle (PED) of the MB. Two biocytin-filled fibers were observed (arrowhead). (C) The lobes of the brain. Two dye-filled fibers are visible (arrowhead, α/β and γ). (D, E) The layers of the lobe were divided into 2 parts from anterior to posterior and stacked. The two biocytin-filled fibers in (C) are shown separately (arrowhead, α/β and γ). Magenta, the fluorescence of UAS-mCD8::EGFP (EGFP, driven by c305a-GAL4, labelling the α’/β’ lobe of the MB); green, biocytin-filled fibers. Arrowhead, the biocytin-filled fibers; arrow, the biocytin-filled somas; dotted circle, the boundary of the calyx; dotted lines, the boundaries of the α’/β’ lobe (white), the α/β lobe (yellow) and the γ lobe (blue). PED, peduncle. Scale bar, 30 μm. (F–H) The biocytin-filled fibers were traced with TrackEM2 and are shown from different perspectives. The α/β fibers are shown in green and the γ fiber in orange. (I) Schematic diagram of the brain. Magenta, α’/β’ somas and presumed fibers; green, the biocytin-filled α/β fiber; orange, the dye-filled γ fiber. “*”, the points where dye coupling likely occurred.
Figure 3—figure supplement 3. Dye coupling between α/β, α’/β’ and non-KC fibers.

Figure 3—figure supplement 3.

(A) Z stack of the soma layer of the MB. Two biocytin-filled somas were observed (arrow). The soma on the right was bright and abnormal in shape and was presumed to be the one that was loaded. (B) Z stack of the soma layer, calyx layer and half of the peduncle layer of the MB, shown from a different angle. Three biocytin-filled fibers are clearly visible (arrowheads). (C) Schematic diagram of the biocytin-filled soma and fibers in the calyx shown in (A, B), presented in different colors. (D) Sagittal view of the brain, showing the three biocytin-filled fibers in the peduncle (arrowheads). Two dye-filled somas are observed (double arrow) on the anterior surface of the brain. (E) Schematic diagram of the biocytin-filled fibers in the peduncle shown in (D). (F) Z stack of the lobes of the MB. Three fibers are visible in the α’/β’ lobe. Two dye-filled somas are observed (double arrow) on the anterior surface of the brain. (G) The biocytin-filled non-KC fibers are shown (double arrowhead). (H) Schematic diagram of the biocytin-filled non-KC fibers shown in (F, G). Magenta, the fluorescence of UAS-mCD8::EGFP (EGFP, driven by c205-GAL4 and labelling the α/β p stratum of the MB and FB); green, the biocytin-filled fibers. Arrowhead, the biocytin-filled fibers; double arrowhead, the biocytin-filled non-KC fibers; arrow, the biocytin-filled KC somas; double arrow, the biocytin-filled non-KC soma. Dotted circle, the boundary of the calyx; dotted lines, the boundaries of the α’/β’ lobe (white), the α/β lobe (yellow) and the γ lobe (blue). “*”, the presumed points of dye coupling. PED, peduncle; VL, vertical lobes; ML, medial lobes; FB, fan-shaped body; CA, calyx; ACA, accessory calyx. Scale bar, 30 μm.

To rule out false-positive results in the dye coupling experiments, blind experiments were performed in flies with inx5/6 double knockdown in all KCs (driven by VT61721-GAL4) and wild type flies. Ninety brains were stained: 38 from wild type flies and 52 from inx5/6 knock down flies. Dye coupling was observed in 5 brains of the wild type flies but in none of the inx5/6 knockdown flies (Table 1). Assuming a binomial process with p=5/38 (k = 5; n = 38 in the control group) to observe false-positive dye coupling, the probability of failing to observe any coupling in the knockdown group (k = 0; n = 52) is about 0.00065, suggesting a small likelihood for false-positive labeling.

Table 1.

Dye coupling in wild type and inx5/6 knockdown flies.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13238.016

WT INX 5&6 KD
Loaded neurons αβ 21 27
α’β’ 9 10
γ 8 15
Dye coupling 5 0

Data are from blind experiments. 38 wild type (WT) and 52 inx5/6 knockdown (KD) brains were loaded with biocytin. The numbers of brains for each condition are shown.

Gap junctions between MBs and MBONs participate in visual learning and memory

In our dye coupling experiments, we found some dye-stained fibers from the area of β’ lobe to areas outside the MBs and two dye-stained non-KC somas (Figure 3—figure supplement 3 and Video 4). Based on the morphology of these fibers and the position of the somas, these non-KC neurons were likely to be MBONs. MBON-β’2mp neurons which have somas on the dorsal anterior surface near the mid-line of the brain, and arborize on the tip of β’ lobe, were supposed to be reasonable candidates. To investigate the possible existence of an efferent pathway mediated by gap junctions in MBs, we explored whether there were electrical synapses between MBONs and KCs. We used either the proton pump eArch3.0 (Mattis et al., 2012) or halorhodopsin (Gradinaru et al., 2010) to hyperpolarize KCs (247LexA > LexAop-eArch3.0-EYFP or 247LexA > LexAop-eNpHR3.0-EYFP), and MBONs were labeled with split GAL4-driven tdTomato (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Using whole-cell recording, we monitored the changes in membrane potential in MBONs whilst KCs were hyperpolarized simultaneously using a yellow laser (Figure 4A). Considering that the electrical coupling coefficient might not be high (Yaksi and Wilson, 2010), we used the maximal power of our laser, which should be the most promising condition to get obvious results. Inasmuch as there was no significant difference between the effect of eArch3.0 and eNpHR3.0 to hyperpolarize KCs (Figure 4—figure supplement 1C), the data using eArch3.0 or eNpHR3.0 were pooled together for analysis. Upon hyperpolarization in KCs, we measured a hyperpolarization in MBON-β’2mp neurons labelled by MB002B-GAL4 or MB011B-GAL4. This response remained unchanged after the application of tetrodotoxin (TTX), which blocks chemical synapses by suppressing action potentials (Figure 4B). In contrast, laser-induced hyperpolarization disappeared when gap junctions were blocked by 2-octanol (2-OCT) (Bohrmann and Haas-Assenbaum, 1993), and was recovered after 2-OCT was washed out (Figure 4C), while 2-OCT application could induce slight hyperpolarization (Figure 4—figure supplement 2). The delivery of 2-OCT did not affect the optogenetic hyperpolarization in KCs (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). These results suggest that β’2mp MBONs are able to receive inputs from KCs through electrical synapses.

Figure 4. Electrical coupling reveals presence of gap junctions between KCs andβ’2mp neurons.

(A) Strategy used to detect electrical coupling between MBONs and KCs. Membrane potentials of MBON somata were recorded during hyperpolarization of KCs using optogenetics. The recorded soma was loaded with biocytin for later identification. (B) Whole-cell recordings were performed on MBON-β’2mp neurons while KCs were hyperpolarized using laser light with emission at 593 nm (yellow bar). Hyperpolarization was observed in the recorded β’2mp neurons. TTX delivery did not affect hyperpolarization. Black traces, average of the membrane-potential traces for each recorded β’2mp neurons (n = 10). Red traces, average of the black traces. Box plots show the peaks of the hyperpolarization amplitudes (Hyper. Amp.) before and after TTX delivery. (C) MBON β’2mp neurons were recorded as in (B). Hyperpolarization was eliminated after the application of the gap junction blocker 2-OCT and recovered after washout of 2-OCT (n=9). Box plots show the hyperpolarization amplitudes. Performance indices represent the mean ± SEM.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13238.017

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. No difference observed between NpHR- and Arch-mediated hyperpolarization of KCs.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(A) Scheme for detecting KC hyperpolarization. (B) Current-clamp traces showing hyperpolarization evoked in KCs using laser light with emission at 593 nm. The KCs expressed eArch3.0 (upper, n = 5) or eNpHR3.0 (lower, n = 13). Black traces, average of the membrane-potential traces for each recorded KCs. Red traces, average of the black traces. (C) Box plots show the peaks of the hyperpolarization amplitudes of KCs expressing eNpHR3.0 or eArch3.0. (D) Hyperpolarization evoked in a KC expressed eArch3.0 (left) or eNpHR3.0 (right) before and after 2-OCT delivery. Gray traces, the membrane potential of the recorded KC. Black traces, the average of the gray traces.
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. Membrane-potential traces of MBON-β’2mp neurons in the OCT experiments.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

The same data shown in Figure 4C, without baseline calibration. Yellow bar, 593 nm laser. Black traces, average of the membrane-potential traces for each recorded β’2mp neurons. Red traces, average of the black traces.

During whole-cell recording, biocytin was loaded into the recorded neuron, therefore, the identity of this recorded neuron could be subsequently confirmed (Figure 4A and Materials and methods). In 3 of the 35 recorded brains, dye coupling was observed between a β’2mp neuron and an α’/β’ neuron. One example is shown in Figure 5, Video 5 and 6. In addition to the arborization of the β’2mp neuron, biocytin also stained neurites in the β’ lobe (Figure 5A,B), vertical lobe stalk (Figure 5A–C), tip of the α’ lobe (Figure 5A,D), peduncle (Figure 5A, D, E) and calyx (Figure 5E), indicating the presence of gap junctions between β’2mp neurons and α’/β’ neurons (Figure 5F).

Video 5. Raw stack of the anterior half of the brain in Figure 5.

Download video file (3.2MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.13238.020

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13238.020

Video 6. Raw stack of the brain in Figure 5.

Download video file (2.3MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.13238.021

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13238.021

Figure 5. Dye coupling between KCs and β’2mp neurons.

Figure 5.

(A) Z stack of MB lobes and half of the peduncle. Biocytin-filled neurites (arrowhead) are observed within the boundary of the α’/β’ lobe (dotted line) and peduncle (PED). (B–D) The lobe layers were divided into 3 parts from anterior to posterior and then stacked. In each stack, biocytin-filled neurites are visible within the boundaries of theα’/β’ lobe and peduncle. (E) Z stack of KC somas, the calyx and half of the peduncle. One biocytin-filled fiber is seen Green, biocytin-loaded neurites; magenta, the fluorescence of YFP-labeled KCs (driven by MB247-lexA); blue, the fluorescence of td-Tomato-labeled MBONs (driven by MB011B-GAL4). Arrow, arborizations of β’2mp neurons projecting to the tip of the β’ lobe; arrowhead, the biocytin-filled KC fibers; dotted line, the boundary of the α’/β’ lobe. PED, peduncle. Scale bar, 30 μm. (F) Schematic diagram of the brain. White, the α’/β’ neuron that was dye-coupled with the β’2mp neurons. “*”, the presumed points of gap junctions.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13238.022

Next, we investigated whether the gap junctions in β’2mp neurons participate in visual learning and memory. Each of the 8 inx genes were knocked down in β’2mp neurons individually using the split-GAL4 MB002B. As shown in Figure 6, visual learning and memory were ablated following inx6 knockdown using two set of RNAi strains from independent sources. Downregulation of inx5 with the RNAi strain TRiP. JF02877 also abolished visual learning and memory (Figure 6). Taken together, our results provide strong evidence that a gap junction network containing KCs and β’2mp neurons is crucially involved in visual learning and memory.

Figure 6. Gap junctions between KCs and β’2mp neurons are necessary for visual learning and memory.

Figure 6.

Knockdown of inx5 or inx6 in β’2mp neurons results in impaired visual learning and memory. The following RNAi lines were used: inx1: v103816; inx2: v102194; inx3: v39094; inx4: v33277; inx5: v102814; inx6: v8638; inx7: v103256; inx8: v26801; JF5: TRiP. JF02877; and JF6: TRiP. JF02168. N=17 to 20 for each data point. Asterisks in grey indicate the level of statistical significance of the performance indices compared against chance level. Asterisks in black indicate the level of statistical significance of the performance indices between groups. (*p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13238.023

Discussion

The function of the MBs, has now been studied for several decades. However, a possible involvement of gap junctions in cognition had not been well addressed, especially in KCs. Here, we showed that in addition to chemical synapses, gap junctions also exist in KCs and KC-associated neural networks, and play important roles in visual learning and memory. Compared with chemical synapses, gap junctions are simple, fast, and non-discrete for information transfer in neural networks. Similar to their function as a low-pass filter in reducing noise during sensory processing (DeVries et al., 2002; Veruki and Hartveit, 2002; Zhang et al., 2013), gap junctions in KCs may contribute to noise reduction, especially those that connect KCs of the same subtype with common inputs. Considering that the different KC subtypes have diverse functions (Guven-Ozkan and Davis, 2014; Vogt et al., 2014; Yi et al., 2013), gap junctions that connect KCs of different subtypes may serve as a communication channel that links multiple processing units. The gating, assembly, internalization and degradation of gap junction channels can be delicately modulated by phosphorylation (Laird, 2005; Moreno, 2005; Solan and Lampe, 2005); and these regulation may underlie memory encoding in MB-MBON networks.

Our results showed that approximately 10% of all KCs possess active gap junction connections, which is very likely to be an underestimate of their density, due to the following reasons: 1) The dye coupling efficiency may be restricted with the gating of gap junction channels, which might not be always open and which might be delicately modulated; 2) filling an entire neuron with sufficient amounts of dye is a difficult and time-consuming (1–2 hr) procedure, due to the slender morphology of their neurites. According to electron microscope results from our lab (unpublished data), KC neurites are extremely narrow in various sections, often only 50 nm in diameter. However, some organelles in the neurites, such as mitochondria, are quite big, with the diameter more than 500 nm. Thus, the diffusion of dye in KCs might be interrupted when neurites were congested by organelles. In general, Alexa Fluor 568 (791.8 Da) stained less details of the neurites than biocytin (372.48 Da) in KCs (Figure 3—figure supplements 1 and 2). This difference also suggested that the KC neurites are not free enough for the dye diffusion. It is therefore unsurprising that the neurites of the coupled neurons were rarely completely filled with dye, with similar reasoning for the soma parts. The low efficiency of dye coupling was also evident in our results from β’2mp neurons. In this study, the probability of dye coupling was approximately 1/12 the probability of electrical coupling, which we believe is reasonable, as electrical currents flow more easily than dyes, both in neurites and through gap junctions. We could not exclude the possibility that the distribution of gap junctions between KCs, and between KCs and MBONs is rare, and possibly differs substantially across individual flies. It is worth noting that the proportion of simultaneously opened gap junctions is probably not very high, thus synchronizing only a minority of KCs. Otherwise, it would be akin to creating a single processing unit, and the MBs would lose their processing capacity. Thus, such fine-tuned modulation of gap junctions may be critical for MB function.

Our findings indicate that INX 5 and 6 in KCs and MBON-β’2mp neurons are necessary for visual learning and memory. Nevertheless, the features of gap junctions involved are not yet determined, such as the constitution (homotypic or heterotypic), the conductance and the rectification properties of particular gap junction channels in particular neural networks. Combining whole-cell recording with both depolarizing and hyperpolarizing optogenetic tools could be helpful to study the electrical physiological properties of gap junctions, and further clarify the dynamics of neural networks with both chemical synapses and gap junctions.

Our results suggest that MBs take part in visual learning and memory via gap junctions. However, in previous reports, visual learning remained intact in the mbm1 mutant and MBs-ablated flies (Liu et al., 1999; Wolf et al., 1998). The experiment was repeated with HU-treated MBs-ablated flies in our flight simulator, yielding similar results (Figure 1—figure supplement 3). Therefore, this unconformity is unlikely to be caused by the upgrading of the flight simulator. Developmental compensation may be a reasonable explanation for this discrepancy in results. HU inhibits ribonucleotide reductase, blocking the synthesis of DNA, and causing the death of dividing cells (Sweeney et al., 2012). It has been reported that the trajectory of the neurons which proliferated during larva period develop independently of each other, although the final branching pattern of these neurons is dependent upon the presence of appropriate neuronal targets (Lovick and Hartenstein, 2015). In the MB ablated flies, MB-targeting neurons might still grow to the MB region and form new circuits with the remainder γ neurons which proliferated before larval hatching and therefore could not be ablated by HU (Figure 1—figure supplement 2C). The developmental compensation might alleviate the change of the topological structure and function of the MB-associated neural network. A comparison of the MB-associated network between the MB-ablated flies and wild type flies, including the topological structure and the physiological responses during visual operant conditioning, would be helpful to understand the neural computations underlying visual cognition.

Recently, it was reported that blocking the output of MB γ lobes with shits affects visual learning and memory when assessed using two color-based, multiple walking fly participation paradigms (Vogt et al., 2014). However, in the experiments using a flight simulator, visual conditioning was not affected when the chemical synaptic output of the α/β and γ lobes was inhibited by shits or TNT. This discrepancy can probably be attributed to the differences between the behavioral paradigms. In the paradigms for the walking flies (Vogt et al., 2014), to ensure the blocking of shits, the flies was tested at high temperature (33°C), but in flight simulator, files could not accomplish the visual learning and memory task when the temperature was higher than 30°C. Moreover, the efficiency of TNT could not be tested in these behavior paradigms. Thus, we cannot formally rule out the possible roles of chemical transmission in visual learning and memory. Our results show that gap junctions in the α/β and α’/β’ lobes, but not those in the γ lobes, are required for the operant visual learning, suggesting that the chemical synaptic circuits and electrical synaptic circuits may be complementary in distribution and function. Therefore, we propose that gap junctions cooperate with chemical synapses to perform the complex function of MBs. Tracing the gap junction networks in Drosophila and clarifying the synergy between gap junctions and chemical synapses should contribute to our understanding of functional neural calculations.

Materials and methods

Fly strains

Flies were reared in standard food vials at 25°C and 60% relative humidity with a 12 hr dark/light (L/D) cycle (Guo et al., 1996), excluding the flies that were indicated as raised in the dark and the flies crossed with UAS-shibirets1;;shibirets1, which were reared at 21°C. We constructed the UAS-NpHR-EYFP, LexAop-eNpHR3.0-EYFP and LexAop-eArch3.0-EYFP fly strains. The NpHR-EYFP plasmid was provided by Dr. Liping Wang (Shenzhen Institutes of Advanced Technology, Shenzhen, China). The NpHR-EYFP fragment was amplified by PCR, cloned into the pUAST vector, and randomly inserted into the genome of W1118 flies. The eNpHR3.0-EYFP and eArch3.0-EYFP plasmids were obtained from Addgene. The eNpHR3.0-EYFP or eArch3.0-EYFP fragment was amplified, cloned into the pJFRC19 vector, and inserted into the 75B1 attP locus in vas-phi-zh2A-VK5flies, which were provided by Dr. Wei Wu (Institute of Biochemistry and Cell Biology, Shanghai, China). The 17d-GAL4, c205-GAL4, c305a-GAL4, c819-GAL4 and UAS-shibirets1;;shibirets1 fly strains were kindly provided by Dr. Li Liu (Institute of Biophysics, Beijing, China); and the MB247-GAL80 and MB247-lexA strains, by Dr. Scott Waddell (University of Oxford, Oxford, UK). The VT61721-GAL4 was a gift from Dr. Ann-Shyn Chiang (National Tsing Hua University, Hsinchu, Taiwan). The split-GAL4 lines MB002B, MB011B, MB131B and MB370B were obtained from the Janelia FlyLight Project Team and Dr. Gerald M. Rubin (Janelia Research Campus, Virginia). The inx5 RNAi JF02877 and inx6 RNAi JF02168 were provided by Dr. Luping Liu (Tsinghua University, Beijing, China), and the other innexin RNAi lines were obtained from the Vienna Drosophila RNAi Center. The specificity and target sequences of RNAi are shown in Supplementary file 1. The cha-GAL4, MB247-GAL4, OK107-GAL4, MB247-GeneSwitch, UAS-eNpHR3.0-EYFP, UAS-Kir2.1-GFP, UAS-CD4-tdTom and UAS-mCD8::EGFP were obtained from the Bloomington Stock Center. All other strains used in this study were extant lines from our lab. The 17d-GAL4, MB247-GAL4, c819-GAL4, c205-GAL4, MB247-GAL80, UAS-NpHR-EYFP, UAS-eNpHR3.0-EYFP and UAS-Kir2.1 fly strains were back-crossed with WCS flies.

Optogenetics

In the optogenetic flight simulator system and the electrophysiological experiments, the yellow light (593 nm) used to activate halorhodopsin was generated using a semiconductor laser (Xi’an He Qi Opto – Electronic Technology Co., Ltd., China). The laser was focused into a Gaussian light spot (diameter: 0.3 mm with 99% of the power). For visual learning and memory experiments, the laser power was 0.1 mW/mm2. Due to the possible low efficiency of electrical coupling, the laser power was maximized to 0.7 mW/mm2 for electrophysiological experiments. Flies for visual conditioning were raised with a 12 hr L/D cycle and fed with food containing 100 μM β-carotene (C9750, Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, Missouri) for 3~5 days before the experiments. For electrophysiological experiments, flies were raised in the dark and fed throughout their lives with food containing 100 μM ATR. In the optogenetic locomotor system, yellow light was supplied by LEDs (590 nm, 0.06–0.1 mW/mm2). ATR (R2500, Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, Missouri) or β-carotene was dissolved in absolute ethyl alcohol and mixed with food to a final concentration of 100 μM.

Visual learning and memory paradigm

The flies were fixed with hooks to the clamp of a torque meter in the center of a circular panorama (44 mm diameter) that could be rotated by a fast electric motor (Figure 1A). The torque meter measured each fly’s yaw torque around its vertical body axis and rotated the panorama around the fly via a negative feedback mechanism. This arrangement allowed the tethered flies to stabilize and choose their flight orientation with respect to the panorama by adjusting their yaw torque. The flies’ yaw torque and the panorama’s angular position were recorded continuously and stored in the computer (sampling rate, 20 Hz) for subsequent analysis. The panorama was divided into 4 quadrants, with horizontal bars at their respective centers. An infrared laser beam (10,600 nm) that projected to the fly’s abdomen was switched on when the fly oriented to the dangerous quadrants and was switched off when the fly oriented to the safe quadrants. For half of the flies, the lower bars were coupled with punishment, and for the other half, the higher bars were coupled with punishment. Standard 24 min training paradigms consisted of 12 blocks of 2 min each: blocks 1–3, pre-training sessions; blocks 4, 5, 7 and 8, training sessions; blocks 6 and 9–12, test sessions. In our experiments, the horizontal bars were black on white backgrounds; the bars had a length of 40° and a width of 12°. The difference in the center of gravity between the higher and lower bars was 44°. Patterns in opposite quadrants were identical, whereas patterns in neighboring quadrants were different. These patterns were printed on paper with a color inkjet printer (Epson Stylus Photo 1390). The paper arena was illuminated by transmitted light from a white LED array. The performance of tethered flies was evaluated quantitatively in 2 min bins using the “performance index” (PI), which was calculated as follows: PI = (t1–t2)/(t1+t2), where t1 is the time spent heading toward safe patterns and t2 is the time spent heading toward dangerous patterns. The final PI was calculated as the average of PIs 9 to 12. The experiments in Figure 2C and D are performed as blind experiments, in which this paradigm was performed on flies by an experimenter who was unaware of the genotypes of the flies.

MB ablation

We followed the procedure described previously (de Belle and Heisenberg, 1994). In brief, wild-type flies (3–7 d old) lay their eggs on agar plates containing sugar and grape juice for half an hour. The eggs were placed at 25°C for 18–20 hr, and newly hatched larvae (within 20 min) were quickly transferred into a yeast suspension containing 50 mg ml−1 hydroxyurea (HU. H8627, Sigma) to ablate the MB neuroblasts. The yeast suspension for the vehicle group did not contain hydroxyurea. After 4 hr of treatment, larvae were washed and transferred into fresh food vials.

Locomotor assay

Female flies aged 3–5 days were immobilized by cold anesthesia and transferred individually into 3.5 mm dishes. After a recovery period of 10–30 min, they were individually recorded. The environmental luminance was 1000–1300 Lux white light. The yellow light used to activate NpHR was supplied by 590 nm LEDs at an intensity of 0.06–0.1 mW/mm2. An infrared source and a web camera with a visible light filter were used to record the movements of the flies. Each fly was recorded for 2 min. For each fly, the distance moved, the time spent moving and the speed during the second min were analyzed. Flies that did not move during the 2 min were excluded. The temperature was 24–26°C and the humidity was 40–60%. The locomotor assay in Drosophila are described in more detail at Bio-protocol (Liu et al., 2017).

Quantitative Real Time PCR

The effectiveness of all innexin RNAi lines were verified with Quantitative Real Time PCR (qPCR). Total RNA extracted from approximately 100 fly heads with TRIzol (Invitrogen, Carsbad, California) was treated with RQ1 DNase (Promega, Fitchburg, Wisconsin) and reverse-transcribed using PrimeScript RT Master Mix (Takara, Japan). Relative quantification PCR was accomplished using a SYBR Premix Ex TaqTM II kit (Takara, Japan) and an ABI PRISM 7300 real-time PCR Detection system (Applied Biosystems, Waltham, Massachussetts). Rp49 was used as an internal control and relative mRNA levels were calculated with the comparative CT method. Gene expression levels of all manipulated flies (elav-Gal4/UAS-inx RNAi) and control flies (UAS-inx RNAi/+) were normalized to elav-GAL4/+. Three separate samples were collected from each condition and measurements were conducted in triplicates.

Electrophysiology

Whole-cell recordings were performed under visual control using a Zeiss Axioskop2 microscope or an Olympus BX61WI microscope with a 40× water-immersion objective. The recordings were performed at 20–25°C with a Multiclamp 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices, Sunnyvale, California). The data were low-pass filtered at 10 kHz and acquired at 20 kHz with a Digidata 1440A digitizer (Molecular Devices, Sunnyvale, California).

Fly brains were dissected out and immersed in extracellular saline solution in a small dish. The saline composition was as follows: 103 mM NaCl, 3 mM KCl, 5 mM N-Tris (hydroxymethyl) methyl-2-aminoethane-sulfonic acid, 10 mM trehalose, 8 mM glucose, 26 mM NaHCO3, 1 mM NaH2PO4, 1.5 mM CaCl2, and 4 mM MgCl2, adjusted to 275 mOsm(Wilson and Laurent, 2005). The saline was bubbled with 95% O2/5% CO2 gas for a final pH of 7.3. The sheath covering the target neuron was removed with fine forceps. For neuron targeting, four GAL4 lines (OK107, MB247, c205, and c305a) were used to drive EGFP expression in KCs, and two split-GAL4 lines (MB002B and MB011B) were used to label β’2mp neurons. Patch-clamp electrodes (20–30 MΩ) were filled with the following solution: 140 mM potassium aspartate, 10 mM HEPES, 1 mM KCl, 4 mM MgATP, 0.5 mM Na3GTP, 1 mM EGTA, and 100 μM Alexa Fluor 568 (Life Technologies, Carsbad, California) or 1% biocytin hydrazide, pH 7.3, adjusted to 265 mOsm. In the majority of the recorded neurons, a small constant hyperpolarizing current (-50–0pA) was injected to keep the membrane potential between −60 and −50 mV. In the drug delivery experiments, the final concentrations of TTX (Taizhou Kang Te, China) and 2-OCT were 1 μM and 1 mM, respectively. All reagents were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich, unless stated otherwise.

Dye coupling

Low-molecular-weight dyes (biocytin and Alexa Fluor 568) were loaded into neurons through an electrode during whole-cell recording. Only one soma was successfully recorded per brain or hemisphere. For each brain, 1~4 attempts were made to get successful recording. Brains without successful recording were discarded after 4 attempts. No obvious dye in the target soma was confirmed by fluorescence after failed attempts. After loading for one to two hours, the dye labeled the soma and neurites of the recorded neuron and diffused to other neurons through open gap-junction channels if present. Because of the small size of KC somas, they were almost always removed with the electrode when the electrodes were drawn out after dye loading was complete. Thus the loaded soma was observed in only a few brains. Next, the brain was fixed in 4% PFA for 12–15 min at room temperature and then washed several times in PBS with 0.3% Triton X-100. The Alexa Fluor 568-labeled brains were mounted in Vectashield H-1000 (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, California) for confocal imaging. The biocytin-labeled brains were incubated with Alexa Fluor 568-conjugated streptavidin (Life Technologies; 1:200) overnight at 4°C. The brains were washed several times and then mounted in Vectashield H-1000 for confocal imaging. A Nikon Eclipse FN1 confocal system with a 40× water-immersion objective and an Olympus FV10I system with a 60× oil-immersion objective were used to obtain the Z-stacks of the brains. The dye-filled neurites were identified based on their position and morphology. The examples that meet the following criteria were counted as dye-coupled examples: (1) More than one dye-filled fibers were observed in peduncle. (2) Only one fiber was observed in peduncle, but fibers in lobe region belonged to different subtypes. (3) Only one fiber was observed in the peduncle and fibers in the lobe region belonging to the same subtype, but were not branches of one neuron. Condition (3) only applied to αβ core neurons, which generally have few branches. In blind experiments, the brain mounting, confocal imaging and identification of dye coupling were performed by an experimenter who was unaware of the genotypes of the brains.

Statistical analysis

Data are presented as the mean ± SEM or by box plot. We applied the Wilcoxon signed rank test to compare the performance indices against chance level (Brembs and Wiener, 2006; Liu et al., 1999), and to evaluate differences between matched samples. Wilcoxon rank sum tests were used for comparisons between two groups. To compare three or more matched groups, we used Kruskal-Wallis test, and the significance of differences between each pair of groups were corrected by Bonferroni method. Statistical analysis was performed using MATLAB. Asterisks indicate the level of statistical significance (*p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001).

Acknowledgements

We sincerely thank Yoshinori Aso for assistance in identification of neurites, Mu-ming Poo for constructive comments, Mingkui Zhang and Jingwu Hou for assistance with experimental setup, and Liping Wang for assistance in optogenetics. This work was supported by the “Strategic Priority Research Program” of the CAS (XDB02040004), by grants from the 973 Program (2011CBA00400), as well as by the National Science Foundation of China (91232000, 91132709, 31130027, and 31070956).

Funding Statement

No external funding was received for this work.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Author contributions

QL, Designed this project, Established the optogenetic-flight simulator setup and conducted the behavioral experiments, Performed the dye coupling experiments, Analyzed the data, Wrote the manuscript.

XY, Designed this project, Assisted with the behavior experiments, Performed the dye coupling experiments, Performed the electrophysiological experiments, Analyzed the data, Wrote the manuscript.

JT, Assisted with the behavior experiments, Analysis and interpretation of data.

ZG, Performed the qPCR experiments, Analysis and interpretation of data.

MW, Assisted with the behavior experiments, Analysis and interpretation of data.

YL, Designed this project, Wrote the manuscript.

AG, Designed this project, Wrote the manuscript.

Additional files

Supplementary file 1. The specificity and target sequences of RNAi for innexin 1~8.

THU, Tsinghua University.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13238.024

elife-13238-supp1.xlsx (11.7KB, xlsx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.13238.024

References

  1. Aso Y, Hattori D, Yu Y, Johnston RM, Iyer NA, Ngo T-TB, Dionne H, Abbott LF, Axel R, Tanimoto H, Rubin GM. The neuronal architecture of the mushroom body provides a logic for associative learning. eLife. 2014a;3:e04577. doi: 10.7554/eLife.04577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aso Y, Sitaraman D, Ichinose T, Kaun KR, Vogt K, Belliart-Guérin G, Plaçais PY, Robie AA, Yamagata N, Schnaitmann C, Rowell WJ, Johnston RM, Ngo TT, Chen N, Korff W, Nitabach MN, Heberlein U, Preat T, Branson KM, Tanimoto H, Rubin GM. Mushroom body output neurons encode valence and guide memory-based action selection in Drosophila. eLife. 2014b;3:e04580. doi: 10.7554/eLife.04580. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Barth M, Heisenberg M. Vision affects mushroom bodies and central complex in Drosophila melanogaster. Learning & Memory. 1997;4:219–229. doi: 10.1101/lm.4.2.219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bissiere S, Zelikowsky M, Ponnusamy R, Jacobs NS, Blair HT, Fanselow MS. Electrical synapses control hippocampal contributions to fear learning and memory. Science. 2011;331:87–91. doi: 10.1126/science.1193785. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bloomfield SA, Völgyi B. The diverse functional roles and regulation of neuronal gap junctions in the retina. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2009;10:495–506. doi: 10.1038/nrn2636. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bohrmann J, Haas-Assenbaum A. Gap junctions in ovarian follicles of Drosophila melanogaster: inhibition and promotion of dye-coupling between oocyte and follicle cells. Cell and Tissue Research. 1993;273:163–173. doi: 10.1007/BF00304623. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brembs B, Wiener J. Context and occasion setting in Drosophila visual learning. Learning & Memory. 2006;13:618–628. doi: 10.1101/lm.318606. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Davis RL. Traces of Drosophila memory. Neuron. 2011;70:8–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. de Belle JS, Heisenberg M. Associative odor learning in Drosophila abolished by chemical ablation of mushroom bodies. Science. 1994;263:692–695. doi: 10.1126/science.8303280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. DeVries SH, Qi X, Smith R, Makous W, Sterling P. Electrical coupling between mammalian cones. Current Biology. 2002;12:1900–1907. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(02)01261-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Gradinaru V, Zhang F, Ramakrishnan C, Mattis J, Prakash R, Diester I, Goshen I, Thompson KR, Deisseroth K. Molecular and cellular approaches for diversifying and extending optogenetics. Cell. 2010;141:154–165. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2010.02.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Guo A, Li L, Xia SZ, Feng CH, Wolf R, Heisenberg M. Conditioned visual flight orientation in Drosophila: dependence on age, practice, and diet. Learning & Memory. 1996;3:49–59. doi: 10.1101/lm.3.1.49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Guven-Ozkan T, Davis RL. Functional neuroanatomy of Drosophila olfactory memory formation. Learning & Memory. 2014;2121:519–526. doi: 10.1101/lm.034363.114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Hervé JC, Derangeon M. Gap-junction-mediated cell-to-cell communication. Cell and Tissue Research. 2013;352:21–31. doi: 10.1007/s00441-012-1485-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Hormuzdi SG, Filippov MA, Mitropoulou G, Monyer H, Bruzzone R. Electrical synapses: a dynamic signaling system that shapes the activity of neuronal networks. Biochimica Et Biophysica Acta. 2004;1662:113–137. doi: 10.1016/j.bbamem.2003.10.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Joesch M, Schnell B, Raghu SV, Reiff DF, Borst A. ON and OFF pathways in Drosophila motion vision. Nature. 2010;468:300–304. doi: 10.1038/nature09545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Kasuya J, Ishimoto H, Kitamoto T. Neuronal mechanisms of learning and memory revealed by spatial and temporal suppression of neurotransmission using shibire, a temperature-sensitive dynamin mutant gene in Drosophila melanogaster. Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience. 2009;2 doi: 10.3389/neuro.02.011.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Laird DW. Connexin phosphorylation as a regulatory event linked to gap junction internalization and degradation. Biochimica Et Biophysica Acta. 2005;1711:172–182. doi: 10.1016/j.bbamem.2004.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Lee T, Lee A, Luo L. Development of the Drosophila mushroom bodies: sequential generation of three distinct types of neurons from a neuroblast. Development. 1999;126:4065–4076. doi: 10.1242/dev.126.18.4065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Lei Z, Chen K, Li H, Liu H, Guo A. The GABA system regulates the sparse coding of odors in the mushroom bodies of Drosophila. Biochemical and Biophysical Research Communications. 2013;436:35–40. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2013.05.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Liu L, Wolf R, Ernst R, Heisenberg M. Context generalization in Drosophila visual learning requires the mushroom bodies. Nature. 1999;400:753–756. doi: 10.1038/23456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Liu Q, Tian J, Yang X, Li Y, Guo A. Locomotor Assay in Drosophila melanogaster. BIO-PROTOCOL. 2017;7:e2283. doi: 10.21769/BioProtoc.2283. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Lovick JK, Hartenstein V. Hydroxyurea-mediated neuroblast ablation establishes birth dates of secondary lineages and addresses neuronal interactions in the developing Drosophila brain. Developmental Biology. 2015;402:32–47. doi: 10.1016/j.ydbio.2015.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Masse NY, Turner GC, Jefferis GS. Olfactory information processing in Drosophila. Current Biology. 2009;19:R700–713. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.06.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Mattis J, Tye KM, Ferenczi EA, Ramakrishnan C, O'Shea DJ, Prakash R, Gunaydin LA, Hyun M, Fenno LE, Gradinaru V, Yizhar O, Deisseroth K. Principles for applying optogenetic tools derived from direct comparative analysis of microbial opsins. Nature Methods. 2012;9:159–172. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1808. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Moreno AP. Connexin phosphorylation as a regulatory event linked to channel gating. Biochimica Et Biophysica Acta. 2005;1711:164–171. doi: 10.1016/j.bbamem.2005.02.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Osterwalder T, Yoon KS, White BH, Keshishian H. A conditional tissue-specific transgene expression system using inducible GAL4. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2001;98:12596–12601. doi: 10.1073/pnas.221303298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Peng Y, Xi W, Zhang W, Zhang K, Guo A. Experience improves feature extraction in Drosophila. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:5139–5145. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0472-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Pereda AE, Curti S, Hoge G, Cachope R, Flores CE, Rash JE. Gap junction-mediated electrical transmission: regulatory mechanisms and plasticity. Biochimica Et Biophysica Acta. 2013;1828:134–146. doi: 10.1016/j.bbamem.2012.05.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Phelan P, Nakagawa M, Wilkin MB, Moffat KG, O'Kane CJ, Davies JA, Bacon JP. Mutations in shaking-B prevent electrical synapse formation in the Drosophila giant fiber system. Journal of Neuroscience. 1996;16:1101–1113. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-03-01101.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Phelan P, Starich TA. Innexins get into the gap. BioEssays. 2001;23 doi: 10.1002/bies.1057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Praefcke GJ, McMahon HT. The dynamin superfamily: universal membrane tubulation and fission molecules? Nature Reviews. Molecular Cell Biology. 2004;5:133–147. doi: 10.1038/nrm1313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Ren Q, Li H, Wu Y, Ren J, Guo A. A GABAergic inhibitory neural circuit regulates visual reversal learning in Drosophila. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:11524–11538. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0827-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Solan JL, Lampe PD. Connexin phosphorylation as a regulatory event linked to gap junction channel assembly. Biochimica Et Biophysica Acta. 2005;1711:154–163. doi: 10.1016/j.bbamem.2004.09.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Stebbings LA, Todman MG, Phillips R, Greer CE, Tam J, Phelan P, Jacobs K, Bacon JP, Davies JA. Gap junctions in Drosophila: developmental expression of the entire innexin gene family. Mechanisms of Development. 2002;113:197–205. doi: 10.1016/S0925-4773(02)00025-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Suh GS, Ben-Tabou de Leon S, Tanimoto H, Fiala A, Benzer S, Anderson DJ. Light activation of an innate olfactory avoidance response in Drosophila. Current Biology. 2007;17:905–908. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.04.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Sweeney ST, Hidalgo A, de Belle JS, Keshishian H. Hydroxyurea ablation of mushroom bodies in Drosophila. Cold Spring Harbor Protocols. 2012;2012:231–234. doi: 10.1101/pdb.prot067777. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Tamás G, Buhl EH, Lörincz A, Somogyi P. Proximally targeted GABAergic synapses and gap junctions synchronize cortical interneurons. Nature Neuroscience. 2000;3:366–371. doi: 10.1038/73936. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Tanaka NK, Tanimoto H, Ito K. Neuronal assemblies of the Drosophila mushroom body. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2008;508:711–755. doi: 10.1002/cne.21692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Tang S, Guo A. Choice behavior of Drosophila facing contradictory visual cues. Science. 2001;294:1543–1547. doi: 10.1126/science.1058237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. van der Bliek AM, Meyerowitz EM. Dynamin-like protein encoded by the Drosophila shibire gene associated with vesicular traffic. Nature. 1991;351:411–414. doi: 10.1038/351411a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Veruki ML, Hartveit E. AII (Rod) amacrine cells form a network of electrically coupled interneurons in the mammalian retina. Neuron. 2002;33:935–946. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00609-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Vogt K, Schnaitmann C, Dylla KV, Knapek S, Aso Y, Rubin GM, Tanimoto H. Shared mushroom body circuits underlie visual and olfactory memories in Drosophila. eLife. 2014;3:e02395. doi: 10.7554/eLife.04580. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Wang T, Jiao Y, Montell C. Dissection of the pathway required for generation of vitamin A and for Drosophila phototransduction. The Journal of Cell Biology. 2007;177:305–316. doi: 10.1083/jcb.200610081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Wilson RI, Laurent G. Role of GABAergic inhibition in shaping odor-evoked spatiotemporal patterns in the Drosophila antennal lobe. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:9069–9079. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2070-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Wolf R, Wittig T, Liu L, Wustmann G, Eyding D, Heisenberg M. Drosophila mushroom bodies are dispensable for visual, tactile, and motor learning. Learning & Memory. 1998;5:166–178. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Wu CL, Shih MF, Lai JS, Yang HT, Turner GC, Chen L, Chiang AS. Heterotypic gap junctions between two neurons in the drosophila brain are critical for memory. Current Biology. 2011;21:848–854. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.02.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Wu Y, Ren Q, Li H, Guo A. The GABAergic anterior paired lateral neurons facilitate olfactory reversal learning in Drosophila. Learning & Memory. 2012;19:478–486. doi: 10.1101/lm.025726.112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Yaksi E, Wilson RI. Electrical coupling between olfactory glomeruli. Neuron. 2010;67:1034–1047. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Yang J, O'Tousa JE. Cellular sites of Drosophila NinaB and NinaD activity in vitamin A metabolism. Molecular and Cellular Neurosciences. 2007;35:49–56. doi: 10.1016/j.mcn.2007.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Yi W, Zhang Y, Tian Y, Guo J, Li Y, Guo A. A subset of cholinergic mushroom body neurons requires Go signaling to regulate sleep in Drosophila. Sleep. 2013;36:1809–1821. doi: 10.5665/sleep.3206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Zhang D, Li Y, Wu S, Rasch MJ. Design principles of the sparse coding network and the role of “sister cells” in the olfactory system of Drosophila. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience. 2013;7:141. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2013.00141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Zhang K, Guo JZ, Peng Y, Xi W, Guo A. Dopamine-mushroom body circuit regulates saliency-based decision-making in Drosophila. Science. 2007;316:1901–1904. doi: 10.1126/science.1137357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
eLife. 2016 May 24;5:e13238. doi: 10.7554/eLife.13238.026

Decision letter

Editor: K VijayRaghavan1

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your work entitled "Gap Junction Networks in Mushroom Bodies Participate in Visual Learning and Memory in Drosophila" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by two peer reviewers, including Benjamin de Bivort, and the evaluation has been overseen by K VijayRaghavan as the Senior Editor.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

In this paper, Liu and colleagues examine an important and understudied question in neuroscience, what is the role of gap-junctions in behavior, specifically visual pattern learning in Drosophila. The authors first reduce expression of a series of innexins, the genes encoding gap junctions, in the mushroom bodies, integrating neuropils which the authors characterize as the cognitive centers of the fly brain. They find that knocking down a specific subset of innexins renders flies' learning scores statistically indistinguishable from zero (no learning). Interestingly, blocking synaptic release using the dominant negative dynamin allele shibire has no effect on learning, suggesting that MB function in this behavior is gap-junction mediated. In a careful sequence of experiments and controls they are able to show that this effect is specific to regional subsets of the mushroom body and that innexin function is required post-developmentally. Next, the authors report diffusion of small dye molecules from patched Kenyon cells (KCs = MB intrinsic neurons) to other KCs. Using optogenetic stimulation paired with whole-cell recording, they show that hyperpolarization can be transmitted between KCs and a specific class of MB output neurons (MBONs), which can be temporarily blocked by the application of 2-octanol, a gap-junction blocker. Lastly, they find corresponding dependence of visual learning on innexin expression in those MBONs.

In general, the paper presents compelling genetic evidence of the requirement of MB-specific gap junction expression for visual learning, and direct evidence of gap junctions by dye diffusion (though as the authors discuss, the observed rate of dye coupling may underestimate the rate of gap-junctioning between pairs of KCs). The paper is well written, with generally solid use of controls. The balance of evidence is convincing that gap junctions likely play an important role in visual learning in the MBs.

Essential revisions:

While this is a very nice study, we do have a major conceptual concern and other important ones too.

1) Previous literature, including from the senior author of this study, has argued that MBs are totally dispensable for flight simulator visual learning except in cases where context is altered or where more complex combinations of features (such as shape and color) are used. This previous literature made use of HU ablation of MB. In other words, flies that have no MB structures at all are able to perform normally in this visual learning paradigm. Defects in visual learning in MB-less flies are only revealed with more complex tasks that were not employed here. This literature includes several classic studies from the Heisenberg group (e.g. Wolf et al. 1998 and Liu et al. 1999), as well as a more recent study by Guo and colleagues (Peng et al., 2007). So if in fact MB are not required for this task, it would obviously undermine the very foundation upon which they test the requirement for gap junctions within MB for this task. The authors correctly point out that a recent study by Vogt et al. points to a role for MB (using shibire to block output) in a visual learning task, and they correctly point to the literature that MB are required for visual learning that involves changes in context. But still, in this flight simulator visual learning assay, the literature is very consistent in the finding that MB ablated animals have no defect. The authors suggest that this discrepancy could be caused by developmental compensation with HU ablation. And we suppose that methodological differences in the flight simulator assay employed here could differ from those circa late 1990s. But this is the crux of the current study, so we were really surprised that they didn’t just do a simple HU ablation and test performance in their current assay. If performance is normal, it would be tough to read past the first figure without further explanation right? We see two types of explanation to reconcile the fact that HU ablation in previous publications has no impact on flight simulator visual learning, whereas acute manipulations of gap junctions does. One possibility is that when MB are ablated developmentally, there is some compensation mechanism in which the brain arrives at a solution that is independent of MB. This is certainly possible. But the other possibility is that the exact details of the current version of the flight simulator behavioral paradigm are sufficiently different from those historically reported. In this case, the current version of the flight simulator, in these researcher's hands, engages the MB in a way that previous versions of the assay did not. These are two really different interpretations. And it would be trivial for the authors to address this: just do an HU ablation of MB and test learning in their flight simulator. This lab has done HU ablation in the past. So this requires no new expertise and is such an obvious experiment that we would guess they have already thought of it. We agree that a positive result here would be an important addition to this study. If the authors find again that developmental ablation of the MB does not impair this behavior, then an in-depth discussion of the plausibility of developmental compensation is needed.

2) Our other major criticism concerns the presentation of statistical findings. Specifically, the authors conflate two statistical observations: that 1) an experimental group is not significantly different from 0 (while its control group is) and 2) that experimental group is therefore significantly different from the control. This faulty inference is made in several places. Relatedly, the author's description of the steps they took to correct for multiple comparisons is insufficient (but it is important to get this right, especially in Figure 1).

3) Next, we found the Introduction to be too short to provide proper background for the findings. It consists of a paragraph about the general function of gap junctions and a paragraph about the role of the mushroom bodies in learning and memory in flies. A major area for which there are previous findings is the role of gap-junctions in other Drosophila circuits, such as the optic lamina, the Horizontal system cells, and the giant fiber system.

4) Lastly, we would recommend that the authors edit their manuscript to make its interpretation easier for non-Drosophila MB experts. Specifically, Gal4 reagents are introduced without any description of their expression pattern. This makes interpreting the findings much harder.

5) In addition, we have few data presentation issues:

A) Figure 3—figure supplement 2: I cannot see the green fills in this figure. Can the images be improved?

B) It is stated that independent RNAi lines were used from different sources, but we could not find any information on the sequences used in the constructs. Were these derived from and targeting different regions of the mRNA? if not, they are not independent in a meaningful way.

3) We did not see any detail of the methods for RT-PCR to confirm that the RNAi lines work (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Was this real time – PCR? End point PCR? PCR details? Are the effects reported as delta-delta CT values? Etc., and no stats were shown for this panel.

eLife. 2016 May 24;5:e13238. doi: 10.7554/eLife.13238.027

Author response


Essential revisions:

While this is a very nice study, we do have a major conceptual concern and other important ones too.

1) Previous literature, including from the senior author of this study, has argued that MBs are totally dispensable for flight simulator visual learning except in cases where context is altered or where more complex combinations of features (such as shape and color) are used. This previous literature made use of HU ablation of MB. In other words, flies that have no MB structures at all are able to perform normally in this visual learning paradigm. Defects in visual learning in MB-less flies are only revealed with more complex tasks that were not employed here. This literature includes several classic studies from the Heisenberg group (e.g. Wolf et al. 1998 and Liu et al. 1999), as well as a more recent study by Guo and colleagues (Peng et al., 2007). So if in fact MB are not required for this task, it would obviously undermine the very foundation upon which they test the requirement for gap junctions within MB for this task. The authors correctly point out that a recent study by Vogt et al. points to a role for MB (using shibire to block output) in a visual learning task, and they correctly point to the literature that MB are required for visual learning that involves changes in context. But still, in this flight simulator visual learning assay, the literature is very consistent in the finding that MB ablated animals have no defect. The authors suggest that this discrepancy could be caused by developmental compensation with HU ablation. And we suppose that methodological differences in the flight simulator assay employed here could differ from those circa late 1990s. But this is the crux of the current study, so we were really surprised that they didn’t just do a simple HU ablation and test performance in their current assay. If performance is normal, it would be tough to read past the first figure without further explanation right? We see two types of explanation to reconcile the fact that HU ablation in previous publications has no impact on flight simulator visual learning, whereas acute manipulations of gap junctions does. One possibility is that when MB are ablated developmentally, there is some compensation mechanism in which the brain arrives at a solution that is independent of MB. This is certainly possible. But the other possibility is that the exact details of the current version of the flight simulator behavioral paradigm are sufficiently different from those historically reported. In this case, the current version of the flight simulator, in these researcher's hands, engages the MB in a way that previous versions of the assay did not. These are two really different interpretations. And it would be trivial for the authors to address this: just do an HU ablation of MB and test learning in their flight simulator. This lab has done HU ablation in the past. So this requires no new expertise and is such an obvious experiment that we would guess they have already thought of it. We agree that a positive result here would be an important addition to this study. If the authors find again that developmental ablation of the MB does not impair this behavior, then an in-depth discussion of the plausibility of developmental compensation is needed.

We thank the reviewers for the comments and suggestions. We have performed the HU ablation experiments and found that ablation of the MBs at early developmental stage does not impair fly’s visual learning and memory, which is in agreement with previous reports (Liu et al., 1999; Peng et al., 2007; Wolf et al., 1998). We have strengthened the discussion of developmental compensation in our manuscript.

2) Our other major criticism concerns the presentation of statistical findings. Specifically, the authors conflate two statistical observations: that 1) an experimental group is not significantly different from 0 (while its control group is) and 2) that experimental group is therefore significantly different from the control. This faulty inference is made in several places. Relatedly, the author's description of the steps they took to correct for multiple comparisons is insufficient (but it is important to get this right, especially in Figure 1).

Thanks for pointing out our mistakes. We have rephrased our expression. When the performance index of the test group was significantly different from the control, we describe the visual learning of these flies as they were impaired. When the performance index of the test group was not significantly different from the control, and significantly different from zero, we describe the visual learning of these flies as they were intact. In Figure 2A, the indices of 17d > inx4 RNAi and 17d > inx5 RNAi flies was nether significantly different from the control nor from zero. We changed the description of “knockdown of inx4, inx5 and inx6 in 17d-GAL4-labeled neurons resulted in impairment of fly learning and memory” to “knockdown of inx6 in 17d-GAL4-labeled neurons resulted in impairment of fly learning and memory. Flies with the knockdown of inx4, inx5 or inx6 were not able to achieve performance indices significantly different to zero, whereas the control groups did.” We have also added the description of the correction for multiple comparisons in the Materials and methods.

3) Next, we found the Introduction to be too short to provide proper background for the findings. It consists of a paragraph about the general function of gap junctions and a paragraph about the role of the mushroom bodies in learning and memory in flies. A major area for which there are previous findings is the role of gap-junctions in other Drosophila circuits, such as the optic lamina, the Horizontal system cells, and the giant fiber system.

Thanks for the suggestions. We have added the introduction of studies on gap junctions in Drosophila circuits, like the giant fiber, the optic lamina, olfactory glomeruli and the APL-DPM neural circuits.

4) Lastly, we would recommend that the authors edit their manuscript to make its interpretation easier for non-Drosophila MB experts. Specifically, Gal4 reagents are introduced without any description of their expression pattern. This makes interpreting the findings much harder.

Thanks for pointing out our negligence. We have added the description of expression pattern of each Gal4.

5) In addition, we have few data presentation issues:

A) Figure 3—figure supplement 2: I cannot see the green fills in this figure. Can the images be improved?

We have uploaded images with higher resolution and stacks of images as videos. We hope these could improve the presentation.

B) It is stated that independent RNAi lines were used from different sources, but we could not find any information on the sequences used in the constructs. Were these derived from and targeting different regions of the mRNA? if not, they are not independent in a meaningful way.

Thanks for pointing out our negligence. We have added the description of the target sequences in Supplementary file 1.

3) We did not see any detail of the methods for RT-PCR to confirm that the RNAi lines work (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Was this real time – PCR? End point PCR? PCR details? Are the effects reported as delta-delta CT values? Etc., and no stats were shown for this panel.

Thanks for this comment. We have added the description of the methods for RT-PCR in the Materials and methods.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Supplementary file 1. The specificity and target sequences of RNAi for innexin 1~8.

    THU, Tsinghua University.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13238.024

    elife-13238-supp1.xlsx (11.7KB, xlsx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.13238.024

    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES