Abstract
Health communication interventions have been effective in promoting fruit and vegetable consumption (FVC). To explore mechanisms underlying health communication effectiveness, we investigated whether information processes mediated the relationship between health communication and FVC, using data from NC STRIDES. NC STRIDES tested the efficacy of two health communication strategies to promote FVC among a diverse population-based sample of older adults. Participants were randomized to one of four groups: control, tailored print communication (TPC), telephone motivational interviewing (TMI), or combined (TPC+TMI). Multi-sample structural equation models were constructed to analyze data from 469 participants. Information processes mediated the effect of TMI and TPC+TMI on FVC. TMI had an indirect effect on FVC through relevance of the communications. TPC+TMI influenced FVC through perceived relevance of the communications, trust in the communications, and dose recall via two paths. In the first path, relevance was associated with trust. Trust was associated with recall, and greater recall predicted FVC. In the second path, relevance was associated with dose recall, and more recall predicted FVC. Thus, we found that key information processes mediated the relationship between a health communication intervention and FVC. Further research should investigate ways to enhance relevance, trust, and recall during the delivery of interventions.
Keywords: health communication, information processing, fruit and vegetable consumption, intervention
Introduction
Unhealthy lifestyle behaviors, such as poor diet, lead to greater morbidity and mortality among American adults (Ness & Powless, 1997; Srinath et al., 2004). A diet low in fruits and vegetables is associated with increased risk for chronic diseases such as cardiovascular disease, hypertension, diabetes, and cancer (Adams & Standridge, 2006; Slattery, 2000). In 1991, the National Cancer Institute and the Produce for Better Health Foundation jointly launched the 5-A-Day Program to increase public awareness of the importance of eating at least 5 servings of fruits and vegetables each day. Despite the health benefits of eating more fruits and vegetables, more than half of all American adults do not meet nationally-recognized guidelines for daily fruit and vegetable consumption (FVC) (Casagrande, et al. 2007).
Health communication interventions can promote healthy eating behaviors when messages are strategically designed and delivered through specific communication channels (Campbell et al., 2004; Emmons et al., 2005; Resnicow et al., 2004). Communication strategies such as tailored print communications (TPC) have been shown to be particularly effective in promoting FVC (Campbell et al., 2004; Emmons et al., 2005). In TPC, the content and style of printed materials are based on individual characteristics, and social-psychological theories provide a basis for determining the characteristics most likely to influence behavior change (Skinner et al., 1999). Telephone motivational interviewing (TMI) is an alternative medium to deliver tailored health communications (Resnicow et al., 2000; Resnicow et al., 2004) through use of specific counseling techniques that increase motivation for behavior change (Miller & Rollnick, 1991).
Health communication experts have developed new and effective communication strategies, such as TPC and TMI, to promote healthy eating behaviors. Less attention, however, has been paid to understanding how these health communication strategies work to exert their effects. Experts recognize the need to examine theory-based information processes of health communication to understand how and why interventions work, and whether different types of communication strategies work differently (Rimer & Glassman, 1999; Skinner et al., 1999).
The information processing model describes ways that persuasive communications can influence information processing and lead to change in attitudes and behaviors (McGuire, 1985; McGuire, 2001). The information processing model is an input/output model. Input factors, such as characteristics of the messages (tailored vs. non-tailored) and channel of information delivery (print vs. interpersonal communication), can influence output factors ranging from perceptive (e.g., attention) and cognitive (e.g., retrieval) to behavior change. McGuire (1985) proposes that for a communication to achieve a higher-order effect (e.g., behavior change), outcomes appearing earlier in the sequence must be achieved first. For example, health communication strategies will be more effective if the recipients are exposed to the communication, attend to it, find it interesting, understand it, learn from it, and can recall the information received (Bull et al., 2001; McGuire, 1985).
Studies on tailored health communications report that these types of communication are effective because they enhance information processing. Findings from process evaluations report that people who receive tailored printed information are more likely to read the material, perceive greater personal relevance, and recall more information, compared to those who receive non-tailored information (Brug et al., 1996; Campbell & Quintiliani, 2006).
Moreover, different processes may be involved when interventions are delivered through print vs. interpersonal communication (Kreuter et al., 2004; McGuire, 1985). Tailored print interventions induced more attention resulting in greater attitude change when compared to non-tailored messages. Interpersonal communications, on the other hand, enhanced credibility of and trust in the information (McGuire, 2001). Motivational interviewing, in particular, begins by establishing rapport and trust with clients (Bundy, 2004; Emmons & Rollnick, 2001). This technique facilitates clients’ perceptions that the counseling sessions are personally relevant to their values and goals (Resnicow & Shaikh, 2007). Although intervention studies with tailored print materials and motivational interviewing provide insight into the different ways in which individuals process health communication interventions, we do not know whether these processes are mediators between health communication and behavior change. To advance the current state of our knowledge about health communication interventions, we must identify processes that mediate the effects of various interventions. We can then enhance specific processes during the delivery of interventions (Rimer & Glassman, 1999; Skinner et al., 1999).
Conceptual Model
Guided by the information processing paradigm, we developed a conceptual model to understand the information processes that mediate the relationship between a health communication intervention and FVC. In this model (Figure 1), we posit that the effect of a health communication intervention strategy on FVC can occur directly, and can also occur indirectly through the relevance of communication, trust in the communication, and dose recall. Relationships between intervention and information processes, and between information processes and behavior change, have already been documented in the literature (Bull et al., 2001; Kreuter et al., 2004). These studies, however, have not tested for the mediation effect of information processes to explain the effect of health communication interventions on FVC (Kreuter et al., 2004).
The Present Study
This study examines two research questions: a) Which information processes mediate the relationship between health communication strategies and FVC and b) Do similar or different processes mediate the different types of health communication strategies?
Method
Sample
Data used in this study come from the North Carolina Strategies to Improve Diet, Exercise, and Screening study (NC STRIDES). NC STRIDES used a classic 2 x 2 randomized factorial design that tested the efficacy of tailored print communication and telephone motivational interviewing to promote FVC. Participants were randomized into one of four intervention groups: tailored print communication (TPC), telephone motivational interviewing (TMI), combined (TPC+TMI), or control. The NC STRIDES intervention was conducted in 33 counties in eastern and central North Carolina. The individuals in the TPC group received a series of four tailored print newsletters, the TMI group received a series of four counseling calls, the combined group received four tailored print newsletters and four counseling calls, and the control group received two “generic” or non-tailored health education mailings on breast or prostate cancer. The intervention has been described in detail elsewhere (James et al., 2006).
Study participants from NC STRIDES were recruited from the North Carolina Colon Cancer Study (NCCCS), a population-based, case-control study of colon cancer patients in North Carolina conducted from 1996–2000. NCCCS study participants were from 33 counties in the central and eastern part of North Carolina, an area including rural, suburban, and urban counties whose residents represent diverse socioeconomic backgrounds and a racial composition, at the time of the study, consisting primarily of African Americans and whites. Eligible cases were individuals with adenocarcinomas of the colon, ages 40–80, of African American or white, non-Hispanic ethnicity, who were being treated in one of 38 non-federal hospitals. Cases were identified using a rapid ascertainment component of the North Carolina Central Cancer Registry. Population-based controls in the NCCCS had been recruited from two sources: those under age 65 came from the NC Department of Motor Vehicles roster and those over age 65 came from the Health Care Financing Administration registry. NCCCS participants were invited to participate in the NC STRIDES study between January 2001 and June 2002.
Detailed recruitment procedures have been described previously (James et al., 2006; Satia et al., 2004). NC STRIDES recruited 922 participants (49.8%), of whom 825 individuals (89.5%) completed the baseline survey (304 cancer cases and 521 controls). Of those 825 participants, 735 (89.1%) completed the follow-up survey (266 cancer cases and 469 controls). Non-responses on the follow-up survey resulted from 18 deaths, 21 people who withdrew from the study for health reasons, 19 refusals, and 32 lost contacts.
Because a colon cancer diagnosis may affect how individuals process information (McGuire, 1985; Petty & Cacioppo, 1981), separate analyses were conducted for colon cancer survivors and for the population-based control group. The analyses for this study used only data from population-based controls (N=469).
Data Collection
After NC STRIDES participants were randomized into intervention groups, researchers collected baseline data using a telephone-administered self-report survey. Data included socio-demographic information, self-rated health, health information, FVC, and psychosocial factors related to FVC. One year after baseline, participants were asked to complete a second telephone questionnaire. That survey asked the same health, behavioral, and psychosocial questions as the baseline survey, and also included information processing questions.
Participants’ baseline age was 67 (±9.5) years (Table 1). More than half of the participants were white (66%). Half of the participants were male, and a little more than one-third (38%) were employed either full-time or part-time. More than half of the participants (57%) had an annual income greater than $30,000, and most had some high school education or had completed high school. Participants reported, on average, eating 5 servings of fruits and vegetables a day. There were no significant demographic differences between the intervention groups.
Table 1.
Variable | Total N = 469 |
Control N = 122 |
TPC N = 111 |
TMI N = 113 |
TPC+TMI N = 123 |
P-value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age in Years | ||||||
Mean (SD) | 67.3 (9.5) | 67.3 (9.8) | 67.1 (9.9) | 67.4 (9.3) | 67.2 (9.2) | .99 |
Race, % (N) | ||||||
White | 66% (309) | 64% (78) | 72% (80) | 60% (68) | 68% (83) | .28 |
Sex, % (N) | ||||||
Male | 50% (234) | 47% (57) | 46% (51) | 57% (64) | 50% (62) | .35 |
Employed, % (N) | ||||||
Yes | 38% (176) | 33% (40) | 40% (44) | 38% (43) | 40% (49) | .63 |
Income, % (N) | ||||||
≥ $30,000 | 57% (244) | 63% (71) | 56% (56) | 48% (51) | 59% (66) | .13 |
Education, % (N) | ||||||
8 years or less | 6% (27) | 5% (5) | 6% (7) | 6% (7) | 6% (7) | .76 |
9–12 years | 40% (187) | 34% (41) | 42% (47) | 41% (46) | 44% (53) | |
13+ | 54% (250) | 61% (73) | 51% (57) | 53% (59) | 50% (61) | |
Daily FVC | ||||||
Mean (Median) | 5.5 (5.2) | 5.7 (5.4) | 5.4 (5.4) | 5.4 (5.1) | 5.5 ( 5.1) | .23 |
Note: FVC = fruit and vegetable consumption
Measures
Independent Variable
Intervention condition
As discussed above, participants were randomized to four intervention groups: control, tailored print communication (TPC), telephone motivational interviewing (TMI), and combined (TPC+TMI).
Dependent Variable
Fruit and vegetable consumption
Average daily FVC was measured using a 36-item modified version of the Block food frequency questionnaire (FFQ), validated by Resnicow and colleagues in their work with a diverse Southern population (Resnicow et al., 2000). Resnicow’s measure was slightly modified to ask how often food was consumed in the last month as opposed to the last week, and food items that were not fruits and vegetables were omitted. For analysis purposes, the item “french fries, fried potatoes, or home fries” was eliminated from calculations; thus, the FVC total was based on 35 items. Fruit and vegetable item frequencies were converted to servings/day and then summed to provide total daily consumption values for fruit, vegetables, and total FVC. The distribution of FVC was skewed to the right; therefore we employed a log transformation (ln+1) in order to improve normality.
Mediator Variables
Relevance of communication
This variable was constructed as a latent variable and defined as the relevance of the intervention’s message to the participant’s life. Three indicators measured this concept: (1) “How important to you personally was the information in the newsletter,” (2) “How much did you feel that the newsletters were designed especially for you,” and (3) “How much did the information in the newsletters apply to your life.” The response categories ranged from 1 (not at all) to 5 (completely). For participants receiving TMI, the three questions were asked about the communication in the phone calls instead of newsletters. For those receiving the combined intervention (TPC+TMI), questions were asked about both newsletters and phone calls. The mean of the responses for newsletters and phone calls was calculated for those receiving both TPC+TMI. Cronbach’s alpha showed good reliability for the three questions on newsletters (α = .73) and phone calls (α = .76) and high reliability for the mean of the two (α = .94).
Trust in the communication
Trust was measured with one question: “How much did you trust that the information in the newsletter was accurate?” Response categories ranged from 1 (not at all) to 5 (completely). For participants receiving TMI, the question asked about communication in the phone calls instead of newsletters. For those receiving the combined intervention, the question asked about trust in both newsletters and phone calls.
Dose recall
This variable was defined as participants’ ability to recall the number of intervention messages that they received, and was measured for TPC participants by the question: “How many newsletters do you remember receiving?” The answers ranged from 1 to 5, where 1 = 1 newsletter, 2 = 2 newsletters, 3 = 3 newsletters, 4 = 4 newsletters, and 5 = more than 4 newsletters. For participants receiving TMI, the question asked about phone calls. To those receiving the combined intervention, questions were about both newsletters and phone calls.
Other Variables
Demographic variables
Age was a continuous variable, calculated using the respondent’s date of birth and the date of the interview. Race was collected as white or Black. Gender was collected as male or female. Education was the highest grade of school completed, collected as a categorical variable with the options of “8 years or less,” “9–12 years,” and “13+ years.” Employment was measured as yes/no. Annual income was collected by asking the total yearly household income and included 6 categories; “less than $10,000,” “$10,000 – $19,999,” “$20,000 – $29,999,” “$30,000 – $49,999,” “$50,000 – $74,999,” and “$75,000 or more.” These categories were dichotomized as less than $30,000 or greater than/equal to $30,000.
Data Analysis
Data formatting, management, and descriptive statistics were conducted using SAS version 9.2. Descriptive analyses generated frequencies for categorical variables and means for continuous variables. Bivariate analyses were conducted with Fisher’s exact test of significance for categorical variables, and t-tests and ANOVA were used for continuous variables. Alpha levels of .05 were used for all analyses. The structural equation model was estimated using MPLUS version 5. A multi-sample structural equation model was specified to test the hypothesized relationships between the variables among the four intervention groups.
Criteria for Establishing Mediation
The structural equation model (SEM) provides a multivariate method for evaluating mediation by first allowing the user to evaluate the effect of the intervention on the outcome (Model 1). A second model (Model 2) is tested to simultaneously evaluate the effects of the intervention on the proposed mediators and their effects on the outcome (MacKinnon & Dwyer, 1993).
Model Fit
Multiple fit indices were used to assess model fit. These included the Chi-square test statistic, the Root-Mean-Square-Error of Approximation (RMSEA), the Standardized Root Square Mean Residual (SRMR), the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), and the Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI). We also relied on standard cutoff recommendations for the RMSEA, SRMR, CFI, and TLI because with a large sample size, the Chi-square test is not a reliable method for assessing model fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999). For the RMSEA and the SRMR, values approximating 0.05 indicate close fit. For the CFI and the TLI, values greater than or equal to 0.95 suggest a model with proportionate improvement in fit from the baseline model (Hu & Bentler, 1999). When models were just identified, that is, when the number of observable variances and covariances equaled the number of parameters of the model to be estimated, the fit indices could not be used to evaluate the model fit (Byrne, 2001).
Model Specification
A SEM was built to test the relationship between the intervention variables (TPC, TMI, TPC+TMI), mediating variables (relevance, trust, and dose recall), and FVC. The SEM analyses proceeded in three steps. First, a confirmatory factor analysis measurement model was specified to evaluate the viability of the proposed latent factor (relevance). Next, two models were specified to establish mediation as described above. The variance and covariance upon which the analyses were based are presented in Appendix A. Finally, a multi-sample SEM was specified to examine whether the mediation pathways varied across the four intervention groups (group 1 = control, group 2 = TPC, group 3 = TMI, and group 4 = TPC+TMI).
Appendix A.
Especially | Importance | Application | FVC | Recall | Trust | TPC+TMI | TPC | TMI | B_FVC | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Especially | 1.085 | |||||||||
Importance | .918 | 1.104 | ||||||||
Application | .926 | .932 | 1.137 | |||||||
FVC | .507 | .412 | .395 | 6.443 | ||||||
Recall | .587 | .599 | .625 | .335 | .988 | |||||
Trust | .986 | 1.003 | 1.031 | .309 | .719 | 1.498 | ||||
TPC+TMI | .088 | .077 | .102 | .102 | .137 | .096 | .193 | |||
TPC | − .015 | − .018 | − .015 | − .050 | .035 | − .023 | − .062 | .181 | ||
TMI | .015 | .044 | .033 | − .007 | − .011 | .016 | − .063 | − .057 | .183 | |
B_FVC | .108 | − .002 | − .041 | 3.616 | − .006 | − .101 | .002 | − .031 | − .025 | 6.278 |
Matrix Key: Especially = communication especially designed for self; Importance = importance of the communication to self; Application = communication application to life; FVC = follow-up fruit and vegetable consumption; Recall = dose recall; Trust = trust in the communication; TPC = tailored print communication; TMI = telephone motivational interviewing; B_FVC = baseline fruit and vegetable consumption
In a multi-sample SEM model, several groups are analyzed at the same time, providing the ability to simultaneously test a theoretical model for its applicability to different groups and to identify similarities and differences in the parameters between these groups (Byrne, 2001; Duncan et al., 1999). Multi-sample SEM was conducted through three major steps (Byrne, 2001). First, two models were built, one where all structural paths were specified as equal across the four intervention groups, and another where all paths were specified to differ across the groups. Second, model equivalence were evaluated using a χ2 difference test. The χ2 difference test is an asymptotically equivalent χ2 test which represents an approximate decrease in model goodness-of-fit resulting from eliminating equality restrictions (Duncan et al., 1999). When the χ2 test favored the model with unequal paths across the intervention groups, nested models were specified. Third, nested models were built by constraining and releasing each individual structural path. Where significant differences in model parameters existed between nested models, constraints were relaxed and the model re-estimated.
Results
Confirmatory Factor Analysis of Relevance of Communication
The measurement model confirmed that the three questions used for this latent variable (communication especially designed for self, importance of communication, and communication’s application to life) were sufficiently empirically related to reliably form one factor. All variable loadings on the hypothesized latent factor were found to be strong and significant. The fit indices could not be calculated because the model was just identified.
Mediational Analysis of Health Communication Strategies, Information Processes, and FVC
The model testing the direct relationship between health communication strategies and FVC, adjusting for baseline FVC, is shown in Figure 2. The combined health communication strategy (TPC+TMI) had a direct significant effect on FVC (β= 0.68, p = .011). There was no significant association between either single strategy, TPC (β= 0.16, p = .56) or TMI (β= 0.32, p = .23), and FVC. This model was just identified.
The model testing the indirect relationship between intervention and FVC through information processes had a good fit with (χ2 (29, N = 469) = 93.93, CFI = .96, TLI = .95, RMSEA = .07, and SRMR = .05). The relationship between the health communication strategies and FVC was mediated by information processes as shown in Figure 3. All three health communication strategies were significantly related to communication relevance (TPC: β = 0.32, p < .05; TMI: β = 0.51, p < .001; TPC+TMI: β = 0.73, p < .001). The participants’ reports of relevance of communication were significantly related to their trust in the communication (β = 1.11, p < .001). Trust was significantly related to dose recall (β = 0.47, p < .001) and recall was significantly associated with FVC (β = 0.37, p < .05). The Sobel (1992) test showed a significant indirect effect for the TMI group (β = 0.01 p = .03) and the TPC+TMI group (β = 0.14, p = .009).
Similar and Different Information Processes in the Four Intervention Groups
Results of the multi-sample SEM showed that when comparing intervention groups, some of the structural paths were similar and some were different. Model comparisons of all paths, constrained vs. non-constrained, showed model non-equivalence; i.e., all paths were not equal across intervention groups. The model fit where all paths were constrained as equal was χ2 (55, N = 469) = 94.63, p < .001, CFI = .97, TLI = .97, RMSEA = .08, and SRMR = .11. The model fit for the unconstrained model was χ2 (34, N = 469) = 43.31, p = .13, CFI = .99, TLI = .99, RMSEA = .05, and SRMR = .03. The χ2 difference test showed that the fit of the constrained model was significantly worse than that of the unconstrained model χ2 (21, N = 469) = 51.32, p < .001, supporting the model where paths were set to differ across the four intervention groups. Nested models, then, were built to identify which paths were equal or different across the four intervention groups.
Model 1 was the reference model with the measurement model constrained. Model 2, constrained on the path relevance and trust, was not significantly different from the reference model when using the χ2 difference test. This finding indicates that the intervention groups were equal in that structural path, χ2 (3, N = 469) = 1.74, p > .05. In model 3, the structural path relevance and dose recall was significantly different between the four intervention groups χ2 (3, N = 469) = 8.29, p < .05, which indicates that the intervention groups were different on that path. In model 4, there was no significant difference in the path trust and dose recall, χ2 (3, N = 469) = 4.34, p > .05, indicating that the intervention groups were similar in that path. In model 5, the structural path relevance and FVC was significantly different between intervention groups, χ2 (3, N = 469) = 10.19, p < .05. Lastly, in model 6, intervention groups also differed in the path dose recall and FVC, χ2 (3, N = 469) = 9.19, p < .05.
Model 4 was selected as the final model. It included constraints on the measurement model and the structural paths relevance and trust and trust and dose recall. Selection was based on model parsimony; that is, we selected a model with relatively few free parameters and more constraints (Preacher, 2006). The final model fit resulted in χ2 (46, N = 469) = 60.83, p = .07, CFI = .99, TLI =.99, RMSEA = .05, and SRMR = .05.
Across all four intervention groups, paths were equal in the relationships between a) relevance and trust, and b) trust and dose recall, as shown in Figure 4. That is, individuals who perceived the intervention as relevant had greater trust in the communication, and greater trust was related to greater dose recall in every type of intervention received.
Paths were different across the four intervention groups in the relationships between a) relevance and dose recall, b) relevance and FVC, and c) dose recall and FVC. Among those who received either the non-tailored information (control group) or the TPC intervention only, information processes did not mediate FVC. Individuals who received the TMI intervention only and perceived that the communication in the phone calls was relevant to them consumed significantly more fruits and vegetables (β = 0.93, p < .05). Receiving a combination of TPC and TMI was significantly related through two paths to eating more fruits and vegetables. First, those who reported that the intervention was relevant to them trusted the communication more (p < .001). More trust was associated with greater dose recall (p < .05) and greater recall increased FVC (β= 0.78, p < .05). In addition, the more the communication was perceived as relevant, the better it was recalled (p< .001) and recall increased FVC (β= 0.78, p < .05).
Discussion
This study examined whether information processes mediated the association between a health communication intervention and FVC among a population-based sample. The relationship between the intervention and FVC was only mediated through relevance of communication for those receiving TMI. Relevance of communication mediated the relationship for participants receiving TPC+TMI as well, but for them the relationship was also mediated by trust and dose call. For those receiving the control message or TPC only, these processes did not mediate the effect of the intervention on FVC. Furthermore, when comparing health communication intervention groups, some of the structural paths were similar and some were different.
Similar Information Processes of Health Communication and FVC
Our findings suggest that participants process messages similarly regardless of the type of interventions received. In all intervention groups, relevance of communication was the first important information process. The elaboration likelihood model (ELM) developed by Petty and Cacioppo (1981) helps explain this finding. The ELM posits that people are more likely to process information thoughtfully if they perceive it as personally relevant. Tailored communication is often reported as being more personally relevant, and when information is seen as more personally relevant, it is more likely to stimulate information processing (Campbell & Quintiliani, 2006; Kreuter et al., 2004; Hawkins et al., 2008). Trust in the communication was also important for our study participants. Petty and colleagues posit that an active information processor considers the information that he or she receives, carefully relating it to prior knowledge, past experiences, and other information to assess its credibility (Hawkins et al. 2008). Lastly, recall was another mediator in our study. Communication that is processed or “elaborated” tends to be retained longer (Petty et al., 1994). Campbell and Quintiliani (2006) found that tailored messages were more likely than non-tailored ones to be remembered by individuals. Both TPC and TMI interventions were personalized for the individual participants; therefore, we were not surprised to find that individuals in the TPC, TMI and combined groups were processing information through relevance, trust, and recall.
Nonetheless, it was surprising to observe that individuals in the control group, who received non-tailored newsletters, also processed through relevance, trust, and recall. Others have found similar phenomena. Kreuter and Wray (2003) reported that when non-tailored communications are a good fit for an individual, it is just as effective as tailored communications in stimulating information processing. The participants in the control group received messages on gender-relevant health topics. Women received information about breast cancer and men about prostate cancer. These topics may have been salient for the participants, who were mainly older individuals. Kreuter and colleagues (2004) reported that women aged 40 and older considered the topic of breast cancer prevention to be important. Similarly, prostate cancer has generated much attention among men, as the most frequently diagnosed cancer in men (American Cancer Society, 2008). Thus, the non-tailored messages may have been a good fit for individuals in the control group. It is, however, important to note that information processes did not influence FVC for this group. One possible explanation is that the non-tailored newsletters did not address FVC. Alternately, when individuals process information, they may also elaborate counterarguments. This process may have lessened the effects of the non-tailored communication (Hawkins et al., 2008; Petty & Cacioppo, 1994).
Differentiated Information Processing by Intervention Groups
Our findings also point to differentiated processes explaining intervention effectiveness. Among individuals in the TMI-only intervention, those who believed that the communication in the phone calls was relevant to them ate more fruits and vegetables. This relationship, however, did not emerge as significant among those receiving the TPC-only intervention. This could have been possible for several reasons. First, the communication via phone may have been perceived as more personal than print newsletters, as it involved interpersonal interaction with a counselor. Additionally, the same counselor was assigned to the same participant each time, enabling the counselor to build rapport during the intervention. Finally, the counseling sessions were client-centered, with counselors trained in the “Spirit of Motivational Interviewing” to use empathy and a non-judgmental counseling style (Miller & Rollnick, 1991). Client-centered counseling sessions are more effective than those that instruct the client on what to do, because participants become motivated as they reflect on their values and explore ways to resolve ambivalence about adopting new behaviors (Miller & Rollnick, 1991). In contrast, individuals in the TPC-only intervention group received a printed menu of advice for goal setting and ways to overcome barriers to eating more fruits and vegetables, and may have lacked the support needed to motivate them to overcome such barriers.
Individuals in the combined intervention group processed information about eating more fruits and vegetables through multiple mediating paths. Studies show that receiving messages through multiple ways can enhance information processes (Cacioppo & Petty, 1979; Grass & Wallace, 1969). In our study, it is likely that the communication from the newsletters and the phone calls may have served to reinforce one another. The telephone counseling calls using MI may have served to resolve ambivalence and clarify questions for our study participants, while the tailored print newsletters may have reinforced the communication from the calls.
Additionally, the combined effect may be a reflection of the dose of the intervention. The individuals in the combined group received four TPCs plus four TMI calls, which was double the number of intervention contacts for each separate intervention. Another study that standardized the intervention dose by giving the combined intervention group two TPCs and two TMI calls, while a TPC-only group received four mailings and a TMI group received four calls, reported that all three intervention groups showed similar effects at follow-up (van Keulen et al., 2008; van Keulen, personal communication, 2009).
It is important to note that, although we did not find a significant direct relationship between TMI and FVC, we found an indirect effect through relevance of the communication. This finding is not uncommon, as several researchers have questioned the necessity of testing a direct association, particularly in an experimental study (MacKinnon et al., 2000; Shrout & Bolger, 2002). For example, Shrout and Bolger explain that when mediation involves proximal causal processes and when the intervention is a strong agent of change in the outcome, then a significant direct effect may occur shortly thereafter. When it involves distal causal processes, however, this relationship may be absent and the mediation effect may be larger, because it assesses the relationship between more proximal variables (intervention to mediator and mediator to outcome). They recommend that researchers not be rigid about the presence of an overall direct effect.
Study Strengths and Limitations
This study has several strengths. First was our ability to extend previous findings by conceptualizing information processes as mediators of health communication and FVC. Past studies have reported a significant bivariate relationship between intervention and information processes, as well as noting a relationship between information processes and behavior change (Bull et al., 2001; Kreuter et al., 2004). We extended this research by conceptualizing and empirically testing theory-based information processes as mediators of health communication and FVC. A methodological strength was the use of multi-sample SEM to test the theoretical model across intervention groups. This technique allowed us to identify the information processes that mediated each intervention group. This study also provides some ability to generalize the results, as the participants from this study were a diverse group of people, including 50% females and 35% African Americans.
The limitations of the study are that the data were collected as part of a larger study that was not designed for testing information processes. Therefore, not all the information processes were included in the conceptual model (McGuire, 1985), and variable selection was dependent on the variables available from the original study dataset. McGuire (1985), however, states that it is not necessary to include all information processes in empirical studies. Finally, the data are retrospective self-reported data, and may have been open to recall bias. However, it is likely that randomization minimized bias across the intervention groups.
Conclusions
This study provides evidence that information processes can be considered as mediators of the relationship between a health communication intervention and FVC. Interventions that focus on FVC may be evaluated with regard to their potential for increasing perception of relevance of communication, trust in the communication, and dose recall. As intervention studies continue to evolve, special attention paid to information processes may yield a more refined understanding of what kinds of processes “matter” for which types of interventions and for whom, as well as the potential influence of information processes on health behaviors.
Acknowledgments
The NC STRIDES study was supported by grants from the National Cancer Institute (RO1-CA81914). This project was supported in part by the UNC Lineberger Cancer Control Education Program (R25 CA057726), Lawrence G. Branch doctoral student research award awarded by the American Public Health Association, and the Vivian Drenckhan Graduate Student Scholarship awarded by the Society for Public Health Education. The authors would like to acknowledge Dr. Joan F. Walsh and Ms. Carol Carr for their helpful comments on an earlier draft of this article.
Contributor Information
Linda K. Ko, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill
Marci K. Campbell, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill
Megan A. Lewis, RTI International
Jo Anne Earp, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill.
Brenda DeVellis, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill.
References
- Adams SM, Standridge JB. What should we eat? Evidence from observational studies. Southern Medical Association. 2006;99:744–748. doi: 10.1097/01.smj.0000220887.52952.f0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Cancer Society (ACS) Cancer facts and figures. 2008 Retrieved April 10, 2008, from ( http://www.cancer.org/downloads/STT/2008CAFFfinalsecured.pdf)
- Brug J, Steenhuis I, van Assema P, de Vries H. The impact of a workplace computer-tailored nutrition education instrument. Preventive Medicine. 1996;25:236–242. doi: 10.1006/pmed.1996.0052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bull FC, Holt CL, Kreuter MW, Clark EM, Scharff D. Understanding the effects of print health education materials: Which features lead to which outcomes. Journal of Health Communication. 2001;6:265–279. doi: 10.1080/108107301752384442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bundy C. Changing behaviour: Using motivational interviewing techniques. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine. 2004;97:43–47. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Byrne B. Structural equation modeling with AMOS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Cacioppo JT, Petty RE. Effects of message repetition and position on cognitive response, recall, and persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1979;37:97–109. [Google Scholar]
- Campbell MK, James A, Hudson MA, Carr C, Jackson E, Oakes V, et al. Improving multiple behaviors for colorectal cancer prevention among African American church members. Health Psychology. 2004;5:492–502. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.23.5.492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campbell MK, Quintiliani L. Tailored interventions in public health. American Behavioral Scientist. 2006;49:1–19. [Google Scholar]
- Casagrande SS, Wang Y, Anderson C, Gary TL. Have Americans increased their fruit and vegetable intake? The trends between 1988 and 2002. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2007;32:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2006.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duncan T, Duncan S, Strycker L, Li F, Alpert A. An introduction to latent variable growth curve modeling: Concepts, issues, and applications. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Emmons KM, McBride CM, Puleo E, Pollak KI, Clipp E, Kuntz K, et al. Project PREVENT: A randomized trial to reduce multiple behavioral risk factors for colon cancer. Cancer Epidemiology Biomarkers and Prevention. 2005;14:1453–1459. doi: 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-04-0620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Emmons KM, Rollnick S. Motivational interviewing in health care settings. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2001;20:68–74. doi: 10.1016/s0749-3797(00)00254-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grass RC, Wallace WH. Satiation effects of TV commercials. Journal of Advertising. 1969;9:3–9. [Google Scholar]
- Hawkins RP, Kreuter M, Resnicow K, Fishbein M, Dijkstra A. Understanding tailoring in communicating about health. Health Education Research. 2008;23:454–466. doi: 10.1093/her/cyn004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling. 1999;6:1–55. [Google Scholar]
- James AS, Campbell MK, DeVellis B, Reedy J, Carr C, Sandler RS. Health behavior correlates among colon cancer survivors: NC STRIDES baseline results. American Journal of Health Behavior. 2006;30:720–730. doi: 10.5555/ajhb.2006.30.6.720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreuter M, Skinner CS, Steger-May K, Holt CL, Bucholtz DC, Clark EM, et al. Responses to behaviorally vs culturally tailored cancer communication among African American women. American Journal of Health Behavior. 2004;28:195–207. doi: 10.5993/ajhb.28.3.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreuter MW, Wray RJ. Tailored and targeted health communication: Strategies for enhancing information relevance. American Journal of Health Behavior. 2003;27:S227–S232. doi: 10.5993/ajhb.27.1.s3.6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacKinnon DP, Dwyer JH. Estimating mediated effects in prevention studies. Evaluation Review. 1993;17:144–158. [Google Scholar]
- MacKinnon DP, Krull JL, Lockwood CM. Equivalence of the mediation, confounding and suppression effects. Prevention Science. 2000;1:173–181. doi: 10.1023/a:1026595011371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGuire WJ. Attitudes and attitude change. In: Gardner L, Elliot A, editors. The handbook of social psychology. 3. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1985. pp. 233–346. [Google Scholar]
- McGuire WJ. Input and output variables currently promising for constructing persuasive communications. In: Rice R, Atkin C, editors. Public communication campaigns. 3. Newbury Park: Sage; 2001. pp. 22–48. [Google Scholar]
- Miller WR, Rollnick S. Motivational interviewing. New York: Guildford Press; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Ness AR, Powless JW. Fruit and vegetables, and cardiovascular disease: A review. International Journal of Epidemiology. 1997;26:1–13. doi: 10.1093/ije/26.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petty RE, Cacioppo JT. Attitudes and persuasion: Classic and contemporary approaches. Dubuque, Iowa: W.C. Brown; 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Petty RE, Priester JR, Wegener DT. Cognitive processes in attitude change. In: Wyer RS, Srull TK, editors. Handbook of social cognition. 2. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1994. pp. 69–142. [Google Scholar]
- Preacher KJ. Quantifying parsimony in structural equation modeling. Multivariate. 2006;41:227–259. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr4103_1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Resnicow K, Campbell MK, Carr C, McCarty F, Wang T, Periasamy S, et al. Body and soul: An effectiveness trial of a dietary intervention conducted through black churches. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2004;27:97–105. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2004.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Resnicow K, Shaikh A. Motivational interviewing in medical settings application to obesity conceptual issues and evidence review. In: Kushner RF, Bessesen DH, editors. Treatment of the obese patients. NJ: Humana Press; 2007. pp. 321–339. [Google Scholar]
- Resnicow K, Wallace D, Jackson A, Digirolamo A, Odom E, Wang T, et al. Dietary change through African American churches: Baseline results and program description of the eat for life trial. Journal of Cancer Education. 2000;15:156–163. doi: 10.1080/08858190009528685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rimer BK, Glassman B. Is there a use for tailored print communications for cancer risk communication? Journal of the National Cancer Institute Monographs. 1999;25:140–148. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.jncimonographs.a024190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Satia J, Campbell M, Galanko J, James A, Carr C, Sandler R. Longitudinal changes and lifestyle behaviors and health status in colon cancer survivors. Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers, and Prevention. 2004;13:1022–1031. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shrout PE, Bolger N. Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: New procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods. 2002;7:422–445. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skinner CS, Campbell MK, Rimer BK, Curry S, Prochaska JO. How effective is tailored print communication? Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 1999;21:290–298. doi: 10.1007/BF02895960. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slattery ML. Diet, lifestyle, and colon cancer. Seminars in Gastrointestinal Disease. 2000;11:142–146. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sobel ME. Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. In: Leinhardt S, editor. Sociological methodology. Washington, DC: American Sociological Association; 1982. pp. 290–312. [Google Scholar]
- Srinath Reddy K, Katan MB. Diet, nutrition and the prevention of hypertension and cardiovascular diseases. Public Health Nutrition. 2004;7:167–186. doi: 10.1079/phn2003587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Keulen HM, Mesters I, Brug J, Ausems M, Campbell M, Resnicow K, et al. Vitalum study design: RCT evaluating the efficacy of tailored print communication and telephone motivational interviewing on multiple health behaviors. BMC Public Health. 2008 doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-8-216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]