Skip to main content
Journal of Epidemiology logoLink to Journal of Epidemiology
. 2016 Jul 5;26(7):361–370. doi: 10.2188/jea.JE20150153

Relationship of Having Hobbies and a Purpose in Life With Mortality, Activities of Daily Living, and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living Among Community-Dwelling Elderly Adults

Kimiko Tomioka 1, Norio Kurumatani 1, Hiroshi Hosoi 1
PMCID: PMC4919481  PMID: 26947954

Abstract

Background

This study’s aim was to clarify the relationship of having hobbies and a purpose in life (PIL; in Japanese, ikigai) with mortality and a decline in the activities of daily living (ADL) and instrumental ADL (IADL) among the community-dwelling elderly.

Methods

Prospective observational data from residents aged ≥65 years who were at increased risk for death (n = 1853) and developing a decline in ADL (n = 1254) and IADL (n = 1162) were analyzed. Cox proportional hazard models were used for mortality analysis of data from February 2011 to November 2014. ADL and IADL were evaluated using the Barthel Index and the Tokyo Metropolitan Institute of Gerontology Index of Competence, respectively. ADL and IADL were assessed at baseline and follow-up and were evaluated using logistic regression models. Fully adjusted models included terms for age, gender, BMI, income, alcohol intake, smoking history, number of chronic diseases, cognitive function, and depression.

Results

During the follow-up of eligible participants, 248 had died, 119 saw a decline in ADL, and 178 saw a decline in IADL. In fully adjusted models, having neither hobbies nor PIL was significantly associated with an increased risk of mortality (hazard ratio 2.08; 95% confidence interval [CI], 1.47–2.94), decline in ADL (odds ratio 2.74; 95% CI, 1.44–5.21), and decline in IADL (odds ratio 1.89; 95% CI, 1.01–3.55) compared to having both hobbies and PIL.

Conclusions

Although effect modifications by cognitive functioning and depression cannot be ruled out, our findings suggest that having hobbies and PIL may extend not only longevity, but also healthy life expectancy among community-dwelling older adults.

Key words: mortality, activities of daily living, elderly, hobbies, purpose in life

INTRODUCTION

Not only behavioral factors but also psychological factors have serious effects on mortality risk in the elderly. Prior studies have reported that behavioral factors, such as smoking,1 excessive alcohol consumption,2 and physical inactivity,2,3 and psychological factors, such as a low subjective sense of well-being,4 hopelessness,5 loneliness,6 and dissatisfaction,7 were associated with an increased risk of mortality. Additionally, behavioral factors, such as hobbies, and psychological factors, such as purpose in life (PIL), are considered important components of successful aging.8 Prior studies of community-dwelling elderly adults have reported that high engagement in hobbies was associated with significantly decreased mortality,911 and that a lack of PIL was significantly associated with an increased risk of mortality.12,13 Although these above-mentioned previous studies suggest that a lack of hobbies and PIL can have grave consequences in older adults, from the early prevention viewpoint, it’s also important to see what factors can directly affect an elderly person’s ability to live independently. Activities of daily living (ADL) and instrumental activities of daily living (IADL) are suitable indicators for these outcomes.14 However, to our knowledge, no studies have been conducted on the relationship of having hobbies and PIL with ADL and IADL among the community-dwelling elderly. Since social participation, including hobby activities and having PIL, are potentially modifiable factors,15 clarifying the association between hobbies, PIL, and functional capacity can provide a better understanding of how to increase the healthy life years of the elderly.

Our study hypothesis is that community-dwelling elderly who have neither hobbies nor PIL are more likely to experience not only an increased risk of mortality but also a decline in their ability to perform ADL or IADL compared with those who have both hobbies and PIL.

METHODS

Study area and subjects

The target area for this study was Shimoichi Town in Nara Prefecture, a rural Japanese town with an approximate population of 6900 residents. The target population was all residents aged 65 years and older. In February 2011, the town office mailed the baseline questionnaires to 2481 community-dwelling older adults (response rate: 83.1%). Figure displays the flow diagram of the enrollment of study participants. Among the 2061 persons (858 males and 1203 females) who participated in the baseline survey, we excluded 208 because of invalid responses for ADL, IADL, hobbies, and/or PIL. Thus, 1853 valid responses were obtained; mortality follow-up of these 1853 participants was performed for the period from February 2011 until November 2014. Compared to persons with valid responses, subjects without valid responses were marginally significantly older and fewer were males, but there was no difference in income (see eTable 1). Of the baseline population, 1556 were independent in their ADL, and 1399 had full ability to perform IADL. In July 2014, similar postal questionnaire surveys were sent out to obtain follow-up data. After excluding individuals with missing follow-up scores (ie, persons who had died, moved out of the study area, had invalid follow-up data for ADL/IADL, or did not respond to the follow-up survey), 1254 (80.6% of 1556) were analyzed for ADL, and 1162 (83.1% of 1399) were analyzed for IADL. Subjects excluded from the follow-up study were significantly older and had lower incomes than analyzed participants (see eTable 1).

Figure. Flow diagram of the enrollment of study participants. ADL, activities of daily living; IADL, instrumental activities of daily living.

Figure.

All study participants provided signed informed consent. This study protocol was approved by the Nara Medical University Ethics Committee (approval number 991).

Assessment of outcomes

The outcomes of interest in this study were all-cause mortality, ADL decline, and IADL decline. Regarding mortality, information about death and migration was obtained from the Shimoichi Town Hall; the vital statuses of the study participants as of the end of November 2014 were determined through their residential registration cards and death certificates. ADL was evaluated using the Barthel index (score range 0–100)16; higher scores indicate better ADL. Subjects with ADL scores ≥90 were considered independent.17 Decline in ADL was defined as a change from a score of ≥90 at baseline to a score of <90 at follow-up. Thus, participants with independent ADL at baseline were divided into two groups according to ADL score at follow-up: decline (<90) and no decline (≥90). IADL was determined using the 5-item Tokyo Metropolitan Institute of Gerontology (TMIG) Index of Competence (ie, the ability to use public transportation, daily shopping for necessities, meal preparation, bill paying, and managing their bank deposits).18 A full score of 5 was categorized as independent, and a score of 0–4 was categorized as dependent.19 Decline in IADL was defined as a change from a maximum score at baseline to anything less than a maximum score at follow-up. Thus, participants with a maximum baseline score on IADL were divided into two groups according to the IADL score at follow-up: decline (<5) and no decline (5 points).

Assessment of hobbies and a purpose in life (PIL)

For assessing hobbies and PIL, the questionnaire asked: “Do you have any hobbies?” and “Do you have ikigai?’’ directly translated as ‘‘Do you have a reason for living?’’. Ikigai is generally meant as feeling that life has a purpose, be it specific to the person themselves or overall, and can be briefly translated into either having a “purpose in life” (overall) or a “reason for living” (specific).13,20 Therefore, in this study, we treated ikigai as PIL. The response to each question was simply ‘yes’ or ‘no’. Subjects who answered ‘yes’ to the questions were defined as persons who have hobbies/PIL. The study participants were classified into four groups according to their responses to these two questions: both hobbies and PIL, hobbies only (ie, subjects who had hobbies but not PIL), PIL only (ie, subjects who had PIL but not hobbies), and neither hobbies nor PIL.

Covariates

Based on previous studies,913,2124 age, gender, income, body mass index (BMI), alcohol consumption, smoking habits, self-reported medical conditions, cognitive functioning, and depression were used as covariates that might correlate with hobbies, PIL, and mortality/disability. Information on age, gender, and income was obtained from the municipal office, and other covariates were gleaned from a questionnaire.

Income was used as an indicator of socioeconomic status. Income was classified into low (annual household income <1 million yen), medium (annual household income ≥1 million yen but annual individual income <1 million yen), and high (annual income of the subject ≥1 million yen). BMI was calculated as weight (kg) divided by the square of height in meters (m2), and defined as underweight (<18.5 kg/m2), normal (18.5–<25.0 kg/m2), and overweight (≥25.0 kg/m2). For lifestyle habits, the subjects were asked about alcohol consumption (nondrinkers, social drinkers, occasional drinkers, or daily drinker) and smoking status (never, former, or current). For medical conditions, the subjects were asked whether they were under medical treatment for any of seven major chronic conditions (hypertension, musculoskeletal disease, heart disease, diabetes mellitus, stroke, dyslipidemia, and cancer). The Cognitive Performance Scale (CPS: score range of 0 to 6) was used to rate cognitive functioning.25 A score of ≥1 was felt to be evidence of lower cognitive functioning. Depression ratings were determined through the use of the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS-5: score range 0–5),26 a 5-item form. Depression was considered a problem if a score of ≥2 was found.

All covariates were dichotomized: age (65–74 vs ≥75 years), gender (male vs female), income (low vs medium or high), BMI (normal vs underweight or overweight), alcohol intake (daily drinkers or not), smoking (former or current smokers vs never-smokers), the number of chronic diseases (≥2 or not), cognitive functioning (CPS score ≥1 or not), and depression (GDS-5 score ≥2 or not).

Statistical analysis

For mortality analysis, survival times were calculated starting from the enrollment date to either the date of death, the follow-up date (end of November 2014), or the latest registration date. Subjects who had relocated during the follow-up period were given censored survival times. Censored survival times were also given to cohort members who remained town residents when the follow-up finished.27 The Cox proportional hazards model was used to evaluate the univariate and multivariate relations between hobbies, PIL, and mortality. For disability analysis, logistic regression models were used to analyze whether hobbies and PIL at baseline were associated with a decline in ADL or IADL at 42-month follow-up. The independent variable was category of hobbies and PIL at baseline. In each model, the category of participants who had both hobbies and PIL was set as the referent category. For analysis of the association of hobbies and PIL with mortality, ADL, and IADL, we first conducted univariate analyses (Model 1) and then adjusted it for age and gender (Model 2). Subsequently, in addition to the variables adjusted in Model 2, income, BMI, alcohol intake, smoking history, and the number of chronic diseases were included in the model (Model 3). Finally, in addition to the variables adjusted in Model 3, cognitive functioning and depression were included in the model (Model 4). Additional stratified analyses by age, income, cognitive functioning, and depression were conducted to confirm whether these factors modified the effect of hobbies and PIL on mortality, ADL, and IADL. The level of significance was 0.05 (two tailed). Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS (version 17.0; SPSS Japan Inc., Tokyo, Japan).

RESULTS

Of the 1853 subjects (42.3% males, mean age 76.4 [standard deviation {SD} 7.1] years) analyzed, 62.4% (n = 1156) had hobbies and PIL, 7.4% (n = 137) had hobbies only, 16.4% (n = 303) had PIL only, and 13.9% (n = 257) had neither hobbies nor PIL. Baseline characteristics of study participants according to hobbies and PIL are shown in Table 1. Compared with those who had both hobbies and PIL, those who had neither hobbies nor PIL were more likely to be older, suffer from depression, and have low income, abnormal BMI, temperance in drinking, a recent medical history of stroke, and poor cognitive functioning. There were no marked differences among the four groups in gender, smoking history, and prevalence of chronic diseases other than stroke.

Table 1. Baseline characteristics of study participants according to hobbies and a purpose in life (ikigai).

Characteristics Both Hobbies only Purpose in life only Neither Pa
n = 1156 n = 137 n = 303 n = 257




n % n % n % n %
Demographics                  
 Age, years                  
  ≥75 557 48.2 87 63.5 209 69.0 203 79.0 <0.001
 Gender                  
  Male 505 43.7 56 40.9 111 36.6 111 43.2 0.162
 Income                  
  Low 404 34.9 64 46.7 152 50.2 149 58.0 <0.001
 Body mass index                  
  Normal (18.5–25.0 kg/m2) 752 65.1 86 62.8 186 61.4 120 46.7 <0.001
Lifestyle habits                  
 Alcohol intake                  
  Daily drinker 208 18.0 24 17.5 39 12.9 27 10.5 0.007
 Smoking history                  
  Ex- or current smokers 411 35.6 49 35.8 86 28.4 90 35.0 0.123
Physical and mental conditions                  
 Chronic diseases under medical treatment              
  Hypertension 490 42.4 54 39.4 139 45.9 97 37.7 0.234
  Musculoskeletal disease 181 15.7 29 21.2 55 18.2 52 20.2 0.132
  Heart disease 142 12.3 16 11.7 51 16.8 43 16.7 0.073
  Diabetes mellitus 144 12.5 15 10.9 41 13.5 31 12.1 0.902
  Stroke 45 3.9 11 8.0 23 7.6 37 14.4 <0.001
  Dyslipidemia 80 6.9 8 5.8 12 4.0 10 3.9 0.112
  Cancer 32 2.8 4 2.9 8 2.6 12 4.7 0.425
 The number of chronic diseases under medical treatment          
  ≥2 282 24.4 34 24.8 87 28.7 75 29.2 0.241
 Cognitive functioning (Cognitive Performance Scale)          
  Poor (score ≥1) 208 18.0 61 44.5 119 39.3 185 72.0 <0.001
 Depression (Geriatric Depression Scale)              
  Depression (score ≥2) 251 21.7 71 51.8 118 38.9 190 73.9 <0.001

aDifferences between the 4 groups were analyzed using Fisher’s exact test.

Follow-up for mortality could be completed for 1822 of the 1853 subjects enrolled (1574 alive and 248 deceased), because 31 had relocated away from Shimoichi Town by the time of follow-up (Figure). Table 2 shows the results from the Cox proportional hazards models of the association between having hobbies, having PIL, and mortality. In all models, having PIL only as well as having neither hobbies nor PIL were significantly associated with an increased risk of mortality. In Model 4, where the data was adjusted for all covariates, the hazard ratios (HRs) of mortality were 1.66 (95% confidence interval [CI], 1.17–2.35) for those who had PIL only and 2.08 (95% CI, 1.47–2.94) for those who had neither hobbies nor PIL compared with those who had both hobbies and PIL. Moreover, in additional analysis that excluded the deaths that occurred within the first 11 months of follow-up, these associations remained statistically significant: HR 1.65 (95% CI, 1.12–2.42) for those who had PIL only, and HR 1.95 (95% CI, 1.31–2.89) for those who had neither hobbies nor PIL.

Table 2. Results from cox proportional hazards models of association between hobbies, a purpose in life (ikigai), and mortality (n = 1853).

  Number of deaths Person-years Ratea Crude HR (95% CI) Adjusted HR (95% CI)


Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
Both 91 4184.7 2.17 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
Hobbies only 18 469.8 3.83 1.77 (1.07–2.93) 1.58 (0.95–2.61) 1.53 (0.92–2.54) 1.14 (0.68–1.91)
Purpose in life only 56 1025.9 5.46 2.51 (1.80–3.50) 2.16 (1.55–3.03) 2.03 (1.44–2.85) 1.66 (1.17–2.35)
Neither 83 782.0 10.61 4.87 (3.62–6.56) 3.82 (2.82–5.17) 3.37 (2.47-4.59) 2.08 (1.47–2.94)

CI, confidence interval; HR, hazard ratio.

aMortality rate per 100 person-years.

Model 1: Unadjusted.

Model 2: Adjusted for age and gender.

Model 3: Adjusted for age, gender, income, BMI, alcohol intake, smoking, and the number of chronic diseases.

Model 4: In addition to Model 3, cognitive functioning and depression were included.

Table 3 shows the results from logistic regression models of the association between having hobbies, having PIL, and a decline in ADL or IADL. Of the 1254 subjects with an independent baseline ADL and valid follow-up scores, 916 had both hobbies and PIL, 81 had hobbies only, 178 had PIL only, and 79 had neither hobbies nor PIL. During the 42-month follow-up, new ADL decline developed in 119 participants: 53 (5.8%) of those with both hobbies and PIL, 13 (16.0%) of those with hobbies only, 32 (18.0%) of those with PIL only, and 21 (26.6%) of those with neither hobbies nor PIL. In the crude model without any covariate adjustments (Model 1), hobbies only, PIL only, and neither hobbies nor PIL were strongly associated with a decline in ADL. However, after adjusting for all covariates, including cognitive functioning and depression (Model 4), the association of having hobbies only with ADL decline disappeared, and significant associations remained for having PIL only and having neither hobbies nor PIL: OR 1.95 (95% CI, 0.96–3.95) for those who had hobbies only, OR 2.56 (95% CI, 1.55–4.22) for those who had PIL only, and OR 2.74 (95% CI, 1.44–5.21) for those who had neither hobbies nor PIL compared with those who had both hobbies and PIL. Of the 1162 subjects with independent baseline IADL and valid follow-up scores, 870 had both hobbies and PIL, 71 had hobbies only, 159 had PIL only, and 62 had neither hobbies nor PIL. Further, 42-month declines in IADL were observed in 178 participants: 113 (13.0%) of those with both hobbies and PIL, 9 (12.7%) of those with hobbies only, 36 (22.6%) of those with PIL only, and 20 (32.3%) of those with neither hobbies nor PIL. In the univariate logistic regression analyses (Model 1), having PIL only as well as having neither hobbies nor PIL were significantly associated with a decline in IADL. After adjusting for all covariates (Model 4), these associations were attenuated but remained significant: OR 1.58 (95% CI, 1.02–2.47) for those who had PIL only and OR 1.89 (95% CI, 1.01–3.55) for those who had neither hobbies nor PIL compared with those who had both hobbies and PIL.

Table 3. Results from logistic regression models of association between hobbies, a purpose in life (ikigai), and a decline in ADL or IADL.

Outcome Outcomea/Total,b n/n (%) Crude OR (95% CI) Adjusted OR (95% CI)


Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
Decline in ADL (n = 1254)
 Both 53/916 (5.8) 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
 Hobbies only 13/81 (16.0) 3.11 (1.62–5.99) 2.66 (1.36–5.19) 2.52 (1.28–4.97) 1.95 (0.96–3.95)
 Purpose in life only 32/178 (18.0) 3.57 (2.23–5.73) 3.04 (1.88–4.92) 2.93 (1.79–4.79) 2.56 (1.55–4.22)
 Neither 21/79 (26.6) 5.90 (3.33–10.44) 4.81 (2.68–8.65) 4.30 (2.36–7.82) 2.74 (1.44–5.21)
Decline in IADL (n = 1162)
 Both 113/870 (13.0) 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00
 Hobbies only 9/71 (12.7) 0.97 (0.47–2.01) 0.88 (0.42–1.84) 0.83 (0.40–1.75) 0.67 (0.31–1.45)
 Purpose in life only 36/159 (22.6) 1.96 (1.29–2.99) 1.84 (1.19–2.82) 1.81 (1.17–2.78) 1.58 (1.02–2.47)
 Neither 20/62 (32.3) 3.19 (1.81–5.63) 2.86 (1.60–5.11) 2.68 (1.49–4.81) 1.89 (1.01–3.55)

ADL, activities of daily living; CI, confidence interval; IADL, instrumental activities of daily living; OR, odds ratio.

aThe number of people who developed an outcome (a decline in ADL or IADL) during follow-up.

bThe number of people who had independent ADL or IADL at baseline.

Model 1: Unadjusted.

Model 2: Adjusted for age and gender.

Model 3: Adjusted for age, gender, income, BMI, alcohol intake, smoking, and the number of chronic diseases.

Model 4: In addition to Model 3, cognitive functioning and depression were included.

Table 4 shows the results from analyses stratified by age, income, cognitive functioning, and depression. Significant associations of having neither hobbies nor PIL with mortality did not vary depending on age, income, or the presence of depression but were influenced by the status of cognitive functioning. Significant associations of having PIL only were observed among all subjects except for those aged 65–74 years and those with intact cognitive functioning. Significant associations of having neither hobbies nor PIL with ADL were unchanged according to age, income, or the status of cognitive functioning but were affected by the presence of depression. Significant associations of having PIL only disappeared for subjects aged 65–74 years and for those with depression. Among subjects with high income, poor cognitive functioning, or depression, there was no association between having hobbies, having PIL, and a decline in IADL. Significant associations of having neither hobbies nor PIL were observed irrespective of age, and among subjects with low income or those with intact cognitive functioning. Significant associations of having PIL only were observed only for subjects with low income and for those without depression.

Table 4. Stratified analyses by age, income, cognitive functioning, and depression.

  Mortality ADL decline IADL decline



n Adjusted HR (95% CI) n Adjusted OR (95% CI) n Adjusted OR (95% CI)
Among subjects aged 65–74 yearsa
 Both 599 1.00 519 1.00 493 1.00
 Hobbies only 50 0.82 (0.20–3.47) 33 2.44 (0.51–11.68) 32 1.08 (0.30–3.85)
 Purpose in life only 94 2.03 (0.92–4.52) 71 0.90 (0.20–4.06) 65 1.65 (0.74–3.68)
 Neither 54 2.45 (1.04–5.79) 27 11.59 (4.02–33.37) 22 5.12 (1.88–13.94)
Among subjects aged ≥75 yearsa
 Both 557 1.00 397 1.00 377 1.00
 Hobbies only 87 1.37 (0.79–2.39) 48 2.82 (1.28–6.21) 39 0.82 (0.31–2.19)
 Purpose in life only 209 1.62 (1.10–2.39) 107 3.14 (1.78–5.54) 94 1.71 (0.99–2.97)
 Neither 203 2.47 (1.72–3.54) 52 2.80 (1.33–5.88) 40 2.31 (1.09–4.91)
Among subjects with high incomeb
 Both 752 1.00 600 1.00 570 1.00
 Hobbies only 73 1.09 (0.47–2.53) 45 1.88 (0.61–5.82) 36 0.61 (0.18–2.09)
 Purpose in life only 151 2.12 (1.31–3.43) 97 3.21 (1.60–6.44) 86 1.68 (0.93–3.04)
 Neither 108 3.05 (1.94–4.80) 38 4.05 (1.63–10.10) 32 2.13 (0.92–4.92)
Among subjects with low incomeb
 Both 404 1.00 316 1.00 300 1.00
 Hobbies only 64 1.96 (1.02–3.78) 36 3.27 (1.35–7.92) 35 1.12 (0.43–2.92)
 Purpose in life only 152 2.06 (1.27–3.34) 81 2.76 (1.38–5.53) 73 2.03 (1.07–3.84)
 Neither 149 3.73 (2.41–5.79) 41 4.73 (2.12–10.58) 30 3.44 (1.49–7.93)
Among subjects with poor cognitive functioningc
 Both 208 1.00 127 1.00 113 1.00
 Hobbies only 61 1.66 (0.85–3.22) 21 0.75 (0.16–3.61) 19 0.75 (0.22–2.58)
 Purpose in life only 119 2.41 (1.46–3.97) 45 2.42 (1.03–5.67) 38 1.62 (0.73–3.59)
 Neither 185 3.26 (2.08–5.09) 34 4.02 (1.61–10.02) 21 1.70 (0.61–4.77)
Among subjects with intact cognitive functioningc
 Both 948 1.00 789 1.00 757 1.00
 Hobbies only 76 0.90 (0.36–2.24) 60 4.58 (2.15–9.76) 52 0.76 (0.29–1.98)
 Purpose in life only 184 1.34 (0.80–2.26) 133 3.48 (1.91–6.32) 121 1.68 (0.99–2.86)
 Neither 72 1.22 (0.58–2.58) 45 4.23 (1.85–9.71) 41 2.91 (1.40–6.05)
Among subjects with depressionc
 Both 251 1.00 156 1.00 147 1.00
 Hobbies only 71 1.23 (0.64–2.38) 34 1.60 (0.62–4.12) 28 0.42 (0.13–1.37)
 Purpose in life only 118 1.77 (1.09–2.87) 56 1.44 (0.63–3.30) 49 1.27 (0.59–2.73)
 Neither 190 1.97 (1.30–3.01) 57 1.96 (0.89–4.29) 44 1.67 (0.77–3.60)
Among subjects without depressionc
 Both 905 1.00 760 1.00 723 1.00
 Hobbies only 66 1.42 (0.61–3.31) 47 2.06 (0.68–6.24) 43 0.96 (0.36–2.55)
 Purpose in life only 185 1.88 (1.15–3.05) 122 3.94 (2.12–7.31) 110 1.90 (1.11–3.25)
 Neither 67 5.71 (3.38–9.64) 22 5.03 (1.66–15.24) 18 2.77 (0.93–8.24)

ADL, activities of daily living; CI, confidence interval; HR, hazard ratio; IADL, instrumental activities of daily living; OR, odds ratio.

aAdjusted for age (continuous variables), gender, income, body mass index, alcohol intake, smoking, and the number of chronic diseases.

bAdjusted for age (65–74 vs ≥75 years), gender, body mass index, alcohol intake, smoking, and the number of chronic diseases.

cAdjusted for age (65–74 vs ≥75 years), gender, income, body mass index, alcohol intake, smoking, and the number of chronic diseases.

DISCUSSION

In this prospective cohort study, we assessed the relationships of having hobbies and PIL with mortality and decline in ADL and IADL among community-dwelling elderly adults. We found that having neither hobbies nor PIL was associated with a significantly higher risk of not only mortality, but also decline in ADL and IADL, after adjusting for potential confounders, including age, income, cognitive functioning, and the presence of depression. Additionally, our findings suggest that having no hobbies may be more strongly linked to the risk of mortality and functional decline than lacking PIL.

There are several possible mechanisms to explain the observed relationships. First, hobbies can possibly reinforce neural networks and musculoskeletal abilities needed to keep independently functioning as a person’s physiological reserve capacity deteriorates as they age (the “use it or lose it” hypothesis).3,28 A prior study has shown that leisure activities, including hobbies, are associated with a slower rate of motor function decline.29 Second, a potential explanation for why having hobbies can prevent mortality and functional decline is an increase in physical activity due to hobbies. Prospective cohort studies of community-dwelling elderly adults have indicated that hobbies high in physical demand are more relevant to incident functional disability prevention than those involving a low level of physical activity.30,31 In our stratified analysis, the young elderly (aged 65–74 years) had no risk of mortality nor a decline in ADL and IADL, not only among the subjects with hobbies only but also those with PIL only. This result is in line with previous studies that showed that physical activity among the young elderly had less impact on survival2,3 and physical capacity32 compared to the older elderly. Third, the association of having hobbies with our outcomes might be mediated by an improvement in quality of life (QOL) derived from having hobbies. Prior studies found that the elderly with hobbies had a significantly higher level of QOL than those without hobbies33 and that better QOL was associated with reduced risk of mortality,34 difficulty in ADL,35 and IADL impairment.36 These mechanisms for the association of having hobbies with QOL and other outcomes are likely to be beneficial regardless of whether or not subjects have PIL, as subjects with hobbies but no PIL had no risk of a decline in ADL and IADL. Fourth, a prior population-based study investigated the association between the sense of having a life worth living (corresponding to PIL) and cause-specific mortality risk and found that increased mortality risk in those not having PIL had a significant risk of dying of cardiovascular disease.13 Other prospective cohort studies have reported that hypertension adversely affected ADL in persons free of diagnosed cardiovascular disease37 and that there was a significant relationship between the accumulation of cardiovascular risk factors based on medical examinations and a 5-year decline in IADL among community-dwelling elderly.38 Negative psychological state related to a lack of PIL could have the effect of changing an elderly person’s disease susceptibility due to the enhancement of sympathetic nervous system activity, decreasing heart rate variability, and increasing blood pressure,39 and this is often related to greater serum rates of both C-reactive protein and inflammatory cytokines and lower serum levels of high-density lipoprotein.4042 Each of these factors are known to be cardiovascular disease risk markers.13 Taken together, a negative psychological condition due to the lack of PIL might promote the risk factor profile for cardiovascular disease, contributing to an increased risk of mortality and a decline in ADL and IADL. Last, reduced stress buffering is also considered a pathway to mortality and disability. Previous studies have shown that having PIL provides people with the ability to integrate stressful psychological events with minimal confusion.43,44 This ability decreases psychiatric/somatic symptoms that occur in stressful conditions,43 which contribute to a protective effect on mortality.45 A prospective cohort study reported that engagement in hobby organizations decreased the risk of incident functional disability in community-dwelling elderly and indicated that the prevention of functional decline may be partly explained by the stress-buffering protective effect of hobbies.30 Taken together, since having hobbies and PIL allows an individual to manage stressful situations better, they can shield elderly people from mortality and the functional decline associated with stressful experiences.

Our findings suggest that a lack of hobbies and PIL may be a cause of ADL and IADL decline, since it was a longitudinal study and no subjects had shown any decline when the follow-up began. However, we cannot completely exclude the possibility of reverse causation. A number of reports have shown that age, low socioeconomic status, cognitive impairment, and depression are predictive factors for disability in elderly people.37,4648 To confirm the effects of these factors in relation to the association between having hobbies, having PIL, and ADL or IADL, we also conducted an analysis stratified by age, income, cognitive functioning, and depression. Our results suggest that age and income have little influence, but the status of cognitive functioning and the presence of depression modify the associations. These findings are consistent with those of previous studies of community-dwelling older persons that depressive symptoms affect social engagement, including hobbies49 and PIL (ikigai),50 and that cognitive impairment is associated with social disengagement51 and poorer sense of PIL.52 Therefore, attention should be paid to the fact that cognitive functioning and depression may modify the effects of our findings.

There are several limitations to our study. First, in the present study, PIL (ikigai) was assessed by a single, simple question. Although a single, simple question is useful for community-based health promotion and is the most commonly used assessment method in studies on “ikigai”,12,13,15,53,54 its validity and reliability have not been examined. The sense of “ikigai”, which is culturally defined as a subjective evaluation of well-being among Japanese, is often seen as an emotional concept in which a person can look back at how they have lived their lives, the joy and pride they have had, and the self-satisfaction it gives them.53 Recently, some researchers have developed scales consisting of multiple items to assess multidimensional aspects of “ikigai”.5557 Therefore, our results should be confirmed using more recent tools. Second, our results may have been biased by the exclusion of subjects who did not provide the required data or did not return the questionnaire, as individuals aged 75 years and older and individuals with low income were more likely to be excluded subjects (eTable 1). Although we have no baseline data for the non-responders, it’s possible that they may have had poor or failing psychological situations or functional capacity that resulted in their discontinuation from the study. We hypothesize that those at high risk for mortality and disability may have been differentially excluded in this study. This may have resulted in an underestimation of the association of having hobbies and PIL with mortality, ADL, and IADL. Third, the hobbies and PIL among our study subjects may have changed—both positively and negatively—while they were in the follow-up period. Unfortunately, we had no access to such data. Last, we realize that some other unforeseen factors could have altered our observations. One example would be our lack of physical activity data. Because population-based surveys of the elderly have shown that those without participation in hobbies10 or without PIL13 are less likely to exercise compared with those with hobbies or PIL and that physical activity is associated with a reduced risk of mortality2,3 and disability,58 including physical activity data, might have changed the associations we observed.

In conclusion, we found that having hobbies and PIL affected not only the risk of mortality but also decline in ADL and IADL among community-dwelling elderly. Although we cannot rule out the possibility that having hobby activities and PIL may be affected by the status of cognitive functioning and the presence of depression, our findings suggest that health-promoting approaches, such as promoting engagement in hobbies and encouraging PIL, may be useful in preventing decline in ADL and IADL as well as reducing the risk of mortality.

ONLINE ONLY MATERIAL

eTable 1. Basic attributes of subjects with or without valid responses at baseline and analyzed or excluded subjects at follow-up.
je-26-361-s001.pdf (59.8KB, pdf)

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This work was supported by center administrative expense from Nara Medical University. The authors wish to express our gratitude to all the participants for their cooperation in this study.

Conflicts of interest: None declared.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Gellert C, Schöttker B, Brenner H. Smoking and all-cause mortality in older people: systematic review and meta-analysis. Arch Intern Med. 2012;172:837–44. 10.1001/archinternmed.2012.1397 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Rizzuto D, Fratiglioni L. Lifestyle factors related to mortality and survival: a mini-review. Gerontology. 2014;60:327–35. 10.1159/000356771 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Woodcock J, Franco OH, Orsini N, Roberts I. Non-vigorous physical activity and all-cause mortality: systematic review and meta-analysis of cohort studies. Int J Epidemiol. 2011;40:121–38. 10.1093/ije/dyq104 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Chida Y, Steptoe A. Positive psychological well-being and mortality: a quantitative review of prospective observational studies. Psychosom Med. 2008;70:741–56. 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31818105ba [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Stern SL, Dhanda R, Hazuda HP. Hopelessness predicts mortality in older Mexican and European Americans. Psychosom Med. 2001;63:344–51. 10.1097/00006842-200105000-00003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Luo Y, Hawkley LC, Waite LJ, Cacioppo JT. Loneliness, health, and mortality in old age: a national longitudinal study. Soc Sci Med. 2012;74:907–14. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.11.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Koivumaa-Honkanen H, Honkanen R, Viinamäki H, Heikkilä K, Kaprio J, Koskenvuo M. Self-reported life satisfaction and 20-year mortality in healthy Finnish adults. Am J Epidemiol. 2000;152:983–91. 10.1093/aje/152.10.983 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Rowe JW, Kahn RL. Successful aging 2.0: conceptual expansions for the 21st century. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2015;70:593–6. 10.1093/geronb/gbv025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Konlaan BB, Bygren LO, Johansson SE. Visiting the cinema, concerts, museums or art exhibitions as determinant of survival: a Swedish fourteen-year cohort follow-up. Scand J Public Health. 2000;28:174–8. 10.1177/14034948000280030501 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Hyyppä MT, Mäki J, Impivaara O, Aromaa A. Leisure participation predicts survival: a population-based study in Finland. Health Promot Int. 2006;21:5–12. 10.1093/heapro/dai027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Fushiki Y, Ohnishi H, Sakauchi F, Oura A, Mori M. Relationship of hobby activities with mortality and frailty among community-dwelling elderly adults: results of a follow-up study in Japan. J Epidemiol. 2012;22:340–7. 10.2188/jea.JE20110057 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Koizumi M, Ito H, Kaneko Y, Motohashi Y. Effect of having a sense of purpose in life on the risk of death from cardiovascular diseases. J Epidemiol. 2008;18:191–6. 10.2188/jea.JE2007388 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Sone T, Nakaya N, Ohmori K, Shimazu T, Higashiguchi M, Kakizaki M, et al. . Sense of life worth living (ikigai) and mortality in Japan: Ohsaki Study. Psychosom Med. 2008;70:709–15. 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31817e7e64 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Lawton MP, Brody EM. Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist. 1969;9:179–86. 10.1093/geront/9.3_Part_1.179 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Nakanishi N, Fukuda H, Tatara K. Changes in psychosocial conditions and eventual mortality in community-residing elderly people. J Epidemiol. 2003;13:72–9. 10.2188/jea.13.72 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Mahoney FI, Barthel DW. Functional evaluation: the Barthel index. Md State Med J. 1965;14:61–5. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Granger CV, Albrecht GL, Hamilton BB. Outcome of comprehensive medical rehabilitation: measures of PULSES profile and Barthel Index. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1979;60:145. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Koyano W, Shibata H, Nakazato K, Haga H, Suyama Y. Measurement of competence: reliability and validity of the TMIG Index of Competence. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 1991;13:103–16. 10.1016/0167-4943(91)90053-S [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Azegami M, Ohira M, Miyoshi K, Kobayashi C, Hongo M, Yanagihashi R, et al. . Effect of single and multi-joint lower extremity muscle strength on the functional capacity and ADL/IADL status in Japanese community-dwelling older adults. Nurs Health Sci. 2007;9:168–76. 10.1111/j.1442-2018.2007.00317.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Nakanishi N. ‘Ikigai’ in older Japanese people. Age Ageing. 1999;28:323–4. 10.1093/ageing/28.3.323 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Ishizaki T, Watanabe S, Suzuki T, Shibata H, Haga H. Predictors for functional decline among nondisabled older Japanese living in a community during a 3-year follow-up. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2000;48:1424–9. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2000.tb02632.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Fujiwara Y, Shinkai S, Kumagai S, Amano H, Yoshida Y, Yoshida H, et al. . Longitudinal changes in higher-level functional capacity of an older population living in a Japanese urban community. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2003;36:141–53. 10.1016/S0167-4943(02)00081-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Tsubota-Utsugi M, Ito-Sato R, Ohkubo T, Kikuya M, Asayama K, Metoki H, et al. . Health behaviors as predictors for declines in higher-level functional capacity in older adults: the Ohasama study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2011;59:1993–2000. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03633.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Tomioka K, Kurumatani N, Hosoi H. Social participation and the prevention of decline in effectance among community-dwelling elderly: a population-based cohort study. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0139065. 10.1371/journal.pone.0139065 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Morris JN, Fries BE, Mehr DR, Hawes C, Phillips C, Mor V, et al. . MDS Cognitive Performance Scale. J Gerontol. 1994;49:M174–82. 10.1093/geronj/49.4.M174 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Hoyl MT, Alessi CA, Harker JO, Josephson KR, Pietruszka FM, Koelfgen M, et al. . Development and testing of a five-item version of the Geriatric Depression Scale. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1999;47:873–8. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1999.tb03848.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Nakanishi N, Fukuda H, Takatorige T, Tatara K. Relationship between self-assessed masticatory disability and 9-year mortality in a cohort of community-residing elderly people. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53:54–8. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53010.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.James BD, Boyle PA, Buchman AS, Bennett DA. Relation of late-life social activity with incident disability among community-dwelling older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2011;66:467–73. 10.1093/gerona/glq231 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Buchman AS, Boyle PA, Wilson RS, Fleischman DA, Leurgans S, Bennett DA. Association between late-life social activity and motor decline in older adults. Arch Intern Med. 2009;169:1139–46. 10.1001/archinternmed.2009.135 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Kanamori S, Kai Y, Aida J, Kondo K, Kawachi I, Hirai H, et al. . Social participation and the prevention of functional disability in older Japanese: the JAGES cohort study. PLoS One. 2014;9:e99638. 10.1371/journal.pone.0099638 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Takeda T, Kondo K, Hirai H. Psychosocial risk factors involved in progressive dementia-associated senility among the elderly residing at home. AGES project—three year cohort longitudinal study. Nihon Koshu Eisei Zasshi. 2010;57:1054–65 (in Japanese). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Manini TM, Pahor M. Physical activity and maintaining physical function in older adults. Br J Sports Med. 2009;43:28–31. 10.1136/bjsm.2008.053736 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Takeda T, Kondo K, Yoshii K, Kuze J, Higuchi K. Hobbies and ikigai among community-dwelling elderly adults. Clues to care prevention by occupational therapists. Gen Rehabil. 2005;33:469–76 (in Japanese). [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Tsai SY, Chi LY, Lee CH, Chou P. Health-related quality of life as a predictor of mortality among community-dwelling older persons. Eur J Epidemiol. 2007;22:19–26. 10.1007/s10654-006-9092-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Atlas A, Grimmer K, Kennedy K. Early indications that low mental quality of life scores in recently unwell older people predict downstream functional decline. Clin Interv Aging. 2015;10:703–12. 10.2147/CIA.S74613 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.d’Orsi E, Xavier AJ, Steptoe A, de Oliveira C, Ramos LR, Orrell M, et al. . Socioeconomic and lifestyle factors related to instrumental activity of daily living dynamics: results from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2014;62:1630–9. 10.1111/jgs.12990 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Pinsky JL, Branch LG, Jette AM, Haynes SG, Feinleib M, Cornoni-Huntley JC, et al. . Framingham Disability Study: Relationship of disability to cardiovascular risk factors among persons free of diagnosed cardiovascular disease. Am J Epidemiol. 1985;122:644–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Hayakawa T, Okamura T, Okayama A, Kanda H, Watanabe M, Kita Y, et al. . Relationship between 5-year decline in instrumental activity of daily living and accumulation of cardiovascular risk factors: NIPPON DATA90. J Atheroscler Thromb. 2010;17:64–72. 10.5551/jat.1727 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Steptoe A. Psychophysiological contributions to behavioral medicine and psychosomatics. In: Cacioppo JT, Tassinary LG, Bernston G, editors. The Handbook of Psychophysiology, 3rd edition. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Horsten M, Wamala SP, Vingerhoets A, Orth-Gomer K. Depressive symptoms, social support, and lipid profile in healthy middle-aged women. Psychosom Med. 1997;59:521–8. 10.1097/00006842-199709000-00009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Carney RM, Freedland KE, Miller GE, Jaffe AS. Depression as a risk factor for cardiac mortality and morbidity: a review of potential mechanisms. J Psychosom Res. 2002;53:897–902. 10.1016/S0022-3999(02)00311-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Lekander M, Elofsson S, Neve IM, Hansson LO, Undén AL. Self-rated health is related to levels of circulating cytokines. Psychosom Med. 2004;66:559–63. 10.1097/01.psy.0000130491.95823.94 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Ishida R, Okada M. Effects of a firm purpose in life on anxiety and sympathetic nervous activity caused by emotional stress: Assessment by psycho-physiological method. Stress Health. 2006;22:275–81. 10.1002/smi.1095 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Ishida R, Okada M. Factors influencing the development of “Purpose in Life” and its relationship to coping with mental stress. Psychology. 2011;2:29–34. 10.4236/psych.2011.21005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Ishida R, Okada M. Emotion and the autonomic nervous activity against psychological stress are affected by firmness of purpose in life. Health. (London). 2011;3:507–11. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Kamiyama T, Muratani H, Kimura Y, Fukiyama K, Abe K, Fujii J, et al. . Factors related to impairment of activities of daily living. Intern Med. 1999;38:698–704. 10.2169/internalmedicine.38.698 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Strine TW, Kroenke K, Dhingra S, Balluz LS, Gonzalez O, Berry JT, et al. . The associations between depression, health-related quality of life, social support, life satisfaction, and disability in community-dwelling US adults. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2009;197:61–4. 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181924ad8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Umegaki H, Suzuki Y, Yanagawa M, Nonogaki Z, Nakashima H, Kuzuya M, et al. . Cognitive impairments and functional declines in older adults at high risk for care needs. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2013;13:77–82. 10.1111/j.1447-0594.2012.00864.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Glass TA, De Leon CF, Bassuk SS, Berkman LF. Social engagement and depressive symptoms in late life: longitudinal findings. J Aging Health. 2006;18:604–28. 10.1177/0898264306291017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Fujimoto K, Okada K, Izumi T, Mori K, Yano E, Konishi M. Factors defining Ikigai of older adults who are living at home. J Healt Welf Stat. 2004;51:24–32 (in Japanese). [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Hughes TF, Flatt JD, Fu B, Chang CC, Ganguli M. Engagement in social activities and progression from mild to severe cognitive impairment: the MYHAT study. Int Psychogeriatr. 2013;25:587–95. 10.1017/S1041610212002086 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Boyle PA, Buchman AS, Barnes LL, Bennett DA. Effect of a purpose in life on risk of incident Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment in community-dwelling older persons. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67:304–10. 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.208 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Shirai K, Iso H, Fukuda H, Toyoda Y, Takatorige T, Tatara K. Factors associated with “Ikigai” among members of a public temporary employment agency for seniors (Silver Human Resources Centre) in Japan; gender differences. Health Qual Life Outcomes. 2006;4:12. 10.1186/1477-7525-4-12 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Tanno K, Sakata K; Japan Collaborative Cohort Study for Evaluation of Cancer . Psychological factors and mortality in the Japan Collaborative Cohort Study for Evaluation of Cancer (JACC). Asian Pac J Cancer Prev. 2007;8 Suppl:113–22. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Kondo T, Kamata J. Construction of ‘the K-I scale for the feeling that life is worth living among the aged’ and the definition of this feeling. Jpn J Soc Welfare. 2003;43:93–101 (in Japanese). [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Hasegawa A, Miyazaki T, Iimori H, Hoshi T, Kawamura N. Construction of IKIGAI objects scales—Measuring IKIGAI objects and type of IKIGAI. Jpn J Psychosom Int Med. 2007;11:5–10 (in Japanese). [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Imai T. [The reliability and validity of a new scale for measuring the concept of Ikigai (Ikigai-9)]. Nihon Koshu Eisei Zasshi. 2012;59:433–9 (in Japanese). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Boyle PA, Buchman AS, Wilson RS, Bienias JL, Bennett DA. Physical activity is associated with incident disability in community-based older persons. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2007;55:195–201. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01038.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

eTable 1. Basic attributes of subjects with or without valid responses at baseline and analyzed or excluded subjects at follow-up.
je-26-361-s001.pdf (59.8KB, pdf)

Articles from Journal of Epidemiology are provided here courtesy of Japan Epidemiological Association

RESOURCES