Abstract
A large body of research indicates that weak expressions of positive psychotic symptoms (“psychotic experiences”) can be measured in the general population, and likely represent the behavioural manifestation of distributed multifactorial (genetic and non‐genetic) risk for psychosis. Psychotic experiences are a transdiagnostic phenomenon: the majority of individuals with these experiences have a diagnosis of non‐psychotic disorder, particularly common mental disorder, in which psychotic experiences predict greater illness severity and poorer treatment response. Some of the people with common mental disorder and psychotic experiences will present to mental health services meeting criteria for “clinical high risk”. Treatment of the transdiagnostic dimension of psychosis in individuals with common mental disorder who meet “clinical high risk” criteria thus may improve outcome (which cannot be interpreted as prevention of “schizophrenia”). Subthreshold psychotic experiences are transitory in about 80% of individuals, while around 20% go on to develop persistent psychotic experiences and 7% a psychotic disorder, with an annual transition rate of 0.5‐1%. Persistence is associated, on the one hand, with environmental exposures, particularly childhood trauma, and, on the other, with network‐type dynamic interactions between psychotic experiences themselves (e.g., interactions between hallucinatory experiences and delusional ideation) and between symptom dimensions (e.g., interactions between affective symptoms and psychotic experiences, or interactions between subthreshold negative symptoms and psychotic experiences). The study of psychotic experiences is helping to elucidate the mechanisms by which environmental and genetic influences shape the transdiagnostic expression of psychosis proneness, that is mostly transitory but may first become persistent over time and eventually give rise to transition to a psychotic disorder.
Keywords: Psychotic experiences, extended psychosis phenotype, ultra‐high‐risk states, genetic risk, socio‐environmental factors, neurocognition, aberrant salience, network models of severity
While there has been no universal consensus on the concept of “psychosis”, since the term was introduced by Canstatt into the psychiatric literature1, one of the most common uses has been to refer to phenomena such as delusions and hallucinations2.
These phenomena have been thought of as key characteristics of psychotic disorders such as schizophrenia for a long time and, somewhat more recently, also referred to as the positive symptom dimension3. However, in recent years, it has become increasingly evident that psychotic experiences are common not only in individuals with psychotic disorder, but also in the general population (i.e., prevalence of ∼7%)4. In addition, while subclinical psychotic experiences are transitory in about 80% of individuals, around 20% go on to develop persistent psychotic experiences and 7% a psychotic disorder, with an annual transition rate below 1%4, 5, 6.
These findings have been taken to suggest an “extended psychosis phenotype”7, i.e. a phenotype that shares demographic, environmental, familial and psychopathological features7 and is both phenomenologically and temporally continuous with clinical psychotic disorder. In other words, while psychotic experiences are not exclusive to, and can occur independently of, psychotic disorder (“phenomenological continuity”), these experiences can endure over time in some individuals, and may be followed by a psychotic disorder (“temporal continuity”)4.
This continuity of psychotic experiences and psychotic disorder implies that, at all phenomenological and temporal stages of the “extended psychosis phenotype”, individuals may become help‐seeking and classified as meeting criteria for an ultra‐high‐risk (UHR) state7. In UHR individuals, much higher annual transition rates have been reported, which may be explained primarily by selection for the presence of help‐seeking behaviour rather than by differences between measures for determining UHR status and presence of psychotic experiences per se 7.
There is evidence that the prevalence of psychotic experiences varies according to place and ethnicity. Nuevo et al8, for example, reported considerable variation in the prevalence of psychotic experiences across countries using data from the World Health Organization (WHO) World Health Survey. Also, in a more recent analysis of data from the WHO World Mental Health Surveys, McGrath et al9 found higher lifetime prevalence estimates in middle‐ and high‐income countries than in low‐income ones. Furthermore, psychotic experiences have been found to be more common in ethnic minority groups4, 10, 11.
The method for assessing psychotic experiences does seem to affect prevalence estimates. A recent meta‐analysis4 reported markedly higher prevalence estimates of psychotic experiences in studies based on self‐report compared with those using interview‐based measures. However, no correlation was found between prevalence estimates and the number of items used4.
A TRANSDIAGNOSTIC PHENOTYPE OF PSYCHOTIC SPECTRUM DISORDER
Most individuals with psychotic experiences have a current diagnosis, primarily one of mood or anxiety disorder12, 13, 14, 15, 16, 17, 18, accounting for the association between psychotic experiences and suicidal ideation and behaviour19. Wigman et al17 reported a more than two times greater prevalence of psychotic experiences in individuals with depression or anxiety disorder than in people without these disorders. The presence of psychotic experiences in individuals with depression or anxiety disorder is commonly associated with a poorer prognosis and, therefore, early treatment of these experiences (rather than mislabelling as UHR status) requires attention and may be beneficial for the course of psychosis expression2.
However, subclinical psychotic experiences are not only common in individuals with depression or anxiety disorder but may also be causally associated with affective disturbance, including anxiety, depressive and hypomanic symptoms13, 20, 21, 22, 23, 24. In a German prospective cohort community study of 2,524 adolescents and young adults24, a dose‐response relationship, suggesting causality, was reported between levels of affective dysregulation (both depression and mania) and psychotic experiences.
There is further evidence that subclinical experiences of negative symptoms are (at least) as prevalent as subclinical experiences of positive symptoms25, 26. In addition, subclinical negative and disorganized symptoms have been found to be predictive of, and co‐occur with, subclinical positive symptoms, and co‐occurrence of subclinical positive, negative and disorganized symptoms seems to predict later functional impairment and help‐seeking behaviour25.
The evidence therefore suggests that subclinical psychotic experiences represent two underlying constructs: a) a distribution of a specific phenotypic expression of attenuated psychotic phenomena (delusional ideation and hallucinatory experiences) and b) a set of transphenotypic fundamental associations between domains of psychopathology (positive, affective, negative, disorganization).
A similar bimodal set of general, transdiagnostic and specific phenotypic expressions is observed at the level of psychotic disorders. Thus, there is growing evidence for a transdiagnostic psychosis phenotype underlying schizophrenia spectrum and bipolar disorder, with overlapping affective and non‐affective psychotic symptoms27, 28, 29 (Figure 1). This transdiagnostic psychosis phenotype has continuity across subclinical24, 29, 30 and clinical27, 28 symptom levels and is further supported by the absence of consistent and clear “points of rarity” across psychosis spectrum disorders3, 31, 32.
Figure 1.
Schematic representation of transdiagnostic psychosis spectrum encompassing non‐affective and affective psychotic experiences
There is further evidence that a general, transdiagnostic psychosis dimension is complemented by five specific diagnostic constructs of psychosis (i.e., positive symptoms, negative symptoms, disorganization, mania, depression), which, when used in combination, allow for a more accurate classification of individuals into categorical diagnoses based on dimensional scores3, 27, 28, 32 (Figure 1). This approach draws on bifactor models for generating quantitative scores of a) a general, transdiagnostic psychosis factor and b) specific psychosis factors27, 28. Then, it adopts a strategy in which: first, quantitative scores on the general, transdiagnostic psychosis dimension may be used to determine whether to place individuals on the affective or non‐affective end of the psychosis spectrum; and, in a second step, based on the profiles for specific symptom dimensions, patients may be classified into specific diagnoses3, 27.
What is more, this approach provides directly measurable general, transdiagnostic as well as specific phenotypes for cross‐disorder investigations to identify transdiagnostically shared genetic and environmental contributions, as well as non‐shared factors contributing to specific symptom dimensions27. Given evidence for a general, transdiagnostic phenotype of psychosis at both the clinical27, 28 and subclinical27, 28 level of psychotic experiences, the existence of an “extended and transdiagnostic phenotype” in the general population can be suggested.
GENETIC AND SOCIO‐ENVIRONMENTAL FACTORS ASSOCIATED WITH THE EXTENDED PSYCHOSIS PHENOTYPE
Several studies have examined the level of psychotic experiences as an indirect measure of expression of the distributed genetic risk for psychotic disorder. Findings from these studies suggest that subclinical psychotic experiences and schizotypal symptoms in twins from the general population33, 34, 35, 36 and relatives of patients with psychosis37 are influenced by genetic effects. There is also evidence that subclinical psychotic experiences may reflect the transitory developmental expression of genetic risk for psychosis in the general population38.
A Danish birth cohort study reported that subclinical psychotic experiences at age 11‐12 years, assessed by clinical interview, were strongly associated with a family history of treated psychotic, but not common mental disorder, identified in an unbiased fashion through the national case register39. Further, studies and meta‐analyses have consistently reported that socio‐environmental risk factors such as ethnicity4, 10, 11, 40, 41, urbanicity23, 42, 43, 44, 45, childhood adversity4, 11, 46, 47, stressful life events21, 46, 48, and cannabis use4, 13, 21, 49, 50, 51, 52, 53, 54, 55 are shared across subclinical psychotic experiences and psychotic disorders.
Wigman et al36, in a general population sample of female twins, showed that childhood trauma and prospectively recorded stressful life events were associated with persistence of psychotic experiences. In addition, psychotic experiences were more likely to persist in monozygotic than in dizygotic twins when persistence occurred in the co‐twin36.
Overall, these findings suggest that both genetic and socio‐environmental factors are associated with the “extended psychosis phenotype”. However, to date, molecular genetic studies have failed to generate replicated findings on similar associations with a priori selected single‐nucleotide polymorphisms56, 57, a limited early version of the polygenic risk score57, or genetic variants identified using a genome‐wide association approach57.
Cross‐disorder investigations and studies using the more powerful recent version of the polygenic risk score are now required for identifying shared genetic and environmental factors (including G × E) of the “transdiagnostic and extended psychosis” phenotype as well as non‐shared factors of specific psychosis constructs.
NEUROCOGNITION, ABERRANT SALIENCE, REASONING BIASES AND THE EXTENDED PSYCHOSIS PHENOTYPE
Neurocognitive alterations, in particular in processing speed and working memory, have been reported to be more common in individuals with psychotic experiences than in those without these experiences58, 59, 60, 61, 62. There is also some evidence of poorer functioning in individuals who report subclinical psychotic experiences, which may potentially in part be due to neurocognitive alterations62.
However, to what degree any association between psychotic experiences and neurocognitive alterations is specific is difficult to examine, as psychotic experiences are strongly associated with a range of non‐psychotic mental disorders which in turn are associated with cognitive alterations63. The fact that neurocognitive alterations have been found in siblings of patients with psychotic disorder and, to a lesser extent, in siblings of patients with non‐psychotic disorders, suggests transdiagnostic overlap even at the level of what is commonly considered a key marker of genetic risk of schizophrenia7, 64.
Not only neurocognitive alterations in processing speed and working memory but also dysregulation in top‐down processing such as white noise speech illusion may be relevant to the “extended psychosis phenotype”65, 66. An association between a tendency to detect affectively salient speech illusions in random noise with higher levels of positive schizotypy has been previously reported in healthy controls66 and in patients with a psychotic disorder65, 66. Recently, aberrant novelty and salience was also found to be associated with more intense psychotic experiences in daily life in patients with first‐episode psychosis, UHR individuals, and healthy controls67. In this experience sampling study, the association between aberrant salience and momentary psychotic experiences was greatest in UHR individuals, which suggests that aberrant salience may be particularly relevant to the development of subclinical and attenuated psychotic experiences67.
Another key cognitive process relevant to psychotic experiences across different phenomenological and temporal stages of psychosis are reasoning biases, most prominently, a tendency to jump to conclusions68, 69, 70, 71, 72, defined as a bias towards gathering less data to reach decisions. Several studies have reported that the jump to conclusions bias is specifically associated with subclinical and clinical delusional experiences in experimental and virtual reality paradigms73, 74, 75, 76, 77, 78, 79, 80, 81.
These findings are consistent with the proposition that responses of aberrant salience to subtle variations in the environment as well as reasoning biases reflect “microphenotypes” that potentially form part of the core vulnerability of the “extended psychosis phenotype”7, 82.
TRANSDIAGNOSTIC AND NETWORK MODELS OF SEVERITY
Several studies have reported that exposure to childhood trauma is associated with both occurrence and persistence of psychotic experiences83, 84, 85, 86, 87. For example, in a recent study87, individuals with childhood trauma reported higher levels of psychotic experiences both at baseline and at 3‐year follow‐up than those without childhood trauma, suggesting that childhood trauma creates a vulnerability for psychotic experiences to persist over time.
If, as van Os and Linscott7 proposed, psychotic experiences persist over a prolonged period of time under the influence of G × E, this may increase the risk for initial onset and sustained expression of psychotic disorder, as demonstrated by Dominguez et al88 in a repeated measures study of psychotic experiences in the general population spanning more than 10 years.
In addition, van Nierop et al89 reported that childhood trauma increases in particular the likelihood of co‐occurrence of hallucinations and delusions (rather than either symptom alone), which has, in turn, been shown to be associated with greater symptom severity90 and familial risk of psychotic disorder39, 91. Since a similar pattern is evident for other socio‐environmental factors, such as cannabis use and urbanicity90, 92, as well as for increased likelihood of co‐occurrence of psychotic experiences with other symptoms including affective and anxiety symptoms93, 94, it has been proposed that a transdiagnostic model of severity may apply, in which coexistence of psychotic experiences, affective and anxiety symptoms reflects greater severity, socio‐environmental risk and poorer functioning.
This may be complemented by, and combined with, a network model of severity (Figure 2), in which symptoms of the transdiagnostic psychosis phenotype do not vary independently, but impact on each other over time, and connectivity of symptoms increases as socio‐environmental load increases95, 96, 97. In this model, as a result of elevated connectivity, more symptoms are recruited and severity of states increased further, which, in the event of exposure to further socio‐environmental adversity, leads to an increased probability of clinical transition to psychotic disorder95, 96, 97.
Figure 2.
Environmental impact on connectivity in the network, resulting in psychosis admixture. In A, there is a low level of environmental exposure, creating a minor disturbance that does not spread extensively through the network of symptoms and remains “contained” in the non‐psychotic domain of psychopathology. In B, environmental exposure is moderate, resulting in a more extensive spread across the network, although not into the psychotic domain of psychopathology. In C, the degree of environmental exposure is high, creating a major disturbance that spreads through the network, also “recruiting” more severe psychotic symptoms.
CONCLUSIONS AND FUTURE PROSPECTS
In recent years, research has revealed a phenomenological and temporal continuity of psychotic experiences with psychotic disorder, as well as the co‐occurrence and overlap of psychotic experiences with affective and anxiety symptoms and disorder, which, taken together, suggests an “extended and transdiagnostic psychosis phenotype” in the general population. Evidence suggests the existence of a general, transdiagnostic factor as well as five specific psychosis factors, which are measurable and best represented by a dimensional bifactor model of psychosis. A bifactor “general” and “specific” model of psychosis may substantially enhance classification accuracy of categorical diagnoses based on dimensional scores.
While there is evidence that subclinical psychotic experiences and psychotic disorder are associated with similar socio‐environmental and genetic variables, cross‐disorder investigations are now required for identifying shared genetic and socio‐environmental variables (including G × E) underlying the transdiagnostic psychosis factor, as well as non‐shared variables underlying specific psychosis factors. Transdiagnostic overlap may be present even at the level of what are commonly considered core markers of genetic risk of schizophrenia such as neurocognitive alterations. Co‐presence of neurocognitive alterations, alterations in salience attribution, and reasoning biases may be particularly relevant on the pathway from persistence of psychotic experiences to initial onset and, ultimately, sustained expression of psychotic disorder.
Initial evidence on transdiagnostic and network models of severity now needs to be strengthened further through prospective studies into the dynamic nature of the “extended psychosis phenotype” cutting across boundaries of diagnostic categories of current classification systems.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This review was funded in part by the European Community's Seventh Framework Program under grant agreement no. HEALTH‐F2‐2009‐241909 (Project EU‐GEI). U. Reininghaus is supported by a Veni grant from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (grant no. 451‐13‐022).
REFERENCES
- 1. Burgy M. The concept of psychosis: historical and phenomenological aspects. Schizophr Bull 2008;34:1200‐10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. van Os J, Murray RM. Can we identify and treat “schizophrenia light” to prevent true psychotic illness? BMJ 2013;346:f304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. van Os J, Kapur S. Schizophrenia. Lancet 2009;374:635‐45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Linscott RJ, van Os J. An updated and conservative systematic review and meta‐analysis of epidemiological evidence on psychotic experiences in children and adults: on the pathway from proneness to persistence to dimensional expression across mental disorders. Psychol Med 2013;43:1133‐49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Kaymaz N, Drukker M, Lieb R et al. Do subthreshold psychotic experiences predict clinical outcomes in unselected non‐help‐seeking population‐based samples? A systematic review and meta‐analysis, enriched with new results. Psychol Med 2012;42:2239‐53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Zammit S, Kounali D, Cannon M et al. Psychotic experiences and psychotic disorders at age 18 in relation to psychotic experiences at age 12 in a longitudinal population‐based cohort study. Am J Psychiatry 2013;170:742‐50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. van Os J, Linscott RJ. Introduction: The extended psychosis phenotype – relationship with schizophrenia and with ultrahigh risk status for psychosis. Schizophr Bull 2012;38:227‐30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Nuevo R, Chatterji S, Verdes E et al. The continuum of psychotic symptoms in the general population: a cross‐national study. Schizophr Bull 2012;38:475‐85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. McGrath JJ, Saha S, Al‐Hamzawi A et al. Psychotic experiences in the general population: a cross‐national analysis based on 31,261 respondents from 18 countries. JAMA Psychiatry 2015;72:697‐705. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Johns LC, Nazroo JY, Bebbington P et al. Occurrence of hallucinatory experiences in a community sample and ethnic variations. Br J Psychiatry 2002;180:174‐8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Morgan C, Fisher H, Hutchinson G et al. Ethnicity, social disadvantage and psychotic‐like experiences in a healthy population based sample. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2009;119:226‐35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Hanssen M, Peeters F, Krabbendam L et al. How psychotic are individuals with non‐psychotic disorders? Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2003;38:149‐54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Morgan C, Reininghaus U, Reichenberg A et al. Adversity, cannabis use and psychotic experiences: evidence of cumulative and synergistic effects. Br J Psychiatry 2014;204:346‐53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. van Os J, Verdoux H, Maurice‐Tison S et al. Self‐reported psychosis‐like symptoms and the continuum of psychosis. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 1999;34:459‐63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Varghese D, Scott J, Welham J et al. Psychotic‐like experiences in major depression and anxiety disorders: a population‐based survey in young adults. Schizophr Bull 2011;37:389‐93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Verdoux H, van Os J, Maurice‐Tison S et al. Increased occurrence of depression in psychosis‐prone subjects: a follow‐up study in primary care settings. Compr Psychiatry 1999;40:462‐8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Wigman JT, van Nierop M, Vollebergh WA et al. Evidence that psychotic symptoms are prevalent in disorders of anxiety and depression, impacting on illness onset, risk, and severity – implications for diagnosis and ultra‐high risk research. Schizophr Bull 2012;38:247‐57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Jeppesen P, Clemmensen L, Munkholm A et al. Psychotic experiences co‐occur with sleep problems, negative affect and mental disorders in preadolescence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2015;56:558‐65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Honings S, Drukker M, Groen R et al. Psychotic experiences and risk of self‐injurious behaviour in the general population: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. Psychol Med 2015;30:1‐15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Armando M, Nelson B, Yung AR et al. Psychotic‐like experiences and correlation with distress and depressive symptoms in a community sample of adolescents and young adults. Schizophr Res 2010;119:258‐65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Johns LC, Cannon M, Singleton N et al. Prevalence and correlates of self‐reported psychotic symptoms in the British population. Br J Psychiatry 2004;185:298‐305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Krabbendam L, Myin‐Germeys I, Hanssen M et al. Development of depressed mood predicts onset of psychotic disorder in individuals who report hallucinatory experiences. Br J Clin Psychol 2005;44:113‐25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. van Os J, Hanssen M, Bijl RV et al. Strauss (1969) revisited: a psychosis continuum in the general population? Schizophr Res 2000;45:11‐20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. van Rossum I, Dominguez MD, Lieb R et al. Affective dysregulation and reality distortion: a 10‐year prospective study of their association and clinical relevance. Schizophr Bull 2011;37:561‐71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Dominguez MD, Saka MC, Lieb R et al. Early expression of negative/disorganized symptoms predicting psychotic experiences and subsequent clinical psychosis: a 10‐year study. Am J Psychiatry 2010;167:1075‐82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Werbeloff N, Dohrenwend BP, Yoffe R et al. The association between negative symptoms, psychotic experiences and later schizophrenia: a population‐based longitudinal study. PLoS One 2015;10:e0119852. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Reininghaus U, Böhnke J, Hosang G et al. Probing the boundaries of the Kraepelinian dichotomy: evidence for a transdiagnostic psychosis spectrum encompassing schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Br J Psychiatry (in press). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Reininghaus U, Priebe S, Bentall RP. Testing the psychopathology of psychosis: evidence for a general psychosis dimension. Schizophr Bull 2013;39:884‐95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Shevlin M, McElroy E, Murphy J. The psychosis continuum: testing a bifactor model of psychosis in a general population sample. Manuscript in preparation. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 30. Caspi AHR, Belsky DW, Goldman‐Mellor SJ et al. The p factor: one general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clin Psychol Sci 2014;2:119‐37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Andrews G, Goldberg DP, Krueger RF et al. Exploring the feasibility of a meta‐structure for DSM‐V and ICD‐11: could it improve utility and validity? Psychol Med 2009;39:1993‐2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. van Os J. The transdiagnostic dimension of psychosis: implications for psychiatric nosology and research. Shanghai Arch Psychiatry 2015;27:82‐6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Kendler K, Hewitt J. The structure of self‐report schizotypy in twins. J Person Disord 1992;6:1‐12. [Google Scholar]
- 34. Linney YM, Murray RM, Peters ER et al. A quantitative genetic analysis of schizotypal personality traits. Psychol Med 2003;33:803‐16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. MacDonald AW 3rd, Pogue‐Geile MF, Debski TT et al. Genetic and environmental influences on schizotypy: a community‐based twin study. Schizophr Bull 2001;27:47‐58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Wigman JT, van Winkel R, Jacobs N et al. A twin study of genetic and environmental determinants of abnormal persistence of psychotic experiences in young adulthood. Am J Med Genet B: Neuropsychiatr Genet 2011;156B:546‐52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Vollema MG, Sitskoorn MM, Appels MC et al. Does the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire reflect the biological‐genetic vulnerability to schizophrenia? Schizophr Res 2002;54:39‐45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Lataster T, Myin‐Germeys I, Derom C et al. Evidence that self‐reported psychotic experiences represent the transitory developmental expression of genetic liability to psychosis in the general population. Am J Med Genet B: Neuropsychiatr Genet 2009;150B:1078‐84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Jeppesen P, Larsen JT, Clemmensen L et al. The CCC2000 birth cohort study of register‐based family history of mental disorders and psychotic experiences in offspring. Schizophr Bull 2015;41:1084‐94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Cantor‐Graae E, Selten JP. Schizophrenia and migration: a meta‐analysis and review. Am J Psychiatry 2005;162:12‐24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Reininghaus U, Craig TK, Fisher HL et al. Ethnic identity, perceptions of disadvantage, and psychosis: findings from the AESOP study. Schizophr Res 2010;124:43‐8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Heinz A, Deserno L, Reininghaus U. Urbanicity, social adversity and psychosis. World Psychiatry 2013;12:187‐97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Kuepper R, van Os J, Lieb R et al. Do cannabis and urbanicity co‐participate in causing psychosis? Evidence from a 10‐year follow‐up cohort study. Psychol Med 2011;41:2121‐9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. McGrath J, Saha S, Welham J et al. A systematic review of the incidence of schizophrenia: the distribution of rates and the influence of sex, urbanicity, migrant status and methodology. BMC Med 2004;2:13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Vassos E, Pedersen CB, Murray RM et al. Meta‐analysis of the association of urbanicity with schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 2012;38:1118‐23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Morgan C, Reininghaus U, Fearon P et al. Modelling the interplay between childhood and adult adversity in pathways to psychosis: initial evidence from the AESOP study. Psychol Med 2014;44:407‐419. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Varese F, Smeets F, Drukker M et al. Childhood adversities increase the risk of psychosis: a meta‐analysis of patient‐control, prospective‐ and cross‐sectional cohort studies. Schizophr Bull 2012;38:661‐71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Beards S, Gayer‐Anderson C, Borges S et al. Life events and psychosis: a review and meta‐analysis. Schizophr Bull 2013;39:740‐7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Arseneault L, Cannon M, Witton J et al. Causal association between cannabis and psychosis: examination of the evidence. Br J Psychiatry 2004;184:110‐7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Henquet C, Murray R, Linszen D et al. The environment and schizophrenia: the role of cannabis use. Schizophr Bull 2005;31:608‐12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Kuepper R, van Os J, Lieb R et al. Continued cannabis use and risk of incidence and persistence of psychotic symptoms: 10 year follow‐up cohort study. BMJ 2011;342:d738. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Minozzi S, Davoli M, Bargagli AM et al. An overview of systematic reviews on cannabis and psychosis: discussing apparently conflicting results. Drug Alcohol Rev 2010;29:304‐17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Moore TH, Zammit S, Lingford‐Hughes A et al. Cannabis use and risk of psychotic or affective mental health outcomes: a systematic review. Lancet 2007;370:319‐28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Semple DM, McIntosh AM, Lawrie SM. Cannabis as a risk factor for psychosis: systematic review. J Psychopharmacol 2005;19:187‐94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. van Winkel R. Family‐based analysis of genetic variation underlying psychosis‐inducing effects of cannabis: sibling analysis and proband follow‐up. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2011;68:148‐57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Sieradzka D, Power RA, Freeman D et al. Are genetic risk factors for psychosis also associated with dimension‐specific psychotic experiences in adolescence? PLoS One 2014;9:e94398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Zammit S, Hamshere M, Dwyer S et al. A population‐based study of genetic variation and psychotic experiences in adolescents. Schizophr Bull 2014;40:1254‐62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Barnett JH, McDougall F, Xu MK et al. Childhood cognitive function and adult psychopathology: associations with psychotic and non‐psychotic symptoms in the general population. Br J Psychiatry 2012;201:124‐30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Blanchard MM, Jacobson S, Clarke MC et al. Language, motor and speed of processing deficits in adolescents with subclinical psychotic symptoms. Schizophr Res 2010;123:71‐6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Cullen AE, Dickson H, West SA et al. Neurocognitive performance in children aged 9‐12 years who present putative antecedents of schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2010;121:15‐23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Kelleher I, Clarke MC, Rawdon C et al. Neurocognition in the extended psychosis phenotype: performance of a community sample of adolescents with psychotic symptoms on the MATRICS neurocognitive battery. Schizophr Bull 2013;39:1018‐26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Kelleher I, Wigman JT, Harley M et al. Psychotic experiences in the population: association with functioning and mental distress. Schizophr Res 2015;165:9‐14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Millan MJ, Agid Y, Brune M et al. Cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: characteristics, causes and the quest for improved therapy. Nat Rev Drug Discov 2012;11:141‐68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Weiser M, Reichenberg A, Kravitz E et al. Subtle cognitive dysfunction in nonaffected siblings of individuals affected by nonpsychotic disorders. Biol Psychiatry 2008;63:602‐8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Catalan A, Simons CJ, Bustamante S et al. Novel evidence that attributing affectively salient signal to random noise is associated with psychosis. PLoS One 2014;9:e102520. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Galdos M, Simons C, Fernandez‐Rivas A et al. Affectively salient meaning in random noise: a task sensitive to psychosis liability. Schizophr Bull 2011;37:1179‐86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Reininghaus U, Kempton M, Craig T et al. Psychological mechanisms underlying the association between childhood adversity and psychosis: an experience sampling study. Schizophr Res 2014;153:S358. [Google Scholar]
- 68. Fine C, Gardner M, Craigie J et al. Hopping, skipping or jumping to conclusions? Clarifying the role of the JTC bias in delusions. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 2007;12:46‐77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Garety PA, Bebbington P, Fowler D et al. Implications for neurobiological research of cognitive models of psychosis: a theoretical paper. Psychol Med 2007;37:1377‐91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Garety PA, Freeman D. Cognitive approaches to delusions: a critical review of theories and evidence. Br J Clin Psychol 1999;38(Pt. 2):113‐54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Lincoln TM, Ziegler M, Mehl S et al. The jumping to conclusions bias in delusions: specificity and changeability. J Abnorm Psychol 2010;119:40‐9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Ross RM, McKay R, Coltheart M et al. Jumping to conclusions about the Beads Task? A meta‐analysis of delusional ideation and data‐gathering. Schizophr Bull 2015;41:1183‐91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Bentall RP, Rowse G, Shryane N et al. The cognitive and affective structure of paranoid delusions: a transdiagnostic investigation of patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders and depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2009;66:236‐47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Broome MR, Johns LC, Valli I et al. Delusion formation and reasoning biases in those at clinical high risk for psychosis. Br J Psychiatry 2007;191(Suppl. 51):s38‐42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Colbert SM, Peters ER. Need for closure and jumping‐to‐conclusions in delusion‐prone individuals. J Nerv Ment Dis 2002;190:27‐31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Freeman D, Pugh K, Antley A et al. Virtual reality study of paranoid thinking in the general population. Br J Psychiatry 2008;192:258‐63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Garety PA, Freeman D, Jolley S et al. Reasoning, emotions, and delusional conviction in psychosis. J Abnorm Psychol 2005;114:373‐84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78. Moritz S, Woodward TS. Jumping to conclusions in delusional and non‐delusional schizophrenic patients. Br J Clin Psychol 2005;44:193‐207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79. Peters E, Garety P. Cognitive functioning in delusions: a longitudinal analysis. Behav Res Ther 2006;44:481‐514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80. Valmaggia LR, Freeman D, Green C et al. Virtual reality and paranoid ideations in people with an ‘at‐risk mental state' for psychosis. Br J Psychiatry 2007;191(Suppl. 51):s63‐8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81. Van Dael F, Versmissen D, Janssen I et al. Data gathering: biased in psychosis? Schizophr Bull 2006;32:341‐51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82. Freeman D, Pugh K, Garety P. Jumping to conclusions and paranoid ideation in the general population. Schizophr Res 2008;102:254‐60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83. Arseneault L, Cannon M, Fisher HL et al. Childhood trauma and children's emerging psychotic symptoms: a genetically sensitive longitudinal cohort study. Am J Psychiatry 2011;168:65‐72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84. Kelleher I, Keeley H, Corcoran P et al. Childhood trauma and psychosis in a prospective cohort study: cause, effect, and directionality. Am J Psychiatry 2013;170:734‐41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85. Mackie CJ, Castellanos‐Ryan N, Conrod PJ. Developmental trajectories of psychotic‐like experiences across adolescence: impact of victimization and substance use. Psychol Med 2011;41:47‐58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86. Schreier A, Wolke D, Thomas K et al. Prospective study of peer victimization in childhood and psychotic symptoms in a nonclinical population at age 12 years. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2009;66:527‐36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87. van Dam DS, van Nierop M, Viechtbauer W et al. Childhood abuse and neglect in relation to the presence and persistence of psychotic and depressive symptomatology. Psychol Med 2015;45:1363‐77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88. Dominguez MD, Wichers M, Lieb R et al. Evidence that onset of clinical psychosis is an outcome of progressively more persistent subclinical psychotic experiences: an 8‐year cohort study. Schizophr Bull 2011;37:84‐93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89. van Nierop M, Lataster T, Smeets F et al. Psychopathological mechanisms linking childhood traumatic experiences to risk of psychotic symptoms: analysis of a large, representative population‐based sample. Schizophr Bull 2014;40(Suppl. 2):S123‐30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90. Smeets F, Lataster T, Dominguez MD et al. Evidence that onset of psychosis in the population reflects early hallucinatory experiences that through environmental risks and affective dysregulation become complicated by delusions. Schizophr Bull 2012;38:531‐42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91. Smeets F, Lataster T, Viechtbauer W et al. Evidence that environmental and genetic risks for psychotic disorder may operate by impacting on connections between core symptoms of perceptual alteration and delusional ideation. Schizophr Bull 2015;41:687‐97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92. Smeets F, Lataster T, van Winkel R et al. Testing the hypothesis that psychotic illness begins when subthreshold hallucinations combine with delusional ideation. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2013;127:34‐47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93. van Nierop M, Viechtbauer W, Gunther N et al. Childhood trauma is associated with a specific admixture of affective, anxiety, and psychosis symptoms cutting across traditional diagnostic boundaries. Psychol Med 2015;45:1277‐88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94. Kelleher I, Keeley H, Corcoran P et al. Clinicopathological significance of psychotic experiences in non‐psychotic young people: evidence from four population‐based studies. Br J Psychiatry 2012;201:26‐32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95. Guloksuz S, van Nierop M, Lieb R et al. Evidence that the presence of psychosis in non‐psychotic disorder is environment‐dependent and mediated by severity of non‐psychotic psychopathology. Psychol Med 2015;45:2389‐401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96. Borsboom D, Cramer AO. Network analysis: an integrative approach to the structure of psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2013;9:91‐121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97. van Os J. The dynamics of subthreshold psychopathology: implications for diagnosis and treatment. Am J Psychiatry 2013;170:695‐8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]