Skip to main content
Frontiers in Physiology logoLink to Frontiers in Physiology
. 2016 Jun 24;7:251. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2016.00251

Emotional Stress and Cardiovascular Complications in Animal Models: A Review of the Influence of Stress Type

Carlos C Crestani 1,*
PMCID: PMC4919347  PMID: 27445843

Abstract

Emotional stress has been recognized as a modifiable risk factor for cardiovascular diseases. The impact of stress on physiological and psychological processes is determined by characteristics of the stress stimulus. For example, distinct responses are induced by acute vs. chronic aversive stimuli. Additionally, the magnitude of stress responses has been reported to be inversely related to the degree of predictability of the aversive stimulus. Therefore, the purpose of the present review was to discuss experimental research in animal models describing the influence of stressor stimulus characteristics, such as chronicity and predictability, in cardiovascular dysfunctions induced by emotional stress. Regarding chronicity, the importance of cardiovascular and autonomic adjustments during acute stress sessions and cardiovascular consequences of frequent stress response activation during repeated exposure to aversive threats (i.e., chronic stress) is discussed. Evidence of the cardiovascular and autonomic changes induced by chronic stressors involving daily exposure to the same stressor (predictable) vs. different stressors (unpredictable) is reviewed and discussed in terms of the impact of predictability in cardiovascular dysfunctions induced by stress.

Keywords: psychological stress, restraint stress, chronic variable stress, social isolation, social defeat, crowding stress, baroreflex, autonomic activity

Introduction

Convergent clinical and preclinical studies have provided evidence on the important role of psychosocial factors in the etiology and progression of cardiovascular diseases (Friedman and Rosenman, 1959; Rosenman et al., 1975; Kawachi et al., 1994; Ford et al., 1998; Rozanski et al., 1999; Rugulies, 2002; Smith et al., 2004; Grippo and Johnson, 2009; Roest et al., 2010; Carnevali et al., 2013a; Sgoifo et al., 2014). Among several psychosocial factors, psychological stress has been recognized as a modifiable risk factor for several cardiovascular dysfunctions (Steptoe and Kivimaki, 2012; von Känel, 2012; Inoue, 2014). Indeed, epidemiological and experimental results on humans and animals have demonstrated the influence of psychosocial stress on cardiovascular health (Rosengren et al., 2004; Kivimaki et al., 2006b; Grippo and Johnson, 2009; Steptoe and Kivimaki, 2012; Jarczok et al., 2013; Sgoifo et al., 2014). The association between stress and cardiovascular diseases has been shown to be independent of traditional cardiovascular risk factors (e.g., age, sex, smoking, diabetes mellitus, and obesity; Rosengren et al., 2004; Kivimaki et al., 2006b; Steptoe and Kivimaki, 2012).

The impact of stress on physiological and psychological processes is determined by stressor characteristics, such as chronicity, predictability, and severity (Nalivaiko, 2011; Steptoe and Kivimaki, 2012; Herman, 2013). Indeed, distinct cardiovascular and neuroendocrine changes have been reported following acute vs. chronic stress regimens (Nalivaiko, 2011; Herman, 2013). Furthermore, studies in animals comparing the impact of predictable vs. unpredictable stressor stimuli demonstrated that the latter induces more severe behavioral and physiological consequences (Bassett et al., 1973; Magariños and McEwen, 1995; Marin et al., 2007; Flak et al., 2012; Kopp et al., 2013; Smith et al., 2013; Duarte et al., 2015a). A dose–response pattern of stress-induced cardiovascular dysfunctions has also been reported, and stress severity may be modulated by the duration and/or frequency of the exposure to stress stimulus (Marmot et al., 1991; Kivimaki et al., 2006a; Chandola et al., 2008; Steptoe and Kivimaki, 2012).

The purpose of the present review is to discuss experimental research in animal models describing the impact of emotional stress on cardiovascular function. The primary focus of this review is to discuss the influence of stress type in stress-induced cardiovascular changes. Therefore, the first section discusses the influence of chronicity by summarizing the cardiovascular responses during acute stress sessions and the cardiovascular dysfunctions following repeated exposure to aversive stimuli (i.e., chronic stress). The second section discusses the impact of the predictability of stressor stimulus in cardiovascular and autonomic changes induced by chronic stress.

Cardiovascular responses to stress: influence of chronicity

Cardiovascular responses during acute stress sessions

A coordinated and complex set of physiological changes is generated during acute stress sessions. Changes in the autonomic nervous system activity promote immediate responses during aversive threats, which are mainly characterized by changes in the cardiovascular function. Cardiovascular responses typically consist of increase in blood pressure, heart rate (HR), and cardiac output (Hubbard et al., 1986; Schadt and Hasser, 1998; Dos Reis et al., 2014). However, these responses are consequences of more complex changes in cardiovascular function. Below is discussed the blood flow redistribution, changes of cardiac autonomic activity, and modulation of baroreflex function observed during an acute stress session. Additionally, the recovery of cardiovascular function after ending the stress session is described.

Blood flow redistribution

Blood flow redistribution occurs from the visceral and cutaneous beds toward the skeletal muscle vasculature during aversive threats (Kirby et al., 1987; Kapusta et al., 1989; Knardahl and Hendley, 1990; Viken et al., 1991; Zhang et al., 1996; Schadt and Hasser, 1998; Blessing, 2003). Blood flow redistribution is mediated mainly by vasoconstriction of the splanchnic, renal, cutaneous, and celiac vascular beds and by vasodilatation of the skeletal muscle vasculature (Iriuchijima et al., 1982; Kirby et al., 1987; Kapusta et al., 1989; Zhang et al., 1992, 1996; Anderson and Overton, 1994; Schadt and Hasser, 1998; Blessing, 2003; Mohammed et al., 2014). The increase in skeletal muscle blood flow is primarily due to an increase in the level of circulating catecholamines and activation of β2-adrenoceptors in the skeletal muscle beds, whereas vasoconstriction in the visceral vasculature is mediated by catecholamine released from both the adrenal medulla and sympathetic nerve fibers and activation of α-adrenoceptors (Iriuchijima et al., 1982; Knardahl and Hendley, 1990; Zhang et al., 1992, 1996).

An increase in the total peripheral resistance during stress was reported in borderline hypertensive rats (Hatton et al., 1997). However, studies in normotensive animals documented little or no change in this parameter (Hubbard et al., 1986; Martin et al., 1996; Zhang et al., 1996; Schadt and Hasser, 1998), indicating that vasodilation mostly balanced vasoconstriction. Therefore, the increase in cardiac output is possibly the main mechanism mediating the moderate increase in arterial pressure during aversive threats in rodents. In this regard, hemodynamic changes during stress also include an increase in venomotor tone via activation of α2-adrenoceptors (Martin et al., 1996; Schadt and Hasser, 1998), which increase venous return and contribute to stress-induced increase in the cardiac output.

Cardiac autonomic activity

The tachycardic response during aversive threats is mediated by an increase in the sympathetic tone to the heart (Iwata and LeDoux, 1988; Baudrie et al., 1997; Sgoifo et al., 1999; van den Buuse et al., 2001; Carrive, 2006; Crestani et al., 2010b; Dos Reis et al., 2014). However, stress-induced tachycardic response was enhanced following systemic treatment with muscarinic cholinergic receptor antagonists (Iwata and LeDoux, 1988; Baudrie et al., 1997; Nijsen et al., 1998; Carrive, 2006; Crestani et al., 2009; Dos Reis et al., 2014), indicating an increase in the parasympathetic tone to the heart. Indeed, coactivation of cardiac sympathetic and parasympathetic activities has been documented during aversive situations (Iwata and LeDoux, 1988; Baudrie et al., 1997; Carrive, 2006; Dos Reis et al., 2014). The increase in the parasympathetic tone to the heart allows precise response control, reducing the amplitude of the response, and counteracting excessive cardiac activation associated with cardiac sympathetic system activation alone. The parasympathetic activation and the consequent reduction of cardiac response possibly contributes to a functional state stabilization of the heart (Paton et al., 2005).

Modulation of baroreflex function

Change in baroreflex function is a relevant mechanism controlling cardiovascular and autonomic responses during aversive threats. The baroreflex is an important mechanism for arterial pressure regulation. For example, an increase in arterial pressure activates baroreceptors within the wall of the carotid arteries and aorta that in turn increase cardiac parasympathetic activity and decrease sympathetic activity, leading to decreased HR, vascular resistance, and venous return (Michelini, 1994; Sved and Gordon, 1994). However, as stated above, the increase in arterial pressure during stress is followed by a parallel enhancement in sympathetic activity and HR. Hilton (1963) addressed first the question regarding how parallel increase in blood pressure, HR, and sympathetic activity occurs during aversive situations. Based on the responses obtained by stimulation of the hypothalamic defense area, which mimics the cardiovascular responses induced by stress, baroreflex function was first claimed to be suppressed during defensive behavior (Hilton, 1963; Coote et al., 1979). However, further studies consistently demonstrated that pressor, tachycardic, sympathetic activation, and regional vasoconstrictor responses observed during emotional stress are enhanced in sinoaortic baroreceptor-denervated rats (Norman et al., 1981; Buchholz et al., 1986; Zhang et al., 1996; Dos Reis et al., 2014), indicating an active role of the baroreflex counteracting stress-induced autonomic and cardiovascular responses during defensive responses. Indeed, baroreflex is clearly functional during aversive threats, but it curves for both renal sympathetic nerve activity and HR shifts to the right and upward (Shammas et al., 1988; Hatton et al., 1997; Schadt and Hasser, 1998; Kanbar et al., 2007; Burke and Head, 2009; Crestani et al., 2010b; Miki and Yoshimoto, 2013), allowing a parallel increase in arterial pressure, HR, and sympathetic activity. A schematic representation of the right and upward shift of the baroreflex curve during aversive situations is presented in Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Schematic representation sketching the modulation of baroreflex function during emotional stress. Figure schematically represents sigmoid baroreflex curves correlating mean arterial pressure (MAP) and sympathetic nerve activity (SNA) or heart rate (HR) in basal condition (basal) and during an aversive situation (stress). Note that baroreflex curves shifts to the right (green arrow) and upward (red arrow). The rightward shift represents a resetting of the operating point of baroreflex toward higher arterial pressure values, whereas the upward shift is due to increase in SNA and HR during stress (possibly mediated by mechanisms independent of baroreflex resetting). For details, see text in the section “Cardiovascular responses during acute sessions of stress.

The rightward shift represents a resetting of the operating point of the baroreflex toward higher arterial pressure values (Figure 1). Therefore, the baroreflex operates under higher arterial pressure values during stress. This idea is supported by evidence that a decrease in the increase in arterial pressure due to treatment with α-adrenoceptor antagonists (i.e., maintaining arterial pressure values below the new operating point of the baroreflex) enhanced the tachycardic response during aversive situations (Baudrie et al., 1997; Carrive, 2002; Dos Reis et al., 2014), possibly to increase arterial pressure to some preset value. The baroreflex resetting seems to be a mechanism allowing the simultaneous increase in arterial pressure and sympathetic activity/HR rather than generating the cardiovascular responses to stress, once sinoaortic baroreceptor denervation facilitated stress-induced cardiovascular and autonomic adjustments, as stated above (Norman et al., 1981; Buchholz et al., 1986; Zhang et al., 1996; Dos Reis et al., 2014). However, Zhang et al. (1996) reported that vasodilatation in the skeletal muscle vasculature was reduced in sinoaortic baroreceptor-denervated rats, indicating that baroreflex activity resetting may contribute to blood flow redistribution during aversive threats.

The abovementioned evidence indicates that autonomic and cardiovascular responses to stress are mediated by mechanisms acting independent of the changes in baroreflex function. However, the increase in sympathetic activity and HR is responsible for the upward shift of the baroreflex curves (Schadt and Hasser, 1998; Crestani et al., 2010b; Miki and Yoshimoto, 2013; Figure 1). Indeed, the organism operates at higher values of HR and sympathetic activity during stress, inducing a vertical change in the baroreflex curves (Schadt and Hasser, 1998; Crestani et al., 2010b; Miki and Yoshimoto, 2013; Figure 1).

Recovery of cardiovascular function after ending stress session

Arterial pressure and HR progressively decrease toward baseline values during recovery from an aversive stimulus, returning to pre-stress levels ~30–60 min after ending stress (McDougall et al., 2000; Vianna and Carrive, 2005; Igosheva et al., 2007; Porter et al., 2007; Crestani et al., 2010b; Krause et al., 2011). Nevertheless, we have previously reported that besides arterial pressure and HR had already returned to pre-stress values 30 min after ending an acute session of restraint stress, the baroreflex activity remained similar to that observed during stress, returning to control values only 60 min after ending the stress session (Crestani et al., 2010b). These findings indicate non-persistent effects of stress on cardiovascular function, but suggest a period of vulnerability during the recovery period, once the baroreflex function is reset to higher arterial pressure values. Nevertheless, persistence of the stress effects on baroreflex activity may be important to counteract excessive activation of depressor mechanisms during the recovery period, thus allowing adequate return of blood pressure to normal values without abrupt falls. Indeed, central and peripheral depressor mechanisms are activated during the recovery period and mediate the return of arterial pressure to resting values (Yip and Krukoff, 2002; D'Angelo et al., 2006).

Cardiovascular responses to chronic stressors

The physiological and behavioral responses during acute stress sessions constitute important adaptive responses, maintaining homeostasis and ensuring survival (Sterling and Eyer, 1988; Sterling, 2012). However, the frequent/prolonged activation (i.e., over-exposure to stress changes) or generation of inadequate responses (i.e., insufficient responses to individual needs) during stressful events can result in disease development (McEwen and Stellar, 1993; McEwen, 1998; Danese and McEwen, 2012). A situation of over-exposure to stress responses occurs during repeated exposure to aversive threats (i.e., chronic stress; McEwen, 1998). Regarding the cardiovascular consequences of stress, besides reports of acute cardiovascular events such as ventricular arrhythmias, myocardial infarction, and cardiomyopathy (potentially fatal) after exposure to acute stressful situations (Cannon, 1942; Lown et al., 1973; Natelson and Cagin, 1979; Sgoifo et al., 1997, 1999; Ueyama et al., 2002; Mostofsky et al., 2014; Lagraauw et al., 2015), enduring cardiovascular changes (days to years) are mainly provoked by long-term exposure to stressful events (Nalivaiko, 2011). Therefore, the frequency of exposure to stress (chronicity) is an important factor in determining the pattern of stress-induced cardiovascular diseases. Below, we discuss the evidence of enduring changes in arterial pressure, HR, cardiac function, autonomic activity, and baroreflex function that lasts beyond the duration of chronic stress protocols.

Arterial pressure

Despite of pronounced neuroendocrine (e.g., increased hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal, HPA, axis activity), somatic (e.g., reduced body weight gain, adrenal hypertrophy, and thymus atrophy), and behavioral (e.g., anxiety- and depression-like behavior) changes following long-term exposure to stressful events, preclinical studies have failed to consistently reproduce epidemiological findings demonstrating a relationship between chronically stressful life events and hypertension in animals (Timio et al., 1988; Markovitz et al., 2004). Indeed, inconsistent findings have been reported regarding the effects of chronic stress on arterial pressure in animal models. Nalivaiko (2011) reviewed in details publications that investigated the effect of different chronic stress models on arterial pressure in several rat strains. An important consideration raised was the influence of the method used to assess arterial pressure. For example, most studies reviewed by Nalivaiko (2011) that identified a significant hypertensive effect following exposure to chronic stressors used the tail-cuff method (indirect) to measure the arterial pressure. Even in studies that validated the findings obtained through the tail-cuff method by direct methods (through implantation of a catheter into the femoral or carotid artery at the end of the stress protocol), the arterial pressure increase obtained in the direct method was lower than that in the tail-cuff method (Nalivaiko, 2011). Similar analysis of studies published after 2007 (date of the more recent study included in the Nalivaiko's review; Table 1) further reinforces observation regarding the influence of the method used to assess arterial pressure in the identification of stress-induced hypertension.

Table 1.

Studies examining the effect of the exposure to chronic stressors in basal parameters of mean arterial pressure (MAP).

Specie/Strain Age/Gender Chronic stressor (length of each session) Protocol stress duration Basal MAP in control MAP change Method of measurement References
Long-Evans rats 275 g/male CVS (variable) 15 days Data not shown No change (dark and light period) Telemetry Goodson et al., 2016
Wistar rat 50 days (adult)/male CVS (variable) 14 days 103 mmHg No change Direct Almeida et al., 2015
Wistar rat 37 days (adolescent)/male RRS (1 h, 5 days/week) 15 weeks 118 mmHg (systolic) +30 mmHg TC Bruder-Nascimento et al. 2015
Wistar rat 28 days (adolescent) 60days (adult) male CVS (variable) RRS (60 min daily) 14 days 95 mmHg (adult) 97 mmHg (adolescent) +15 mmHg (RRS/adult) +10 mmHg (CVS/adult) +9 mmHg (CVS/adolescent) Direct Duarte et al., 2015a
Wistar rat 28 days (adolescent) 60 days (adult) male CVS (variable) RRS (60 min daily) 10 days 100 mmHg (adult) 97 mmHg (adolescent) No change Direct Duarte et al., 2015b
Wistar rat Adult RRS (4 h daily) 6 weeks ~120 mmHg (systolic) ~+25 mmHg TC Habib et al., 2015
WKY and SHR rats 5 weeks/male and female Crowding (continuous) 2 weeks ~160 mmHg (SHR) ~110 mmHg (WKY) (systolic) +20 mmHg (only SHR male) TC Ledvenyiova-Farkasova et al., 2015
Wistar rat 350 g (adult)/male CVS (10–14h, 7 days/week) 8 weeks 104 mmHg No change TC Matchkov et al., 2015
Wistar rat 300–400 g (adult)/male CVS (variable, 7 days/week) 8 weeks 95 mmHg No change Direct Demirtas et al., 2014
Wistar rat 200–250 g (adult)/male CVS (variable, 7 days/week) 8 weeks 99 mmHg (systolic) No change TC Ismail et al., 2014
WKY, BHR, and SHR rats 5 weeks/female Crowding 2 weeks 100 mmHg (WKY) 110 mmHg (BHR) 160 mmHg (SHR) (systolic) No change TC Slezak et al., 2014
BALB/cJ mice 9 weeks/male-female CVS (variable, 7 days/week) 8 weeks Male 87 mmHg Female 91 mmHg No change Direct Stanley et al., 2014
Sprague–Dawley rat 8 weeks/male RRS (2 h daily) 14 days Data not shown Increase TC Yang et al., 2014
Wistar rat 250–300 g/male CVS (variable, 7 days/week) 8 weeks 102 mmHg (systolic) No change TC Bayramgurler et al., 2013
Wistar rat 7–10 weeks/male Footshock (40 min, 3−7 days/week) 6 weeks ~110 mmHg No change (increased SAP during, but not after stress) Telemetry Bobrovskaya et al., 2013
WKY rat 12 weeks/male Crowding 8 and 12 weeks ~110 mmHg (8 weeks) ~105 mmHg (12 weeks) (systolic) No change TC Puzserova et al., 2013
Sprague–Dawley rat 250–300 g/male Social defeat 4 days + 5 days 102 mmHg No change Direct Sévoz-Couche et al., 2013
Wistar rat 5–6 weeks/male CVS (12 h, 7 days/week) 4 or 8 weeks 121 mmHg No change TC Bouzinova et al., 2012
Wistar rat 30 days (adolescent)/male RRS (1 h, 5days/week) 15 weeks ~120 mmHg (systolic) ~+30 mmHg TC Bruder-Nascimento et al., 2012
Sprague–Dawley rat 275–300 g/male RRS (60 min daily) 5 days ~100 mmHg +10 mmHg (light period) Telemetry Daubert et al., 2012
Rabbits 2 kg(adult)/male CVS+HD 8 and 16 weeks 116 mmHg (8 week) 118 mmHg (16 week) (systolic) +29 mmHg (8 week) +46 mmHg (16 week) Indirect Lu et al., 2012
WKY rat 12 weeks/male Crowding 8 weeks ~110 mmHg (systolic) No change TC Puzserova et al., 2012
Wistar rat 5–6 weeks/male Social isolation 14 days ~90 mmHg (diastolic) ~125 mmHg (systolic) No change Telemetry Tsvirkun et al., 2012
Wistar rat 230–250 g/male CVS (variable, 7 days/week) 2 weeks ~120 mmHg (systolic) No change Direct Xie et al., 2012*
Sprague–Dawley rat 250–275 g (adult)/male CVS (variable, 7 days/week) 2 weeks ~105 mmHg No change (24 h recording) Telemetry Flak et al., 2011
WKY and SHR rats Adult/male Crowding 8 weeks 110 mmHg (WKY) 189 mmHg (SHR) +8 mmHg (only SHR) TC Ravingerova et al., 2011
WKY and SHR/y rat Colony social stress 6 months 120 mmHg (WKY) 140 mmHg (SHR/y) (systolic) +20 mmHg (WKY) +15 mmHg (SHR) Toot et al., 2011
BHR and Wistar rat 15 weeks (adult)/male and female Crowding 6 weeks ~110 mmHg (Wistar) ~130 mmHg/male ~120 mmHg/female (BHR) +10 mmHg (only BHR male) TC Bernatova et al., 2010
Sprague–Dawley rat 8–10 weeks (adult)/male CVS (variable, 7 days/week) 4 weeks ~115 mmHg No change Direct Cudnoch-Jedrzejewska et al., 2010
WKY 12 weeks (adult)/male Crowding 8 weeks 111 mmHg (systolic) No change TC Puzserova and Bernatova, 2010
BHR/WKY rats BHR-385 g/WKY-414 g (adults) RRS (1–2 h, 5 days/week) 21–25 days BHR-122 mmHg WKY-110 mmHg No change Direct Bechtold et al., 2009
Sprague–Dawley rat 250–300 g/male CVS (variable) 4 weeks ~120 mmHg ~+6 mmHg Direct (24 h recovery) Grippo et al., 2008
WKY rat 12 weeks/male Crowding 8 weeks 110 mmHg (systolic) No change TC Bernatova et al., 2007
Wistar, BHR, and SHR 12 weeks/male Crowding 8 weeks Wistar 111 mmHg BHR 135 mmHg SHR 182 mmHg (systolic) No change +6 mmHg +9 mmHg TC Bernatowa et al., 2007

BHR, borderline hypertensive rat; CVS, chronic variable stress; Dahl-S, Dahl salt-sensitive rats; Dahl-R, Dahl salt-resistant rats; HD, high-cholesterol diet; ISIAH-inherited stress-induced arterial hypertension; LE, Long Evans rat; RRS, repeated restraint stress; SHR, spontaneously hypertensive rats; SHR/Y, borderline hypertensive consomic rat strain; WKY, Wistar-Kyoto rat.

Bold indicates studies that identified effect of chronic emotional stress in baseline MAP.

*

Cardiovascular recording in anesthetized animals.

Animals need to be restrained (similar to the restraint stress model) in the tail-cuff method, and marked pressor and tachycardic responses are observed, even following recommended habituation (Grundt et al., 2009). Based on this, Nalivaiko (2011) proposed that the identification of hypertensive effect preferentially by the tail-cuff method would be due to a more vigorous reaction of the animals stressed to the tail-cuff procedure. Hence, it has been reported that exposure to chronic stressors increases pressor response during acute aversive stimuli (Grippo et al., 2002, 2006; Cudnoch-Jedrzejewska et al., 2010, 2014; Maslova et al., 2010).

Another factor mentioned by Nalivaiko (2011) could be the possible influence of coping strategies in models providing actual or perceived control over a stressful situation, which may occur, for example, in highly predictable protocols (e.g., during repeated exposure to the same stressor). We recently addressed this issue by comparing the impact on arterial pressure (measured by the direct method) of a predictable (repeated restraint stress, RRS) vs. an unpredictable (chronic variable stress, CVS) chronic stress protocol (Duarte et al., 2015a) (see the next section for details). We observed a similar small pressor effect (+10–15 mm Hg) following exposure to either RRS or CVS for 10 days (Duarte et al., 2015a). Other studies using longer CVS protocols (4–8weeks) reported either similar effect (~+8 mm Hg) (Grippo et al., 2008) or absence of changes on arterial pressure measured by either direct methods or telemetry (Grippo et al., 2002, 2006; Cudnoch-Jedrzejewska et al., 2010; Demirtas et al., 2014; Stanley et al., 2014). In addition, Bobrovskaya et al. (2013) did not identify changes in mean arterial pressure measured by telemetry (only a small increase in systolic arterial pressure was identified) following repeated exposure to footshock (more intense aversive stimulus) for 6 weeks in an unpredictable schedule. Thus, the small hypertensive effect obtained by direct measurement of arterial pressure seems to be independent of intensity, predictability, and duration of chronic stress protocol.

An additional factor considered in our review (Table 1) was an elevated baseline arterial pressure in the control group, which would indicate some degree of stress induced by the experimental procedure (independent of chronic stressor) that in turn could buffer effects of chronic stress. However, independently of the chronic stress paradigm, a similar small hypertensive effect (~10 mm Hg) was observed following long-term stress exposure in studies wherein baseline mean arterial pressure in control groups ranged from 95 to 120 mmHg (Grippo et al., 2008; Daubert et al., 2012; Duarte et al., 2015a; Cruz et al., 2016) (Table 1). It has also been documented that cardiovascular dysfunctions induced by chronic stressors may be related to animals age (Crestani, 2016); hence, an influence of age (e.g., young vs. adult) was considered in this review (Table 1). However, the influence of age seems to be related to the type of chronic stressor. For example, social isolation for 3 weeks increased arterial pressure in adolescent rats, without affecting this parameter in adult animals (Cruz et al., 2016). In contrast, RRS induced a mild hypertensive effect in adult animals alone (Duarte et al., 2015a).

Heart rate and cardiac dysfunctions

More consistent results have been reported for HR. Table 2 summarizes the main findings of changes in baseline HR following exposure to chronic stressors. Increase in baseline HR has been reported in many studies, but this effect seems to be related to the chronic stressor type. Indeed, resting tachycardia has been consistently documented following exposure to two animal models of chronic stress: the CVS and chronic social isolation in prairie voles (a highly social rodent species; Table 2). Resting tachycardia induced by both these models was independent of the protocol length. For example, increased baseline HR was observed following exposure to protocols ranging from 10 days to 8 weeks of CVS (Grippo et al., 2002, 2008; Mercanoglu et al., 2008; Bouzinova et al., 2012; Duarte et al., 2015a) and 5 days to 8 weeks of social isolation in prairie voles (Grippo et al., 2007, 2011; McNeal et al., 2014). Moreover, the increase in HR was not affected by baseline HR in control animals (indicative of some degree of stress of the experimental procedure, see discussion above) and by animal age (Table 2).

Table 2.

Studies examining the effect of the exposure to chronic stressors in basal parameters of heart rate (HR).

Specie/Strain Age/Gender Chronic stressor (length of sessions) Protocol length HR basal in control group HR change Method of measurement References
Long-Evans rats 275 g/male CVS (variable) 15 days Data not shown No change (dark and light period) Telemetry Goodson et al., 2016
Wistar rat 50 days (adult)/male CVS (variable) 14 days 368 bpm No change Direct Almeida et al., 2015
WKY rat 3 months male Social defeat (30 min contact without physical interaction/10 min physical interaction) 5 weeks ~320 bpm No change Carnevali et al., 2015
Wistar rat 28 days (adolescent) 60days (adult) male Social isolation (continuous) 3 weeks 338 bpm (adult) 371 bpm (adolescent) No change Direct Cruz et al., 2016
Wistar rat 28 days (adolescent) 60 days (adult) male CVS (variable) RRS (60 min daily) 14 days 343 bpm (adult) 390 bpm (adolescent) +30 bpm (CVS/adolescent)
+32 mmHg (CVS/adult)
RRS-No change
Direct Duarte et al., 2015a
Wistar rat 28 days (adolescent) 60days (adult) male CVS (variable) RRS (60 min daily) 10 days 329 bpm (adult) 387 bpm (adolescent) +34 bpm (CVS/adolescent)
+11 mmHg (CVS/adult)
RRS-No change
Direct Duarte et al., 2015b
WKY and SHR rats 5 weeks/male and female Crowding (continuous) 2 weeks Data not shown No change TC Ledvenyiova-Farkasova et al., 2015
Wistar rat 350 g(adult)/male CVS (10–14 h) 8 weeks ~350 bpm +100 bpm TC Matchkov et al., 2015
Wistar rat 200–250 g/male Footshock (1 h daily) 7 days ~300 bpm No change Isolated heart Rakhshan et al., 2015
Wistar rat 200–250 g (adult)/male CVS 8 weeks 344 bpm No change TC Ismail et al., 2014
Prairie voles 60–90 days/male Social isolation 5 days ~400 bpm ~+50 bpm Telemetry McNeal et al., 2014
WKY, BHR, and SHR rats 5 weeks/female Crowding 2 weeks 420 bpm (WKY) 440 bpm (BHR) 497 bpm(SHR) No change TC Slezak et al., 2014
Wistar rat 7–10 weeks/male Footshock (40 min, 3–7 days/week) 6 weeks ~400 bpm No change Telemetry Bobrovskaya et al., 2013
WT Groningen rat 10 weeks/male Social defeat (12 episodes/75min) 25 days + 2days ~167ms (R-R interval) No change Telemetry Carnevali et al., 2013b
Sprague–Dawley rat 250–300 g/male Social defeat 4 days + 5 days 321 bpm +24 bpm Electrocardiogram Sévoz-Couche et al., 2013
Wistar rat 5–6 weeks/male CVS (12 h) 4 or 8 weeks 354 bpm +90 bpm TC Bouzinova et al., 2012
5-HT1A KO and WT mice 12 weeks/male Social defeat (cohabited with resident, 5 defeat episodes) 15 days + 3 days ~530 bpm No change Telemetry Carnevali et al., 2012
Sprague–Dawley rat 275–300 g/male RRS (60 min daily) 5 days ~375 bpm No change (day time and night time) Telemetry Daubert et al., 2012
Prairie voles 81 days/female Social isolation 4 weeks ~390 bpm +20 bpm Telemetry Grippo et al., 2012a
Prairie voles 60–90 days/female Social isolation 4 weeks ~350 bpm ~+70 bpm Telemetry Grippo et al., 2012b
Rabbits 2 kg(adult)/male CVS+HD 8 and 16 weeks 257 bpm (8 week) 254 bpm (16 week) (systolic) +25 bpm (8 week) +91 bpm (16 week) Indirect Lu et al., 2012
WKY rat 12 weeks/male Crowding 8 weeks ~400 bpm No change TC Puzserova et al., 2012
Wistar rat 5–6 weeks old Social isolation (standard plastic cages) 14 days ~300 bpm No change Telemetry Tsvirkun et al., 2012
Wistar rat 230–250 g/male CVS (variable, 7 days/week) 2 weeks ~390 bpm No change Direct Xie et al., 2012*
Sprague–Dawley rat 275–300 g/male Social defeat 30 min/session 7 days 370 bpm No change Telemetry Wood et al., 2012
Sprague–Dawley rat 10 weeks/male Footshock (1 h daily) 5 days 416 bpm (night time) 359 bpm(day time) –23 bpm (night time) –20 bpm (day time) Telemetry Carnevali et al., 2011
Sprague–Dawley rat 250–275 g (adult)/male CVS (variable, 7 days/week) 2 weeks ~350 bpm No change (24 h recording) Telemetry Flak et al., 2011
Prairie voles 75 days/female Social isolation 2 and 4 weeks ~390 bpm ~+35 bpm(24 h recording) Telemetry Grippo et al., 2011
WKY and SHR rats Adult/male Crowding 8 weeks 306 bpm (WKY) 317 bpm(SHR) No change Isolated heart Ravingerova et al., 2011
Sprague–Dawley rat 8–10 weeks (adult) CVS 4 weeks ~350 bpm No change Direct (24 h recovery) Cudnoch-Jedrzejewska et al., 2010
Prairie voles 60–90 days/female Social isolation 2 and 4 weeks ~360 bpm ~+40 bpm(24 h recording) Telemetry Grippo et al., 2010
WKY 12 weeks (adult)/male Crowding 8 weeks 385 bpm No change TC Puzserova and Bernatova, 2010
BHR/WKY rats BHR-385 g/WKY-414 g (adults) RRS (1–2 h, variable) 21–25 days BHR-295 bpm WKY-312bpm No change Direct (24 h recovery) Bechtold et al., 2009
Prairie voles 60–90 days/female Social isolation 2 and 4 weeks ~360 bpm ~+40 bpm Telemetry Grippo et al., 2009
Sprague–Dawley rat 250–300 g/male CVS (variable) 4 weeks ~380 bpm ~+20 mmHg Direct (24 h recovery) Grippo et al., 2008
Sprague–Dawley rat 250–300 g/male CVS (variable) 20 days 253 bpm +24 bpm ECG Mercanoglu et al., 2008*
Wistar, BHR, and SHR 12 weeks/male Crowding 8 weeks Wistar411 bpm BHR415 bpm SHR453 bpm No change TC Bernatowa et al., 2007
WKY rat 12 weeks/male Crowding 8 weeks 335 bpm No change TC Bernatova et al., 2007
Prairie voles 60–90 days/female Social isolation 2 and 4 weeks ~360 bpm ~+40 bpm Telemetry Grippo et al., 2007
BHR 15 weeks/male Crowding 6 weeks 446 bpm No change TC Bernatova and Csizmadiova, 2006
Sprague–Dawley rat 250–300 g/male CVS(variable, 7 days/week) 4 weeks 371 bpm +26 bpm Direct (48 h recovery) Grippo et al., 2006
Sprague–Dawley rat 300–400 g/male CVS(variable) 4 weeks ~240 bpm ~+20 bpm Direct(24 h recovery) Grippo et al., 2004*
Swiss CD-1 mice 12 weeks, male Social defeat (cohabited with resident, 5 min agonistic interaction/day) 15 days ~530 bpm (day) ~600 bpm (night) No change (post-stress period) Telemetry Costoli et al., 2004
Sprague–Dawley rat 5 weeks/male Restraint/heat stress (45 min, twice daily) 2 weeks 447 bpm No change Direct Porter et al., 2004
Swiss CD-1 mice 12 weeks, male Social defeat (cohabited with resident, 5-15min agonistic interaction/day) 15 days ~500 bpm (day) ~600 bpm (night) No change (post-stress period) Telemetry Bartolomucci et al., 2003
Sprague–Dawley rats 250–300 g (adult)/male CVS 4 weeks 329 bpm +32 bpm Direct (48 h recovery) Grippo et al., 2003
Sprague–Dawley rat 300-400 g (adult)/male CVS 4 weeks 364 bpm +18 bpm Direct (72 h recovery) Grippo et al., 2002
WKY 12 weeks/male RRS(10–20 min daily) 5 days 340 bpm No change Direct(24 h recovery) Conti et al., 2001
WT Groningen rat 20 weeks/male Social defeat/victory (15 min/day) 10 days 200 ms (R-R interval) No change (basal) Telemetry Sgoifo et al., 2001
WKY and SHR 15–16 weeks/male RRS (60 min dalily, isolated) 10 days WKY 285 bpm No change Telemetry McDougall et al., 2000
SHR 288 bpm No change
Lister hooded rat 180 g/male Social defeat (10 min/day) 10 days 440 bpm No change Telemetry Chung et al., 1999
Wistar rat 150–250 g/male Social isolation RRSCrowding 7, 15, and 30 days 362 bpm +25 bpm (isolation, 7 days)
+68 bpm (RRS, 7 days)
Electrocardiogram Nagaraja and Jeganathan, 1999
Sprague–Dawley rat RRS (1, 1.5, or 2 h daily) 8–14 days 430 bpm No change Direct (30 min after surgery) Scheuer and Mifflin, 1998
LE rat 280–340 g/male Social defeat 5 days ~340 bpm No change (24 h recording) decreased circadian rhythmicity Telemetry Tornatzky and Miczek, 1993
Sprague–Dawley rat 275–300 g/male RRS Cold swim Footshock (30 min/daily) 26 days 350 bpm No change Direct (24 h recovery) Konarska et al., 1989
338 bpm No change
346 bpm No change
Dahl-S 70–80 days (adult) Footshock (25 min) 5 days 440 bpm No change TC Adams et al., 1987
Social Defeat (25 min) 5 days 440 bpm No change
Dahl-S+salt 70–80 days (adult) Social Defeat (25 min) 6 days 452 bpm No change TC Adams and Blizard, 1986
Dahl-R+salt 388 bpm
Brattleboro and LE rats Brattleboro 280–340 g LE 310–380 male Social isolation 5 and 11 days ~375 bpm (LE) ~410 bpm (Brattleboro) No change TC Gardiner and Bennett, 1983

BHR, borderline hypertensive rat; CVS, chronic variable stress; Dahl-S, Dahl salt-sensitive rats; Dahl-R, Dahl salt-resistant rats; ECG, electrocardiogram; HD, high-cholesterol diet; ISIAH, inherited stress-induced arterial hypertension; KO, knockout mice; LE, Long Evans rat; RRS, repeated restraint stress; SHR, spontaneously hypertensive rats; WKY, Wistar-Kyoto rat.

Bold indicates studies that identified effect of chronic stress in baseline HR.

*

Cardiovascular recording in anesthetized animals.

Although, most studies investigating the effect of chronic social isolation in prairie voles have used female animals (Table 2), resting tachycardia was also reported in male prairie voles following a period of social isolation (McNeal et al., 2014). CVS-induced increase in HR was evaluated only in males animals, but was documented in both Wistar and Sprague–Dawley rats (Grippo et al., 2002, 2006, 2008; Bouzinova et al., 2012; Duarte et al., 2015a; Matchkov et al., 2015), indicating that the effect is independent of the rat strain. As discussed for the effect of stress on arterial pressure, we note higher responses in studies that measured HR by the tail-cuff method compared with data obtained by other methods (Table 2). As discussed above, it can be related to a more vigorous reaction of the stressed animals to the tail-cuff procedure (Nalivaiko, 2011).

Despite the absence of changes in baseline HR, some models induce profound changes in cardiac function. For example, repeated social defeat episodes caused accumulation of fibrous tissue in the left ventricular myocardium, maladaptive cardiac hypertrophy, changes in electrical conduction system of the heart (e.g., reduced myocardial refractoriness and impaired conduction), and increased susceptibility to cardiac arrhythmias (Gelsema et al., 1994; Costoli et al., 2004; Carnevali et al., 2013b, 2015; Sgoifo et al., 2014). Disruption of the circadian rhythm for HR has also been reported following repeated exposure to social defeat (Tornatzky and Miczek, 1993; Sgoifo et al., 2002; Carnevali et al., 2015).

RRS is another chronic stressor that does not induce significant changes in baseline HR (Table 2), but cardiac hypertrophy (Nagaraja and Jeganathan, 1999; Bruder-Nascimento et al., 2012; Duarte et al., 2015a), electrocardiogram (ECG) abnormality, myocardium injury, and cardiac dysfunction (Zhao et al., 2007; Roth et al., 2015) have been reported in animals exposed to this experimental model of stress. Moreover, RRS increased the size of infarction and the incidence of potentially fatal arrhythmias induced by myocardial ischemia (Scheuer and Mifflin, 1998), increased plaque formation in the coronary arteries and occurrence of myocardial infarctions in mice susceptible to atherosclerosis (Roth et al., 2015), and exacerbated hypertension and left ventricular hypertrophy, fibrosis, and diastolic dysfunction related to metabolic syndrome (Habib et al., 2015; Matsuura et al., 2015). These results indicate that RRS may affect the outcome of cardiac diseases.

Consistent findings have also indicated that crowding stress (i.e., social stress that causes competition for resources such as space, food, and water) does not affect baseline HR (Table 2). Nevertheless, analysis of responses to myocardial ischemia indicated an impairment of post-ischemic recovery of cardiac mechanical function and aggravation of tachyarrhythmia and ventricular fibrillation in animals subjected to crowding stress (Ravingerova et al., 2011; Ledvenyiova-Farkasova et al., 2015), suggesting that exposure to this social stressor reduces the tolerance to cardiac ischemia. Maladaptive cardiac hypertrophy, ECG abnormality, and impairment of coronary perfusion of the myocardium have also been reported following exposure to crowding stress (Nagaraja and Jeganathan, 1999; Ravingerova et al., 2011).

In addition to causing tachycardia, cardiac dysfunctions have also been reported following exposure to either CVS or chronic social isolation in prairie voles. Indeed, increased susceptibility to experimentally and stress-induced cardiac arrhythmias was reported in animals subjected to either stressor (Grippo et al., 2004, 2010, 2012a; Liang et al., 2015), and CVS increased the infarcted area after myocardial ischemia (Mercanoglu et al., 2008). These findings indicate that these experimental models of stress may increase the vulnerability and severity of cardiac complications. In addition, cardiac contractile dysfunction was reported in animals subjected to CVS protocols (Grippo et al., 2006; Xie et al., 2012).

Studies in the literature also investigated a possible impact of chronic stressors in the pacemaker activity of the sinoatrial node, referred as intrinsic HR. It can be assessed experimentally by dual blockade of sympathetic and parasympathetic activities of the heart via combined systemic treatment with β-adrenoceptor antagonists (i.e., sympathetic blocker; e.g., propranolol or atenolol) and muscarinic cholinergic receptors (i.e., parasympathetic blocker; e.g., methylatropine). However, intrinsic HR was not affected after exposure to either CVS (Grippo et al., 2002; Almeida et al., 2015; Duarte et al., 2015a), chronic social isolation in prairie voles (Grippo et al., 2007, 2009; McNeal et al., 2014), or RRS (Duarte et al., 2015a). This finding seems to be independent of the duration of chronic stress protocol. For example, intrinsic HR was investigated in CVS protocols ranged from 10 days to 4 weeks (Grippo et al., 2002; Almeida et al., 2015; Duarte et al., 2015a), whereas social isolation in prairie voles ranged from 5 days to 4 weeks (Grippo et al., 2007; McNeal et al., 2014). To the best of my knowledge, a possible impact of other animal models in cardiac pacemaker activity has never been evaluated.

Autonomic activity

Reduction in HR variability following exposure to different animal models of chronic stress has been described (Grippo et al., 2002, 2009; Wood et al., 2012; Sévoz-Couche et al., 2013), which indicates changes in cardiac autonomic activity. Indeed, analysis of cardiac autonomic activity by either power spectral analysis of oscillatory components of HR or pharmacological blockade of cardiac sympathetic (e.g., treatment with propranolol) and parasympathetic (e.g., treatment with methylatropine) activities indicated significant stress-induced changes in cardiac autonomic activity. Nevertheless, these alterations seem to be specific to the chronic stressor type. For example, pharmacological blockade of cardiac autonomic activity revealed that CVS increased the sympathetic tone to the heart, without significantly affecting cardiac parasympathetic activity (Grippo et al., 2002; Duarte et al., 2015a). It was also evidenced by demonstration of an increase in LF/HF ratio of HR variability (Bundzikova-Osacka et al., 2015), which indicates a change in cardiac sympathovagal balance toward a sympathetic predominance. In contrast, the changes in cardiac autonomic activity induced by chronic social isolation in prairie voles, evidenced by pharmacological autonomic blockade, were characterized by both a decrease in parasympathetic and an increase in sympathetic tone to the heart (Grippo et al., 2007, 2009; McNeal et al., 2014). Despite specific changes in sympathetic and parasympathetic activities, the changes induced by either CVS or social isolation in prairie voles were characterized by a change in cardiac sympathovagal balance toward a sympathetic predominance, which is in line with the resting tachycardia induced by these experimental models. Moreover, the increase in sympathetic contribution of cardiac autonomic balance corroborates the increased susceptibility to cardiac arrhythmias induced by both models (Grippo et al., 2004, 2006, 2012a), as well as with cardiac contractile dysfunction (Grippo et al., 2006; Xie et al., 2012) and increased severity of myocardial ischemia (Mercanoglu et al., 2008) observed after exposure to CVS. Studies have not reported changes in cardiac sympathovagal balance following exposure to RRS protocols (Daubert et al., 2012; Duarte et al., 2015a,b), which is in line with the absence of changes in baseline HR.

Cardiac autonomic imbalance has been reported in Sprague–Dawley rats subjected to repeated sessions of social defeat (Wood et al., 2012; Sévoz-Couche et al., 2013). By analyzing HR variability, results indicated a shift in sympathovagal balance toward sympathetic predominance (Wood et al., 2012; Sévoz-Couche et al., 2013), which was likely mediated by both a decrease in parasympathetic activity and increase in sympathetic tone to the heart (Sévoz-Couche et al., 2013). This finding is in line with the increase in baseline HR observed in Sprague–Dawley rats following repeated exposure to social defeat (Sévoz-Couche et al., 2013). However, Sprague–Dawley rats seem to be selectively susceptible to resting tachycardia induced by repeated social defeat once studies did not identify changes in baseline HR in either mice (Bartolomucci et al., 2003; Costoli et al., 2004; Carnevali et al., 2012) or wide-type Groningen (Sgoifo et al., 2001; Carnevali et al., 2013b), Long-Evans (Tornatzky and Miczek, 1993), Wistar-Kyoto (Carnevali et al., 2015), Lister hooded (Chung et al., 1999), and Dahl-s (Adams and Blizard, 1986; Adams et al., 1987) rats. Nevertheless, differences in experimental protocol may also account for the differences in findings of autonomic/cardiovascular changes because exposures to social defeat in different studies ranged from 5 to 25 days (Table 2). Moreover, the defeated animal cohabited with its aggressor in some protocols, being subjected to intermittent episodes of aggressive interaction but with continuous sensorial contact (Bartolomucci et al., 2003; Costoli et al., 2004), whereas animals were exposed only to intermittent episodes of social defeat in other studies (Sgoifo et al., 2001; Carnevali et al., 2013b), including those that identified changes in HR (Wood et al., 2012; Sévoz-Couche et al., 2013).

The increase in sympathetic activity induced by chronic stressors seems not to be restricted to the heart. For instance, Grippo et al. (2008) reported that a 4-week CVS protocol increased lumbar sympathetic nerve activity. In addition, an increase in tyrosine hydroxylase expression and activity has been reported in the sympathetic ganglia and adrenal medulla following exposure to chronic stressors (Nankova et al., 1994, 1996; Bobrovskaya et al., 2013). The relevance of the response in the adrenal medulla is unclear since changes in plasma concentration of adrenaline and noradrenaline were not identified following exposure to several chronic stress protocols, including RRS, footshock, social isolation, crowding, and CVS (Konarska et al., 1989; Dronjak et al., 2004; Gavrilovic et al., 2005; Spasojevic et al., 2009). Therefore, sympathoexcitation induced by chronic stressors seems to be mediated by an increase in activity of sympathetic nerves rather than changes in the sympatho-adrenomedullary system.

Long-term potentiation was reported in the sympathetic ganglia of animals subjected to cage-switch stress for 4 weeks (Alkadhi et al., 2005). It is an activity-dependent sustained increase in ganglionic transmission, which is similar to that described in the brain. Therefore, this sustained enhancement of synaptic efficacy may constitute an important mechanism underlying the increase in sympathetic tone following long-term exposure to aversive stimuli (Alkadhi et al., 2005).

Baroreflex function

Changes in the baroreflex control of HR were reported following exposure to several chronic stressors, including CVS, RRS, repeated social defeat stress, and chronic social isolation (Conti et al., 2001; Porter et al., 2004; Daubert et al., 2012; Xie et al., 2012; Sévoz-Couche et al., 2013; Almeida et al., 2015; Duarte et al., 2015a; Cruz et al., 2016). However, the effects in the baroreflex activity seem to be dependent on the duration of chronic stress. For example, exposure to CVS protocols of either 10 (Duarte et al., 2015a) or 14 days (Xie et al., 2012; Almeida et al., 2015) induced changes in the baroreflex control of HR, but a 4-week protocol did not affect baroreflex HR responses (Grippo et al., 2008). Nevertheless, the reflex increase in lumbar sympathetic nerve activity induced by hypotension was decreased in animals following exposure to a CVS protocol for 4 weeks (Grippo et al., 2008), indicating that lumbar sympathetic nerve and cardiac autonomic innervation are affected differently by CVS. In addition, different effects of CVS in reflex bradycardia induced by increased blood pressure were obtained when baroreflex function was evaluated in animals anesthetized (facilitation; Xie et al., 2012) and unanesthetized rats (impairment; Almeida et al., 2015; Duarte et al., 2015a), indicating that anesthesia may affect the analysis of cardiovascular changes induced by chronic stressors.

Most studies describing stress effects evaluated the baroreflex activity using the classical pharmacological approach (i.e., arterial pressure changes were induced by intravenous infusion of vasoactive agents). However, analysis of the baroreflex responses over the physiological range of fluctuations in arterial pressure without any pharmacological manipulations (i.e., spontaneous baroreflex) has also provided evidence of an impact of chronic stressors, but the effects are stress-specific type. For example, changes in spontaneous baroreflex activity were reported in animals subjected to repeated exposure to social defeat (Sévoz-Couche et al., 2013) and CVS (Almeida et al., 2015), but not following RRS (Daubert et al., 2012; Duarte et al., 2015b). Therefore, an impact of RRS in baroreflex activity was evidenced by the classical pharmacological approach alone (Conti et al., 2001; Porter et al., 2004; Duarte et al., 2015a). Acute ablation of specific central nervous system regions has different effects on the baroreflex responses assessed by the classical pharmacological approach and the sequence analysis technique (Crestani et al., 2010a; de Andrade et al., 2014), indicating that differences in the neural circuitry of reflex responses within the narrow range of physiological variations and during more pronounced arterial pressure changes could explain the specific influence of RRS on the baroreflex responses over the full range of arterial pressure changes. Nevertheless, further studies are necessary to clarify this issue.

The studies that evaluated cardiovascular function in unanesthetized animals provided evidence that both CVS (Grippo et al., 2008; Almeida et al., 2015; Duarte et al., 2015a) and repeated social defeat stress (Sévoz-Couche et al., 2013) impaired the baroreflex function. Impairment of the baroreflex function is proposed to be involved in the physiopathology of hypertension (Grassi et al., 2006; Honzikova and Fiser, 2009), and is associated with overactivity of sympathetic activity (Grassi et al., 2004). Therefore, sympathoexcitation and mild hypertension (reported in some studies, Table 1) induced by these chronic stressors may be mediated by changes in the baroreflex activity. In contrast, an increase in baroreflex sensitivity was observed following exposure to RRS (Conti et al., 2001; Duarte et al., 2015a). Moreover, a recent study did not identify an impact of chronic social isolation in baroreflex function in adult rats (Cruz et al., 2016). Therefore, an involvement of baroreflex changes in the physiopathology of stress-induced cardiovascular complications is dependent on the chronic stress type.

Cardiovascular responses to stress: influence of predictability

Most studies that characterized the influence of predictability of stressor in its responses evaluated alterations in neuroendocrine function, behavioral responses, somatic parameters, and brain morphology/function. Indeed, a possible difference in the impact of predictable vs. unpredictable stressors in cardiovascular function and autonomic activity was addressed only recently (Duarte et al., 2015a,b). Therefore, before discussing the influence of predictability in cardiovascular/autonomic responses to stress, a summary of the impact of predictable vs. unpredictable stressors in the HPA axis, anxiety- and depression-like behaviors, and somatic parameters is presented in order to discuss experimental results in rodents that characterized the influence of predictability of stressor stimulus and its consequences.

Characterization of influence of predictability

The impact of predictability of stressor stimulus was initially examined by comparing responses to signaled (e.g., aversive stimulus preceded by light) vs. unsignaled footshock. Some of these studies indicated that signaling minimized stress reactions (e.g., corticosterone release) and stress-induced pathology (e.g., stomach ulceration; Perkins, 1955; Seligman, 1968; Weiss, 1970), which supported the hypothesis that a reliable predictor of an aversive stimulus minimizes its responses (Seligman, 1968). However, these results were not consistent (Brady et al., 1962; Paré, 1971; Bassett et al., 1973), and the definition of predictability as identical with the presence or absence of a signal preceding a regularly occurring aversive stimulus was criticized (Bassett et al., 1973). Indeed, the predictability of a stressor in terms of time has been proposed as an important dimension of its consequences. For example, repeated stress applied varying the interval between each exposure (i.e., irregular) induced greater and more persistent increase in plasma corticosterone, fatty acid, and glucose levels than regular exposure (Bassett et al., 1973; Quirce et al., 1981; Smith et al., 2013). Increase in anxiety- and depression-like behaviors and somatic changes such as adrenal hypertrophy and reduction in body weight gain were also rather observed following stressor applied irregularly than regularly (Martí and Armario, 1997; Smith et al., 2013), besides some of these effects may be related to stressor intensity (Martí and Armario, 1997). Studies have also used protocols that vary the duration of each stress session, but with constant interval between the sessions (Rockman et al., 1987; Ortiz et al., 2015). Nevertheless, a comparison of the responses induced by this stress paradigm with those induced by a stressor applied regularly has never been investigated.

Studies have also evaluated the predictability by varying the type of stressor. These studies have compared the impact of daily exposure to the same stressor type (i.e., homotypic) vs. the exposure to different aversive stimuli (i.e., heterotypic). Typically, these studies have compared the effects of the RRS applied in a predictable schedule vs. the CVS, which is a widely used paradigm that involves daily exposure of rodents to different stressors at unpredictable times (Willnér, 2005; Grippo, 2009; Frisbee et al., 2015). In this regard, the CVS has been demonstrated to induce more severe somatic changes such as adrenal hypertrophy and thymus involution (Magariños and McEwen, 1995; Zucchi et al., 2009; Flak et al., 2012; Kopp et al., 2013; Duarte et al., 2015a), which is possibly related to an increase in baseline HPA axis activity observed mainly following exposure to heterotypic stressors (Magariños and McEwen, 1995; Marin et al., 2007). Increase in anxiety- and depression-like behaviors was also more severe following exposure to CVS than to RRS (Haile et al., 2001; Pastor-Ciurana et al., 2014; Yoon et al., 2014; Zhu et al., 2014; Gao et al., 2016). Although some studies report that CVS induces increased reduction in body weight gain (Marin et al., 2007; Gao et al., 2016), several studies have demonstrated that homotypic and heterotypic stressors similarly affect this parameter (Magariños and McEwen, 1995; Vyas et al., 2002; Flak et al., 2012; Yoon et al., 2014; Duarte et al., 2015a).

The lesser impact of homotypic stressors in neuroendocrine, behavioral, and some somatic parameters is possibly related to a habituation process upon repeated exposure to the same stressor, which is reduced during exposure to heterotypic stressors (Herman, 2013). This habituation process is mainly evidenced by a progressive reduction in HPA axis activation (Grissom and Bhatnagar, 2009). Indeed, adaptation to chronic stress determined by habituation of responses upon repeated stress exposure has been proposed, which limits the long-term impact of stress (Grissom and Bhatnagar, 2009; Herman, 2013). In this regard, the habituation process has been demonstrated to be impaired when homotypic stressors are applied irregularly (De Boer et al., 1989; Martí and Armario, 1997; Smith et al., 2013), which is in line with evidence discussed above wherein stressors applied irregularly induce more severe responses.

Influence of predictability in cardiovascular responses to stress

Cardiovascular and autonomic changes have been reported following exposure to both predictable and unpredictable stressors. However, few studies to date have investigated the influence of predictability by directly comparing cardiovascular and autonomic changes induced by predictable vs. unpredictable stress protocols. De Boer et al. (1989) initially demonstrated that noise applied regularly, but not irregularly, increased plasma noradrenaline concentration, whereas noradrenaline response to noise was decreased in animals subjected to irregular protocol. Plasma adrenaline response to noise was reduced by both regular and irregular protocols (De Boer et al., 1989). These results do not support an influence of predictability in the sympatho-adrenomedullary response to stress. However, a possible influence of regular vs. irregular aversive stimuli on cardiovascular parameters, such as blood pressure and HR, has never been evaluated. Indeed, an impact of predictable vs. unpredictable stressors in cardiovascular function was addressed only recently. In this regard, Duarte et al. (2015a) compared the effect of the homotypic stressor RRS (predictable) vs. the heterotypic stressor CVS (unpredictable) on the baseline values of arterial pressure and HR, baroreflex function, and cardiac autonomic activity. They observed that both chronic stressors increased the baseline arterial pressure values (Duarte et al., 2015a). However, increase in baseline HR values and cardiac sympathetic activity as well as impaired baroreflex function was observed only in animals subjected to CVS (Duarte et al., 2015a). Although, these results provide evidence of a more severe impact of unpredictable vs. predictable stressors in cardiovascular function and autonomic activity, relevant cardiovascular changes were also detected following exposure to the predictable stressor RRS.

As stated above, adaptation to chronic stress determined by habituation of physiological responses upon repeated exposure to the same stressor has been proposed, which limits its long-term impact (Grissom and Bhatnagar, 2009; Herman, 2013). This idea has been supported mainly by consistent findings of habituation of HPA axis activation (Grissom and Bhatnagar, 2009). Table 3 summarizes the studies that compared cardiovascular and autonomic responses during an acute stress session and after repeated exposure to the stressor. We note that studies have not consistently demonstrated a habituation of cardiovascular responses upon repeated exposure to a stressor (Table 3). For example, some findings indicated a decrease in pressor and tachycardiac response upon repeated exposure to restraint stress (Chen and Herbert, 1995; Bechtold et al., 2009), but several other studies reported similar cardiovascular responses to this stressor during both acute and repeated exposures (McDougall et al., 2000, 2005; Conti et al., 2001; Daubert et al., 2012). Analysis of cardiovascular and autonomic responses during repeated exposure to social defeat has also identified inconsistent results. Indeed, some results evidenced habituation (Adams et al., 1987; Chung et al., 1999; Costoli et al., 2004), whereas several other studies reported similar responses during repeated exposure to social defeat (Adams and Blizard, 1986; Meehan et al., 1995; Sgoifo et al., 2001, 2002; Carnevali et al., 2012, 2013b). Regardless of inconsistency of the results, evidence that cardiovascular responses do not readily habituate indicates a reduced process of adaptation (Grissom and Bhatnagar, 2009; Herman, 2013), which supports the findings of Duarte et al. (2015a) and other researchers (see above sections) demonstrating significant cardiovascular dysfunctions following exposure to predictable stressors (e.g., RRS and repeated social defeat).

Table 3.

Studies comparing arterial pressure, heart rate (HR), and autonomic responses during an acute session of stress and after repeated exposure to the stressor.

Specie/Strain Age/Gender Stressor and Protocol length Arterial pressure HR Autonomic activity References
WT Groningen rat 10 weeks/male Social defeat (12 episodes/75 min in 25 days) Tachycardia = Carnevali et al., 2013b
5-HT1A KO mice WT mice 12 weeks/male Social defeat (cohabited with resident, 5 defeat episodes in 15 days) Tachycardia = RMMSD = Carnevali et al., 2012
Sprague-Dawley rat 275–300 g/male Restraint 60 min/session 5 sessions Pressor response = Tachycardia = Daubert et al., 2012
WKY rat BHR rat WKY – 414 g BHR – 385 g male Restraint 12 h/session 57 sessions/week 2125 days Pressor response(both strain) Tachycardia(both strain) Bechtold et al., 2009
WKY rat SHR rat Sprague-Dawley rat 15–16 weeks/male Handling ~30s/session 10 days Air-jet stress 1h/day 10 days Restraint 1h/day 10 days Pressor response = Tachycardia = McDougall et al., 2005
Swiss CD-1/mice 12 weeks/male Social defeat (cohabited with resident, 515 min agonistic interaction/day) 15 days Tachycardia(only post-interaction) Bartolomucci et al., 2003
Swiss CD-1/mice 12 weeks/male Social defeat (cohabited with resident, 515 min agonistic interaction/day) 15 days RR interval SDRR and r-MSSD Costoli et al., 2004
WT Groningen rat 5 months/male Social defeat 10 days Open field 10 days RR interval = Cardiac arrhythmia = Cardiac autonomic response-
No habituation
Sgoifo et al., 2002
WKY/male 12 weeks Restraint 20 min/day 5 days Pressor response = Tachycardia = Conti et al., 2001
WT Groningen rat 5 months/male Social defeat Social victory 15min/session 10 sessions Victory RR interval and cardiac arrhythmias ↓ Defeat RR and cardiac arrhythmias = Victory SDRR and r-MSSD = Defeat SDRR and r-MSSD = Sgoifo et al., 2001
WKY rat SHR rat 1516 weeks male Restraint 1 h/day 10 sessions WKY = SHR = WKY = SHR(duration) McDougall et al., 2000
Lister hooded rat 180 g/male Social defeat 20 min/session (10 min interaction without contact+10 min direct interaction) 10 days (experiments light off) Tachycardia – partial habituation Chung et al., 1999
Wistar rat Adult/male Acoustic stimuli 5 stimuli in 60 s Pressor response = Tachycardia followed by bradycardia = Blanc et al., 1997
Lister Hooded rats 250 g/male Restraint 1 h/day 10 days Tachycardia Chen and Herbert, 1995
Long Evans rat 350–400 g male Social defeat Submission + 55 min protected contact (experiments light off) Pressor response = Tachycardia = Meehan et al., 1995
S/JR rat 7080 days male Social defeat 25 min/session 6 sessions Footshock 25min/session(intermittent) 6 sessions Defeat – habituation of depressor response Footshock – habituation pressor response Direct measurement over 2 sessions- Defeat – facilitation depressor response in second session Footshock –Non-significant response Defeat – non-significant effect Footshock = over days Direct measurement over 2 sessions- Defeat – habituation Footshock - habituation Adams et al., 1987*
Dahl-S+salt Dahl-R+salt 70–80 days (adult) Social Defeat (25 min) 6 sessions S/JR depressor response =
R/JR Non-significant response
S/JR Non-significant response
R/JR Tachycardia =
Adams and Blizard, 1986*

BHR, borderline hypertensive rat; Dahl-S, Dahl salt-sensitive rats; Dahl-R, Dahl salt-resistant rats; KO, knockout mice; SHR, spontaneously hypertensive rats; WKY, Wistar-Kyoto rat.

Bold indicates studies that identified reduction of cardiovascular and/or autonomic responses upon repeated exposure to the same stressor (i.e., habituation).

*

Tail-cuff method of cardiovascular recording.

Reasons for the discrepancy in findings of habituation of cardiovascular responses are unclear, but may be the result of methodological differences. For example, the protocol of one of the studies that identified habituation included some stress-free days (i.e., animals were subjected to restraint stress 5–7 days/week for 21–25 days; Bechtold et al., 2009), which is different from the studies that did not identify habituation to restraint stress wherein animals were continuously subjected to stress. In this regard, stress-free periods between periods of repeated stress exposure has been proposed to facilitate the habituation process (Grissom and Bhatnagar, 2009). Differences between species and strains may also contribute. For example, more consistent evidence of habituation to social defeat was observed in mice (Bartolomucci et al., 2003; Costoli et al., 2004), whereas similar responses upon repeated exposure to social defeat was observed in rats (Adams and Blizard, 1986; Meehan et al., 1995; Sgoifo et al., 2001, 2002; Carnevali et al., 2013b). A study using rats reported habituation upon repeated exposure to social defeat (Chung et al., 1999), whereas another report in mice did not identify habituation (Carnevali et al., 2012). However, strains of mice and rats used in these studies were different from those wherein a habituation process was observed in mice rather than in rats (Table 3), indicating that the strain may also affect the habituation process. Nevertheless, further studies are necessary to better address the habituation of the cardiovascular responses upon repeated exposure to the same stressor.

Concluding remarks

A significant increase in research using animal models on the impact of emotional stress in cardiovascular function and autonomic activity has occurred in last years. These studies have provided evidence that the impact of stress on the cardiovascular function is determined by stressor stimulus characteristics, such as chronicity and predictability. Regarding chronicity, changes in cardiovascular function and autonomic activity are well-documented during acute stress sessions. These responses constitute important short-term adaptive mechanisms, maintaining homeostasis and ensuring survival. However, enduring autonomic imbalance and cardiovascular dysfunctions are provoked mainly by long-term exposure to stressful events (i.e., chronic stress). In this sense, studies have provided consistent results regarding the effect of chronic emotional stress in baseline HR values, autonomic activity, and baroreflex function. Nevertheless, the chronic stress paradigm seems to be an important factor that determines the pattern of changes in these parameters. A detailed analysis of the effects of chronic stress in arterial pressure reveals inconsistent results; hence, an animal model of stress-induced hypertension is still missing.

Analysis of neuroendocrine and behavioral responses to stress has clearly demonstrated an influence of predictability of stressor in its effects. Nevertheless, only a limited number of studies compared the cardiovascular and autonomic changes following exposure to predictable vs. unpredictable stressors. These studies have not demonstrated a clear influence of predictability in cardiovascular dysfunctions induced by chronic stress. However, a possible influence of predictability in stress-induced cardiovascular complications is only beginning to be investigated, and a number of issues need to be addressed before more conclusive analysis can be performed. For example, adaptation to chronic stress determined by habituation of physiological responses upon repeated exposure to the same stressor has been proposed (Grissom and Bhatnagar, 2009; Herman, 2013), which explains the more severe impact of homotypic vs. heterotypic stressors in HPA axis activity, behavioral responses, and somatic parameters. However, habituation of the cardiovascular and autonomic responses is still a matter of debate. Furthermore, evaluation of cardiovascular changes following exposure to homotypic stressors applied regularly vs. irregularly is a relevant unvalued aspect in determining the influence of predictability, because this approach compares responses with an aversive stimulus of similar intensity applied at predictable vs. unpredictable schedules. Therefore, further studies are necessary to elucidate the influence of predictability of stressor in its cardiovascular responses.

Author contributions

CC designed, drafted, and revised the manuscript; and prepared the figures and tables.

Conflict of interest statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The author wishes to acknowledge the financial support from FAPESP (grants # 2012/14376-0 and 2015/05922-9), CNPq (grant #456405/2014-3), and Programa de Apoio ao Desenvolvimento Científico da Faculdade de Ciências Farmacêuticas da UNESP – PADC. CC is a CNPq research fellow (process #305583/2015-8).

References

  1. Adams N., Blizard D. A. (1986). Effects of social defeat on acute cardiovascular response in salt-sensitive and salt-resistant rats. Behav. Neural Biol. 46, 325–336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Adams N., Lins M. D., Blizard D. A. (1987). Contrasting effects of social stress and foot-shock on acute cardiovascular response in salt-sensitive rats. Behav. Neural Biol. 48, 368–381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Alkadhi K. A., Alzoubi K. H., Aleisa A. M., Tanner F. L., Nimer A. S. (2005). Psychosocial stress-induced hypertension results from in vivo expression of long-term potentiation in rat sympathetic ganglia. Neurobiol. Dis. 20, 849–857. 10.1016/j.nbd.2005.05.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Almeida J., Duarte J. O., Oliveira L. A., Crestani C. C. (2015). Effects of nitric oxide synthesis inhibitor or fluoxetine treatment on depression-like state and cardiovascular changes induced by chronic variable stress in rats. Stress 18, 462–474. 10.3109/10253890.2015.1038993 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Anderson G. A., Overton J. M. (1994). Acute exercise and cardiovascular responses to stress in rats. Physiol. Behav. 56, 639–644. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bartolomucci A., Palanza P., Costoli T., Savani E., Laviola G., Parmigiani S., et al. (2003). Chronic psychosocial stress persistently alters autonomic function and physical activity in mice. Physiol. Behav. 80, 57–67. 10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00209-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bassett J. R., Cairncross K. D., King M. G. (1973). Parameters of novelty, shock predictability and response contigency in corticosterone release in the rat. Physiol. Behav. 10, 901–907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Baudrie V., Tulen J. H., Blanc J., Elghozi J. L. (1997). Autonomic components of the cardiovascular responses to an acoustic startle stimulus in rats. J. Auton. Pharmacol. 17, 303–309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bayramgurler D., Karson A., Yazir Y., Celikyurt I. K., Kurnaz S., Utkan T. (2013). The effect of etanercept on aortic nitric oxide-dependent vasorelaxation in an unpredictable chronic, mild stress model of depression in rats. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 710, 67–72. 10.1016/j.ejphar.2013.04.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bechtold A. G., Patel G., Hochhaus G., Scheuer D. A. (2009). Chronic blockade of hindbrain glucocorticoid receptors reduces blood pressure responses to novel stress and attenuates adaptation to repeated stress. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 296, R1445–R1454. 10.1152/ajpregu.00095.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bernatova I., Csizmadiova Z. (2006). Effect of chronic social stress on nitric oxide synthesis and vascular function in rats with family history of hypertension. Life Sci. 78, 1726–1732. 10.1016/j.lfs.2005.08.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bernatova I., Puzserova A., Dubovicky M. (2010). Sex differences in social stress-induced pressor and behavioral responses in normotensive and prehypertensive rats. Gen. Physiol. Biophys. 29, 346–354. 10.4149/gpb_2010_04_346 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bernatova I., Puzserova A., Navarova J., Csizmadiova Z., Zeman M. (2007). Crowding-induced alterations in vascular system of Wistar-Kyoto rats: role of nitric oxide. Physiol. Res. 56, 667–669. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bernatowa I., Csizmadiova Z., Kopincova J., Puzserova A. (2007). Vascular function and nitric oxide production in chronic social-stress-exposed rats with various family history of hypertension. J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 58, 487–501. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Blanc J., Baudrie V., Tulen J., Ponchon P., Gaudet E., Elghozi J. L. (1997). Social isolation affects the pattern of cardiovascular responses to repetitive acoustic startle stimuli. Clin. Exp. Pharmacol. Physiol. 24, 40–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Blessing W. W. (2003). Lower brainstem pathways regulating sympathetically mediated changes in cutaneous blood flow. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 23, 527–538. 10.1023/A:1025020029037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bobrovskaya L., Beard D., Bondarenko E., Beig M. I., Jobling P., Walker F. R., et al. (2013). Does exposure to chronic stress influence blood pressure in rats? Auton. Neurosci. 177, 217–223. 10.1016/j.autneu.2013.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Bouzinova E. V., Moller-Nielsen N., Boedtkjer D. B., Broegger T., Wiborg O., Aalkjaer C., et al. (2012). Chronic mild stress-induced depression-like symptoms in rats and abnormalities in catecholamine uptake in small arteries. Psychosom. Med. 74, 278–287. 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31824c40a9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Brady J. P., Thornton D. R., De Fisher D. (1962). Deleterious effects of anxiety elicited by conditioned pre-aversive stimuli in the rat. Psychosom. Med. 24, 590–595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Bruder-Nascimento T., Campos D. H., Cicogna A. C., Cordellini S. (2015). Chronic stress improves NO− and Ca2+ flux-dependent vascular function: a pharmacological study. Arq. Bras. Cardiol. 104, 226–233. 10.5935/abc.20140207 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Bruder-Nascimento T., Campos D. H., Leopoldo A. S., Lima-Leopoldo A. P., Okoshi K., Cordellini S., et al. (2012). Chronic stress improves the myocardial function without altering L-type Ca+2 channel activity in rats. Arq. Bras. Cardiol. 99, 907–914. 10.1590/S0066-782X2012005000082 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Buchholz R. A., Hubbard J. W., Keeton T. K., Nathan M. A. (1986). Cardiovascular and neuroendocrine responses to behavioral stress after central or peripheral barodenervation in rats. Brain Res. 365, 360–364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Bundzikova-Osacka J., Ghosal S., Packard B. A., Ulrich-Lai Y. M., Herman J. P. (2015). Role of nucleus of the solitary tract noradrenergic neurons in post-stress cardiovascular and hormonal control in male rats. Stress 18, 221–232. 10.3109/10253890.2015.1013531 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Burke S. L., Head G. A. (2009). Cardiac and renal baroreflex control during stress in conscious renovascular hypertensive rabbits: effect of rilmenidine. J. Hypertens. 27, 132–141. 10.1097/HJH.0b013e328317a7a7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Cannon W. B. (1942). “Voodoo” death. Am. Anthropol. 44, 169–181. 3041754 [Google Scholar]
  26. Carnevali L., Bondarenko E., Sgoifo A., Walker F. R., Head G. A., Lukoshkova E. V., et al. (2011). Metyrapone and fluoxetine suppress enduring behavioral but not cardiac effects of subchronic stress in rats. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 301, R1123–R1131. 10.1152/ajpregu.00273.2011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Carnevali L., Mastorci F., Audero E., Graiani G., Rossi S., Macchi E., et al. (2012). Stress-induced susceptibility to sudden cardiac death in mice with altered serotonin homeostasis. PLoS ONE 7:e41184. 10.1371/journal.pone.0041184 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Carnevali L., Trombini M., Porta A., Montano N., de Boer S. F., Sgoifo A. (2013a). Vagal withdrawal and susceptibility to cardiac arrhythmias in rats with high trait aggressiveness. PLoS ONE 8:e68316. 10.1371/journal.pone.0068316 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Carnevali L., Trombini M., Rossi S., Graiani G., Manghi M., Koolhaas J. M., et al. (2013b). Structural and electrical myocardial remodeling in a rodent model of depression. Psychosom. Med. 75, 42–51. 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318276cb0d [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Carnevali L., Vacondio F., Rossi S., Callegari S., Macchi E., Spadoni G., et al. (2015). Antidepressant-like activity and cardioprotective effects of fatty acid amide hydrolase inhibitor URB694 in socially stressed Wistar Kyoto rats. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 25, 2157–2169. 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.07.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Carrive P. (2002). Cardiovascular and behavioural components of conditioned fear to context after ganglionic and alpha-adrenergic blockade. Auton. Neurosci. 98, 90–93. 10.1016/S1566-0702(02)00039-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Carrive P. (2006). Dual activation of cardiac sympathetic and parasympathetic components during conditioned fear to context in the rat. Clin. Exp. Pharmacol. Physiol. 33, 1251–1254. 10.1111/j.1440-1681.2006.04519.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Chandola T., Britton A., Brunner E., Hemingway H., Malik M., Kumari M., et al. (2008). Work stress and coronary heart disease: what are the mechanisms? Eur. Heart J. 29, 640–648. 10.1093/eurheartj/ehm584 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Chen X., Herbert J. (1995). Regional changes in c-fos expression in the basal forebrain and brainstem during adaptation to repeated stress: correlations with cardiovascular, hypothermic and endocrine responses. Neuroscience 64, 675–685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Chung K. K., Martinez M., Herbert J. (1999). Central serotonin depletion modulates the behavioural, endocrine and physiological responses to repeated social stress and subsequent c-fos expression in the brains of male rats. Neuroscience 92, 613–625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Conti L. H., Shannon M. H., Murry J. D., Printz M. P. (2001). Repeated restraint stress-induced increase in baroreceptor reflex sensitivity: role of corticotropin-releasing factor. Neuropeptides 35, 71–81. 10.1054/npep.2001.0847 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Coote J. H., Hilton S. M., Perez-Gonzales J. F. (1979). Inhibition of the baroreceptor reflex on stimulation in the brain stem defence centre. J. Physiol. 288, 549–560. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Costoli T., Bartolomucci A., Graiani G., Stilli D., Laviola G., Sgoifo A. (2004). Effects of chronic psychosocial stress on cardiac autonomic responsiveness and myocardial structure in mice. Am. J. Physiol. Heart Circ. Physiol. 286, H2133–H2140. 10.1152/ajpheart.00869.2003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Crestani C. C., Alves F. H., Busnardo C., Resstel L. B., Corrêa F. M. (2010a). N-methyl-D-aspartate glutamate receptors in the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus modulate cardiac component of the baroreflex in unanesthetized rats. Neurosci. Res. 67, 317–326. 10.1016/j.neures.2010.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Crestani C. C., Alves F. H., Tavares R. F., Correa F. M. (2009). Role of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis in the cardiovascular responses to acute restraint stress in rats. Stress 12, 268–278. 10.1080/10253890802331477 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Crestani C. C., Tavares R. F., Alves F. H., Resstel L. B., Correa F. M. (2010b). Effect of acute restraint stress on the tachycardiac and bradycardiac responses of the baroreflex in rats. Stress 13, 61–72. 10.3109/10253890902927950 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Crestani C. C. (2016). Adolescent vulnerability to cardiovascular consequences of chronic emotional stress: review and perspectives for future research. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.027. [Epub ahead of print]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Cruz F. C., Duarte J. O., Leão R. M., Hummel L. F., Planeta C. S., Crestani C. C. (2016). Adolescent vulnerability to cardiovascular consequences of chronic social stress: immediate and long-term effects of social isolation during adolescence. Dev. Neurobiol. 76, 34–46. 10.1002/dneu.22297 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Cudnoch-Jedrzejewska A., Czarzasta K., Puchalska L., Dobruch J., Borowik O., Pachucki J., et al. (2014). Angiotensin converting enzyme inhibition reduces cardiovascular responses to acute stress in myocardially infarcted and chronically stressed rats. Biomed Res. Int. 2014:385082. 10.1155/2014/385082 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Cudnoch-Jedrzejewska A., Szczepanska-Sadowska E., Dobruch J., Gomolka R., Puchalska L. (2010). Brain vasopressin V(1) receptors contribute to enhanced cardiovascular responses to acute stress in chronically stressed rats and rats with myocardial infarcton. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 298, R672–R680. 10.1152/ajpregu.00543.2009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Danese A., McEwen B. S. (2012). Adverse childhood experiences, allostasis, allostatic load, and age-related disease. Physiol. Behav. 106, 29–39. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.08.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. D'Angelo G., Mintz J. D., Tidwell J. E., Schreihofer A. M., Pollock D. M., Stepp D. W. (2006). Exaggerated cardiovascular stress responses and impaired beta-adrenergic-mediated pressor recovery in obese Zucker rats. Hypertension 48, 1109–1115. 10.1161/01.HYP.0000247306.53547.d4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Daubert D. L., McCowan M., Erdos B., Scheuer D. A. (2012). Nucleus of the solitary tract catecholaminergic neurons modulate the cardiovascular response to psychological stress in rats. J. Physiol. 590(Pt 19), 4881–4895. 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.232314 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. de Andrade O., Borghi S. M., de Souza H. C., Fontes M. A., Martins-Pinge M. C. (2014). Paraventricular nucleus of hypothalamus participates in the sympathetic modulation and spontaneous fluctuation of baroreflex during head up tilt in unanesthetized rats. Neurosci. Lett. 558, 1–7. 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.09.039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. De Boer S. F., Van der Gugten J., Slangen J. L. (1989). Plasma catecholamine and corticosterone responses to predictable and unpredictable noise stress in rats. Physiol. Behav. 45, 789–795. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Demirtas T., Utkan T., Karson A., Yazir Y., Bayramgurler D., Gacar N. (2014). The link between unpredictable chronic mild stress model for depression and vascular inflammation? Inflammation 37, 1432–1438. 10.1007/s10753-014-9867-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Dos Reis D. G., Fortaleza E. A., Tavares R. F., Corrêa F. M. (2014). Role of the autonomic nervous system and baroreflex in stress-evoked cardiovascular responses in rats. Stress 17, 362–372. 10.3109/10253890.2014.930429 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Dronjak S., Gavrilovic L., Filipovic D., Radojcic M. B. (2004). Immobilization and cold stress affect sympatho-adrenomedullary system and pituitary-adrenocortical axis of rats exposed to long-term isolation and crowding. Physiol. Behav. 81, 409–415. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.01.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Duarte J. O., Cruz F. C., Leao R. M., Planeta C. S., Crestani C. C. (2015a). Stress vulnerability during adolescence: comparison of chronic stressors in adolescent and adult rats. Psychosom. Med. 77, 186–199. 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000141 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Duarte J. O., Planeta C. S., Crestani C. C. (2015b). Immediate and long-term effects of psychological stress during adolescence in cardiovascular function: comparison of homotypic vs heterotypic stress regimens. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 40, 52–59. 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2014.11.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Flak J. N., Jankord R., Solomon M. B., Krause E. G., Herman J. P. (2011). Opposing effects of chronic stress and weight restriction on cardiovascular, neuroendocrine and metabolic function. Physiol. Behav. 104, 228–234. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.03.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Flak J. N., Solomon M. B., Jankord R., Krause E. G., Herman J. P. (2012). Identification of chronic stress-activated regions reveals a potential recruited circuit in rat brain. Eur. J. Neurosci. 36, 2547–2555. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08161.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Ford D. E., Mead L. A., Chang P. P., Cooper-Patrick L., Wang N. Y., Klag M. J. (1998). Depression is a risk factor for coronary artery disease in men: the precursors study. Arch. Intern. Med. 158, 1422–1426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Friedman M., Rosenman R. H. (1959). Association of specific overt behavior pattern with blood and cardiovascular findings; blood cholesterol level, blood clotting time, incidence of arcus senilis, and clinical coronary artery disease. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 169, 1286–1296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Frisbee J. C., Brooks S. D., Stanley S. C., d'Audiffret A. C. (2015). An unpredictable chronic mild stress protocol for instigating depressive symptoms, behavioral changes and negative health outcomes in rodents. J. Vis. Exp. [Epub ahead of print]. 10.3791/53109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Gao Y., Chen F., Kong Q. Q., Ning S. F., Yuan H. J., Lian H. Y., et al. (2016). Stresses on female mice impair oocyte developmental potential: effects of stress severity and duration on oocytes at the growing follicle stage. Reprod. Sci. 10.1177/1933719116630416. [Epub ahead of print]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Gardiner S. M., Bennett T. (1983). The cardiovascular and renal responses to short-term isolation in Brattleboro rats. Clin. Sci. 64, 377–382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Gavrilovic L., Spasojevic N., Dronjak S. (2005). Novel stressors affected catecholamine stores in socially isolated normotensive and spontaneously hypertensive rats. Auton. Neurosci. 122, 38–44. 10.1016/j.autneu.2005.07.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Gelsema A. J., Schoemaker R. G., Ruzicka M., Copeland N. E. (1994). Cardiovascular effects of social stress in borderline hypertensive rats. J. Hypertens. 12, 1019–1028. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Goodson M. L., Packard A. E., Buesing D. R., Maney M., Myers B., Fang Y., et al. (2016). Chronic stress and rosiglitazone increase indices of vascular stiffness in male rats. Physiol. Behav. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.03.031. [Epub ahead of print]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Grassi G., Seravalle G., Dell'Oro R., Facchini A., Ilardo V., Mancia G. (2004). Sympathetic and baroreflex function in hypertensive or heart failure patients with ventricular arrhythmias. J. Hypertens. 22, 1747–1753. 10.1097/00004872-200409000-00019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Grassi G., Trevano F. Q., Seravalle G., Scopelliti F., Mancia G. (2006). Baroreflex function in hypertension: consequences for antihypertensive therapy. Prog. Cardiovasc. Dis. 48, 407–415. 10.1016/j.pcad.2006.03.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Grippo A. J., Beltz T. G., Johnson A. K. (2003). Behavioral and cardiovascular changes in the chronic mild stress model of depression. Physiol. Behav. 78, 703–710. 10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00050-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Grippo A. J., Beltz T. G., Weiss R. M., Johnson A. K. (2006). The effects of chronic fluoxetine treatment on chronic mild stress-induced cardiovascular changes and anhedonia. Biol. Psychiatry 59, 309–316. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.07.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Grippo A. J., Carter C. S., McNeal N., Chandler D. L., Larocca M. A., Bates S. L., et al. (2011). 24-hour autonomic dysfunction and depressive behaviors in an animal model of social isolation: implications for the study of depression and cardiovascular disease. Psychosom. Med. 73, 59–66. 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31820019e4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Grippo A. J., Johnson A. K. (2009). Stress, depression and cardiovascular dysregulation: a review of neurobiological mechanisms and the integration of research from preclinical disease models. Stress 12, 1–21. 10.1080/10253890802046281 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Grippo A. J., Lamb D. G., Carter C. S., Porges S. W. (2007). Social isolation disrupts autonomic regulation of the heart and influences negative affective behaviors. Biol. Psychiatry 62, 1162–1170. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.04.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Grippo A. J., Moffitt J. A., Johnson A. K. (2002). Cardiovascular alterations and autonomic imbalance in an experimental model of depression. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 282, R1333–R1341. 10.1152/ajpregu.00614.2001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Grippo A. J., Moffitt J. A., Johnson A. K. (2008). Evaluation of baroreceptor reflex function in the chronic mild stress rodent model of depression. Psychosom. Med. 70, 435–443. 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31816ff7dd [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Grippo A. J., Moffitt J. A., Sgoifo A., Jepson A. J., Bates S. L., Chandler D. L., et al. (2012a). The integration of depressive behaviors and cardiac dysfunction during an operational measure of depression: investigating the role of negative social experiences in an animal model. Psychosom. Med. 74, 612–619. 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31825ca8e5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Grippo A. J., Pournajafi-Nazarloo H., Sanzenbacher L., Trahanas D. M., McNeal N., Clarke D. A., et al. (2012b). Peripheral oxytocin administration buffers autonomic but not behavioral responses to environmental stressors in isolated prairie voles. Stress 15, 149–161. 10.3109/10253890.2011.605486 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Grippo A. J., Santos C. M., Johnson R. F., Beltz T. G., Martins J. B., Felder R. B., et al. (2004). Increased susceptibility to ventricular arrhythmias in a rodent model of experimental depression. Am. J. Physiol. Heart Circ. Physiol. 286, H619–H626. 10.1152/ajpheart.00450.2003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Grippo A. J., Sgoifo A., Mastorci F., McNeal N., Trahanas D. M. (2010). Cardiac dysfunction and hypothalamic activation during a social crowding stressor in prairie voles. Auton. Neurosci. 156, 44–50. 10.1016/j.autneu.2010.03.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Grippo A. J., Trahanas D. M., Zimmerman R. R. II, Porges, S. W., Carter C. S. (2009). Oxytocin protects against negative behavioral and autonomic consequences of long-term social isolation. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 1542–1553. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.05.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Grippo A. J. (2009). Mechanisms underlying altered mood and cardiovascular dysfunction: the value of neurobiological and behavioral research with animal models. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33, 171–180. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.07.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Grissom N., Bhatnagar S. (2009). Habituation to repeated stress: get used to it. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 92, 215–224. 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.07.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Grundt A., Grundt C., Gorbey S., Thomas M. A., Lemmer B. (2009). Strain-dependent differences of restraint stress-induced hypertension in WKY and SHR. Physiol. Behav. 97, 341–346. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.02.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Habib M., Shaker S., El-Gayar N., Aboul-Fotouh S. (2015). The effects of antidepressants “fluoxetine and imipramine” on vascular abnormalities and Toll like receptor-4 expression in diabetic and non-diabetic rats exposed to chronic stress. PLoS ONE 10:e0120559 10.1371/journal.pone.0120559 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Haile C. N., GrandPre T., Kosten T. A. (2001). Chronic unpredictable stress, but not chronic predictable stress, enhances the sensitivity to the behavioral effects of cocaine in rats. Psychopharmacology 154, 213–220. 10.1007/s002130000650 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Hatton D. C., Brooks V., Qi Y., McCarron D. A. (1997). Cardiovascular response to stress: baroreflex resetting and hemodynamics. Am. J. Physiol. 272(5 Pt 2), R1588–R1594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Herman J. P. (2013). Neural control of chronic stress adaptation. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 7:61. 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00061 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Hilton S. M. (1963). Inhibition of baroreceptor reflexes on hypothalamic stimulation. J. Physiol. 165, 56–57. [Google Scholar]
  88. Honzíková N., Fiser B. (2009). Baroreflex sensitivity and essential hypertension in adolescents. Physiol. Res. 58, 605–612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Hubbard J. W., Cox R. H., Sanders B. J., Lawler J. E. (1986). Changes in cardiac output and vascular resistance during behavioral stress in the rat. Am. J. Physiol. 251(1 Pt 2), R82–R90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Igosheva N., Taylor P. D., Poston L., Glover V. (2007). Prenatal stress in the rat results in increased blood pressure responsiveness to stress and enhanced arterial reactivity to neuropeptide Y in adulthood. J. Physiol. 582(Pt 2), 665–674. 10.1113/jphysiol.2007.130252 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Inoue N. (2014). Stress and atherosclerotic cardiovascular disease. J. Atheroscler. Thromb. 21, 391–401. 10.5551/jat.21709 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Iriuchijima J., Kawaue Y., Teranishi Y. (1982). Blood flow redistribution in the transposition response of the rat. Jpn. J. Physiol. 32, 807–816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Ismail B., Aboul-Fotouh S., Mansour A. A., Shehata H. H., Salman M. I., Ibrahim E. A., et al. (2014). Behavioural, metabolic, and endothelial effects of the TNF-alpha suppressor thalidomide on rats subjected to chronic mild stress and fed an atherogenic diet. Can. J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 92, 375–385. 10.1139/cjpp-2013-0446 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Iwata J., LeDoux J. E. (1988). Dissociation of associative and nonassociative concomitants of classical fear conditioning in the freely behaving rat. Behav. Neurosci. 102, 66–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Jarczok M. N., Jarczok M., Mauss D., Koenig J., Li J., Herr R. M., et al. (2013). Autonomic nervous system activity and workplace stressors–a systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 1810–1823. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.07.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Kanbar R., Orea V., Barres C., Julien C. (2007). Baroreflex control of renal sympathetic nerve activity during air-jet stress in rats. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 292, R362–R367. 10.1152/ajpregu.00413.2006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Kapusta D. R., Knardahl S., Koepke J. P., Johnson A. K., DiBona G. F. (1989). Selective central alpha-2 adrenoceptor control of regional haemodynamic responses to air jet stress in conscious spontaneously hypertensive rats. J. Hypertens. 7, 189–194. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Kawachi I., Sparrow D., Vokonas P. S., Weiss S. T. (1994). Symptoms of anxiety and risk of coronary heart disease. The normative aging study. Circulation 90, 2225–2229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Kirby R. F., Callahan M. F., Johnson A. K. (1987). Regional vascular responses to an acute stressor in spontaneously hypertensive and Wistar-Kyoto rats. J. Auton. Nerv. Syst. 20, 185–188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Kivimäki M., Head J., Ferrie J. E., Brunner E., Marmot M. G., Vahtera J., et al. (2006a). Why is evidence on job strain and coronary heart disease mixed? An illustration of measurement challenges in the Whitehall II study. Psychosom. Med. 68, 398–401. 10.1097/01.psy.0000221252.84351.e2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Kivimaki M., Virtanen M., Elovainio M., Kouvonen A., Vaananen A., Vahtera J. (2006b). Work stress in the etiology of coronary heart disease–a meta-analysis. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 32, 431–442. 10.5271/sjweh.1049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Knardahl S., Hendley E. D. (1990). Association between cardiovascular reactivity to stress and hypertension or behavior. Am. J. Physiol. 259(1 Pt 2), H248–H257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Konarska M., Stewart R. E., McCarty R. (1989). Sensitization of sympathetic-adrenal medullary responses to a novel stressor in chronically stressed laboratory rats. Physiol. Behav. 46, 129–135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Kopp B. L., Wick D., Herman J. P. (2013). Differential effects of homotypic vs. heterotypic chronic stress regimens on microglial activation in the prefrontal cortex. Physiol. Behav. 122, 246–252. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.05.030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Krause E. G., de Kloet A. D., Flak J. N., Smeltzer M. D., Solomon M. B., Evanson N. K., et al. (2011). Hydration state controls stress responsiveness and social behavior. J. Neurosci. 31, 5470–5476. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6078-10.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Lagraauw H. M., Kuiper J., Bot I. (2015). Acute and chronic psychological stress as risk factors for cardiovascular disease: insights gained from epidemiological, clinical and experimental studies. Brain Behav. Immun. 50, 18–30. 10.1016/j.bbi.2015.08.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Ledvenyiova-Farkasova V., Bernatova I., Balis P., Puzserova A., Bartekova M., Gablovsky I., et al. (2015). Effect of crowding stress on tolerance to ischemia-reperfusion injury in young male and female hypertensive rats: molecular mechanisms. Can. J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 93, 793–802. 10.1139/cjpp-2015-0026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Liang J., Yuan X., Shi S., Wang F., Chen Y., Qu C., et al. (2015). Effect and mechanism of fluoxetine on electrophysiology in vivo in a rat model of postmyocardial infarction depression. Drug Des. Devel. Ther. 9, 763–772. 10.2147/DDDT.S75863 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Lown B., Verrier R., Corbalan R. (1973). Psychologic stress and threshold for repetitive ventricular response. Science 182, 834–836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Lu X. T., Liu Y. F., Zhang L., Yang R. X., Liu X. Q., Yan F. F., et al. (2012). Unpredictable chronic mild stress promotes atherosclerosis in high cholesterol-fed rabbits. Psychosom. Med. 74, 604–611. 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31825d0b71 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Magariños A. M., McEwen B. S. (1995). Stress-induced atrophy of apical dendrites of hippocampal CA3c neurons: comparison of stressors. Neuroscience 69, 83–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Marin M. T., Cruz F. C., Planeta C. S. (2007). Chronic restraint or variable stresses differently affect the behavior, corticosterone secretion and body weight in rats. Physiol. Behav. 90, 29–35. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.08.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Markovitz J. H., Matthews K. A., Whooley M., Lewis C. E., Greenlund K. J. (2004). Increases in job strain are associated with incident hypertension in the CARDIA Study. Ann. Behav. Med. 28, 4–9. 10.1207/s15324796abm2801_2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Marmot M. G., Smith G. D., Stansfeld S., Patel C., North F., Head J., et al. (1991). Health inequalities among British civil servants: the Whitehall II study. Lancet 337, 1387–1393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Martí O., Armario A. (1997). Influence of regularity of exposure to chronic stress on the pattern of habituation of pituitary-adrenal hormones, prolactin and glucose. Stress 1, 179–189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Martin D. S., Appelt C., Rodrigo M. C., Egland M. C. (1996). Acute stress increases venomotor tone in conscious rats. Am. J. Physiol. 271(4 Pt 2), H1375–H1383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Maslova L. N., Bulygina V. V., Amstislavskaya T. G. (2010). Prolonged social isolation and social instability in adolescence in rats: immediate and long-term physiological and behavioral effects. Neurosci. Behav. Physiol. 40, 955–963. 10.1007/s11055-010-9352-y [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  118. Matchkov V. V., Kravtsova V. V., Wiborg O., Aalkjaer C., Bouzinova E. V. (2015). Chronic selective serotonin reuptake inhibition modulates endothelial dysfunction and oxidative state in rat chronic mild stress model of depression. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 309, R814–R823. 10.1152/ajpregu.00337.2014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Matsuura N., Nagasawa K., Minagawa Y., Ito S., Sano Y., Yamada Y., et al. (2015). Restraint stress exacerbates cardiac and adipose tissue pathology via beta-adrenergic signaling in rats with metabolic syndrome. Am. J. Physiol. Heart Circ. Physiol. 308, H1275–H1286. 10.1152/ajpheart.00906.2014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. McDougall S. J., Lawrence A. J., Widdop R. E. (2005). Differential cardiovascular responses to stressors in hypertensive and normotensive rats. Exp. Physiol. 90, 141–150. 10.1113/expphysiol.2004.028308 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. McDougall S. J., Paull J. R., Widdop R. E., Lawrence A. J. (2000). Restraint stress: differential cardiovascular responses in Wistar-Kyoto and spontaneously hypertensive rats. Hypertension 35(1 Pt 1), 126–129. 10.1161/01.HYP.35.1.126 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. McEwen B. S., Stellar E. (1993). Stress and the individual. Mechanisms leading to disease. Arch. Intern. Med. 153, 2093–2101. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. McEwen B. S. (1998). Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. N. Engl. J. Med. 338, 171–179. 10.1056/NEJM199801153380307 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. McNeal N., Scotti M. A., Wardwell J., Chandler D. L., Bates S. L., Larocca M., et al. (2014). Disruption of social bonds induces behavioral and physiological dysregulation in male and female prairie voles. Auton. Neurosci. 180, 9–16. 10.1016/j.autneu.2013.10.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Meehan W. P., Tornatzky W., Miczek K. A. (1995). Blood pressure via telemetry during social confrontations in rats: effects of clonidine. Physiol. Behav. 58, 81–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Mercanoglu G., Safran N., Uzun H., Eroglu L. (2008). Chronic emotional stress exposure increases infarct size in rats: the role of oxidative and nitrosative damage in response to sympathetic hyperactivity. Methods Find. Exp. Clin. Pharmacol. 30, 745–752. 10.1358/mf.2008.30.10.1316822 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Michelini L. C. (1994). Vasopressin in the nucleus tractus solitarius: a modulator of baroreceptor reflex control of heart rate. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res. 27, 1017–1032. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Miki K., Yoshimoto M. (2013). Sympathetic nerve activity during sleep, exercise, and mental stress. Auton. Neurosci. 174, 15–20. 10.1016/j.autneu.2012.12.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Mohammed M., Ootsuka Y., Blessing W. (2014). Brown adipose tissue thermogenesis contributes to emotional hyperthermia in a resident rat suddenly confronted with an intruder rat. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 306, R394–R400. 10.1152/ajpregu.00475.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Mostofsky E., Penner E. A., Mittleman M. A. (2014). Outbursts of anger as a trigger of acute cardiovascular events: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur. Heart J. 35, 1404–1410. 10.1093/eurheartj/ehu033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Nagaraja H. S., Jeganathan P. S. (1999). Influence of different types of stress on selected cardiovascular parameters in rats. Indian J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 43, 296–304. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Nalivaiko E. (2011). Animal models of psychogenic cardiovascular disorders: what we can learn from them and what we cannot. Clin. Exp. Pharmacol. Physiol. 38, 115–125. 10.1111/j.1440-1681.2010.05465.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Nankova B., Kvetnanské R., Hiremagalur B., Sabban B., Rusnak M., Sabban E. L. (1996). Immobilization stress elevates gene expression for catecholamine biosynthetic enzymes and some neuropeptides in rat sympathetic ganglia: effects of adrenocorticotropin and glucocorticoids. Endocrinology 137, 5597–5604. 10.1210/endo.137.12.8940389 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Nankova B., Kvetnansky R., McMahon A., Viskupic E., Hiremagalur B., Frankle G., et al. (1994). Induction of tyrosine hydroxylase gene expression by a nonneuronal nonpituitary-mediated mechanism in immobilization stress. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 91, 5937–5941. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Natelson B. H., Cagin N. A. (1979). Stress-induced ventricular arrhythmias. Psychosom. Med. 41, 259–262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Nijsen M. J., Croiset G., Diamant M., Stam R., Delsing D., de Wied D., et al. (1998). Conditioned fear-induced tachycardia in the rat: vagal involvement. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 350, 211–222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Norman R. A., Jr., Coleman T. G., Dent A. C. (1981). Continuous monitoring of arterial pressure indicates sinoaortic denervated rats are not hypertensive. Hypertension 3, 119–125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Ortiz J. B., Taylor S. B., Hoffman A. N., Campbell A. N., Lucas L. R., Conrad C. D. (2015). Sex-specific impairment and recovery of spatial learning following the end of chronic unpredictable restraint stress: potential relevance of limbic GAD. Behav. Brain Res. 282, 176–184. 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.12.051 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Paré W. P. (1971). Six-hour escape-avoidance work shift and production of stomach ulcers. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 74, 459–466. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Pastor-Ciurana J., Rabasa C., Ortega-Sánchez J. A., Sanchis-Ollé M., Gabriel-Salazar M., Ginesta M., et al. (2014). Prior exposure to repeated immobilization or chronic unpredictable stress protects from some negative sequels of an acute immobilization. Behav. Brain Res. 265, 155–162. 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.02.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Paton J. F., Boscan P., Pickering A. E., Nalivaiko E. (2005). The yin and yang of cardiac autonomic control: vago-sympathetic interactions revisited. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 49, 555–565. 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2005.02.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Perkins C. C., Jr. (1955). The stimulus conditions which follow learned responses. Psychol. Rev. 62, 341–348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Porter J. P., King S. H., Honeycutt A. D. (2007). Prenatal high-salt diet in the Sprague-Dawley rat programs blood pressure and heart rate hyperresponsiveness to stress in adult female offspring. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 293, R334–R342. 10.1152/ajpregu.00887.2006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Porter J. P., Phillips A., Rich J., Wright D. (2004). Effect of chronic stress on the cardiac baroreflex in the post-weanling rat. Life Sci. 75, 1595–1607. 10.1016/j.lfs.2004.03.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Puzserova A., Bernatova I. (2010). Chronic social stress increases nitric oxide-dependent vasorelaxation in normotensive rats. Interdiscip. Toxicol. 3, 109–117. 10.2478/v10102-010-0049-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Puzserova A., Slezak P., Balis P., Bernatova I. (2013). Long-term social stress induces nitric oxide-independent endothelial dysfunction in normotensive rats. Stress 16, 331–339. 10.3109/10253890.2012.725116 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Puzserova A., Torok J., Sotnikova R., Zemancikova A., Bernatova I. (2012). Reactivity of the mesenteric bed arteries of normotensive rats exposed to chronic social stress. Gen. Physiol. Biophys. 31, 279–290. 10.4149/gpb_2012_032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Quirce C. M., Odio M., Solano J. M. (1981). The effects of predictable and unpredictable schedules of physical restraint upon rats. Life Sci. 28, 1897–1902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Rakhshan K., Imani A., Faghihi M., Nabavizadeh F., Golnazari M., Karimian S. (2015). Evaluation of Chronic physical and psychological stress induction on cardiac ischemia / reperfusion injuries in isolated male rat heart: the role of sympathetic nervous system. Acta Med. Iran. 53, 482–490. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Ravingerova T., Bernatova I., Matejikova J., Ledvenyiova V., Nemcekova M., Pechanova O., et al. (2011). Impaired cardiac ischemic tolerance in spontaneously hypertensive rats is attenuated by adaptation to chronic and acute stress. Exp. Clin. Cardiol. 16, e23–e29. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Rockman G. E., Hall A., Hong J., Glavin G. B. (1987). Unpredictable cold-immobilization stress effects on voluntary ethanol consumption in rats. Life Sci. 40, 1245–1251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Roest A. M., Martens E. J., de Jonge P., Denollet J. (2010). Anxiety and risk of incident coronary heart disease: a meta-analysis. J. Am. Coll. Cardiol. 56, 38–46. 10.1016/j.jacc.2010.03.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Rosengren A., Hawken S., Ounpuu S., Sliwa K., Zubaid M., Almahmeed W. A., et al. (2004). Association of psychosocial risk factors with risk of acute myocardial infarction in 11119 cases and 13648 controls from 52 countries (the INTERHEART study): case-control study. Lancet 364, 953–962. 10.1016/S0140-6736(04)17019-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Rosenman R. H., Brand R. J., Jenkins D., Friedman M., Straus R., Wurm M. (1975). Coronary heart disease in Western Collaborative Group Study. Final follow-up experience of 8 1/2 years. JAMA 233, 872–877. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Roth L., Rombouts M., Schrijvers D. M., Lemmens K., De Keulenaer G. W., Martinet W., et al. (2015). Chronic intermittent mental stress promotes atherosclerotic plaque vulnerability, myocardial infarction and sudden death in mice. Atherosclerosis 242, 288–294. 10.1016/j.atherosclerosis.2015.07.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Rozanski A., Blumenthal J. A., Kaplan J. (1999). Impact of psychological factors on the pathogenesis of cardiovascular disease and implications for therapy. Circulation 99, 2192–2217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Rugulies R. (2002). Depression as a predictor for coronary heart disease. a review and meta-analysis. Am. J. Prev. Med. 23, 51–61. 10.1016/S0749-3797(02)00439-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Schadt J. C., Hasser E. M. (1998). Hemodynamic effects of acute stressors in the conscious rabbit. Am. J. Physiol. 274(3 Pt 2), R814–R821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Scheuer D. A., Mifflin S. W. (1998). Repeated intermittent stress exacerbates myocardial ischemia-reperfusion injury. Am. J. Physiol. 274(2 Pt 2), R470–R475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Seligman M. E. (1968). Chronic fear produced by unpredictable electric shock. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 66, 402–411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Sévoz-Couche C., Brouillard C., Camus F., Laude D., De Boer S. F., Becker C., et al. (2013). Involvement of the dorsomedial hypothalamus and the nucleus tractus solitarii in chronic cardiovascular changes associated with anxiety in rats. J. Physiol. 591(Pt 7), 1871–1887. 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.247791 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Sgoifo A., Carnevali L., Grippo A. J. (2014). The socially stressed heart. Insights from studies in rodents. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 39, 51–60. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.12.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Sgoifo A., de Boer S. F., Westenbroek C., Maes F. W., Beldhuis H., Suzuki T., et al. (1997). Incidence of arrhythmias and heart rate variability in wild-type rats exposed to social stress. Am. J. Physiol. 273(4 Pt 2), H1754–H1760. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Sgoifo A., Koolhaas J. M., Musso E., De Boer S. F. (1999). Different sympathovagal modulation of heart rate during social and nonsocial stress episodes in wild-type rats. Physiol. Behav. 67, 733–738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Sgoifo A., Pozzato C., Costoli T., Manghi M., Stilli D., Ferrari P. F., et al. (2001). Cardiac autonomic responses to intermittent social conflict in rats. Physiol. Behav. 73, 343–349. 10.1016/S0031-9384(01)00455-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  166. Sgoifo A., Pozzato C., Meerlo P., Costoli T., Manghi M., Stilli D., et al. (2002). Intermittent exposure to social defeat and open-field test in rats: acute and long-term effects on ECG, body temperature and physical activity. Stress 5, 23–35. 10.1080/102538902900012387 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  167. Shammas R. A., Denison A. L., Pfennig T. W., Hemker D. P., Stephenson R. B. (1988). Baroreflex unimpaired by operant avoidance or classical aversive conditioning in dogs. Am. J. Physiol. 254(6 Pt 2), R1025–R1034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  168. Slezak P., Puzserova A., Balis P., Sestakova N., Majzunova M., Dovinova I., et al. (2014). Genotype-related effect of crowding stress on blood pressure and vascular function in young female rats. Biomed Res. Int. 2014:413629. 10.1155/2014/413629 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  169. Smith A. S., Lieberwirth C., Wang Z. (2013). Behavioral and physiological responses of female prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) to various stressful conditions. Stress 16, 531–539. 10.3109/10253890.2013.794449 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  170. Smith T. W., Glazer K., Ruiz J. M., Gallo L. C. (2004). Hostility, anger, aggressiveness, and coronary heart disease: an interpersonal perspective on personality, emotion, and health. J. Pers. 72, 1217–1270. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00296.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  171. Spasojevic N., Gavrilovic L., Kovacevic I., Dronjak S. (2009). Effects of antidepressants maprotiline and fluxilan on sympatho-adrenomedullary system in stressed rats. Auton. Neurosci. 145, 104–107. 10.1016/j.autneu.2008.11.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  172. Stanley S. C., Brooks S. D., Butcher J. T., d'Audiffret A. C., Frisbee S. J., Frisbee J. C. (2014). Protective effect of sex on chronic stress- and depressive behavior-induced vascular dysfunction in BALB/cJ mice. J. Appl. Physiol. (1985) 117, 959–970. 10.1152/japplphysiol.00537.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  173. Steptoe A., Kivimaki M. (2012). Stress and cardiovascular disease. Nat. Rev. Cardiol. 9, 360–370. 10.1038/nrcardio.2012.45 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  174. Sterling P., Eyer P. (1988). Allostasis: a new paradigm to explain arousal pathology, in Handbook of Life Stress, Cognition and Health, eds Fisher S., Reason J. (New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons; ), 629–649. [Google Scholar]
  175. Sterling P. (2012). Allostasis: a model of predictive regulation. Physiol. Behav. 106, 5–15. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.06.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  176. Sved A. F., Gordon F. J. (1994). Amino acids as central neurotransmitter in the baroreceptor reflex pathway. News Physiol. Sci. 9, 243–246. [Google Scholar]
  177. Timio M., Verdecchia P., Venanzi S., Gentili S., Ronconi M., Francucci B., et al. (1988). Age and blood pressure changes. A 20-year follow-up study in nuns in a secluded order. Hypertension 12, 457–461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  178. Toot J. D., Reho J. J., Novak J., Dunphy G., Ely D. L., Ramirez R. J. (2011). Colony social stress differentially alters blood pressure and resistance-sized mesenteric artery reactivity in SHR/y and WKY male rats. Stress 14, 33–41. 10.3109/10253890.2010.491876 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  179. Tornatzky W., Miczek K. A. (1993). Long-term impairment of autonomic circadian rhythms after brief intermittent social stress. Physiol. Behav. 53, 983–993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  180. Tsvirkun D., Bourreau J., Mieuset A., Garo F., Vinogradova O., Larina I., et al. (2012). Contribution of social isolation, restraint, and hindlimb unloading to changes in hemodynamic parameters and motion activity in rats. PLoS ONE 7:e39923. 10.1371/journal.pone.0039923 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  181. Ueyama T., Kasamatsu K., Hano T., Yamamoto K., Tsuruo Y., Nishio I. (2002). Emotional stress induces transient left ventricular hypocontraction in the rat via activation of cardiac adrenoceptors: a possible animal model of “tako-tsubo” cardiomyopathy. Circ. J. 66, 712–713. 10.1253/circj.66.712 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  182. van den Buuse M., Van Acker S. A., Fluttert M., De Kloet E. R. (2001). Blood pressure, heart rate, and behavioral responses to psychological “novelty” stress in freely moving rats. Psychophysiology 38, 490–499. 10.1111/1469-8986.3830490 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  183. Vianna D. M., Carrive P. (2005). Changes in cutaneous and body temperature during and after conditioned fear to context in the rat. Eur. J. Neurosci. 21, 2505–2512. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04073.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  184. Viken R. J., Johnson A. K., Knutson J. F. (1991). Blood pressure, heart rate, and regional resistance in behavioral defense. Physiol. Behav. 50, 1097–1101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  185. von Känel R. (2012). Psychosocial stress and cardiovascular risk: current opinion. Swiss Med. Wkly. 142, w13502. 10.4414/smw.2012.13502 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  186. Vyas A., Mitra R., Shankaranarayana Rao B. S., Chattarji S. (2002). Chronic stress induces contrasting patterns of dendritic remodeling in hippocampal and amygdaloid neurons. J. Neurosci. 22, 6810–6818. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  187. Weiss J. M. (1970). Somatic effects of predictable and unpredictable shock. Psychosom. Med. 32, 397–408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  188. Willnér P. (2005). Chronic mild stress (CMS) revisited: consistency and behavioural-neurobiological concordance in the effects of CMS. Neuropsychobiology 52, 90–110. 10.1159/000087097 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  189. Wood S. K., McFadden K. V., Grigoriadis D., Bhatnagar S., Valentino R. J. (2012). Depressive and cardiovascular disease comorbidity in a rat model of social stress: a putative role for corticotropin-releasing factor. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 222, 325–336. 10.1007/s00213-012-2648-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  190. Xie F., Sun L., Su X., Wang Y., Liu J., Zhang R., et al. (2012). Neuropeptide Y reverses chronic stress-induced baroreflex hypersensitivity in rats. Cell. Physiol. Biochem. 29, 463–474. 10.1159/000338500 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  191. Yang H. J., Kim K. Y., Kang P., Lee H. S., Seol G. H. (2014). Effects of Salvia sclarea on chronic immobilization stress induced endothelial dysfunction in rats. BMC Complement. Altern. Med. 14:396. 10.1186/1472-6882-14-396 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  192. Yip A. W., Krukoff T. L. (2002). Endothelin-A receptors and NO mediate decrease in arterial pressure during recovery from restraint. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 282, R881–R889. 10.1152/ajpregu.00308.2001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  193. Yoon S. H., Kim B. H., Ye S. K., Kim M. H. (2014). Chronic non-social stress affects depressive behaviors but not anxiety in mice. Korean J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 18, 263–268. 10.4196/kjpp.2014.18.3.263 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  194. Zhang Z. Q., Julien C., Barres C. (1996). Baroreceptor modulation of regional haemodynamic responses to acute stress in rat. J. Auton. Nerv. Syst. 60, 23–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  195. Zhang Z. Q., Julien C., Cerutti C., Paultre C., Barres C. (1992). Role of sympathetic nerve fibers in hemodynamic responses to stress in rats. Arch. Mal. Coeur Vaiss. 85, 1141–1144. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  196. Zhao Y., Wang W., Qian L. (2007). Hsp70 may protect cardiomyocytes from stress-induced injury by inhibiting Fas-mediated apoptosis. Cell Stress Chaperones 12, 83–95. 10.1379/CSC-231R.1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  197. Zhu S., Shi R., Wang J., Wang J. F., Li X. M. (2014). Unpredictable chronic mild stress not chronic restraint stress induces depressive behaviours in mice. Neuroreport 25, 1151–1155. 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000243 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  198. Zucchi F. C., Kirkland S. W., Jadavji N. M., van Waes L. T., Klein A., Supina R. D., et al. (2009). Predictable stress versus unpredictable stress: a comparison in a rodent model of stroke. Behav. Brain Res. 205, 67–75. 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.06.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Physiology are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES