Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Aug 1.
Published in final edited form as: Autism Res. 2015 Dec 21;9(8):810–828. doi: 10.1002/aur.1590

The integrity of lexical acquisition mechanisms in autism spectrum disorders: A research review

Sudha Arunachalam 1, Rhiannon J Luyster 2
PMCID: PMC4916034  NIHMSID: NIHMS752749  PMID: 26688218

Scientific Abstract

Research on autism spectrum disorders (ASD) has rapidly expanded in recent years, yielding important developments in both theory and practice. While we have gained important insights into how children with ASD differ from typically developing (TD) children in terms of phenotypic features, less has been learned about if and how development in ASD differs from typical development in terms of underlying mechanisms of change. This paper aims to provide a review of processes subserving lexical development in ASD, with the goal of identifying contributing factors to the heterogeneity of language outcomes in ASD. The focus is on available evidence of the integrity or disruption of these mechanisms in ASD, as well as their significance for vocabulary development; topics include early speech perception and preference, speech segmentation, word learning and category formation. Significant gaps in the literature are identified and future directions are suggested.


In recent years, the study of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) has, in a word, exploded. The Interagency Autism Coordinating Committee reported in 2012 that the past three decades had seen a 12-fold increase in the number of annual ASD-related publications (https://iacc.hhs.gov/publications-analysis/july2012). Though this rapid expansion certainly marks important advances, there remain gaps. Many researchers of human development have highlighted the importance of combining the study of typical and atypical development; in the words of Sroufe (1997): “the same laws that govern normal development govern the pathological as well” (p. 266). That is, rather than conceptualizing “disorder” as manifesting qualitatively different patterns of development than what we observe in “typical” development, we should instead perceive it as a variant produced by different combinations of endogenous or exogenous factors (i.e., biology and/or environment). While ASD research conventionally includes comparison samples of children matched on chronological or mental age to identify how children with ASD differ from TD children, however, these studies often focus on profiling differences in children’s language knowledge rather than understanding why such differences arise—that is, whether the cognitive and learning mechanisms by which children develop language themselves differ. This is a marked shortcoming in our efforts to understand ASD; our aim should be to identify continuities and discontinuities in underlying mechanisms, not simply in observable features (Rutter & Sroufe, 2000).

In this review we focus on mechanisms subserving language development, and specifically, lexical development (which we interchangeably term lexical, semantic, or vocabulary development). The five domains of language (phonology, morphology, syntax, semantics and pragmatics) have been studied to varying degrees in ASD. Pragmatic deficits have been well studied, but these are not the only impaired aspects of language and communication in ASD. Individuals with ASD often have language impairments ranging in severity, encompassing some or all of phonological, morphosyntactic, and lexical/semantic development (see Boucher, 2012 for a survey). We focus our attention on the latter.

Though lexical knowledge (per se) in ASD is not the focus of this review—we focus primarily on the processes that allow children to acquire this knowledge—it is a suitable launching point. Most individuals with ASD acquire their first words later than their typically-developing (TD) peers (Le Couteur, Bailey, Rutter, & Gottesman, 1989; Lord, Rutter & Le Couteur, 1994; Charman, Drew, Baird & Baird, 2003; Luyster, Lopez & Lord, 2007), with language delay being one of the most commonly reported initial parent concerns (Herlihy, Knoch, Vibert & Fein, 2013; Chawarska et al., 2007). However, language outcome in early adulthood is extremely heterogeneous across the autism spectrum: current estimates suggest that nearly 70% percent of individuals will attain verbal fluency (e.g., use of sentences that are spontaneous, generative and communicative), and roughly 10% will have receptive and expressive language approximating age expectations (Pickles, Anderson & Lord, 2014). Studies have repeatedly found this variability in language outcome to be closely associated with nonverbal cognitive ability (Pickles, Anderson & Lord, 2014; Anderson et al., 2007; Thurm, Lord, Lee & Newschaffer, 2007). As with all areas of development in ASD, individual differences are significant (e.g., Kjelgaard & Tager-Flusberg, 2001), but for individuals who do acquire phrase speech, vocabulary may be a relative strength (Mayes & Calhoun, 2003; Kjelgaard & Tager-Flusberg, 2001). Nevertheless, lexical mastery—even if considerable—may not be “normal”: individuals with ASD often do not show a pronounced advantage in receptive relative to expressive language skills as TD children do (Ellis Weismer, Lord & Esler, 2010; Charman et al., 2003; Luyster, Lopez & Lord, 2007; but see Kwok et al., 2015), though the extent to which these apparent deficits in comprehension are entangled with impairments in social response remains unclear.

This review aims to go beyond characterizing lexical knowledge to explore possible explanations for its diversity in ASD. Research on typical development has identified several mechanisms supporting lexical acquisition at multiple levels; for instance, from perceptual sensitivity to speech sounds, to parsing out word forms from the speech stream, determining their grammatical category and morphosyntactic properties, and assigning a semantic representation. These capabilities have been explored (to varying degrees) in individuals with ASD. Below, we summarize what is known about the integrity of lexical acquisition mechanisms in ASD, with the goal of “bridging the gap” between research on typical and atypical populations. The first section explores children’s attention to and processing of lexical input through a review of perceptual narrowing, preference for speech, and statistical learning. The second section reviews abilities supporting mapping to meaning, including the use of social cues, availability of word learning constraints, and formation of lexical categories.

The heterogeneity associated with ASD introduces a number of questions that are beyond the scope of the present paper. For instance, what role does IQ play in the integrity of lexical mechanisms in ASD? Are these processes diminished in individuals with higher levels of symptom severity? How do underlying systems such as executive function or motor control, influence lexical development? Although these inquiries will not be addressed here, we argue that if mechanistic disruptions are defining—that is, universal—features of ASD, group-level differences should nevertheless emerge. As a first step towards answering these questions, however, we note relevant characteristics of the studied populations in the research we review. It is our hope that our synthesis of the literature will help clarify the defining features of ASD, and provide leads for future research efforts as well as evidence-based interventions.

Access to a Lexicon: Using language input

Tuning to native sounds

One of the earliest milestones in the long road of language development is the “tuning” of auditory perception to native speech sounds. Foundational research has indicated that, while infants younger than 6–8 months are equally sensitive to phonemic distinctions across native and non-native languages, older infants (~10 months) are much less sensitive to non-native sounds, showing reduced ability to discriminate similar phonemes (e.g., Werker & Tees, 1999). This developmental shift, whereby the infant retains heightened discriminatory skill with native speech sounds relative to non-native ones, is called “perceptual narrowing”, and is thought to be one of the first signs that the child is specializing, both neurologically and functionally, in his or her native language. Indeed, individual differences in the timing of perceptual narrowing are associated with later lexical performance, such that children who remain sensitive to non-native speech contrasts past 8 months show slowed vocabulary development at 2 years of age (Kuhl, Conboy, Padden, Nelson, & Pruitt, 2005).

Although perceptual narrowing in children later diagnosed with ASD has not yet been measured in developmentally-appropriate longitudinal designs, several studies have explored related questions with the hope of extrapolating whether this process might be related to language outcomes in ASD. One vein of research has observed that auditory discrimination is an area of strength in ASD (Lepistö et al., 2005). A commonly cited example is that a disproportionate number of individuals with ASD have absolute pitch (Miller, 1999; Rimland & Fein, 1988; DePape, Hall, Tillman & Trainor, 2012) or heightened pitch discrimination ability (Bonnel et al., 2010; Stanutz, Wapnick & Burack, 2014; O’Riordan & Passetti, 2006), leading some experts to suggest that “fundamental differences in the perception of sounds may be part of the autistic profile” (Jones et al., 2009, p. 2851). Although this evidence is not about speech sounds specifically, pitch discrimination may play a pivotal role in early language acquisition, facilitating the identification of word boundaries and processing of syntactic structures (e.g., Eigsti & Fein, 2013; Goodsit, Morgan & Kuhl, 1993; Jusczyk et al., 1992).

Several studies have suggested a link between enhanced pitch discrimination and lexical delays in individuals with ASD: infants with heightened sensitivity to auditory contrasts may be slower to focus on the particular contrasts most relevant for their native language (see Eigsti et al., 2013 for discussion), thus delaying the process of word segmentation. Jones et al. (2009) reported that exceptional skill in discriminating frequency differences in auditory stimuli was significantly more common in adolescents with ASD and a history of delayed first words than in children with no history of language delays; similar findings were reported by Bonnel and colleagues (2010). In a study of children with ASD—some with “optimal outcomes” who had shed their diagnosis, and others still diagnosed on the autism spectrum—Eigsti et al. (2013) reported that individuals who had an early diagnosis of ASD and a history of delayed language (regardless of current symptom presentation) were better in one of three experimental measures of pitch discrimination than those who did not have a history of language delay. Interestingly, when averaging across all three tasks, the ASD group showed an advantage in pitch discrimination relative to the typical controls, whereas the optimal outcome group did not, pointing to a link between pitch discrimination and current symptomatology. Overall, the results addressing pitch discrimination are consistent with the aforementioned results in TD children (Kuhl et al., 2005) and together suggest that lexical delays may be linked with heightened sensitivity to sounds that continues beyond a “developmentally appropriate” time window.

Relatedly, Constantino and colleagues (2007) measured the ability to perceive non-native phonemic contrasts in school-aged, verbal children with ASD, with the hypothesis that children with ASD might have retained this ability even through late childhood. The hypothesis was not supported; the ASD and TD groups performed similarly. However, it is worth noting that the authors consider their sample to be “high-functioning”, although they do not report on history of language delays or current level of language ability.

The perceptual abilities shaped through perceptual narrowing form the foundation for phoneme production. Consequently, another inroad to the question of whether children with ASD tune to their native language is to explore production—rather than perception—of speech sounds. Schoen, Paul and Chawarska (2011) measured the vocal productions of toddlers with ASD, as well as age- and language-matched controls. Groups were similar in their productions of some nonspeech sounds, like laughter and crying/fussing, but toddlers with ASD produced significantly more atypical vocalizations than their peers, primarily in the form of high-pitched squeals (see also Schoen, Paul & Chawarska, 2009; but cf. Brisson et al., 2014). The authors concluded that one feature of speech and language development in children with ASD may be a “failure to shape their production toward the sound parameters of the ambient language” (Schoen, Paul & Chawarska, 2011, 2009, p. 186). Although this is a promising path of investigation, it remains to be seen whether these perturbations in vocal production are sequelae of perceptual deficits.

Early speech preferences

From the earliest months of life, typically-developing infants prefer to listen to speech over non-speech sounds (e.g., Vouloumanos & Werker, 2004; 2007; Shultz & Vouloumanos, 2010). This preference predicts later vocabulary development in healthy, TD populations (Vouloumanos & Curtin, 2014). In contrast, early speech preferences (relative to non-speech) are disrupted in infants at risk for ASD (Curtin & Vouloumanos, 2013), and other studies have reported a lack of preference for speech over non-speech in children with ASD in both behavioral (Kuhl et al., 2005; Watson et al., 2012) and event-related potential (Whitehouse & Bishop, 2008) paradigms.

The speech that infants hear from their mothers (and other primary caregivers) is often characterized by a particular style of speech called infant-directed speech (IDS), with higher pitch and exaggerated pitch contours compared to adult-directed speech. Infants generally prefer IDS over adult-directed speech (Fernald, 1985), and this preference facilitates language development in TD children (Thiessen, Hill, & Saffran, 2005; Singh, Nestor, Parikh & Yull, 2009; Saint-Georges et al., 2013).

A small body of literature has explored the salience and developmental role of IDS to infants at risk for or diagnosed with ASD, but a methodological note is warranted: in the studies described below, there is considerable variation in the comparison stimuli employed (i.e., adult-directed speech, analogs of IDS, digitized musical tracks). Parents of infants with ASD use IDS at the same rate as comparison groups (Cassel et al., 2013; Cohen et al., 2013; Brisson et al., 2014), although parental utterances may be shorter (Brisson et al., 2014). Nevertheless, infants at high-risk for ASD have a weaker preference for IDS than their low-risk peers, showing either a smaller preference for IDS over adult-directed speech (Droucker, Curtin & Vouloumanos, 2013) or a slight preference, instead, for adult-directed speech (Nadig et al., 2007). This lack of preference could be due, in part, to reduced attention or sensitivity to the features of IDS. For instance, Ference and Curtin (2013) found that high-risk infants did not differentially attend to contrasting lexical stress patterns highlighted in IDS (and also failed to recognize stress as a marker of word meaning, Ference & Curtin, 2015). Toddlers and preschoolers with ASD showed a similarly damped preference for infant-directed speech (as compared to non-speech analogs of IDS speech, Paul et al., 2007; Kuhl, Coffey-Corina, Padden & Dawson, 2005), but Paul and colleagues (2007) suggest that this effect could be due partly to degree of language impairment. Follow-up studies confirmed that, although toddlers with ASD showed a markedly diminished IDS preference relative to chronologically-matched peers, they differed only from language-matched TD peers in the degree of IDS preference (as compared to non-social stimuli) under live speech (rather than videotaped) conditions (Watson et al., 2012). Lack of preference for IDS and its exaggerated pitch contours is perhaps surprising given the enhanced pitch sensitivity reviewed above; it may be that heightened sensitivity to pitch makes IDS overly stimulating and thus dispreferred, or that the social nature of IDS is what makes it specifically dispreferred.

The ramifications of reduced IDS preference for language development are mixed. Two studies with infants at high risk for ASD (Droucker, Curtin & Vouloumanos, 2013; Ference & Curtin, 2013) found no relationship between IDS preference and later vocabulary development. On one hand, these results might suggest that, for infants with a genetic liability for ASD, preference for IDS (or lack thereof) may not play a lasting role in language development. On the other hand, however, other studies report lasting effects of reduced IDS preference. In a study of toddlers with ASD, preference for IDS was correlated with receptive language skills, both concurrently and at a one-year follow-up (Paul et al., 2007). Watson et al. (2010) similarly reported that, for preschoolers with ASD, preference for IDS predicted concurrent and later expressive and receptive abilities, and Nadig and colleagues (2007) reported that, for 6-month-olds at risk for ASD, adult-speech preference was negatively associated with concurrent standardized expressive language scores (Nadig et al., 2007).

In sum, although children with ASD do not—at a group level—show a preference for speech (over non-speech) or IDS (over a range of comparison stimuli), associations with concurrent and later language attest to the lasting effects of heterogeneity in early preferences; in fact, Kuhl and colleagues (2005) measured response to a native contrast (/ba/ and /wa/) in preschoolers with ASD, and found evidence for phonemic discrimination only for those children who preferred to listen to IDS over a non-speech analog signal. Clearly, then, there are meaningful individual differences in speech preferences for infants at risk for and children with ASD.

Identifying the words: Statistical and distributional learning

Statistical learning is the ability to extract statistical regularities from a stimulus. Though now widely considered a domain-general learning mechanism (e.g., Kirkham et al., 2002; Saffran et al., 1999), it has been well studied in language acquisition, particularly with respect to children’s identification of word boundaries and extraction of grammatical rules. In the first demonstration of statistical learning in human infants, Saffran et al. (1996) presented 8-month-olds with streams of connected speech syllables. The distribution of the syllables was such that some strings of three syllables appeared together more often than others, and infants were tested to see if they recognized the cohesion between these syllable groupings. They did, demonstrating that they had segmented the ongoing speech stream into parts (“words”) that frequently co-occurred. Many subsequent studies have replicated and extended this finding, and linked it to lexical development; for instance, Graf Estes et al. (2007) found that 17-month-olds could map the “words” they segmented to novel objects (see also Lany & Saffran, 2010).

There are individual differences in segmentation ability, suggesting that this is an important place to seek predictive value for language outcomes. Newman et al. (2006) found that performance on a segmentation task before age 12 months predicted expressive vocabulary at age 2 years and other language measures at ages 4 to 6 years (MacArthur Bates Communicative Development Inventory (Fenson et al., 1994), Test of Language Development (Newcomer & Hammill, 1997)). Further, statistical learning difficulties may be implicated in Specific Language Impairment (SLI); Evans et al. (2009) showed impaired identification of word boundaries in a statistical learning task in children with SLI as compared to age- and IQ-matched comparison groups.

In individuals with ASD, the evidence about statistical learning is scant, and somewhat conflicting (see also Eigsti & Mayo, 2011, for review). Scott-Van Zeeland et al. (2010), using a similar paradigm to Saffran et al. (1996), found different patterns of neural activation in high-functioning 9- to 16-year-olds with ASD than TD age- and IQ-matched controls. These atypical activation patterns were linked to communication domain scores on the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised, suggesting that disrupted speech segmentation abilities may underlie language and communication deficits. Mayo and Eigsti (2012), however, found similar performance in high-functioning 7- to 17-year-olds with ASD as TD controls on a similar task—although the exposure period was considerably longer than in Scott-Van Zeeland’s (21 minutes, compared to 2 minutes)—and they found no links between task performance and language or cognitive measures.

Statistical learning with visual stimuli (e.g., shapes), instead of auditory or linguistic stimuli, may be intact in ASD (Jeste et al., 2014; Roser et al., 2014). A recent meta-analysis also finds no evidence that sequence learning or other types of implicit learning for non-language stimuli are impaired (Foti et al., 2014; see also Boucher et al., 2008). Thus, although statistical learning itself is considered domain-general, it may be intact for non-speech/language stimuli but not for language. This does not necessarily indicate that the mechanism is domain-specific in ASD; rather, it likely indicates difficulty processing language stimuli in a way that permits typical use of the mechanism, and may be related to the aforementioned reduced attention to speech.

Forming a Lexicon: Mapping Words to Meaning

When children with ASD do successfully segment a new word from the speech stream (even if they may require more input to do so than TD children), how do they assign it meaning? A first step is to recognize that word forms have meaning. This ability has been demonstrated in the first year of life: words, but not non-verbal sounds, promote object categorization and object individuation in infancy (e.g., Ferry, Hespos, & Waxman, 2010; Fulkerson & Waxman, 2007; Xu, 2002). McDuffie, Yoder, and Stone (2006a) reported similar behaviors in 2-year-olds with ASD (and comprehension-matched TD children), who show increased attention to novel objects when they hear a novel label.

But because there are likely to be multiple objects, people, ongoing actions, and spatial relations evident in the child’s environment, assigning the correct meaning to a word is an important challenge, requiring the child to identify which referent is being labeled. Even if only one referent is salient, it may be construed in multiple ways (e.g., a puppy might be labeled dog or animal, and a game of catch as throwing, catching, or playing a game). Research with TD children has delineated several linguistic/cognitive mechanisms they bring to the word-learning task. Here, we outline the evidence about these mechanisms in typical development and ASD in three domains: social cues, word learning constraints, and linguistic cues.

Using social cues

An important set of social cognitive skills is thought to drive language learning, such as inferring what a speaker intends to refer to (e.g., a speaker is likely to label something she appears interested in at the moment), and using the speaker’s direction of gaze, pointing, or affect to determine his or her attentional state (e.g., Baldwin, 1993; Baldwin & Tomasello, 1998; Nappa et al., 2009; Poulin-Dubois & Forbes, 2002; Tomasello & Barton, 1994). To capitalize on such cues, the child must have some prerequisite social-cognitive abilities. A review of this rich literature is beyond the scope of this review; however, it is essential to note the heterogeneity of ability in these areas for children with ASD. As a group, individuals with ASD show deficits in gaze following (Leekam et al., 1997; Leekam, Hunnisett & Moore, 1998; Gillespie et al., 2013), joint attention (Osterling & Dawson, 1994; Mundy, Sigman & Kasari, 1990; Leekam & Ramsden, 2006; Dawson et al., 2004), and understanding others’ internal states (Happé, 1994, 1995; Baron-Cohen, Jolliffe, Mortimore & Robertson, 1997); but these gross differences belie remarkable individual differences in each of these areas. Not surprisingly, these social cognitive skills strongly predict language ability (both concurrent and at later points) for children with ASD (Brooks & Meltzoff, 2015; Yoder, Watson & Lambert, 2015; Luyster, Kadlec, Carter, & Tager-Flusberg, 2008; Adamson, Bakeman, Deckner & Romski, 2009; Anderson et al., 2007), suggesting that they may be “gate-keepers” for children’s access to essential linguistic input (e.g., Bono, Daley & Sigman, 2004; Thurm, Lord, Lee & Newschaffer, 2007). Indeed, Parish-Morris et al. (2007) found that understanding of social intention predicted vocabulary size in children with ASD (but not TD children). Similarly, McDuffie, Yoder, and Stone (2006b) found that attention-following at 2.5 years of age predicted success in a fast-mapping task, as well as vocabulary size 6 months later.

The literature addressing use of social cues in word learning in children with ASD has primarily focused on two questions: (1) can children with ASD to use speaker’s gaze to guide mapping of new words to their intended referents; and (2) what role does attention modulation play in successful mapping? We address each of these issues in turn.

In a seminal study, Baron-Cohen, Baldwin and Crowson (1997) adapted Baldwin’s (1993a,b) word learning paradigm asking whether children would map a novel word to the object that the speaker was currently attending to, or to the object they themselves were holding at the time. Baron-Cohen et al. demonstrated that children with ASD do not typically use the speaker’s direction of gaze to assign word meanings; Preissler & Carey (2005) reported similar results. Note that both samples included children who were quite a bit older than Baldwin’s typically-developing sample and had marked language deficits. In a follow-up study using the same experimental design with younger, higher-functioning children with ASD, Luyster and Lord (2009) found that children with ASD were able to use the speaker’s direction of gaze in a word-learning task. This ability has since been confirmed across a range of paradigms, with a range of age groups and language abilities, though most studied individuals have been relatively high-functioning (Akechi et al., 2011; Akechi et al., 2013; Aldaqre et al., 2014; Franken et al., 2010; Hani et al., 2013; McGregor et al., 2013; Norbury et al., 2010).

Some researchers have suggested that children’s use of social cues is more based on attention than social understanding per se (e.g., Akechi et al., 2011; Hani et al., 2013; McGregor et al., 2013), and that social cues work insofar as they increase the perceptual salience of an object. Akechi et al. (2013), for example, using a similar paradigm to Baron-Cohen et al., but adding eye-tracking to identify children’s locus of visual attention, found that 6- to 12-year-old TD children looked longer at the object the speaker was gazing at than the object they themselves were holding, while children with ASD showed no difference; however, children with ASD reversed this pattern and successfully mapped a novel label to an object in the speaker’s gaze when that object was more perceptually salient than their own object. Even high-functioning adults with ASD may be affected by perceptual salience in a word-learning task requiring attention to speaker gaze (Aldaqre, Paulus, & Sodian, 2014). Also supportive of this is evidence that attention to gaze direction does not necessarily lead to word learning; Gliga et al. (2012) found that 3-year-olds with a family history of ASD followed a speaker’s direction of gaze but those who scored poorly on social and communication domains on the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS; Lord et al., 2000) did not use that information to assign a referent to the novel word. Importantly, in many of the studies documenting successful learning in children with ASD, multiple converging social cues and/or increased repetitions were provided, scaffolding performance in valuable ways (Akechi et al., 2013; Hani et al., 2013; Luyster & Lord, 2009; Norbury et al., 2010). These findings point to the importance of attention and memory in word learning (e.g., Norbury et al., 2010; Bedford et al. 2013).

Using word learning constraints/biases

In typical development, learners are thought to use certain constraints or biases to inform word learning—in other words, cognitive heuristics that aid the formation of links between word forms and meanings. Among these are the whole object assumption (Markman, 1990), the taxonomic assumption (Markman, 1990), the noun bias (Gentner, 1982), the noun-category bias (Waxman, 1991), and mutual exclusivity (Markman, 1990) or the Principle of Contrast (Clark, 1987). It is still a matter of debate whether these constraints or biases are innate or learned, domain-specific or domain-general (e.g., Hollich et al., 2000; Samuelson, 2011). For current purposes, we ask to what extent children with ASD appear to use these same constraints or biases, regardless of their source; we hope that with continued study, findings from TD and disordered populations will inform each other with respect to the specific processes underlying these constraints.

Several of these word learning constraints or biases have been studied in children with ASD. The noun bias, or a preference to map a novel word to an object rather than an action, appears intact in 2- to 3-year-olds with ASD (Swensen et al., 2007). Mutual exclusivity is the assumption that only one word applies to each object, and therefore that unfamiliar words label objects for which children do not yet have a label (Markman, 1990); several studies have found that this, too, is intact (de Marchena et al., 2011; Parish-Morris et al., 2007; Preissler & Carey, 2005; Wilkinson, 2005). De Marchena et al. (2011), studying high-functioning children and adolescents with ASD, specifically addressed the issue of whether the ability to use mutual exclusivity in word learning is more closely related to lexical or pragmatic development, and found that it was the former. The same is true with TD children: Bion, Borovsky, and Fernald (2013) found a positive relationship between accuracy at looking to an unfamiliar object when hearing a novel word and vocabulary size for TD 2-year-olds. We take these studies as evidence that these constraints are not in themselves absent or notably impaired in ASD.

Using linguistic cues

The linguistic context in which a word appears can also provide cues to its meaning. Verb acquisition is most studied in this context, because verb meaning is tightly linked to the sentence structures in which it appears (e.g., Baker, 2001; Grimshaw, 1984; Levin, 1993; Pinker, 1984). The ability to use these syntax-semantics links to acquire the meanings of unfamiliar verbs is known as syntactic bootstrapping (Gleitman, 1990; Landau & Gleitman, 1985), and has been documented beginning at about 1.5 years of age (e.g., Arunachalam et al., 2013; Fisher, 1996, 2002; Fisher et al., 1994; Messenger et al., 2014; Naigles, 1990). In a classic demonstration, Naigles (1990) presented 28-month-olds with novel verbs in either transitive sentences (e.g., “The duck is gorping the bunny”) or intransitive sentences (e.g., “The duck and the bunny are gorping”), accompanied by a visual scene depicting two simultaneous events (e.g., the duck pushed the bunny, and the duck and bunny waved). At test, these two events displayed on different screens, and children were asked to “find gorping!” Children who had heard the transitive sentence preferred the scene in which the duck pushed the bunny, revealing their knowledge that transitive syntax can be mapped to causative events in which one event participant acts on another.

Prerequisite abilities for syntactic bootstrapping include knowledge of some nouns and comprehension of basic linguistic structures (e.g., Gleitman et al., 2005). This latter ability was partly demonstrated by children with ASD by Swensen et al. (2007), who found that 2- to 3-year-old English-acquiring children with ASD understood that word order in English dictates the assignment of event participants to semantic roles. With this prerequisite skill empirically established, two studies went on to document syntactic bootstrapping in children with ASD using Naigles’s (1990) task: Shulman and Guberman (2007) in Hebrew-acquiring 5-year-olds, and Naigles et al. (2011) in English-acquiring 3-year-olds. Naigles et al. (2011) further found that syntactic bootstrapping performance was related to vocabulary and sentence processing abilities as measured 8 months earlier.

Methodologically, an interesting feature of experimental paradigms for studying syntactic bootstrapping is that they are remarkably non-social, only requiring children to attend to linguistic cues, with little social context or cuing (see also Arunachalam (2013) for an even less social version of Naigles’s (1990) task). The minimal social demands involved in these tasks suggest that they may be promising for understanding children’s lexical and syntactic abilities while bypassing confounding social impairments. We suspect that pursuit of this line of research will reveal which learning mechanisms require social attention and social understanding and which proceed without these skills.

Interestingly, one category of verbs that originally motivated the study of syntactic bootstrapping does appear to be poorly learned in ASD: mental state verbs. Mental state verbs are ideal for examining syntactic bootstrapping because they represent abstract concepts that would be difficult to learn by observing the world, such as think, remember, and know; however, they often appear in a syntactic structure in which they take a complement clause (e.g., “I think that…”), which serves as an indicator of the verb’s meaning. Nevertheless, children with ASD have difficulty understanding mental state verbs (e.g., Baron-Cohen et al., 1994; Kazak, Collis, & Lewis, 1997; Kelley et al., 2006; Tager-Flusberg, 1992; Ziatas, Durkin, & Pratt, 1998), suggesting an interaction between semantic and syntactic ability: While syntactic knowledge can support acquisition of a broad meaning category, it cannot override difficulties children may have with particular concepts. Taken together, the literature indicates that syntactic bootstrapping is relatively intact in ASD but (unsurprisingly) relies on the earlier acquisition of prerequisite linguistic skills and is constrained by conceptual understanding.

Generalizing words to the appropriate category

Of course, the brief exposure to a new word that is typically presented in word-learning tasks does not itself result in robust or precise representations of word meaning. In Carey and Bartlett’s (1978) seminal “fast mapping” study, 3-year-olds successfully chose an olive green tray rather than a red tray when asked to “Bring me the chromium tray, not the red one,” indicating that they inferred that the unfamiliar word chromium (a) referred, (b) referred to a color, and (c) referred to a color for which they did not have a name. However, they were only slightly more successful than a control group of children in subsequently picking out the same color in a memory task. To truly learn a word’s meaning, children must engage in a process of extended mapping during which they refine and consolidate their lexical representation. At least two specific abilities have been tested in ASD: extension to appropriate category members, and integration into existing lexical knowledge and semantic networks.

Extension involves generalizing words beyond a specific referent to the entire category to which they apply: The noun “dog” refers not just to the family pet but to all dogs, and the verb “kick” refers not only to a girl kicking a football, but also a boy kicking a soccer ball, a baby kicking a mobile, and so on. It has long been suggested that children with ASD have difficulty generalizing information (though the precise mechanisms underlying this difficulty are poorly understood) (e.g., Happé & Frith, 2006; Plaisted, 2001). However, studies examining generalization of new words in ASD have yielded mixed findings (McGregor & Bean, 2012; Menyuk & Quill, 1985; Tager-Flusberg, 1988; Vogindroukas et al., 2003). In particular, when children with ASD who have low expressive language levels (Hartley & Allen, 2014, 2015) and/or IQs below 70 (Preissler, 2008) are targeted, studies suggest impairments in extension to appropriate category members. One specific type of extension that may be impaired in ASD is the shape bias (Field et al., 2015; Potrzeba et al., 2015; Tek et al., 2008), which informs children’s generalizations of nouns from one category exemplar to others: TD children prefer to generalize nouns by shape, rather than by other attributes such as color or texture, by about 2 years of age (Landau, Smith, & Jones, 1988). Tek et al. (2008) and Potrzeba et al. (2015) found that children with ASD as a group did not use the shape bias to categorize novel nouns even at 4 years, although Field et al. (2015) found that children with a relatively high verbal mental age did use the shape bias (and children with a lower verbal mental age did not), and Potrzeba et al. (2015) reported intriguing associations between vocabulary size and fine motor skill in a subset of children with ASD who did show evidence of the shape bias. Tek et al. (2008) suggest that difficulty using the shape bias in children with ASD might be related to the pervasive difficulties with generalization evidenced in ASD. However, the shape bias is also reported to be deficient in late-talking children (Jones, 2003) and children with SLI (Collison et al., 2014), suggesting that the interaction between language learning and (visual) attention may be implicated (Vales & Smith, 2015), rather than ASD itself.

A few studies have examined the composition of the lexicon more broadly to ask whether children with ASD know the same kinds of words as TD children. Rescorla and Safyer (2013) found that though children with ASD had smaller vocabularies than TD children, there were no significant differences in the kinds of words they knew whether by grammatical category (e.g., noun, verb), or semantic category (e.g., animal names, toy names). Similar results have been reported using the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory (Charman et al., 2003; Luyster, Lopez & Lord, 2007), and in comparison to late-talking toddlers (Weismer et al., 2011), although non-semantic properties like word length do appear to matter (Kover & Ellis Weismer, 2014).

However, other studies have highlighted differences in semantic knowledge. Learners must discover relationships between words, for example, that kick is hierarchically related to verbs like move and punt but taxonomically related to ball and foot. Although children with ASD perform similarly to TD children on tasks probing their arrangement of words into superordinate and basic levels (Tager-Flusberg, 1985, 2001), they nevertheless appear less sensitive to the semantic relationships among words in some tasks (e.g., Battaglia, 2012; Bowler, Gaigg, & Gardiner, 2008; Gastgeb et al., 2006; Hermelin & O’Connor, 1970; Norbury, 2005). Schafer et al. (2013) found that children with ASD have sparser semantic networks, such that they know fewer words that are highly connected in the lexicon than their TD counterparts. Relatedly, Kelley et al. (2006) and Naigles et al. (2013) found that “optimal-outcome” children struggled with tasks tapping into conceptual knowledge and semantic networks, such as categorical induction. These authors suggest that pervasive difficulties with generalization, as well as pragmatic/social abilities to infer semantically relevant relationships may be to blame; we agree that this is an area ripe for future research.

We do not yet know what underlies group differences, but the fact that children with and without ASD seem to know the same kinds of words suggests that they may have access to the same mechanisms to acquire these words. Indeed, we believe that assuming similarity at first is more useful than starting with the assumption that learning mechanisms are fundamentally different. A starting hypothesis, for example, is that children with ASD are capable of forming robust and robustly connected semantic networks that are similar to those evident in TD children, but that they need more time and input to do so, or that the input must be presented in a way that is easier for them to take in and integrate, or that takes into account their characteristically restricted interests. We return to this latter notion of “intake” of the input below, but one intriguing finding in support of this possibility is that children with ASD with poor language skills have difficulty with cross-situational learning (McGregor et al., 2013), or more specifically, the ability to use information from multiple trials when positing a representation for a new word. If this is the case it could be that retention of information about new words is compromised in ASD, resulting in sparser or more poorly integrated lexical semantic knowledge. Indeed, two studies have found poorer retention of word meanings in children and siblings of children with ASD (Bedford et al., 2013; Norbury et al., 2010).

Summary and Directions for Future Research

We have presented several mechanisms underlying lexical acquisition in TD children and reviewed the evidence for the integrity of these processes in ASD. Below, we synthesize our findings in each area and identify the most pressing needs for future research.

Perceptual narrowing is relatively understudied. Although there is suggestive evidence for disruption in terms of timing or completeness, ASD is not characterized by an absence of perceptual narrowing. Indeed, Yi and colleagues (2015) reported intact perceptual narrowing for faces in individuals with ASD, suggesting that, at least as a domain-general process, narrowing to environmental input proceeds typically in ASD. Future research, particularly prospective designs in high-risk infants, should focus on replicating results from traditional paradigms addressing sensitivity to speech sounds in the first year of life. This work could confirm the unfolding of this process in children later diagnosed with ASD and explore whether it is delayed or occurs in a more “piecemeal” fashion; research should also address how individual variability in perceptual narrowing to speech sounds predicts language development.

There is strong evidence that IDS evokes a less robust preference (relative to adult-directed speech, IDS analogs or non-social stimuli, depending on the study) in children with ASD than in TD children; however, because IDS is not required for healthy language development (as evidenced by cross-cultural research, e.g., Ochs & Schieffelin, 1984; Rogoff et al., 1993), their lack of preference should not by itself result in language deficits. Much more concerning is evidence that human speech does not ensnare the attention of children with ASD (as a group) to the extent that it does in TD children; this is likely to have downstream effects for many of the mechanisms we have reviewed. We suspect that the core issue for speech preference, then, is less about IDS and more about the degree to which children later diagnosed with ASD prioritize speech of any kind over other environmental input.

Consequently, in addition to investigating the neurobiological underpinnings and mutability of dampened speech preferences in ASD and the directional relationship between these biases and children’s ability to capitalize on linguistic information from the speech signal, future research should consider ways to heighten infant attention to speech. For instance, is it as simple as markedly increasing the amount of input (e.g., bombardment)? Is it helpful to reduce visual distractions (e.g., movement, color, contrast) or lessen social demands (e.g., overheard rather than child-directed speech)? Or, on the other hand, is it useful to pair speech with another reinforcing sensory experience (e.g., parental touch, actions with objects)? There is much to be gained from exploration of these questions.

With respect to statistical learning, the literature suggests that it is available to children with ASD, but that they may require more input to use it, perhaps especially so for linguistic stimuli compared to non-linguistic stimuli. Reported difficulties with statistical language learning may thus be tied to the difficulties with attention to and preference for speech discussed above, a hypothesis demanding research. Future work should also probe statistical learning abilities in prospective studies starting in infancy, using now well-established paradigms to establish foundational empirical data on similarities and differences in statistical learning associated with the ASD phenotype.

To identify word meanings, children with ASD apparently rely on the same kinds of biases and cues as TD children. Word learning constraints such as mutual exclusivity are largely intact. With respect to social cues, there is now a general consensus that children with ASD can use gaze cues to learn words. However, a number of questions remain about the role of domain-general abilities like memory and learning, as well as the extent to which learners can interpret the deeper meaning of these gaze cues. Indeed, when children with ASD are asked to move beyond gaze cues to use non-visible cues of speaker reference or intention, including perspective-taking and Theory of Mind (e.g., Jing & Fang, 2014), performance suffers. This suggests that there is much to learn from studies requiring children to invoke higher order social cognitive skills or navigate different kinds of word-learning situations (e.g., when non-ostensive cues are used, when the language is being addressed to someone other than the child, etc.).

With respect to linguistic cues, and syntactic bootstrapping in particular, children who have the prerequisite syntactic and lexical skills (which are often delayed in ASD) show successful use of this mechanism. Children with intellectual disability and ASD are expected to struggle, however (Kover et al., 2014). Future work should address the degree to which bootstrapping can be “turned on” earlier by increasing early lexical and syntactic knowledge, as this could have exponential consequences for lexical development. Importantly, in addition to prerequisite lexical and grammatical abilities, syntactic bootstrapping also requires rapid language processing skills; in order to benefit from the information available in a linguistic context, children must be able to quickly process it (e.g., Fernald et al., 2008; Trueswell & Gleitman, 2007). Indeed, language processing speed—or the speed with which children look to a picture in a display after hearing it named—predicts language and cognitive outcomes in both TD children (Marchman & Fernald, 2008) and children with ASD (Venker et al., 2013), and so future research should focus on this issue. However, the research on online language processing in young children with ASD is new; while there may not be systematic differences between TD children and children with or at risk for ASD, there are large individual differences (Brock et al., 2008; Chita-Tegmark et al., 2015; Venker et al., 2013).

An important gap in the literature on mapping words to meaning is that most studies are limited to noun learning, and even further, to highly concrete and imageable concepts that can be depicted easily. Some work has been done on verbs, prepositions, and adjectives, for example, but even within these categories, easily imageable referents have been the primary targets. Varied techniques must be used to improve our understanding of the lexicon, including less imageable referents and function morphemes that serve grammatical functions but convey little content.

Children with ASD do have difficulty extending word meanings appropriately and situating them in semantic networks. Notably, the shape bias appears perturbed. But because shape bias difficulties are also evident in late talkers and SLI, this is unlikely to be related to ASD specifically. Future research should address the extent to which language impairment is characterized by weaknesses in shape bias and other kinds of extension, as well as what kinds of learning situations might promote children’s abilities to generalize word meanings appropriately—such as situations that do not rely on social understanding, or simply present more repetitions/exemplars than TD children require.

Another question demanding additional study is why semantic networks appear to be different in ASD. It is not yet clear whether the differences are qualitative or quantitative, conceptual or linguistic. One possibility is that poor memory for and integration of new word meanings into children’s existing knowledge is partially to blame. (Henderson et al. (2014) argue that word form integration is impaired in ASD; whether word meaning integration is as well remains to be seen.) Memories, including memories for words, undergo consolidation over time, and during this memory consolidation process they become integrated with existing lexical knowledge (e.g., Dumay & Gaskell, 2007). If this is impaired, it will be important to study the role of sleep in learning and memory in ASD. Although sleep is important for memory consolidation (e.g., Stickgold & Walker, 2005), sleep problems are pervasive in ASD (e.g., Goodlin-Jones et al., 2008; Hollway & Aman, 2011; Malow et al., 2006; Richdale, 1999), suggesting that memory consolidation may proceed differently in these children. Although no studies have yet investigated this issue, it is promising that slow-wave sleep, which promotes consolidation of semantic memories like word knowledge (e.g., Gais & Born, 2004), may be relatively intact in children with ASD (Buckley et al., 2010) (but maybe not in adults with ASD, Limoges et al., 2005). Unfortunately, research on the role of sleep in word learning even in typical development is only just beginning (e.g., Werchan & Gómez, 2014; Williams & Horst, 2014).

Much of the research we have reviewed on the underlying structure and use of lexical categories in ASD suggests that it is relatively intact (e.g., Charman et al., 2003; Luyster et al., 2007; Rescorla and Safyer, 2013). One caveat is that lexical entries that are highly embedded in complex social cognitive skills (such as perspective-taking and Theory of Mind) may be impaired, including deictic terms (Hobson, Garcia-Perez, & Lee, 2010) and mental state and emotion words (e.g., Hobson & Lee, 1989; Tager-Flusberg, 2000). However, we suggest that any observed lexical differences in these domains are secondary to a primary difficulty in social cognition (e.g., Boucher, 2012; Pelphrey et al., 2011).

To evaluate this hypothesis rigorously it will be necessary to accrue evidence on relationships between social cognition and language development from a variety of domains. One intriguing new vein of research documenting language development in native-signers (that is, deaf children of Deaf parents exposed to a sign language from birth) with ASD (Shield, 2014; Shield & Meier, 2012; Shield, Meier & Tager-Flusberg, 2015) serves as a useful example. An important finding from this research is that although native signers with ASD do not reverse pronouns in the same way as children with ASD who use spoken language (Shield, 2014), they do use sign language pronouns less often than TD deaf children, even though sign pronouns are semantically transparent in that they point in the direction of their referent (Shield et al., 2015). They further show unique patterns of sign formation, such as reversal of the palm while signing (Shield & Meier, 2012), which suggest a different approach to gesture imitation and sign learning. Although there is no obvious analog to palm reversal in speech, the authors hypothesize that “the same underlying deficit (in the understanding of the relationship between self and other) results in two different modality-dependent linguistic phenomena: pronoun reversals in speech, and reversals of the palm in sign” (Shield et al., 2015, p. 2141). Future work on signing and speaking children will clarify the relative contributions of linguistic and social cognition to lexical development in ASD.

Conclusions

We suggest that while children with ASD have the same fundamental machinery for lexical development as typically-developing children, there are apparent differences in the efficacy of these learning mechanisms due to (1) disruptions in supporting systems; and (perhaps causing) (2) differences in children’s intake of the language input they receive. With regard to the former, even for typically-developing children, attendant cognitive systems like attention, memory, cognitive control, and processing speed develop throughout early childhood (e.g., Courage & Cowen, 2009; Fernald et al., 1998; Mazuka, Jincho, & Oishi, 2009; Trueswell et al., 1999), and therefore the representations that children form from the input are “filtered” through these systems (e.g., Omaki & Lidz, 2015). An array of other systems also moderate the relationship between input and lexical acquisition; for instance, variations in lexical development are linked to individual differences in social attention (Morales et al., 2000; Morales, Mundy, & Rojas, 1998) and motor function (Iverson, 2010; He, Walle & Campos, 2015). Not surprisingly, parallels are observed in children with ASD, for whom cognitive function consistently predicts language development (e.g., Luyster et al., 2007; Thurm et al., 2007; McDuffie, Yoder & Stone, 2005; Venker et al., 2013); however, additional research is necessary to determine definitively whether this is because higher nonverbal IQ allows children access to ‘normative’ language learning, or to compensatory strategies like rote learning or association (Boucher, 2012). Social attention (Charman, 2003; Luyster & Lord, 2009) and motor control (Gernsbacher et al., 2008) are also predictive of language development in children with ASD.

Therefore, children’s intake of the input signal will be affected by these other abilities. We suggest that individual differences in these factors may go a long way toward explaining individual differences in language behavior--and more specifically in lexical development--in ASD; that is, children use the same learning mechanisms and are able to attend to the same aspects of the input, but they differ both in the degree to which they access this input and in how they process and integrate it into their developing semantic knowledge. A stronger focus on individual differences in future research may help to clarify these issues.

Importantly, despite the social impairments associated with ASD, children are sensitive to many of the same reciprocal aspects of parental language as TD children. In general, parents of children with ASD provide a similar quantity and quality of language input as parents of TD children in naturalistic play interactions (Bang & Nadig, 2015) and when presenting new words (Adamson et al., 2014; Hani et al., 2013), and just as with TD children, quantity and quality of input predict language skill (Bang & Nadig, 2015). In fact, as Naigles (2013) points out, “What is most remarkable about the extant research … is how similar the role of input seems to be for children with ASD, demonstrating effects of maternal responsiveness, effects of the child’s role in establishing and maintaining joint attention, and effects of specific components of maternal speech on subsequent language development” (p. 245). The mechanisms by which these features support language development include not only social-pragmatic mechanisms that children with ASD may struggle with, but also high rates of contingency and contiguity (e.g., parents tend to label objects that infants are looking at) as well as multimodal features (e.g., parents often simultaneously label and touch objects) that increase salience (Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2014). Nevertheless, there is evidence that (as with TD children), the benefits of parental responsiveness differ depending on the child’s language level (Haebig et al., 2013a,b), with low-language children benefitting more from general responsiveness and high-language children benefitting more from specific linguistic features of the input (Naigles, 2013). Apparent differences between children with ASD and TD children in this regard could be due to a larger discrepancy between chronological age, language ability, and an array of other cognitive/social/motor skills, thus making it more difficult for parents to identify the best ways to provide high-quality developmentally-appropriate input.

An important issue for future research, then, is how we can increase children’s intake from the input—that is, we must identify under what conditions children with ASD benefit from information available in their environment, and how we can maximize those conditions. An overarching theme from the research reviewed above is that children with ASD may simply require more exposure to language. However, there may also be malleable aspects of how exposure is presented, such as reducing social interaction demands, separating auditory and visual input (minimizing the need for online integration), introducing highly motivating contexts, and modifying instructional settings to capitalize on personal interests or strengths. Situations that integrate these features are likely to be most successful in increasing children’s access to linguistic information, thereby setting the stage for optimal lexical development.

We hope this review represents a step forward in clarifying the ways in which lexical acquisition in ASD is similar to and different from what is seen in typical development. Throughout we have aimed to view differences through Sroufe’s (1997) lens, initially assuming that the mechanisms governing lexical development in ASD are fundamentally the same as those in typical development. We hope that future research will continue to take this approach, and to explore the roles of related non-linguistic systems in contributing to the profiles of children with ASD.

Scientific Summary for Families with ASD.

Research on autism spectrum disorders (ASD) has rapidly expanded in recent years, yielding important developments in both theory and practice. Many children with ASD struggle with vocabulary and vocabulary development. We survey the research on how vocabulary development and knowledge in ASD are similar to and different from typical development, with the goal of identifying contributing factors to the heterogeneity of language outcomes in ASD. Our focus is on available evidence about the cognitive processes that are thought to underlie language learning in typical development, and whether these processes are intact or disrupted in ASD. We also discuss the significance of these processes for vocabulary development. Topics include early speech perception and preference, speech segmentation, word learning and category formation. Significant gaps in the literature are identified and future directions are suggested.

Acknowledgments

Preparation of this manuscript was partially funded by K01DC013306 to the first author. Thanks to four anonymous reviewers, Peter Mundy, and David Amaral for helpful comments, and to Julia Bird for assistance with formatting.

References

  1. Adamson LB, Bakeman R, Deckner DF, Romski M. Joint engagement and the emergence of language in children with autism and Down syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2009;39(1):84–96. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0601-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Adamson LB, Bakeman R, Deckner DF, Nelson PB. From interactions to conversations: The development of joint engagement during early childhood. Child Development. 2014;85(3):941–955. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Akechi H, Senju A, Kikuchi Y, Tojo Y, Osanai H, Hasegawa T. Do children with ASD use referential gaze to learn the name of an object? An eye-tracking study. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2011;5(3):1230–1242. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2011.01.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Akechi H, Kikuchi Y, Tojo Y, Osanai H, Hasegawa T. Brief report: Pointing cues facilitate word learning in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2013;43(1):230–235. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1555-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Aldaqre I, Paulus M, Sodian B. Referential gaze and word learning in adults with autism. Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice. 2014 doi: 10.1177/1362361314556784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Anderson DK, Lord C, Risi S, Shulman C, Welch K, DiLavore PS, … Pickles A. Patterns of growth in verbal abilities among children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2007;75(4):594–604. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.75.4.594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Arunachalam S. Two-year-olds can begin to acquire verb meanings in socially impoverished contexts. Cognition. 2013;129(3):569–573. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.08.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Arunachalam S, Escovar E, Hansen MA, Waxman SR. Out of sight, but not out of mind: 21-month-olds use syntactic information to learn verbs even in the absence of a corresponding event. Language and Cognitive Processes. 2013;28(4):417–425. doi: 10.1080/01690965.2011.641744. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Baker Mark C. Atoms of language. New York: Basic Books; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  10. Baldwin D. Infants’ ability to consult the speaker for clues to word reference. Journal of Child Language. 1993a;20:395–418. doi: 10.1017/s0305000900008345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Baldwin D. Early referential understanding: Infants’ ability to recognize referential acts for what they are. Developmental Psychology. 1993b;29:832–843. [Google Scholar]
  12. Baldwin DA, Tomasello M. Word learning: a window on early pragmatic understanding. In: Clark EV, editor. Proceedings of the twenty-ninth annual Child Language Research Forum. Stanford, CA: CSLI; 1998. pp. 3–23. [Google Scholar]
  13. Bang J, Nadig A. Learning language in autism: Maternal linguistic input contributes to later vocabulary. Autism Research. 2015;8(2):214–223. doi: 10.1002/aur.1440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Baron-Cohen S, Ring H, Moriarty J, Schmitz B, Costa D, Ell P. Recognition of mental state terms. Clinical findings in children with autism and a functional neuroimaging study of normal adults. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 1994;165(5):640–649. doi: 10.1192/bjp.165.5.640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Baron-Cohen S, Baldwin DA, Crowson M. Do children with autism use the speaker’s direction of gaze strategy to crack the code of language? Child Development. 1997;68(1):48. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.ep9707256797. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Battaglia D. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. City University of New York; New York: 2012. Word association and semantic priming in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. [Google Scholar]
  17. Bedford R, Gliga T, Frame K, Hudry K, Chandler S, Johnson MH, Charman T. Failure to learn from feedback underlies word learning difficulties in toddlers at risk for autism. Journal of Child Language. 2013;40(01):29–46. doi: 10.1017/S0305000912000086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Bion RA, Borovsky A, Fernald A. Fast mapping, slow learning: Disambiguation of novel word–object mappings in relation to vocabulary learning at 18, 24, and 30 months. Cognition. 2013;126(1):39–53. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.08.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Bono MA, Daley T, Sigman M. Relations among joint attention, amount of intervention and language gain in autism. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders. 2004;34(5):495–505. doi: 10.1007/s10803-004-2545-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Boucher J. Research Review: Structural language in autistic spectrum disorder - characteristics and causes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2012;53(3):219–233. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02508.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Boucher J, Mayes A, Bigham S. Memory, language, and intellectual ability in low-functioning autism. In: Boucher J, Bowler DM, editors. Memory in autism: theories and evidence. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press; 2008. pp. 268–290. [Google Scholar]
  22. Bonnel A, McAdams S, Smith B, Berthiaume C, Bertone A, Ciocca V, …Mottron L. Enhanced pure-tone pitch discrimination among persons with autism but not asperger syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48(9):2465–2475. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.04.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Bowler DM, Gaigg SB, Gardiner JM. Effects of related and unrelated context on recall and recognition by adults with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46(4):993–999. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Buckley W, Rodriguez AJ, Jennison K, Buckley J, Thurm A, Sato S, Swedo S. Rapid eye movement sleep percentage in children with autism compared with children with developmental delay and typical development. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine. 2010;164:1032–1037. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2010.202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Brisson J, Martel K, Serres J, Sirois S, Adrien JL. Acoustic analysis of oral productions of infants later diagnosed with autism and their mother. Infant Mental Health Journal. 2014;35(3):285–295. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Brock J, Norbury C, Einav S, Nation K. Do individuals with autism process words in context? Evidence from language-mediated eye-movements. Cognition. 2008;108(3):896–904. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Brooks R, Meltzoff AN. Connecting the dots from infancy to childhood: A longitudinal study connecting gaze following, language, and explicit theory of mind. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2015;130:67–78. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.09.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Carey S, Bartlett E. Acquiring a single new word. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development. 1978;15:17–29. [Google Scholar]
  29. Cassel RS, Saint-Georges C, Mahdhaoui A, Chetouani M, Laznik MC, Muratori F, … Cohen D. Course of maternal prosodic incitation (motherese) during early development in autism. Interaction Studies. 2013;14(3):480–495. doi: 10.1075/is.14.3.08cas. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. Charman T, Drew A, Baird C, Baird G. Measuring early language development in preschool children with autism spectrum disorder using the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (Infant Form) Journal of Child Language. 2003;30(01):213–236. doi: 10.1017/s0305000902005482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Chawarska K, Volkmar F. Impairments in monkey and human face recognition in 2-year-old toddlers with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Developmental Delay. Developmental Science. 2007;10(2):266–279. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00543.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Chita-Tegmark M, Arunachalam S, Nelson CA, Tager-Flusberg H. Eye-tracking measurements of language processing: Developmental differences in children at high risk for ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2015:1–12. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2495-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Clark E. The principle of contrast: A constraint on language acquisition. In: MacWhinney B, editor. Mechanisms of language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc; 1987. pp. 1–33. [Google Scholar]
  34. Cohen D, Cassel RS, Saint-Georges C, Mahdhaoui A, Laznik MC, Apicella F, … Chetouani M. Do parentese prosody and fathers’ involvement in interacting facilitate social interaction in infants who later develop autism? Plos One. 2013;8(5):e61402. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0061402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Collisson BA, Grela B, Spaulding T, Rueckl JG, Magnuson JS. Individual differences in the shape bias in preschool children with specific language impairment and typical language development: theoretical and clinical implications. Developmental science. 2015;18(3):373–388. doi: 10.1111/desc.12219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Constantino JN, Yang D, Gray TL, Gross MM, Abbacchi AM, Smith SC, …Kuhl PK. Clarifying the associations between language and social development in autism: A study of non-native phoneme recognition. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2007;37(7):1256–1263. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0269-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Courage ML, Cowan N. The development of memory in infancy and childhood. New York, NY: Psychology Press; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  38. Curtin S, Vouloumanos A. Speech preference is associated with autistic-like behavior in 18-months-olds at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2013;43(9):2114–2120. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1759-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Dawson G, Toth K, Abbott R, Osterling J, Munson J, Estes A, Liaw J. Early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology. 2004;40:271–283. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. DeCasper AJ, Fifer WP. Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers’ voices. Science. 1980;208(4448):1174–1176. doi: 10.1126/science.7375928. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. De Marchena A, Eigsti IM, Worek A, Ono KE, Snedeker J. Mutual exclusivity in autism spectrum disorders: Testing the pragmatic hypothesis. Cognition. 2011;119(1):96–113. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. DePape AMR, Hall GB, Tillmann B, Trainor LJ. Auditory Processing in High-Functioning Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:44084. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0044084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Droucker D, Curtin S, Vouloumanos A. Linking infant-directed speech and face preferences to language outcomes in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2013;56(2):567–576. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0266). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Dumay N, Gaskell MG. Sleep-associated changes in the mental representation of spoken words. Psychological Science. 2007;18:35–39. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01845.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Eigsti IM, Fein DA. More is less: Pitch discrimination and language delays in children with optimal outcomes from autism. Autism Research. 2013;6(6):605–613. doi: 10.1002/aur.1324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Eigsti IM, Mayo J. Implicit learning in ASD. In: Fein D, editor. The neuropsychology of autism. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 2011. pp. 267–279. [Google Scholar]
  47. Estes KG, Evans JL, Alibali MW, Saffran JR. Can infants map meaning to newly segmented words? Statistical segmentation and word learning. Psychological Science. 2007;18(3):254–260. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01885.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Evans JL, Saffran JR, Robe-Torres K. Statistical learning in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2009;52(2):321–335. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/07-0189). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Fenson L, Dale P, Reznick J, Bates E, Thal D, Pethick S. Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1994;59(5) Serial No. 242. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Fernald A. Four-month-old infants prefer to listen to motherese. Infant Behavior and Development. 1985;8(2):181–195. [Google Scholar]
  51. Fernald A, Pinto JP, Swingley D, Weinbergy A, McRoberts GW. Rapid gains in speed of verbal processing by infants in the 2nd year. Psychological Science. 1998;9(3):228–231. [Google Scholar]
  52. Fernald A, Marchman VA, Hurtado N. Input affects uptake: How early language experience influences processing efficiency and vocabulary learning. Paper presented at the 7th IEEE International Conference on Development and Learning.2008. Aug, [Google Scholar]
  53. Ferry AL, Hespos SJ, Waxman SR. Categorization in 3-and 4-month-old infants: an advantage of words over tones. Child Development. 2010;81(2):472–479. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01408.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Ference J, Curtin S. Attention to lexical stress and early vocabulary growth in 5-month-olds at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2013;116(4):891–903. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Ference J, Curtin S. The Ability to Map Differentially Stressed Labels to Objects Predicts Language Development at 24 months in 12-month-olds at High Risk for Autism. Infancy. 2015;20(3):242–262. [Google Scholar]
  56. Field C, Allen ML, Lewis C. Attentional learning helps language acquisition take shape for atypically developing children, not just children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2015:1–12. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2401-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Fisher C, Hall DG, Rakowitz S, Gleitman L. When it is better to receive than to give: Syntactic and conceptual constraints on vocabulary growth. Lingua. 1994;92:333–375. [Google Scholar]
  58. Fisher C. Structural limits on verb mapping: The role of analogy in children’s interpretations of sentences. Cognitive Psychology. 1996;31(1):41–81. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1996.0012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Fisher C. Structural limits on verb mapping: the role of abstract structure in 2.5-year-olds’ interpretations of novel verbs. Developmental Science. 2002;5(1):55–64. [Google Scholar]
  60. Foti F, De Crescenzo F, Vivanti G, Menghini D, Vicari S. Implicit learning in individuals with autism spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine. 2015;45(05):897–910. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714001950. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Franken TE, Lewis C, Malone SA. Brief report: Are children with autism proficient word learners? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2010;40(2):255–259. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0847-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Fulkerson AL, Waxman SR. Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization: Evidence from 6-and 12-month-olds. Cognition. 2007;105(1):218–228. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.09.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Gais S, Born J. Declarative memory consolidation: mechanisms acting during human sleep. Learning & Memory. 2004;11(6):679–685. doi: 10.1101/lm.80504. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Gastgeb HZ, Strauss MS, Minshew NJ. Do individuals with autism process categories differently? The effect of typicality and development. Child Development. 2006;77(6):1717–1729. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00969.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Gentner D. Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In: Kuczaj SA, editor. Language Development: Vol. 2. Language, Thought and Culture. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1982. pp. 301–334. [Google Scholar]
  66. Gernsbacher MA, Sauer EA, Geye HM, Schweigert EK, Hill Goldsmith H. Infant and toddler oral- and manual-motor skills predict later speech fluency in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2008;49(1):43–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01820.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Gillespie-Lynch K, Elias R, Escudero P, Hutman T, Johnson SP. Atypical gaze following in autism: A comparison of three potential mechanisms. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2013;43(12):2779–2792. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1818-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Gleitman L. The structural sources of verb meanings. Language Acquisition. 1990;1(1):3–55. [Google Scholar]
  69. Gliga T, Elsabbagh M, Hudry K, Charman T, Johnson MH. Gaze following, gaze reading, and word learning in children at risk for autism. Child Development. 2012;83(3):926–938. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01750.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Goodlin-Jones BL, Tang K, Liu J, Anders TF. Sleep patterns in preschool-age children with autism, developmental delay, and typical development. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2008;47(8):930–938. doi: 10.1097/CHI.ObO13e3181799f7c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Goodsitt JV, Morgan JL, Kuhl PK. Perceptual strategies in prelingual speech segmentation. Journal of Child Language. 1993;20(2):229–52. doi: 10.1017/s0305000900008266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Grimshaw Jane. Form, function, and the language acquisition device. In: Baker CL, McCarthy JJ, editors. The logical problem of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1984. pp. 165–182. [Google Scholar]
  73. Haebig E, McDuffie A, Weismer SE. The contribution of two categories of parent verbal responsiveness to later language for toddlers and preschoolers on the autism spectrum. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 2013a;22(1):57–70. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2012/11-0004). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Haebig E, McDuffie A, Weismer SE. Brief report: Parent verbal responsiveness and language development in toddlers on the autism spectrum. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2013b;43(9):2218–2227. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1763-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Hani HB, Gonzalez-Barrero ANA, Nadig AS. Children’s referential understanding of novel words and parent labeling behaviors: Similarities across children with and without autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Language. 2013;40(5):971–1002. doi: 10.1017/S0305000912000426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Happé FG. An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1994;24(2):129–154. doi: 10.1007/BF02172093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Happé F. Autism: An introduction to psychological theory. Harvard, MA: Harvard University Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  78. Happé F, Frith U. The weak coherence account: detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2006;36(1):5–25. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0039-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Hartley C, Allen ML. Brief Report: Generalisation of Word–Picture Relations in Children with Autism and Typically Developing Children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2014;44(8):2064–2071. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2074-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Hartley C, Allen ML. Symbolic understanding of pictures in low-functioning children with autism: the effects of iconicity and naming. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2015;45(1):15–30. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-2007-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. He M, Walle EA, Campos JJ. A Cross-National Investigation of the Relationship Between Infant Walking and Language Development. Infancy. 2015;20(3):283–305. [Google Scholar]
  82. Henderson L, Powell A, Gareth Gaskell M, Norbury C. Learning and consolidation of new spoken words in autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Science. 2014;17(6):858–871. doi: 10.1111/desc.12169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Herlihy L, Knoch K, Vibert B, Fein D. Parents’ first concerns about toddlers with autism spectrum disorder: Effect of sibling status. Autism. 2013 doi: 10.1177/1362361313509731. 1362361313509731. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Hermelin B, O’Connor N. Psychological experiments with autistic children. Oxford, England: Pergamon Press; 1970. [Google Scholar]
  85. Hobson RP, García-Pérez RM, Lee A. Person-centred (deictic) expressions and autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2010;40(4):403–415. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0882-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Hobson RP, Lee A. Emotion-related and abstract concepts in autistic people: Evidence from the British Picture Vocabulary Scale. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1989;19(4):601–623. doi: 10.1007/BF02212860. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Hollich GJ, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM, Brand RJ, Brown E, Chung HL, Bloom L. Breaking the language barrier: An emergentist coalition model for the origins of word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2000:i-135. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Hollway JA, Aman MG. Sleep correlates of pervasive developmental disorders: A review of the literature. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2011;32(5):1399–1421. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Iverson JM. Developing language in a developing body: The relationship between motor development and language development. Journal of Child Language. 2010;37(02):229–261. doi: 10.1017/S0305000909990432. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Jeste SS, Kirkham N, Senturk D, Hasenstab K, Sugar C, Kupelian C, Johnson SP. Electrophysiological evidence of heterogeneity in visual statistical learning in young children with ASD. Developmental Science. 2015;18(1):90–105. doi: 10.1111/desc.12188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Jing W, Fang J. Brief Report: Do children with Autism gather information from social contexts to aid their word learning? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2014;44(6):1478–1482. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1994-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Jones CR, Happé F, Baird G, Simonoff E, Marsden AJ, Tregay J, …Charman T. Auditory discrimination and auditory sensory behaviours in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(13):2850–2858. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Jones SS. Late talkers show no shape bias in a novel name extension task. Developmental Science. 2003;6(5):477–483. [Google Scholar]
  94. Jusczyk PW, Hirsh-Pasek K, Nelson DG, Kennedy LJ, Woodward A, Piwoz J. Perception of acoustic correlates of major phrasal units by young infants. Cognitive Psychology. 1992;24(2):252–293. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(92)90009-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Kazak S, Collis GM, Lewis V. Can young people with autism refer to knowledge states? Evidence from their understanding of “know” and “guess”. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 1997;38:1001–1009. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01617.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Kelley E, Paul JJ, Fein D, Naigles LR. Residual language deficits in optimal outcome children with a history of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2006;36(6):807–828. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0111-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Kirkham NZ, Slemmer JA, Johnson SP. Visual statistical learning in infancy: Evidence for a domain general learning mechanism. Cognition. 2002;83(2):B35–B42. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(02)00004-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Kjelgaard MM, Tager-Flusberg H. An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups. Language and Cognitive Processes. 2001;16(2–3):287–308. doi: 10.1080/01690960042000058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Kover ST, Weismer SE. Lexical characteristics of expressive vocabulary in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2014;57(4):1428–1441. doi: 10.1044/2014_JSLHR-L-13-0006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Kover ST, Haebig E, Oakes A, McDuffie A, Hagerman RJ, Abbeduto L. Sentence comprehension in boys with autism spectrum disorder. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 2014;23(3):385–394. doi: 10.1044/2014_AJSLP-13-0073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Kuhl PK, Coffey-Corina S, Padden D, Dawson G. Links between social and linguistic processing of speech in preschool children with autism: behavioral and electrophysiological measures. Developmental Science. 2005;8(1):F1–F12. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00384.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Kuhl PK, Conboy BT, Padden D, Nelson T, Pruitt J. Early speech perception and later language development: implications for the “Critical Period”. Language Learning and Development. 2005;1(3–4):237–264. [Google Scholar]
  103. Kwok EY, Brown HM, Smyth RE, Cardy JO. Meta-analysis of receptive and expressive language skills in autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2015;9:202–222. [Google Scholar]
  104. Landau B, Gleitman LR. Language and experience: Evidence from the blind child. Vol. 8. Harvard University Press; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  105. Landau B, Smith LB, Jones SS. The importance of shape in early lexical learning. Cognitive Development. 1988;3(3):299–321. [Google Scholar]
  106. Lany J, Saffran JR. From statistics to meaning infants’ acquisition of lexical categories. Psychological Science. 2010;21(2):284–291. doi: 10.1177/0956797609358570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. LeCouteur A, Bailey A, Rutter M, Gottesman I. Epidemiologically based twin study of autism. Paper presented at the First World Congress on Psychiatric Genetics; Cambridge, England. 1989. Aug, [Google Scholar]
  108. Leekam SR, Hunnisett E, Moore C. Targets and Cues: Gaze-following in Children with Autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1998;39(7):951–962. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Leekam S, Baron-Cohen S, Perrett D, Milders M, Brown S. Eye-direction detection: a dissociation between geometric and joint attention skills in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 1997;15(1):77–95. [Google Scholar]
  110. Leekam SR, Ramsden CA. Dyadic orienting and joint attention in preschool children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2006;36(2):185–197. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0054-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Lepistö T, Kujala T, Vanhala R, Alku P, Huotilainen M, Näätänen R. The discrimination of and orienting to speech and non-speech sounds in children with autism. Brain Research. 2005;1066(1):147–157. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.10.052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Levin Beth. English verb classes and alternations. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  113. Limoges E, Mottron L, Bolduc C, Berthiaume C, Godbout R. Atypical sleep architecture and the autism phenotype. Brain. 2005;128:1049–61. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A. Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1994;24(5):659–685. doi: 10.1007/BF02172145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, … Rutter M. The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2000;30(3):205–223. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Luyster RJ, Kadlec MB, Carter A, Tager-Flusberg H. Language assessment and development in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2008;38(8):1426–1438. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0510-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Luyster R, Lopez K, Lord C. Characterizing communicative development in children referred for Autism Spectrum Disorders using the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory (CDI) Journal of Child Language. 2007;34(3):623–654. doi: 10.1017/S0305000907008094. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Luyster R, Lord C. Word Learning in Children With Autism Spectrum Disorders. Developmental Psychology. 2009;45(6):1774–1786. doi: 10.1037/a0016223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Malow BA, Marzec ML, McGrew SG, Wang L, Henderson LM, Stone WL. Characterizing sleep in children with autism spectrum disorders: a multidimensional approach. SLEEP. 2006;29(12):1563. doi: 10.1093/sleep/29.12.1563. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Marchman VA, Fernald A. Speed of word recognition and vocabulary knowledge in infancy predict cognitive and language outcomes in later childhood. Developmental Science. 2008;11(3):F9–F16. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00671.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Markman EM. Constraints children place on word meanings. Cognitive Science. 1990;14(1):57–77. [Google Scholar]
  122. Mayo J, Eigsti IM. Brief report: A comparison of statistical learning in school-aged children with high functioning autism and typically developing peers. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2012;42(11):2476–2485. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1493-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Mayes SD, Calhoun SL. Analysis of WISC-III, Stanford-Binet: IV, and academic achievement test scores in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2003;33(3):329–341. doi: 10.1023/a:1024462719081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Mazuka R, Jincho N, Oishi H. Development of executive control and language processing. Language and Linguistics Compass. 2009;3(1):59–89. [Google Scholar]
  125. McDuffie AS, Yoder PJ, Stone WL. Labels increase attention to novel objects in children with autism and comprehension-matched children with typical development. Autism. 2006a;10(3):288–301. doi: 10.1177/1362361306063287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. McDuffie A, Yoder P, Stone W. Fast-mapping in young children with autism spectrum disorders. First Language. 2006b;26(4):421–438. doi: 10.1177/0142723706067438. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  127. McDuffie A, Yoder P, Stone W. Prelinguistic predictors of vocabulary in young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research. 2005;48(5):1080–1097. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2005/075). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. McGregor KK, Rost G, Arenas R, Farris-Trimble A, Stiles D. Children with ASD can use gaze in support of word recognition and learning. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2013;54(7):745–753. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. McGregor KK, Bean A. How children with autism extend new words. Journal of Speech, Language, & Hearing Research. 2012;55:70–83. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2011/11-0024). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Messenger K, Yuan S, Fisher C. Learning Verb Syntax via Listening: New Evidence From 22-Month-Olds. Language Learning and Development. 2014:1–13. doi: 10.1080/15475441.2014.978331. ahead-of-print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Menyuk P, Quill K. Semantic problems in autistic children. In: Schopler E, Mesibov G, editors. Communication Problems in Autism. New York, NY: Plenum Press; 1985. pp. 127–145. [Google Scholar]
  132. Miller LK. The savant syndrome: Intellectual impairment and exceptional skill. Psychological Bulletin. 1999;(125):31–46. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.125.1.31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Morales M, Mundy P, Delgado CE, Yale M, Neal R, Schwartz HK. Gaze following, temperament, and language development in 6-month-olds: A replication and extension. Infant Behavior and Development. 2000;23(2):231–236. [Google Scholar]
  134. Morales M, Mundy P, Rojas J. Following the direction of gaze and language development in 6-month-olds. Infant Behavior and Development. 1998;21(2):373–377. [Google Scholar]
  135. Mundy P, Sigman M, Kasari C. A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1990;20(1):115–128. doi: 10.1007/BF02206861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Nadig A, Ozonoff S, Singh L, Young G, Rogers S, Caunt-Nulton H, …Woo IH. Do 6-month-olds at risk for autism display an infant-directed speech preference?. Proceedings from the 31st Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development; Boston, MA. 2007. pp. 451–461. [Google Scholar]
  137. Naigles L. Children use syntax to learn verb meanings. Journal of Child Language. 1990;17:357–374. doi: 10.1017/S0305000900013817. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Naigles LR, Kelty E, Jaffery R, Fein D. Abstractness and continuity in the syntactic development of young children with autism. Autism Research. 2011;4(6):422–437. doi: 10.1002/aur.223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Naigles LR. Input and language development in children with autism. Seminars in Speech and Language. 2013;34(4):237–248. doi: 10.1055/s-0033-1353446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Naigles LR, Kelley E, Troyb E, Fein D. Residual difficulties with categorical induction in children with a history of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2013;43(9):2048–2061. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1754-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Nappa R, Wessel A, McEldoon KL, Gleitman LR, Trueswell JC. Use of speaker’s gaze and syntax in verb learning. Language Learning and Development. 2009;5(4):203–234. doi: 10.1080/15475440903167528. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Newcomer P, Hammill D. Test of Language Development—Primary (TOLD-P:3) Austin, TX: Pro-Ed; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  143. Newman R, Ratner NB, Jusczyk AM, Jusczyk PW, Dow KA. Infants’ early ability to segment the conversational speech signal predicts later language development: a retrospective analysis. Developmental Psychology. 2006;42(4):643. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.4.643. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Norbury CF. Barking up the wrong tree? Lexical ambiguity resolution in children with language impairments and autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2005;90:42–171. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Norbury CF, Griffiths H, Nation K. Sound before meaning: Word learning in autistic disorder. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:4012–4019. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Norbury CF, Griffiths H, Nation K. Sound before meaning: Word learning in autistic disorders. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48(14):4012–4019. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. O’Riordan M, Passetti F. Discrimination in autism within different sensory modalities. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2006;36(5):665–675. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0106-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Ochs E, Schieffelin BB. Language acquisition and socializations: Three developmental stories and their implications. In: Shweder RA, LeVine RA, editors. Culture theory: Essays on mind, self and emotion. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1984. pp. 276–320. [Google Scholar]
  149. Omaki A, Lidz J. Linking parser development to acquisition of syntactic knowledge. Language Acquisition. 2015;22(2):158–192. [Google Scholar]
  150. Osterling J, Dawson G. Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1994;24(3):247–257. doi: 10.1007/BF02172225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Parish-Morris J, Hennon EA, Hirsh-Pasek K, Golinkoff RM, Tager-Flusberg H. Children with autism illuminate the role of social intention in word learning. Child Development. 2007;78(4):1265–1287. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01065.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Paul R, Chawarska K, Fowler C, Cicchetti D, Volkmar F. “Listen my children and you shall hear”: Auditory preferences in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2007;50(5):1350–1364. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/094). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Pelphrey KA, Shultz S, Hudac CM, Vander Wyk BC. Research review: constraining heterogeneity: the social brain and its development in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2011;52(6):631–644. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02349.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Pickles A, Anderson DK, Lord C. Heterogeneity and plasticity in the development of language: a 17 year follow up of children referred early for possible autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2014;55(12):1354–1362. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Pinker S. Language learnability and language development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1984. [Google Scholar]
  156. Plaisted KC. Reduced generalization in autism: An alternative to weak central coherence. In: Burack J, Charman T, Nurit Y, Zelazo P, editors. The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; pp. 149–169. [Google Scholar]
  157. Poulin-Dubois D, Forbes JN. Toddlers’ attention to intentions-in-action in learning novel action words. Developmental Psychology. 2002;38(1):104. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Potrzeba ER, Fein D, Naigles L. Investigating the shape bias in typically developing children and children with autism spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Psychology. 2015;6:446. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00446. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Preissler MA, Carey S. The role of inferences about referential intent in word learning: Evidence from autism. Cognition. 2005;97(1):B13–B23. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Preissler MA. Associative learning of pictures and words by low-functioning children with autism. Autism. 2008;12(3):231–248. doi: 10.1177/1362361307088753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Rescorla L, Safyer P. Lexical composition in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) Journal of Child Language. 2013;40(01):47–68. doi: 10.1017/S0305000912000232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Richdale AL. Sleep problems in autism: prevalence, cause, and intervention. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 1999;41(01):60–66. doi: 10.1017/s0012162299000122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Rimland B, Fein D. Special talents of autistic savants. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  164. Rogoff B, Mistry J, Göncü A, Mosier C. Guided participation in cultural activity by toddlers and caregivers. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1993;58(8):i-179. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Roser ME, Aslin RN, McKenzie R, Zahra D, Fiser J. Enhanced visual statistical learning in adults with autism. Neuropsychology. 2015;29(2):163. doi: 10.1037/neu0000137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  166. Rutter M, Sroufe L. Developmental psychopathology: Concepts and challenges. Development and Psychopathology. 2000;12(03):265–296. doi: 10.1017/s0954579400003023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  167. Saffran JR, Aslin RN, Newport EL. Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science. 1996;274(5294):1926–1928. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5294.1926. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  168. Saffran JR, Johnson EK, Aslin RN, Newport EL. Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults. Cognition. 1999;70(1):27–52. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(98)00075-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  169. Samuelson LK. Building biases: Word learning as a developmental cascade. Psychological Science Agenda. 2011;25(2) [Google Scholar]
  170. Saint-Georges C, Chetouani M, Cassel R, Apicella F, Mahdhaoui A, Muratori F, …Cohen D. Motherese in interaction: at the cross-road of emotion and cognition?(A systematic review) PloS One. 2013;8(10):e78103. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0078103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  171. Schafer G, Williams TI, Smith PT. Which words are hard for Autistic children to learn? Mind & Language. 2013;28(5):661–698. [Google Scholar]
  172. Schoen E, Paul R, Chawarska K. Phonology and vocal behavior in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research: Official Journal of the International Society for Autism Research. 2011;4(3):177–188. doi: 10.1002/aur.183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  173. Schoen E, Paul R, Chawarska K. Vocal productions in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. In: Paul R, Flipsen P, editors. Speech Sound Disorders in Children. San Diego: Plural Publishing Inc; 2009. pp. 181–204. [Google Scholar]
  174. Scott-Van Zeeland AA, McNealy K, Wang AT, Sigman M, Bookheimer SY, Dapretto M. No neural evidence of statistical learning during exposure to artificial languages in children with autism spectrum disorders. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;68(4):345–351. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.01.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  175. Shield A. Preliminary findings of similarities and differences in the signed and spoken language of children with Autism. Seminars in Speech and Language. 2014;35(4):309–320. doi: 10.1055/s-0034-1389103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  176. Shield A, Meier RP. Palm reversal errors in native-signing children with autism. Journal of Communication Disorders. 2012;45(6):439–454. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2012.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  177. Shield A, Meier RP, Tager-Flusberg H. The use of sign language pronouns by natives-signing children with Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2015;45(7):2128–2145. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2377-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  178. Shulman C, Guberman A. Acquisition of verb meaning through syntactic cues: A comparison of children with autism, children with specific language impairment (SLI) and children with typical language development (TLD) Journal of Child Language. 2007;34(02):411–423. doi: 10.1017/s0305000906007963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  179. Singh L, Nestor S, Parikh C, Yull A. Influences of infant-directed speech on early word recognition. Infancy. 2009;14(6):654–666. doi: 10.1080/15250000903263973. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  180. Shultz S, Vouloumanos A. Three-month-olds prefer speech to other naturally occurring signals. Language Learning and Development. 2010;6(4):241–257. [Google Scholar]
  181. Sroufe LA. Emotional development: The organization of emotional life in the early years. Cambridge University Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  182. Stanutz S, Wapnick J, Burack JA. Pitch discrimination and melodic memory in children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism. 2014;18(2):137–147. doi: 10.1177/1362361312462905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  183. Stickgold R, Walker MP. Memory consolidation and reconsolidation: What is the role of sleep? Trends in Neurosciences. 2005;28(8):408–415. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  184. Swensen LD, Kelley E, Fein D, Naigles LR. Processes of language acquisition in children with autism: Evidence from preferential looking. Child Development. 2007;78(2):542–557. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01022.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  185. Tager-Flusberg H. Basic level and superordinate level categorization by autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1985;40(3):450–469. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(85)90077-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  186. Tager-Flusberg H. On the nature of a language acquisition disorder: The example of autism. In: Kessel F, editor. The development of language and language researchers: Essays in honor of Roger Brown. Hillsdale, NJ: Psychology Press; 1988. pp. 249–267. [Google Scholar]
  187. Tager-Flusberg H. Autistic children’s talk about psychological states: Deficits in the early acquisition of a theory of mind. Child Development. 1992;63:161–172. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  188. Tager-Flusberg H. Language and understanding minds: Connections in autism. In: Baron-Cohen S, Tager-Flusberg H, Cohen DJ, editors. Understanding other minds: perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press; 2000. pp. 124–149. [Google Scholar]
  189. Tager-Flusberg H. A reexamination of the theory of mind hypothesis of autism. In: Burack J, Charman T, Yirmiya N, Zelazo PR, editors. The Development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2001. pp. 173–193. [Google Scholar]
  190. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Kuchirko Y, Song L. Why is infant language learning facilitated by parental responsiveness? Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2014;23:121–126. [Google Scholar]
  191. Tek S, Jaffery G, Fein D, Naigles LR. Do children with autism spectrum disorders show a shape bias in word learning? Autism Research. 2008;1(4):208–222. doi: 10.1002/aur.38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  192. Thiessen ED, Hill EA, Saffran JR. Infant-directed speech facilitates word segmentation. Infancy. 2005;7(1):53–71. doi: 10.1207/s15327078in0701_5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  193. Thurm A, Lord C, Lee LC, Newschaffer C. Predictors of language acquisition in preschool children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2007;37(9):1721–1734. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0300-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  194. Tomasello M, Barton ME. Learning words in nonostensive contexts. Developmental Psychology. 1994;30(5):639. [Google Scholar]
  195. Trueswell JC, Gleitman LR. Learning to parse and its implications for language acquisition. In: Gaskell G, editor. Oxford Handbook of Psycholinguistics. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press; 2007. pp. 635–56. [Google Scholar]
  196. Trueswell JC, Sekerina I, Hill NM, Logrip ML. The kindergarten-path effect: Studying on-line sentence processing in young children. Cognition. 1999;73(2):89–134. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(99)00032-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  197. Vales C, Smith LB. Words, shape, visual search and visual working memory in 3-year-old children. Developmental Science. 2015;18(1):65–79. doi: 10.1111/desc.12179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  198. Venker CE, Eernisse ER, Saffran JR, Weismer SE. Individual Differences in the Real-Time Comprehension of Children with ASD. Autism Research. 2013;6(5):417–432. doi: 10.1002/aur.1304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  199. Venker CE. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Wisconsin-Madison; Madison, Wisconsin: 2014. Statistical word learning and non-social visual attention in children with autism. [Google Scholar]
  200. Vogindroukas I, Papageorgiou V, Vostanis P. Pattern of semantic errors in autism - A brief research report. Autism. 2003;7(2):195–203. doi: 10.1177/1362361303007002006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  201. Vouloumanos A, Werker JF. Tuned to the signal: the privileged status of speech for young infants. Developmental Science. 2004;7(3):270–276. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00345.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  202. Vouloumanos A, Werker JF. Listening to language at birth: Evidence for a bias for speech in neonates. Developmental Science. 2007;10(2):159–164. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00549.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  203. Vouloumanos A, Curtin S. Foundational Tuning: How Infants’ Attention to Speech Predicts Language Development. Cognitive Science. 2014;38(8):1675–1686. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  204. Watson LR, Baranek GT, Roberts JE, David FJ, Perryman TY. Behavioral and physiological responses to child-directed speech as predictors of communication outcomes in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2010;53(4):1052–1064. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/09-0096). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  205. Watson LR, Roberts JE, Baranek GT, Mandulak KC, Dalton JC. Behavioral and physiological responses to child-directed speech of children with autism spectrum disorders or typical development. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2012;42(8):1616–1629. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1401-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  206. Waxman SR. Convergences between semantic and conceptual organization in the preschool years. In: Gelman SA, Byrnes JP, editors. Perspectives on language and thought. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1991. pp. 107–145. [Google Scholar]
  207. Weismer SE, Gernsbacher MA, Stronach S, Karasinski C, Eernisse ER, Venker CE, Sindberg H. Lexical and grammatical skills in toddlers on the autism spectrum compared to late talking toddlers. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2011;41(8):1065–1075. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1134-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  208. Weismer SE, Lord C, Esler A. Early language patterns of toddlers on the autism spectrum compared to toddlers with developmental delay. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2010;40(10):1259–1273. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-0983-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  209. Werker JF, Tees RC. Influences on infant speech processing: Toward a new synthesis. Annual Review of Psychology. 1999;50(1):509–535. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  210. Whitehouse AJ, Bishop DV. Do children with autism ‘switch off’ to speech sounds? An investigation using event-related potentials. Developmental Science. 2008;11(4):516–524. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00697.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  211. Werchan DM, Gómez RL. Wakefulness (not sleep) promotes generalization of word learning in 2.5-year-old children. Child Development. 2014;85(2):429–436. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  212. Wilkinson KM. Disambiguation and mapping of new word meanings by individuals with intellectual/developmental disabilities. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2005;110(2):71–86. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2005)110<71:DAMONW>2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  213. Williams SE, Horst JS. Goodnight book: sleep consolidation improves word learning via storybooks. Frontiers in Psychology. 2014;5:184. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  214. Xu F. The role of language in acquiring object kind concepts in infancy. Cognition. 2002;85(3):223–250. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(02)00109-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  215. Yi L, Quinn PC, Feng C, Li J, Ding H, Lee K. Do individuals with autism spectrum disorder process own- and other-race faces differently? Vision Research. 2015;107:124–132. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2014.11.02. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  216. Yoder P, Watson LR, Lambert W. Value-Added Predictors of Expressive and Receptive Language Growth in Initially Nonverbal Preschoolers with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2014;45(5):1254–1270. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2286-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  217. Ziatas K, Durkin K, Pratt C. Belief term development in children with autism, Asperger syndrome, specific language impairment, and normal development: Links to theory of mind development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 1998;39:755–763. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES