Abstract
Objective
When college students violate campus alcohol policies, they typically receive disciplinary sanctions that include alcohol education or counseling. This meta-analysis evaluated the efficacy of these “mandated interventions” to prevent future alcohol misuse.
Methods
Studies were included if they evaluated an individual- or group-level intervention, sampled students mandated to an alcohol program, used a pretest-posttest design, and assessed alcohol use as an outcome. Thirty-one studies with 68 separate interventions (N = 8,621 participants; 35% women; 85% White) were coded by independent raters with respect to sample, design, methodological features, and intervention content; the raters also calculated weighted mean effect sizes, using random-effects models. A priori predictors were examined to explain variability in effect sizes.
Results
In the five studies that used assessment-only control groups, mandated students reported significantly less drinking relative to controls (between-group contrasts), d+ ranged from 0.13-0.20 for quantity and intoxication outcomes. In the 31 studies that provided within-group contrasts, significant effects were observed for all outcomes in the short-term (i.e., ≤ 3 months post-intervention), with d+ ranging from 0.14-0.27; however, fewer significant effects appeared at longer follow-ups. Four commercially-available intervention protocols (i.e., BASICS, e-CHUG, Alcohol 101, and Alcohol Skills Training Program) were associated with risk reduction.
Conclusions
Providing mandated interventions to students who violate campus alcohol policies is an effective short-term risk reduction strategy. Continued research is needed to maintain initial gains, identify the most useful intervention components, and determine the cost-effectiveness of delivery modes.
Keywords: alcohol, drinking, intervention, mandated, alcohol sanctions, meta-analysis
College is a time when emerging adults explore adult behaviors, roles, and relationships, including alcohol and other drug use (Arnett, 2005). This youthful exploration can result in two related but distinct consequences: drinking by underage students, and excessive drinking regardless of age. Although more than half of college students are under the minimum legal drinking age of 21 (American College Health Association, 2012), 81% report lifetime alcohol use (L. D. Johnson, O'Malley, Bachman, & Schulenberg, 2012). Relative to students over 21, underage students are more likely to drink to excess (Wechsler, Lee, Nelson, & Kuo, 2002). A substantial proportion of college students endanger themselves and their fellow students by excessive consumption of alcohol. Young adults attending college are more likely to engage in heavy episodic drinking (5 [4] or more drinks on occasion for men [women] in the last month) than their peers not in college (40% vs. 35%) (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2013), and this increased risk for heavy drinking holds for both underage students and students of legal age (Hingson, Zha, & Weitzman, 2009). Nationally representative samples indicate that 40% of college students admit getting drunk in the past 30 days and 1 out of 7 college students reported consuming 10 or more drinks in a single day (L. D. Johnson et al., 2012). The college environment appears to promote a hazardous style of drinking.
More than half of students who drink report one or more negative alcohol-related problems. Some of the most frequent involve doing something later regretted, partial or full blackouts, unprotected sex, and alcohol-related injury (American College Health Association, 2012). Alcohol-related problems are associated with poorer academic functioning (Read, Merrill, Kahler, & Strong, 2007). Adverse consequences of college student drinking also affect other members of the campus community (Perkins, 2002). Nondrinking students experience collateral effects of others' drinking, such as interrupted studying, aggression and assault, and having to tend to the safety of drunken peers (Hingson et al., 2009; Wechsler et al., 2002). Institutions incur expenses related to property damage, security and emergency services, and nearby communities deal with noise and other drinking-related violations (Carey, McClurg, et al., 2009; Gebhardt, Kaphingst, & DeJong, 2000; Perkins, 2002). When underage drinking or problematic alcohol use is detected, college administrators are required by law to implement disciplinary sanctions (U.S. Department of Education, 2006). This legal obligation is accompanied by a fundamental duty to promote student development and to ensure a healthy and safe campus environment. Thus, for both legal and educational reasons, campus administrators often require both underage and of-age students who violate campus alcohol policies to receive alcohol-specific education or counseling to learn to use alcohol more safely and responsibly.
A national survey of nearly 200 institutions of higher education reveals a variety of sanctions in use, including participation in an alcohol education group (74%), parental notification (54%), individual motivational interviews (46%), and computer-delivered alcohol education (40%) (Anderson & Gadaleto, 2006). Although a growing number of empirical studies have evaluated the efficacy of mandated interventions, these studies vary on a range of methodological dimensions (e.g., mode of intervention administration, intervention dose, and targeted outcome measures), making interpretation of this growing literature challenging. Administrators and health care professionals responsible for choosing among a wide variety of intervention options often have little guidance with respect to their efficacy.
Both qualitative (Cronce & Larimer, 2011; Larimer & Cronce, 2007) and quantitative (Carey, Scott-Sheldon, Carey, & DeMartini, 2007; Carey, Scott-Sheldon, Elliott, Garey, & Carey, 2012; Fachini, Aliane, Martinez, & Furtado, 2012; Scott-Sheldon, Carey, Elliott, Garey, & Carey, 2014)reviews of alcohol interventions targeting college students support their efficacy both relative to control conditions (between-group contrasts) and over time (within-group contrasts). Meta-analyses often find small but statistically significant effect sizes; however, the distribution of the effect sizes across a number of alcohol use outcomes is usually heterogeneous. Differences among weighted mean effect sizes (between-groups) have been associated with student characteristics such as heavy drinking and mandated status (Carey et al., 2012) as well as specific intervention components (Scott-Sheldon et al., 2014). The results of extant reviews suggest that it may be fruitful to evaluate the extent to which mandated alcohol interventions produce changes in alcohol use and related problems.
Mandated students have been characterized as a special population at high risk for negative outcomes (Cronce & Larimer, 2011). On average, mandated students drink more than other students (Barnett et al., 2004; Fromme & Corbin, 2004; Merrill, Carey, Lust, Kalichman, & Carey, 2014). Mandated students are often more defensive and less ready to change than volunteers participating in the same intervention (Barnett et al., 2008; Dimeff, Baer, Kivlahan, & Marlatt, 1999; Palmer, Kilmer, Ball, & Larimer, 2010). Furthermore, males are represented disproportionately in mandated samples (Fromme & Corbin, 2004; Merrill et al., 2014), male students drink more than female students (American College Health Association, 2012), and males are more resistant to changing their alcohol use relative to females (Carey & DeMartini, 2010; Henson, Pearson, & Carey, 2015). These behavioral, contextual, and motivational factors may influence outcomes of mandated interventions and/or maintenance of risk reduction over time. A focused review of intervention efficacy offered in the context of sanctions for campus alcohol violations is needed.
We conducted a meta-analytic review with two over-arching goals, namely, (a) to evaluate the effects of interventions for mandated students across multiple outcomes, and (b) to identify student or intervention characteristics associated with changes in alcohol consumption and problems after mandated interventions. We addressed five research questions. First, do students mandated to alcohol interventions reduce their alcohol use and related problems compared to controls? This question focuses on between-group effects to address whether interventions (vs. wait-list controls) improve alcohol outcomes. Because most schools require students who violate campus alcohol policy to complete an intervention, our ability to test for the efficacy of delivering an intervention is limited to the few studies that randomized mandated students to wait-list control conditions. Second, over time, do students mandated to alcohol interventions drink less and report fewer related problems? Due to the requirement that all mandated students receive an appropriate intervention, we evaluate the magnitude and duration of within-group effects. We assess pre- to post-test change because most studies evaluating alcohol interventions with mandated students compare two active intervention conditions. Active comparison conditions with alcohol content are likely to produce some risk reduction among college drinkers (Carey, Scott-Sheldon, Elliott, Bolles, & Carey, 2009; Scott-Sheldon et al., 2014) and so between-group differences between two active interventions can be small and difficult to interpret. Consistent with the broader college alcohol intervention literature, we hypothesize that mandated students who receive any alcohol intervention will report lower alcohol consumption and fewer alcohol-related problems relative to baseline. Effect sizes representing within-group changes over time are subject to alternative explanations for change (such as maturation), however this interpretive limitation is addressed in part by the next set of questions regarding factors that predict magnitude of change.
Third, we examine whether the magnitude of drinking reduction varied by student gender. We predict that samples with more women would be associated with larger effect sizes, based on prior research (Carey et al., 2007). Fourth, we determine what types of interventions are associated with larger drinking reductions. To do this we coded structural and content features of interventions to evaluate their association with outcomes. Hypothesized predictors of change over time included (a) mode of administration (i.e., individual vs. group; face-to-face vs. computer-delivered interventions; length of intervention), and (b) intervention components (e.g., education, personalized feedback, moderation strategies, and goal-setting). We hypothesized that interventions will be more effective when they were longer and used delivery formats (i.e., individual and face-to-face interventions) that have been associated with stronger effects in non-mandated student samples (Carey et al., 2007; Larimer & Cronce, 2007). We also hypothesized that interventions will be more effective when they used intervention components that are personalized (i.e., drinking feedback, moderation strategies, challenges to alcohol expectancies, goal-setting, identification of high-risk situations) consistent with results obtained with non-mandated students (Scott-Sheldon et al., 2014). Finally, we characterize the efficacy of commercially-available intervention protocols (e.g., BASICS, Alcohol 101) that have been marketed and widely disseminated to campuses, in order to evaluate the evidence base for these products.
Methods
Sample of Studies and Selection Criteria
Following established guidelines (Reed & Baxter, 2009; Rosenthal, 1991), studies were retrieved from (1) electronic databases, (2) reference sections of relevant papers, (3) professional journals, and (4) author responses to requests. First, we searched electronic databases (PubMed, PsycInfo, ERIC, CINAHL, Dissertation Abstracts, The Cochrane Library) using a Boolean search strategy that included the following broad search terms: (alcohol OR drink* OR binge) AND (college OR university) AND (intervention OR prevention). Search terms were adjusted based on the specific requirements of each electronic database. We conducted a broad search of alcohol interventions targeting college students rather than restricting the search to mandated students only. Second, we reviewed the references of papers obtained through the database searches. Third, we examined the tables of contents and/or abstracts available from relevant electronic journals (e.g., Addiction, Addictive Behaviors, Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs). Finally, we searched the National Institutes of Health Research Portfolio Online Reporting Tools (NIH RePORTER) database for relevant awards, sent requests for papers to individual researchers, and posted messages to several electronic mailing lists. We continued to review manuscripts received from researchers and obtained through journals for the 6-month period following the last database search.
Both published and unpublished studies fulfilling the selection criteria and available by the end of December 2012 were included if they (a) examined an individual- or group-level intervention to reduce alcohol use, (b) sampled students mandated to an alcohol intervention, (c) used either a single-group or an independent-groups pretest-posttest design, (d) measured alcohol use, and (e) provided statistical information needed to calculate effect sizes. Studies were excluded if they (a) did not focus on alcohol use (e.g., combined substance use interventions), (b) exclusively sampled non-mandated college students, or (c) included a mass media or structural-level intervention component. If a study reported an intervention delivered to non-mandated comparison sample, that condition was not included in the analyses. No-intervention/wait-list control conditions were also excluded from the within-groups analyses. Thirty-one studies (68 interventions) were included (Figure 1). We refer to all active alcohol-focused interventions (whether listed as experimental or control) as “interventions” [k] throughout this manuscript.
Coding and Reliability
Two trained coders independently rated the study descriptive information, sample characteristics (e.g., sex), design and measurement specifics (e.g., number of follow-ups), and length and content of intervention (e.g., number of total minutes, skills-training). Study quality was assessed using 14 items (e.g., manualized treatment, retention, reactivity of assessment procedures) adapted from validated measures (Jadad et al., 1996; Miller et al., 1995). A random selection of 20 studies was used to assess inter-rater reliability. For the categorical variables, raters agreed on 84% of the judgments (mean Cohen's kappa = 0.65, signifying substantial agreement) (Landis & Koch, 1977). Reliability for the continuous variables (using the intraclass correlation coefficient; ρ) yielded an average ρ of 0.82 (median = 0.97). Discrepancies between coders were resolved through discussion.
Study Outcomes
Effect size (ES) estimates were calculated for alcohol consumption and related problems. Consumption outcomes included: quantity consumed (a) over a period of time (e.g., week or month) and (b) during specific drinking periods (e.g., weekends, spring break); (c) frequency of drinking days over a period of time (e.g., week or month); (d) frequency of heavy drinking, usually defined as 5 or more drinks for men and 4 or more drinks for women (Wechsler, Dowdall, Davenport, & Rimm, 1995); (e) maximum amount of alcohol consumed on a single occasion; and (f) peak (maximum) and (g) typical (average) estimated blood alcohol concentration (BAC, or the percentage of alcohol contained in the blood, measured as weight/volume). Alcohol-related problems (e.g., hangover, blackouts, missed work or classes) were typically operationalized using multi-item scales.
Effect Size Calculations
Two methods were used for calculating ES: (a) the standardized mean difference (between-group contrasts) and (b) the standardized mean gain (within-group contrasts). ES were calculated as the mean differences between the pre- and post-test divided by the pre-test standard deviation for each condition (Becker, 1988; Morris & DeShon, 2002). For the between-group contrasts, the standardized mean gain for each wait-list control group was subtracted from the standardized mean gain for each intervention group. Thus, all ES were adjusted for baseline. This is the preferred method for repeated-measures designs according to the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ) Methods Research Report (B. T. Johnson & Huedo-Medina, 2013). When means and standard deviations were not provided, other measures (e.g., t- or F-test) were used (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). All ES were corrected for sample size bias (Hedges, 1981).
Multiple ES were calculated from individual studies when they reported more than one outcome variable, when outcomes were reported separately by sample characteristics (i.e., gender), or studies reported outcomes across multiple follow-ups. We grouped follow-up intervals into short-term (≤ 13 weeks, equivalent to ≤3 months), intermediate (14-51 weeks) and longer-term (≥ 52 weeks, equivalent to one year or more). If a study conducted multiple assessments within one of these intervals, we report the ES for the longest follow-up within the interval. When a study reported multiple measures of a single outcome at the same assessment interval, the ES were averaged. Two coders independently calculated ES; ES were examined for consistency and discrepancies were corrected. Positive ES indicate that mandated students receiving an intervention reported the intended effects (lower alcohol consumption and fewer alcohol-related problems relative to wait-list controls or at post-test).
Statistical Analysis
Weighted mean ES (d+) were calculated using a random-effects model following maximum likelihood assumptions (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). Consistent with procedures described by Lipsey and Wilson (2001; pp. 107-108) we examined each dependent variable for outliers prior to conducting statistical analyses. Because few outliers were detected (see table notes, Table 2 and 3), we eliminated outliers from subsequent analyses. ES calculated for a single study by sample characteristic (i.e., men, women) were analyzed as separate studies because the subgroups are independent from each other (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2009; Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). To assess the null hypothesis of homogeneity (i.e., that all studies share a common ES), the Q statistic and associate p-value for each outcome were calculated. The proportion of the observed dispersion was assessed using the I2 index and its corresponding 95% uncertainty intervals (Higgins & Thompson, 2002; Huedo-Medina, Sanchez-Meca, Marin-Martinez, & Botella, 2006; Ioannidis, Patsopoulos, & Evangelou, 2007). The I2 index ranges from 0 to 100% with percentages of 25%, 50%, and 75%, considered low, moderate, and high levels of observed variance reflecting true differences in ES (Higgins, Thompson, Deeks, & Altman, 2003). Asymmetries in the distributions of ES, indicating a possible reporting bias (Rosenthal, 1979), were examined by inspecting funnel plots (Sterne & Egger, 2001) and assessing the degree of funnel plot asymmetry using recommended methods (Begg & Mazumdar, 1994). Consistent with meta-analytic recommendations, tests for funnel plot asymmetry were conducted only for variables with at least 10 studies (Higgins & Green, 2011).
Table 2.
Outcome | k | d+ (95% CI) | Heterogeneity | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
| |||||
Random effects | Q | p | I2(95% UI) | ||
Short-term assessment (≤13 weeks) | |||||
| |||||
Alcohol consumption | |||||
Quantity, per week/month | 10 | 0.13 (0.02, 0.25) | 9.70 | .375 | 7 (0, 45) |
Quantity, specific intervals/drinking day | 8 | 0.08 (-0.05, 0.21) | 6.85 | .445 | 0 (0, 00) |
Peak consumption | 2 | 0.28 (-0.15, 0.71) | 1.18 | .277 | 15 (0, 54) |
Frequency of drinking days | 3 | 0.09 (-0.13, 0.31) | 2.13 | .345 | 6 (0, 35) |
Frequency of heavy drinking | 7 | 0.10 (-0.02, 0.23) | 5.86 | .439 | 0 (0, 00) |
Peak BAC | 7 | 0.20 (0.06, 0.33) | 9.68 | .139 | 38 (0, 74) |
Typical BAC | 5 | 0.16 (0.01, 0.31) | 0.85 | .932 | 0 (0, 51) |
Alcohol-related problems | 11 | -0.02 (-0.15, 0.12) | 7.82 | .646 | 0 (0, 61) |
Note. All dependent variables were examined for outliers (Emerson & Strenio, 1983). Of the 53 effect sizes, one outlier was detected for typical BAC (short-term). The unadjusted and adjusted (i.e., trimmed) weighted mean effect size-differed in magnitude for typical BAC (d+unadjusted = 0.10, 95% CI = -0.03, 0.23, k = 6; d+adjusted = 0.16, 95% CI = 0.01, 0.31, k = 5). The adjusted effect size is reported above. Positive weighted mean effect sizes (d+) indicate that the treatment group reduced consumption or problems relative to the control group. k, number of interventions; d+, weighted mean effect size; Q, heterogeneity statistic; I2, consistency of effect sizes; CI, confidence interval; UI, uncertainty interval; BAC, blood alcohol concentration. Bold font indicates significant weighted mean difference effects.
Table 3.
Outcome | k | d+ (95% CI) | Heterogeneity | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
| |||||
Random effects | Q | p | I2(95% UI) | ||
Short-term assessment (≤13 weeks) | |||||
| |||||
Alcohol consumption | |||||
Quantity, per week/month | 42 | 0.22 (0.14, 0.31) | 58.59 | .037 | 30 (0, 52) |
Quantity, specific intervals/drinking day | 28 | 0.14 (0.06, 0.23) | 41.72 | .035 | 35 (0, 59) |
Peak consumption | 19 | 0.27 (0.13, 0.41) | 32.21 | .009 | 49 (13, 70) |
Frequency of drinking days | 23 | 0.17 (0.04, 0.30) | 88.66 | <.001 | 75 (63, 83) |
Frequency of heavy drinking | 22 | 0.23 (0.13, 0.33) | 20.58 | .485 | 0 (0, 0) |
Peak BAC | 23 | 0.26 (0.17, 0.36) | 43.80 | .004 | 50 (19, 69) |
Typical BAC | 22 | 0.17 (0.08, 0.26) | 31.13 | .071 | 33 (0, 60) |
Alcohol-related problems | 50 | 0.25 (0.19, 0.31) | 84.08 | <.001 | 42 (18, 58) |
| |||||
Intermediate assessment (14-51 weeks) | |||||
| |||||
Alcohol consumption | |||||
Quantity, per week/month | 18 | 0.08 (-0.03, 0.18) | 40.60 | .001 | 58 (29, 75) |
Quantity, specific intervals/drinking day | 15 | 0.07 (-0.03, 0.18) | 27.47 | .017 | 49 (7, 72) |
Peak consumption | 9 | 0.06 (-0.14, 0.27) | 14.06 | .080 | 43 (0, 74) |
Frequency of drinking days | 5 | 0.12 (-0.11, 0.36) | 56.90 | <.001 | 93 (87, 96) |
Frequency of heavy drinking | 18 | 0.14 (0.04, 0.23) | 15.39 | .567 | 0 (0, 59) |
Peak BAC | 16 | 0.25 (0.14, 0.36) | 43.59 | <.001 | 66 (42, 80) |
Typical BAC | 10 | 0.17 (0.04, 0.29) | 33.02 | <.001 | 73 (48, 86) |
Alcohol-related problems | 25 | 0.13 (0.06, 0.21) | 113.68 | <.001 | 79 (69, 85) |
| |||||
Long-term assessment (≥52 weeks) | |||||
| |||||
Alcohol consumption | |||||
Quantity, per week/month | 10 | -0.06 (-0.25, 0.12) | 21.09 | .012 | 57 (13, 79) |
Quantity, specific intervals/drinking day | 11 | 0.03 (-0.11, 0.17) | 9.87 | .452 | 0 (0, 0) |
Peak consumption | 2 | -0.13 (-0.78, 0.52) | 0.01 | .904 | 0 (0, 100) |
Frequency of drinking days | 6 | -0.15 (-0.42, 0.13) | 14.34 | .014 | 65 (16, 85) |
Frequency of heavy drinking | 14 | -0.02 (-0.15, 0.11) | 16.20 | .239 | 20 (0, 57) |
Peak BAC | 10 | 0.11 (-0.03, 0.25) | 54.38 | <.001 | 83 (71, 91) |
Typical BAC | 10 | 0.12 (0.01, 0.25) | 9.83 | .364 | 8 (0, 48) |
Alcohol-related problems | 10 | 0.05 (-0.09, 0.20) | 29.72 | <.001 | 70 (42, 84) |
Note. All dependent variables were examined for outliers (Emerson & Strenio, 1983). Of the 428 effect sizes, 10 outliers were detected (2% of the total number of effect size estimates). The weighted mean effect sizes for each dependent variable were consistent in magnitude and direction for unadjusted and adjusted (i.e., trimmed) effect sizes, so only the adjusted effect sizes are reported above. Positive weighted mean effect sizes (d+) indicate reduced consumption or problems at post-test. k, number of interventions; d+, weighted mean effect size; Q, heterogeneity statistic; I2, consistency of effect sizes; CI, confidence interval; UI, uncertainty interval; BAC, blood alcohol concentration. Bold font indicates significant weighted mean gain effects.
Predictors of Outcome
Meta-regression was used to assess the association between sample, methodological, or intervention characteristics and the magnitude of within-group effects. Predictors were selected a priori and included proportion of women in the sample, intervention delivery mode (individual vs. group, face-to-face vs. computer-delivered, length of intervention), and content (e.g., feedback on consumption) (Harbord & Higgins, 2008; Hedges, 1981; Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). Mixed-effect models were used such that the inverse variance for each ES included error associated with (a) within-study level sampling error and (b) additional between-study population variance. We selected this model because it is more conservative than purely fixed-effects models, minimizing the Type I error rate (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). Significant univariate predictors were simultaneously entered into multiple regression models to evaluate whether they explained unique variance. Permutation tests were conducted to adjust the p-values for multiple testing (Harbord & Higgins, 2008; Higgins & Thompson, 2004). All analyses were conducted in Stata 12.1 (StataCorp, 2012).
Results
Study, Sample, and Intervention Characteristics
Study, sample, and intervention characteristics of the included studies are provided in Table 1. Thirty-one studies (from 30 publications) met the inclusion criteria and were included in the meta-analysis. (Details for the included studies are provided in Table A, Supplemental Digital Content.) Most studies (29 out of 31) were published in journals (2 studies were unpublished dissertations) with a median publication date of 2007 (range = 1991 to 2015).1
Table 1.
Study Characteristics | |
---|---|
| |
Publication year | |
Mdn | 2007 |
Range | 1991-2015 |
| |
Data collection year | |
Mdn | 2004 |
Range | 1989-2010 |
| |
Published in journals, % | 94 |
| |
Funded study, % | 68 |
| |
Sample Characteristics | |
| |
Sample size, Mdn/M | 151/287 |
| |
Retention, M% (SD) | 77 (21) |
| |
Age, M (SD) (n = 24) | 19 (0.51) |
| |
Sex, % women (n = 30) | 35 |
| |
Ethnicity, % White (n = 28) | 85 |
| |
Risk Characteristics | |
| |
Baseline alcohol use, % (n = 29) | 100 |
| |
Quantity, M (SD), n = 23 | 13 (6) |
| |
Problems, M (SD), n = 16 | 7 (4) |
| |
Year in school, % (n = 24) | |
First | 58 |
Second | 27 |
Third | 12 |
Fourth | 5 |
| |
Greek members, % (n = 9) | 22 |
| |
Institution Characteristics | |
| |
Region, no. of studies | |
U.S. Northeast | 15 |
U.S. Southeast | 4 |
U.S. Midwest | 3 |
U.S. Northwest | 6 |
U.S. Southwest | 3 |
| |
Institution(s), % | |
Public university | 61 |
Private university | 36 |
Private university/liberal arts college | 3 |
| |
Size of institution, % | |
Small (<5,000) | 9 |
Medium (5,000-15,000) | 19 |
Large (>15,000) | 72 |
| |
Design and Measurement | |
| |
Randomized controlled trial, % | 68 |
| |
Provided incentives, % | 61 |
| |
Follow-up length, % | |
<3 months | 29 |
3 to 5 months | 26 |
≥ 6 months | 45 |
| |
Treatment Characteristics | |
| |
Dose, Mdn (Range) | |
Sessions | 1 (1 - 4) |
Minutes | 60 (5 - 450) |
| |
Delivery method, % | |
FTF | 74 |
CDI | 19 |
Computer-facilitated FTF | 4 |
FTF and CDI | 1 |
Written (facilitator provided) | 1 |
| |
Type of delivery, % | |
Individual | 59 |
Group | 39 |
Both individual and group | 1 |
| |
Group size, Mdn (Range) | 10 (2-19) |
| |
Group composition, % mixed-sex | 74 |
| |
Facilitators, Mdn (Range) | 1 (0 – 6) |
| |
Type of Facilitators, % | |
Peers | 12 |
Paraprofessionals | 18 |
Professionals-in-training | 31 |
Professionals | 0 |
Multiple | 16 |
None | 24 |
| |
Treatment Components | |
| |
Personalized feedback, % | 82 |
| |
Normative comparisons, % | 90 |
| |
Alcohol/BAC education, % | 85 |
| |
Moderation strategies, % | 79 |
| |
Goal-setting, % | 62 |
| |
Challenges to expectancies, % | 60 |
| |
Focus on high-risk situations, % | 44 |
| |
Skills training, % | 29 |
| |
Decisional balance exercise, % | 26 |
| |
Values clarification, % | 10 |
| |
Writing/journaling, % | 9 |
| |
Provided materials, % | |
Generic | 34 |
Tailored | 41 |
| |
Boosters | 6 |
Note. n, number of studies. FTF, face-to-face. CDI, computer-delivered intervention.
Studies were typically conducted at large public universities in the northeast U.S. Of the 8,621 college students mandated to an alcohol program, 65% were men and most were White (85%); the average age was 19 years (range = 18.6 to 20.4). Participants were typically first (58%) or second (27%) year students. Most studies randomized participants to two or more conditions (68%), and reported providing incentives to participants for completing the assessments (61%). On average, participants completed 2 (SD = 2) post-intervention assessments, typically occurring at 13 weeks after intervention but ranging from 0 to 65 weeks.
Interventions were typically delivered during a single one-hour session. Most of the sessions were delivered face-to-face (74%) but some (19%) were delivered via computer or included a computer-based component and/or session (4%). When interventions involved facilitators, 12% used peer facilitators; non-peer facilitators included paraprofessionals (18%), professionals-in-training (31%), or a combination of paraprofessionals, professionals-in training, and professionals (16%). Most interventions were delivered to individuals (59%) but some were delivered in small groups (39%; Mdn = 10 participants, range = 2 to 19); one study used a combination of individual and group sessions. All of the group interventions, but only 59% of the individual interventions, were delivered face-to-face. Less than half (44%) of the interventions used a publicly available intervention protocol (video, computer, or published manual). Content included normative comparisons (90%), alcohol education (85%), personalized feedback on alcohol consumption, alcohol-related problems, or alcohol-related risks (82%), strategies to modify alcohol consumption (79%), goal setting (62%), or expectancy challenge exercises (60%).
Methodological Quality
The studies satisfied an average of 57% (SD = 17%; range = 20% to 85%) of the methodological quality criteria.2 Ratings for the 14 methodological quality items are as follows: (1) 68% of the studies randomized participants to treatment and/or control conditions, (2) 100% of the studies used a pre/post-test design, (3) 100% controlled the quality of the study by standardizing intervention delivery (using a manual and/or systematic training, or delivery by computer), (4) 45% had post-intervention assessments of 12 months or longer, (5) 50% of the studies reported ≥85% of their samples completed follow-ups, (6) 3% reported that the study was anonymous, (7) 87% used methods to minimize reactivity of measure completion (e.g., using different personnel for intervention and measurement), (8) 3% used objective measures (e.g., medical records, breathalyzer) in their assessments, (9) 3% used collateral verification, (10) 48% used assessment methods (e.g., interviewers, computerized questionnaires) that were blind to the group assignment, (11) 75% of studies reported on the students who withdrew from the study, (12) 55% included those lost to follow-up in outcome reporting (e.g., intent-to-treat analyses), (13) 100% of the studies reported acceptable statistical analyses for the study design (e.g., assessed groups for differences at baseline), and (14) 3% reported conducting the study at more than one site.
Efficacy of the Alcohol Interventions Compared to Wait-List Controls
Weighted mean ES (d+) for alcohol consumption and alcohol-related problems between the intervention and wait-list controls are presented in Table 2. Mandated students who received an active alcohol intervention reduced their quantity of drinking (d+ = 0.13, 95% CI = 0.02, 0.25), peak BAC (d+ = 0.20, 95% CI = 0.06, 0.33), and typical BAC (d+ = 0.16, 95% CI = 0.01, 0.31) at the short-term assessment compared to wait-list controls. There were no differences between the intervention and wait-list controls on quantity of drinking during specific intervals/drinking days, peak consumption, frequency of drinking days or heavy drinking, and alcohol-related problems. The null hypothesis of homogeneity was confirmed for all outcomes (i.e., non-significant Q). I2 ranged from 0% to 38% (low to moderate); however uncertainty limits were wide for many outcomes and exceeded the 50% threshold. These analyses suggest that some heterogeneity between ES estimates may exist but our ability to test for heterogeneity is limited due to the small number of included studies (Ioannidis et al., 2007).
Efficacy of the Alcohol Interventions over Time
Table 3 displays the weighted mean ES (d+) for the change in alcohol consumption and alcohol-related problems at the short-term, intermediate, and long-term assessment intervals. Overall, students who participated in a mandated intervention reduced their alcohol consumption (drinks per week/month, drinks per drinking interval, peak consumption, frequency of drinking days and heavy drinking, typical and peak BAC) and alcohol-related problems at the short-term assessment (d+ = 0.14 to 0.27). The hypothesis of homogeneity was rejected for all outcomes except for the frequency of heavy drinking (Q [22] = 20.58, p = .485, I2 = 0, 95% UI = 0, 0).I2 ranged from 30% to 75% (low to high), however uncertainty limits were wide for many outcomes and exceeded the 50% threshold. We examined whether a priori determined sample or intervention characteristics explained the variability in the ES estimates (see below).
Changes in alcohol consumption (frequency of heavy drinking, typical and peak BAC) and alcohol-related problems were also found at the intermediate assessment (d+ = 0.13 to 0.25) but only a single outcome (i.e., typical BAC) was found to be significant at the long-term assessment (d+ = 0.12, 95% CI = 0.01, 0.25, k = 10). The hypothesis of homogeneity was rejected for all of these outcomes except for the frequency of heavy drinking at the intermediate assessment (Q [18] = 15.39, p = .567, I2 = 0, 95% UI = 0, 59) and typical BAC at the long-term assessment (Q [10] = 9.83, p = .364, I2 = 8, 95% UI = 42, 84). I2 ranged from 0% to 79% (low to high) but the uncertainty limits were wide for the frequency of heavy drinking (intermediate assessment) and typical BAC (long-term assessment), exceeding the 50% threshold.
Predictors of the Pre- to Post-test Changes in Alcohol Outcomes at Short-Term Assessment
Univariate analyses, using mixed-effects models, were conducted to examine a priori selected predictors of change in alcohol consumption and alcohol-related problems at the short-term assessment only (within-group contrasts). We restricted our analyses to the short-term assessment because this interval contained the largest number of studies available for multiple predictor tests. Table 4 presents the 10 predictors, with results from the univariate regression analyses; these are organized below by outcome. P-values were adjusted based on permutation tests with 5,000 permutations (Harbord & Higgins, 2008). We controlled for methodological quality in testing predictors of change in the frequency of heavy drinking and alcohol-related problems.
Table 4.
Quantity, per week/month | Quantity, Specific Interval | Peak Consumption | Drinking Days | Heavy Drinking | Peak BAC | Typical BAC | Alcohol Problems | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sample Characteristics | B (SE) | p | B (SE) | p | B (SE) | p | B (SE) | p | B (SE) | p | B (SE) | p | B (SE) | p | B (SE) | p |
Sex | .00 (.09) | .98 | -.11 (.09) | .25 | .13 (.15) | .40 | -.13 (.16) | .43 | -.09 (.08) | .28 | -.15 (.12) | .21 | -.10 (.11) | .39 | .05 (.08) | .51 |
Intervention Delivery | ||||||||||||||||
Individual vs. group | .04 (.07) | .54 | -.09 (.06) | .17 | -.04 (.11) | .75 | -.09 (.11) | .41 | -- | -- | -.01 (.15) | .96 | -.04 (.17) | .80 | -.03 (.05) | .57 |
FTF vs. CDI | .04 (.07) | .50 | .14 (.06) | .03 | .03 (.12) | .78 | -.11 (.14) | .45 | -.10 (.06) | .11 | .10 (.08) | .26 | .08 (.08) | .29 | .04 (.06) | .53 |
Duration in minutes | -.00 (.00) | .50 | .00 (.00) | .89 | .00 (.00) | .50 | -.00 (.00) | .97 | .00 (.00) | .83 | .00 (.00) | .14 | .00 (.00) | .10 | .00 (.00) | .56 |
Intervention Components | ||||||||||||||||
Alcohol education | -.31 (.12) | .01 | -.42 (.10) | <.01 | -.03 (.14) | .84 | -.43 (.16) | .01 | -- | -- | .31 (.19) | .12 | .18 (.19) | .35 | -.22 (.09) | .02 |
Personalized feedback | .06 (.09) | .49 | .14 (.11) | .24 | .24 (.16) | .16 | .18 (.24) | .47 | .05 (.20) | .79 | -.02 (.24) | .94 | .12 (.20) | .57 | .16 (.07) | .03 |
Moderation strategies | -.09 (.09) | .33 | -.11 (.10) | .30 | .00 (.12) | .99 | .13 (.17) | .46 | -- | -- | .31 (.19) | .12 | .18 (.19) | .35 | -.01 (.07) | .94 |
Goal-setting | -.01 (.07) | .84 | .14 (.07) | .06 | .18 (.11) | .12 | .25 (.12) | .04 | .01 (.06) | .90 | .14 (.09) | .14 | .13 (.13) | .34 | .13 (.05) | .02 |
Expectancy challenges | -.07 (.06) | .25 | -.00 (.07) | .96 | .14 (.11) | .24 | .26 (.12) | .04 | .01 (.06) | .87 | -.02 (.09) | .82 | -.06 (.09) | .48 | .03 (.06) | .57 |
Identifying risk situations | -.01 (.06) | .91 | .10 (.06) | .10 | .14 (.10) | .20 | .18 (.10) | .09 | .03 (.05) | .53 | -.05 (.08) | .54 | -.02 (.08) | .80 | .06 (.05) | .24 |
Note. FTF = face-to-face. CDI = computer delivered interventions. Reported coefficients are unstandardized. Predictors that were significant (mixed-effects models) are in bold. Predictors that remained significant using the adjusted p-values obtained from the permutation tests (5,000 permutations) are indicated by underline.
Quantity of alcohol consumed
Less change in the quantity of alcohol consumed was observed for interventions providing alcohol education (B = -0.31, SE = .12, p = .01), accounting for 44% of the between-study variance, but this variable did not retain significance when adjusted for multiple univariate tests (adjusted p-value = .231). All other tests of predictors were non-significant (adjusted ps >.994).
Quantity of alcohol consumed during specific intervals
Alcohol consumption during specific intervals (e.g., drinking days) reduced significantly if the intervention was delivered face-to-face (vs. computer; B = 0.14 SE = 0.06, p=.03) and the intervention content did not emphasize alcohol education (B = -0.42, SE = .10, p<.01), but only alcohol education retained significance when adjusting for multiple univariate tests (adjusted p-value = .005). All other tests for predictors were non-significant (ps >.398). Delivery mode and alcohol education were simultaneously entered into a regression analysis; only (lack of) alcohol education (B = -0.39, SE = 0.10, p< .001; I2Residual = 0%) remained a significant predictor of change in the quantity of alcohol consumed during specific intervals, accounting for 100% of the between-study variance. Alcohol education retained significance when adjusting for multiple meta-regression (adjusted p-value = .003).
Peak consumption
No significant predictors of change in peak consumption were found (adjusted p-values >.736).
Drinking days
Interventions were associated with reduced drinking frequency when they did not provide alcohol education (B = -.43, SE = .16, p = .01), they encouraged goal setting (B = .25, SE = .12, p = .04),and they challenged expectancies (B = 0.25, SE = 0.12, p=.04); none of these pre reached significance when they were adjusted for multiple univariate testing (ps = .185, .071, and .431, respectively). These variables were simultaneously entered into a multiple regression analysis. Only alcohol education (B =-0.36, p<.02; I2Residual = 54%) remained a significant predictor of change in the frequency of drinking days, and accounted for 34% of the between-study variance (model testing alcohol education alone; adjusted R2 = 56% for the full model). Alcohol education did not reach statistical significance when the p-value was adjusted for multiple meta-regression (adjusted p-value = .085).
Heavy drinking frequency
No significant predictors of change in heavy drinking frequency were found (adjusted p-values >.444).
Peak and Typical BAC
No significant predictors of change in peak BAC (adjusted p-values >.717) or typical BAC (adjusted p-values >.607) were found.
Alcohol-related problems
Reductions in students' alcohol-related problems were greater when interventions included personalized feedback (B = 0.16, SE = 0.09, p=.03) and goal-setting (B = 0.13, SE = 0.05, p=.02), and when they did not include alcohol education (B = -0.22, SE = 0.09, p = .02) but these variables did not retain significance when adjusting for multiple univariate testing (adjusted p-values = .954, .714, and .142, respectively). These variables were simultaneously entered into a regression analysis with goal-setting remaining a significant predictor of the change in alcohol-related problems (B = 0.13, SE = .06, p = .03, I2 Residual = 10%), and accounted for 65% of the between-study variance (adjusted R2 = 80% for the full model). Goal-setting did not reach statistical significance when the p-value was adjusted for multiple meta-regression (adjusted p-value = .090).
Exploratory Analyses: Intervention Protocols
Interventions protocols that have published manuals for use by campus staff or that are commercially available as intact interventions are particularly useful, as these features facilitate dissemination. These can be contrasted to interventions that are available primarily in research contexts and are not easily disseminated in practice settings. Four intervention protocols meet these criteria (a published manual or a commercial available product) and have been evaluated in at least two studies, which allows for ES estimation (i.e., BASICS, e-CHUG, Alcohol 101, and Alcohol Skills Training Program). Stratified analyses examined the weighted mean ES for quantity of alcohol consumed and alcohol-related problems (the two most commonly reported outcomes) by specific intervention protocols at the short-term assessment. Mixed-effects models, using maximum likelihood estimates, were used. Interventions based on BASICS (d+ = 0.26, 95% CI = 0.17,0.50; k = 14), Alcohol 101 (d+ = 0.15, 95% CI = 0.06,0.24; k = 11), and e-CHUG (d+ = 0.41, 95% CI = 0.05,0.77; k = 2) achieved significant reductions in alcohol consumption (Figure 2), QB (3) = 13.90, p<.001. Significant short-term improvements were associated with all four of the intervention protocols for alcohol-related problems (Figure 2): BASICS (d+ = 0.31, 95% CI = 0.21,0.41; k = 14), Alcohol 101 (d+ = 0.14, 95% CI = 0.03,0.25; k = 11), e-CHUG (d+ = 0.39, 95% CI = 0.13,0.65; k = 3), and the Alcohol Skills Training Program (d+ = 0.36, 95% CI = 0.16,0.55; k = 3), QB (3) = 12.89, p =.005.
Publication Bias
Both published and unpublished manuscripts were included in this meta-analysis to reduce the possibility of publication bias; nonetheless, we also examined our data using graphical and statistical tools to test for the possibility of publication bias. (Funnel plots and results of the statistical tests are available in the Supplemental Online Figures A1-A27 and Table B.) Inspection of the funnel plots revealed some asymmetries that might be interpreted as publication bias (e.g., positive skew of data for quantity of alcohol consumed). Results for Begg's test (Begg & Mazumdar, 1994) were non-significant for all outcomes except for alcohol-related problems at the short-term interval (p = .02). Trim-and-fill procedures (Duval & Tweedie, 2000) were used to estimate and correct for the possibility of missing studies. These analyses found that six studies measuring the quantity of alcohol consumption, four studies measuring the quantity of alcohol consumed at specific intervals, four studies measuring the frequency of heavy drinking, six studies measuring peak BAC, and fourteen studies measuring alcohol-related problems are estimated to be missing at the short-term assessment. The random-effect weighted mean ES would be similar in magnitude to the estimate mean ES from the original dataset for all of the listed outcomes. The findings from the trim-and-fill measures should be interpreted with caution as performance of this method is poor when significant heterogeneity is present (Lau, Ioannidis, Terrin, Schmid, & Olkin, 2006; Sutton, 2009; Terrin, Schmid, Lau, & Olkin, 2003).
Discussion
This meta-analysis summarizes the efficacy of alcohol interventions for college students who were mandated to participate after violating campus alcohol policy. Our analyses showed that mandated interventions are associated with reductions in alcohol use and related problems across a range of measured variables. Reductions across all reported outcomes were observed at follow-ups up to 3 months, however, the magnitude and number of significant reductions diminished over longer assessment intervals.
Only five studies compared outcomes of active alcohol interventions to a pure (i.e., no intervention or wait list) control; evidence from these trials support the efficacy of providing an alcohol intervention even when it is mandated. Finding small effects on selected outcomes when comparing alcohol interventions to wait-list controls is consistent with observations that the process of being sanctioned can prompt self-initiated change even in the absence of an active intervention (e.g.,(Carey, Henson, Carey, & Maisto, 2009; Hustad et al., 2011; Morgan, White, & Mun, 2008). However, even if students waiting for an intervention reduce how much they drink, the active intervention produced larger reductions in standard drinks consumed and average and peak BACs. Therefore, these findings confirm the additional benefit of offering an alcohol intervention to students who have been sanctioned for alcohol violations.
These data provide empirical validation for use of alcohol risk reduction interventions as part of institutional sanctions for those who violate campus alcohol policies (U.S. Department of Education, 2006). Finding that mandated alcohol prevention programs reduce alcohol use among college students corroborates observations from community-based treatment studies that mandatory treatment results in positive outcomes (Kelly, Finney, & Moos, 2005; Yeterian, Greene, Bergman, & Kelly, 2013). Thus, enforcement of campus alcohol policy is an opportunity to reach students who can benefit from an alcohol risk reduction interventions.
Given the large number of within-group intervention effects, many of them coming from randomized controlled trials that evaluated multiple active interventions, we supplemented our analyses with tests of change over time. The observed effects of these mandated interventions are small but are consistent with other meta-analyses evaluating within-group changes in alcohol-related outcomes for college drinkers (Carey, Scott-Sheldon, et al., 2009; Samson & Tanner-Smith, 2015; Scott-Sheldon, Terry, Carey, Garey, & Carey, 2012).Given that most campuses deliver some intervention to students violating alcohol policy, if such evidence-based interventions were to be applied widely, they could have significant public health impact. It is notable that most within-group effects were heterogeneous, meaning that there was variability across active interventions in the amount of change observed. Variability in outcomes was explained, in part, using characteristics of the interventions.
Our predictor analyses identified intervention components associated with stronger effects. Most consistently, we found that an explicit focus on alcohol education may be counterproductive. The presence of this component was associated with less change on several outcomes, consistent with prior reviews of that have concluded that information or educational interventions do not change drinking behavior (e.g., Larimer & Cronce, 2007). This finding is notable given that alcohol education groups are the most commonly employed sanction (Anderson & Gadaleto, 2006).
In contrast, several of the other intervention components were associated with larger reductions in drinking. Interventions containing goal setting and expectancy challenges were associated with greater reduction in drinking frequency. Interventions containing personalized feedback and goal setting were associated with fewer alcohol problems. Goal setting is an established self-management strategy known to facilitate behavior change (Hester, 1995; Locke & Latham, 2002). Personalized feedback can raise awareness for students who do not realize that they are accumulating alcohol-related problems, consistent with the Transtheoretical Model (Prochaska, DiClemente, & Norcross, 1992). In addition, expectancy challenge interventions have been shown to reduce drinking behavior in the short-term in general college samples (Labbe & Maisto, 2011; Scott-Sheldon et al., 2012).
Several of the hypothesized predictors of change were not supported. With regard to intervention length, all but one intervention was delivered in 1 or 2 sessions. Although alcohol risk reduction may be achieved by relatively short interventions (cf.(Samson & Tanner-Smith, 2015), this review could not fully evaluate the potential effects of more intensive multi-session interventions. Evaluations of multiple-session interventions are needed to test whether longer interventions could optimize outcomes for mandated students.
We also did not find strong evidence for the superiority of face-to-face interventions. Because mandated students are more likely to drink more and express defensiveness regarding the circumstances of their referral (Barnett et al., 2004; Palmer et al., 2010), an intervention delivered by a skilled facilitator may be more engaging than a computer-delivered intervention. However, delivery mode was not consistently associated with intervention effects. Furthermore, few trials have evaluated some of the promising computer-delivered interventions (e.g., e-CHUG), indicating the need for additional investigation of the relative value of face-to-face versus computer delivery with mandated students.
This quantitative review updates the qualitative review of Barnett and Read (2005), which summarized 16 studies of mandated alcohol interventions. Our larger sample of 31 studies reflects the growth of the field with more controlled outcome studies, larger samples, more evaluations of different intervention modalities, more detailed descriptions of intervention components, and longer follow-ups. The intervention literature now contains many examples of comparative efficacy of different interventions. Despite these improvements, few comparisons have been replicated, limiting our ability to compare one specific intervention to another. A growing number of studies now support the efficacy of commercially-available intervention protocols, including both face-to-face and computer-delivered modalities. The absence of cost-effectiveness analyses remains a glaring gap in the literature.
To advance the field, we make the following suggestions for future research. First, given the evidence that mandated interventions help to change behavior, systematic exploration is needed to determine which interventions optimize outcomes and for whom (Kraemer, Frank, & Kupfer, 2006). Heterogeneity in ES suggests that differences in efficacy exist among interventions or in the response of certain individuals to a given intervention, and these predictors of response need to be identified. Second, systematic exploration of the efficacy of intervention components is warranted. The predictors of change that were identified in this meta-analysis can guide the design and evaluation of comparative efficacy trials that test optimal combinations of intervention components. Third, given the decay of ES over time, future studies should evaluate booster sessions or other strategies to enhance maintenance, and trials need to incorporate both short-term and longer-term follow-ups to evaluate the maintenance of intervention gains.
We acknowledge several limitations of this meta-analysis. First, the small number of no-treatment or waitlist control groups in studies of mandated students constrained between-groups analyses. Thus, our conclusions are limited to establishing that any intervention produces better outcomes than no intervention; as the literature matures, it will be important to test which interventions are more efficacious. Second, alternative interpretations of the observed within-group change cannot be ruled out; these include short-term self-initiated change after the sanction event (Carey, Henson, et al., 2009; Hustad et al., 2011; Morgan et al., 2008), maturation, or regression to the mean (Campbell & Stanley, 1963). We note, however, that regression to the mean cannot account for the observed associations of ES with intervention content (e.g., goal setting). Therefore, not all of the observed changes can be explained by temporal or statistical artifact. Third, intervention components are often delivered in combination, making it difficult to discern which components or combination might explain outcomes. We encourage the use of dismantling designs to confirm if the components identified by this review are the active ingredients of change. Fourth, our analyses of predictors of change should not be interpreted as moderator analyses; we identified correlates of the degree of change associated with participation in an alcohol intervention, not differential responsiveness to one intervention or another. Fifth, missing data, a common problem in meta-analysis (Cooper & Hedges, 2009), limited our ability to fully test predictors of interest. Finally, we acknowledge the limitations of the primary level studies included in this meta-analysis; these limitations include not providing the reasons for mandate, predominance of brief interventions, limited descriptions of intervention content, brief follow-ups, reliance on self-report assessment, and limited reporting of non-drinking outcomes (e.g., retention or additional alcohol violations).
In summary, mandated alcohol interventions for students who violate campus alcohol policy reduce alcohol-related risk relative to no intervention. Participation in a mandated intervention is associated with lower alcohol consumption and fewer alcohol-related problems. Change over time was seen across most consumption and problems measures, indicating that the process of participating in mandated interventions has a broad impact on drinking. However, intervention-related gains weakened over time, with almost no benefit apparent one year later. This meta-analysis revealed that certain components were associated with better outcomes (i.e., goal setting, personalized feedback, expectancy challenge) whereas alcohol education was associated with poorer outcomes. Future intervention research might identify which interventions and components optimize outcomes for mandated students, develop strategies to maintain initial gains, evaluate interventions of varying intensity, and provide information regarding cost-effectiveness.
Supplementary Material
Public Health Significance.
Providing mandated interventions to college students who have violated alcohol policy has significant but modest effects on reducing drinking and alcohol-related problems.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported in part by NIH grants R01AA012518 (PI: K. Carey) and T32DA031099 (PI: D. Hasin).
Footnotes
Only studies available through December 2012 were included; however, a single study (Terlecki, 2011) was subsequently published (Terlecki, Buckner, Larimer, & Copeland, 2015) and the content coding for this study was updated to reflect the new publication date.
Methodological quality was correlated with three alcohol outcome (all within-group contrasts): (a) frequency of heavy drinking at short-term assessment (r = -0.48, p = .02), (b) frequency of heavy drinking at intermediate assessment (r = 0.53, p = .03), and (c) alcohol-related problems at the short-term assessment (r = -0.34, p = .02). Therefore, we controlled for methodological quality in our meta-regression analyses predicting change for the frequency of heavy drinking and alcohol-related problems at the short-term assessment.
References
- Alfonso J. PhD Dissertation. University of Central Florida; 2008. Feedback-based alcohol interventions for mandated students: A comparison of individual, group, and electronic formats. [Google Scholar]
- Alfonso J, Hall TV, Dunn ME. Feedback-based alcohol interventions for mandated students: An effectiveness study of three modalities. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy. 2013;20(5):411–423. doi: 10.1002/cpp.1786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allison DL. PhD Dissertation. Texas A&M University; Commerce, TX: 2002. Comparable effects of the use of a computer-assisted, facilitative teaching approach to a traditional, lecture teaching approach on college students' learning about and subsequent use of alcohol. [Google Scholar]
- Amaro H, Ahl M, Matsumoto A, Prado G, Mule C, Kemmemer A, et al. Mantella P. Trial of the university assistance program for alcohol use among mandated students. Journal on Studies of Alcohol and Drugs Supplement. 2009;(16):45–56. doi: 10.15288/jsads.2009.s16.45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American College Health Association. American College Health Association-National College Health Assessment II: Undergraduate reference group exectutive summary Spring 2012. Hanover, MD: American College Health Association; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Anderson DS, Gadaleto AF. Results of the 2006 College Alcohol Survey. 2006 Retrieved February 5, 2014, from http://www.caph.gmu.edu/files/CAS/2006%20College%20Alcohol%20Survey%20Results.pdf.
- Arnett JJ. The Developmental Context of Substance Use in Emerging Adulthood. Journal of Drug Issues. 2005;35:235–254. [Google Scholar]
- Barnett NP, Borsari B, Hustad JT, Tevyaw TO, Colby SM, Kahler CW, Monti PM. Profiles of college students mandated to alcohol intervention. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2008;69:684–694. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2008.69.684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnett NP, Murphy JG, Colby SM, Monti PM. Efficacy of counselor vs. computer-delivered intervention with mandated college students. Addictive Behaviors. 2007;32(11):2529–2548. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.06.017. doi:S0306-4603(07)00188-8[pii]10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.06.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnett NP, Read JP. Mandatory alcohol intervention for alcohol-abusing college students: a systematic review. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 2005;29(2):147–158. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2005.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnett NP, Tevyaw TO, Fromme K, Borsari B, Carey KB, Corbin WR, Colby SM, Monti PM. Brief alcohol interventions with mandated or adjudicated college students. Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research. 2004;28(6):966–975. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000128231.97817.c7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Becker BJ. Synthesizing standardized mean-change measures. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology. 1988;41(2):257–278. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8317.1988.tb00901.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Begg CB, Mazumdar M. Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics. 1994:1088–1101. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JPT, Rothstein HR. Complex Data Structures Introduction to Meta-Analysis. West Sussex, U.K.: John Wiley & Sons; 2009. pp. 215–245. [Google Scholar]
- Borsari B. PhD Dissertation. ProQuest Information & Learning, US; 2003. Two brief alcohol interventions for referred college students. Retrieved from http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2003-95016-119&site=ehost-live Available from EBSCOhost psyh database. [Google Scholar]
- Borsari B, Carey KB. Two brief alcohol interventions for mandated college students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2005;19(3):296–302. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.19.3.296. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borsari B, Hustad JTP, Mastroleo NR, Tevyaw TOL, Barnett NP, Kahler CW, Short EE, Monti PM. Addressing alcohol use and problems in mandated college students: A randomized clinical trial using stepped care. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2012;80(6):1062–1074. doi: 10.1037/a0029902. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borsari B, Tevyaw TOL, Barnett NP, Kahler CW, Monti PM. Stepped care for mandated college students: A pilot study. American Journal on Addictions. 2007;16(2):131–137. doi: 10.1080/10550490601184498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campbell DT, Stanley JC. Experimental and Quasi-Experimental Designs for Research. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin; 1963. [Google Scholar]
- Carey KB, Carey MP, Henson JM, Maisto SA, DeMartini KS. Brief alcohol interventions for mandated college students: comparison of face-to-face counseling and computer-delivered interventions. Addiction. 2011;106(3):528–537. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03193.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carey KB, DeMartini KS. The motivational context for mandated alcohol interventions for college students by gender and family history. Addictive Behaviors. 2010;35:218–223. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2009.10.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carey KB, DeMartini KS, Prince MA, Luteran C, Carey MP. Effects of choice on intervention outcomes for college students sanctioned for campus alcohol policy violations. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2013;27(3):596–603. doi: 10.1037/a0030333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carey KB, Henson JM, Carey MP, Maisto SA. Computer versus in-person intervention for students violating campus alcohol policy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2009;77(1):74–87. doi: 10.1037/a0014281. doi:2009-00563-016[pii]10.1037/a0014281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carey KB, Henson JM, Carey MP, Maisto SA. Perceived norms mediate effects of a brief motivational intervention for sanctioned college drinkers. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 2010;17:58–71. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2850.2009.01194.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carey KB, McClurg AJ, Bolles JR, Hubbell SJ, Will HA, Carey MP. College student drinking and ambulance utilization. Journal of Public Health Management and Practice. 2009;15(6):524–528. doi: 10.1097/PHH.0b013e3181a5d279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carey KB, Scott-Sheldon LAJ, Carey MP, DeMartini KS. Individual-level interventions to reduce college student drinking: a meta-analytic review. Addictive Behaviors. 2007;32(11):2469–2494. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carey KB, Scott-Sheldon LAJ, Elliott JC, Bolles JR, Carey MP. Computer-delivered interventions to reduce college student drinking: a meta-analysis. Addiction. 2009;104(11):1807–1819. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02691.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carey KB, Scott-Sheldon LAJ, Elliott JC, Garey L, Carey MP. Face-to-face versus computer-delivered alcohol interventions for college drinkers: a meta-analytic review, 1998 to 2010. Clinical Psychology Review. 2012;32(8):690–703. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cimini MD, Martens MP, Larimer ME, Kilmer JR, Neighbors C, Monserrat JM. Assessing the effectiveness of peer-facilitated interventions addressing high-risk drinking among judicially mandated college students. Journal on Studies of Alcohol and Drugs Supplement. 2009;(16):57–66. doi: 10.15288/jsads.2009.s16.57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper H, Hedges LV. Potentials and limitations. In: Cooper H, Hedges LV, Valentine JC, editors. The Handbook of Research Synthesis and Meta-Analysis. 2nd. New York: Sage; 2009. pp. 561–572. [Google Scholar]
- Cronce JM, Larimer ME. Individual-focused approaches to the prevention of college student drinking. Alcohol Research and Health. 2011;34(2):210–221. doi:SPS-AR&H-33. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Difulvio GT, Linowski SA, Mazziotti JS, Puleo E. Effectiveness of the Brief Alcohol and Screening Intervention for College Students (BASICS) program with a mandated population. Journal of American College Health. 2012;60(4):269–280. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2011.599352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dimeff LA, Baer JS, Kivlahan DR, Marlatt GA. Brief alcohol screening and interventions for college students: A harm reduction approach. New York: Guilford Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Doumas DM, McKinley LL, Book P. Evaluation of two web-based alcohol interventions for mandated college students. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 2009;36(1):65–74. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2008.05.009. doi:S0740-5472(08)00084-6[pii]10.1016/j.jsat.2008.05.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doumas DM, Workman C, Smith D, Navarro A. Reducing high-risk drinking in mandated college students: evaluation of two personalized normative feedback interventions. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 2011;40(4):376–385. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2010.12.006. doi:S0740-5472(11)00007-9[pii]10.1016/j.jsat.2010.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doumas DM, Workman CR, Navarro A, Smith D. Evaluation of web-based and counselor-delivered feedback interventions for mandated college students. Journal of Addictions & Offender Counseling. 2011;32(1-2):16–28. doi: 10.1002/j.2161-1874.2011.tb00204.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Duval S, Tweedie R. Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics. 2000;56(2):455–463. doi: 10.1111/j.0006-341x.2000.00455.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Emerson JD, Strenio J. Boxplots and batch comparisons. In: Hoaglin DC, Mosteller F, Tukey JW, editors. Understanding Robust and Exploratory Data Analysis. New York: Wiley; 1983. pp. 58–96. [Google Scholar]
- Fachini A, Aliane PP, Martinez EZ, Furtado EF. Efficacy of brief alcohol screening intervention for college students (BASICS): a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy. 2012;7:40. doi: 10.1186/1747-597X-7-40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischer Potts KA. Efficacy of the alcohol skills training program, a brief group intervention for high-risk college student drinkers. 63. ProQuest Information & Learning, US; 2002. Retrieved from http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2002-95024-161&site=ehost-live Available from EBSCOhost psyh database. [Google Scholar]
- Flynn CA, Brown WE. The effects of a mandatory alcohol education program on college student problem drinkers. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education. 1991;37(1):15–24. [Google Scholar]
- Fromme K, Corbin W. Prevention of heavy drinking and associated negative consequences among mandated and voluntary college students. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2004;72(6):1038–1049. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.6.1038. doi:2004-21587-014[pii]10.1037/0022-006X.72.6.1038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gebhardt TL, Kaphingst K, DeJong W. A campus-community coalition to control alcohol-related problems off campus: an environmental management case study. Journal of American College Health. 2000;48(5):211–215. doi: 10.1080/07448480009599306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harbord RM, Higgins JPT. Meta-regression in Stata. Stata Journal. 2008;8(4):493–519. [Google Scholar]
- Hedges LV. Distribution theory for Glass's estimator of effect size and related estimators. Journal of Education Statistics. 1981;6:107–128. [Google Scholar]
- Henson JM, Pearson MR, Carey KB. Defining and characterizing differences in college alcohol intervention efficacy: A growth mixture modeling application. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2015;83(2):370–381. doi: 10.1037/a0038897. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hester RK. Behavioral self-control training. In: Hester RK, Miller WR, editors. Handbook of alcoholism treatment approaches: Effective alternatives. 2nd. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon; 1995. pp. 148–159. [Google Scholar]
- Higgins JPT, Green S. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions The Cochrane Collaboration. 2011 Retrieved from http://handbook.cochrane.org/
- Higgins JPT, Thompson SG. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Statistics in Medicine. 2002;21(11):1539–1558. doi: 10.1002/sim.1186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higgins JPT, Thompson SG. Controlling the risk of spurious findings from meta-regression. Statistics in Medicine. 2004;23(11):1663–1682. doi: 10.1002/sim.1752. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higgins JPT, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. British Medical Journal. 2003;327(7414):557–560. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hingson RW, Zha W, Weitzman ER. Magnitude of and trends in alcohol-related mortality and morbidity among U.S. college students ages 18-24, 1998-2005. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2009;(16):12–20. doi: 10.15288/jsads.2009.s16.12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huedo-Medina TB, Sanchez-Meca J, Marin-Martinez F, Botella J. Assessing heterogeneity in meta-analysis: Q statistic or I2 index? Psychological Methods. 2006;11(2):193–206. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.11.2.193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hustad JT, Eaton Short E, Borsari B, Barnett NP, O'Leary Tevyaw T, Kahler CW. College alcohol citations result in modest reductions in student drinking. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 2011;40(3):281–286. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2010.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ioannidis JPA, Patsopoulos NA, Evangelou E. Uncertainty in heterogeneity estimates in meta-analyses. 2007;335 doi: 10.1136/bmj.39343.408449.80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jadad AR, Moore RA, Carroll D, Jenkinson C, Reynolds DJ, Gavaghan DJ, McQuay HJ. Assessing the quality of reports of randomized clinical trials: is blinding necessary? Controlled Clinical Trials. 1996;17(1):1–12. doi: 10.1016/0197-2456(95)00134-4. doi:0197-2456(95)00134-4[pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson BT, Huedo-Medina TB. AHRQ Methods for Effective Health Care Meta-Analytic Statistical Inferences for Continuous Measure Outcomes as a Function of Effect Size Metric and Other Assumptions. Rockville (MD): Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (US); 2013. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson LD, O'Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE. Monitoring the Future national survey results on drug use, 1975-2011. II. Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research, The University of Michigan; 2012. p. 314. [Google Scholar]
- Keillor RM, Perkins WB, Horan JJ. Effects of videotaped expectancy challenges on alcohol consumption of adjudicated students. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy. 1999;13(3):179–187. [Google Scholar]
- Kelly JF, Finney JW, Moos R. Substance use disorder patients who are mandated to treatment: characteristics, treatment process, and 1- and 5-year outcomes. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 2005;28(3):213–223. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2004.10.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kraemer HC, Frank E, Kupfer DJ. Moderators of treatment outcomes: clinical, research, and policy importance. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2006;296(10):1286–1289. doi: 10.1001/jama.296.10.1286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Labbe AK, Maisto SA. Alcohol expectancy challenges for college students: a narrative review. Clinical Psychology Review. 2011;31(4):673–683. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LaBrie JW, Lamb TF, Pedersen ER, Quinlan T. A group motivational interviewing intervention reduces drinking and alcohol-related consequences in adjudicated college students. Journal of College Student Development. 2006;47(3):267–280. [Google Scholar]
- LaBrie JW, Thompson AD, Huchting K, Lac A, Buckley K. A group Motivational Interviewing intervention reduces drinking and alcohol-related negative consequences in adjudicated college women. Addictive Behaviors. 2007;32(11):2549–2562. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.05.014. doi:S0306-4603(07)00155-4[pii]10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.05.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LaChance H. Group motivational intervention for underage college student drinkers in mandated university-based programming. 65. ProQuest Information & Learning, US; 2004. Retrieved from http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2004-99024-300&site=ehost-live Available from EBSCOhost psyh database. [Google Scholar]
- LaChance H, Feldstein Ewing SW, Bryan AD, Hutchison KE. What makes group MET work? A randomized controlled trial of college student drinkers in mandated alcohol diversion. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2009;23(4):598–612. doi: 10.1037/a0016633. doi:2009-24023-006[pii]10.1037/a0016633. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Landis JR, Koch GG. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics. 1977;33(1):159–174. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larimer ME, Cronce JM. Identification, prevention, and treatment revisited: individual-focused college drinking prevention strategies 1999-2006. Addictive Behaviors. 2007;32(11):2439–2468. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lau J, Ioannidis JP, Terrin N, Schmid CH, Olkin I. The case of the misleading funnel plot. British Medical Journal. 2006;333(7568):597–600. doi: 10.1136/bmj.333.7568.597. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipsey MW, Wilson DB. Practical Meta-Analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Locke EA, Latham GP. Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task motivation: A 35-year odyssey. American Psychologist. 2002;57(9):705–717. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.57.9.705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mastroleo NR, Murphy JG, Colby SM, Monti PM, Barnett NP. Incident-specific and individual-level moderators of brief intervention effects with mandated college students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2011;25(4):616–624. doi: 10.1037/a0024508. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merrill JE, Carey KB, Lust SA, Kalichman SC, Carey MP. Do students mandated to intervention for campus alcohol-related violations drink more than nonmandated students? Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2014;28(4):1265–1270. doi: 10.1037/a0037710. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller WR, Brown JM, Simpson TL, Handmaker NS, Bien TH, Luckie LF. What works? A methodological analysis of the alcohol treatment outcome literature. In: Hester RK, Miller WR, editors. Handbook of alcoholism treatment approaches: Effective alternatives. 2nd. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon; 1995. pp. 12–44. [Google Scholar]
- Morgan TJ, White HR, Mun EY. Changes in drinking before a mandated brief intervention with college students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2008;69(2):286–290. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2008.69.286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris SB, DeShon RP. Combining effect size estimates in meta-analysis with repeated measures and independent-groups designs. Psychological Methods. 2002;7(1):105–125. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.7.1.105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy JG, Barnett NP, Colby SM. Alcohol-related and alcohol-free activity participation and enjoyment among college students: a behavioral theories of choice analysis. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2006;14(3):339–349. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.14.3.339. doi:2006-10166-008[pii]10.1037/1064-1297.14.3.339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oswalt SB, Shutt MD, English E, Little SD. Did it work? Examining the impact of an alcohol intervention on sanctioned college students. Journal of College Student Development. 2007;48(5):543–557. [Google Scholar]
- Palmer RS. PhD Dissertation. ProQuest Information & Learning, US; 2004. Efficacy of the alcohol skills training program in mandated and nonmandated heavy drinking college students. Retrieved from http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2004-99022-049&site=ehost-live Available from EBSCOhost psyh database. [Google Scholar]
- Palmer RS, Kilmer JR, Ball SA, Larimer ME. Intervention defensiveness as a moderator of drinking outcome among heavy-drinking mandated college students. Addictive Behaviors. 2010;35(12):1157–1160. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.08.009. doi:S0306-4603(10)00245-5[pii]10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perkins HW. Surveying the damage: a review of research on consequences of alcohol misuse in college populations. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2002;(14):91–100. doi: 10.15288/jsas.2002.s14.91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prochaska JO, DiClemente CC, Norcross JC. In search of how people change. Applications to addictive behaviors. American Psychologist. 1992;47(9):1102–1114. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.47.9.1102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Read JP, Merrill JE, Kahler CW, Strong DR. Predicting functional outcomes among college drinkers: reliability and predictive validity of the Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Questionnaire. Addictive Behaviors. 2007;32(11):2597–2610. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.06.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reed JG, Baxter PM. Using reference databases. In: Cooper H, Hedges LV, Valentine JC, editors. The Handbook of Research Synthesis and Meta-Analysis. 2nd. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 2009. pp. 73–101. [Google Scholar]
- Rosenthal R. The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychological Bulletin. 1979;86:638–641. [Google Scholar]
- Rosenthal R. The Literature Search Stage Meta-Analytic Procedures for Social Research. 2nd. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications, Inc; 1991. pp. 40–77. [Google Scholar]
- Samson JE, Tanner-Smith EE. Single-session alcohol interventions for heavy drinking college students: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2015;76(4):530–543. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2015.76.530. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott-Sheldon LAJ, Carey KB, Elliott JC, Garey L, Carey MP. Efficacy of alcohol interventions for first-year college students: A meta-analytic review of randomized controlled trials. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2014;82:177–188. doi: 10.1037/a0035192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott-Sheldon LAJ, Terry DL, Carey KB, Garey L, Carey MP. Efficacy of expectancy challenge interventions to reduce college student drinking: A meta-analytic review. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2012;26(3):393. doi: 10.1037/a0027565. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- StataCorp. Stata Statistical Software: Release 12.1. College Station, TX: StataCorp, LP; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Sterne JAC, Egger M. Funnel plots for detecting bias in meta-analysis: guidelines on choice of axis. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 2001;54(10):1046–1055. doi: 10.1016/s0895-4356(01)00377-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. Results from the 2012 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: Summary of National Findings, NSDUH Series H-46, HHS Publication No (SMA) 13-4795. Rockville, MD: Substance Abuse and Mental Health Administration; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Sutton AJ. Publication Bias. In: Cooper H, Hedges LV, Valentine JC, editors. The Handbook of Research Synthesis and Meta-Analysis. 2nd. New York: Sage; 2009. pp. 435–452. [Google Scholar]
- Terlecki MA. PhD Dissertation. Louisiana State University; 2011. The long-term effect of brief motivational alcohol intervention for heavy drinking mandated college students. [Google Scholar]
- Terlecki MA, Buckner JD, Larimer ME, Copeland AL. Randomized controlled trial of brief alcohol screening and intervention for college students for heavy-drinking mandated and volunteer undergraduates: 12-month outcomes. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2015;29(1):2–16. doi: 10.1037/adb0000056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Terlecki MA, Larimer ME, Copeland AL. Clinical outcomes of a brief motivational intervention for heavy drinking mandated college students: a pilot study. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2010;71(1):54–60. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2010.71.54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Terrin N, Schmid CH, Lau J, Olkin I. Adjusting for publication bias in the presence of heterogeneity. Statistics in Medicine. 2003;22(13):2113–2126. doi: 10.1002/sim.1461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tevyaw TO, Borsari B, Colby SM, Monti PM. Peer enhancement of a brief motivational intervention with mandated college students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2007;21(1):114–119. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.21.1.114. doi:2007-03272-012[pii]10.1037/0893-164X.21.1.114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- U.S. Department of Education. Complying with the Drug-Free Schools and Campuses Regulations [EDGAR Part 86]: A Guide for University and College Administrators. Washington, DC: Office of Safe and Drug-Free Schools, Higher Education Center for Alcohol and Other Drug Abuse and Violence Prevention; 2006. p. 64. [Google Scholar]
- Walters ST, Gruenewald DA, Miller JH, Bennett ME. Early findings from a disciplinary program to reduce problem drinking by college students. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 2001;20(1):89–91. doi: 10.1016/s0740-5472(00)00142-2. doi:S0740-5472(00)00142-2[pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler H, Dowdall GW, Davenport A, Rimm EB. A gender-specific measure of binge drinking among college students. American Journal of Public Health. 1995;85(7):982–985. doi: 10.2105/ajph.85.7.982. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler H, Lee JE, Nelson TF, Kuo M. Underage college students' drinking behavior, access to alcohol, and the influence of deterrence policies. Findings from the Harvard School of Public Health College Alcohol Study. Journal of American College Health. 2002;50(5):223–236. doi: 10.1080/07448480209595714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White HR, Morgan TJ, Pugh LA, Celinska K, LaBouvie EW, Pandina RJ. Evaluating two brief substance-use interventions for mandated college students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2006;67(2):309–317. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2006.67.309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White HR, Mun EY, Morgan TJ. Do brief personalized feedback interventions work for mandated students or is it just getting caught that works? Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2008;22(1):107–116. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.22.1.107. doi:2008-01797-011[pii]10.1037/0893-164X.22.1.107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeterian JD, Greene MC, Bergman BG, Kelly JF. Does mandated treatment benefit youth? A prospective investigation of adolescent justice system involvement, treatment motivation, and substance use outcomes. Alcohol Treatment Quarterly. 2013;31(4):431–449. doi: 10.1080/07347324.2013.831671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.