Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Jul 1.
Published in final edited form as: Int J Law Psychiatry. 2014 Mar 15;37(4):359–369. doi: 10.1016/j.ijlp.2014.02.006

The Influence of Neighborhood Characteristics on Police Officers’ Encounters with Persons Suspected to Have a Serious Mental Illness

Shaily Krishan 1, Roger Bakeman 2, Beth Broussard 3, Sarah L Cristofaro 1, Dana Hankerson-Dyson 1, Letheshia Husbands 1, Amy C Watson 4, Michael T Compton 3,5,*
PMCID: PMC4929689  NIHMSID: NIHMS571553  PMID: 24636571

Abstract

Objective

Police officers’ decisions and behaviors are impacted by the neighborhood context in which police encounters occur. For example, officers may use greater force and be more likely to make arrests in disadvantaged neighborhoods. We examined whether neighborhood characteristics influence police encounters with individuals suspected to have a serious mental illness, addictive disorder, or developmental disability.

Method

We obtained data on 916 encounters from 166 officers in six jurisdictions in Georgia, USA and abstracted geographical data pertaining to the location of these encounters from United States Decennial Census data. Encounters were nested within 163 census tracts. Officer-reported data covered general encounter characteristics, the officer’s perception of the subject’s condition, subject demographics, use of force, and disposition of the encounter (e.g., arrest v. referral or transport to treatment services). Geographical data included 17 variables representing population and housing characteristics of the census tracts, from which three indices pertaining to neighborhood income, stability, and immigration status were derived using factor-analytic techniques. We then examined associations of these indices with various encounter-related variables using multi-level analysis.

Results

Encounters taking place in higher-income and higher-stability census tracts were more likely to be dispatch-initiated and take place in a private home compared to those in lower-income and lower-stability neighborhoods. In higher-income neighborhoods, encounters were more likely to involve a subject suspected to have a mental illness (as opposed to an addictive disorder or developmental disability) and less likely to involve a subject suspected to have alcohol problems. The officer’s level of force used was not associated with neighborhood factors. Regarding disposition, although the likelihood of arrest was unrelated to neighborhood characteristics, encounters taking place in higher-immigrant neighborhoods were more likely to result in referral or transport to services than those in lower-immigrant neighborhoods.

Conclusion

Neighborhood characteristics are important to consider in research on police interactions with individuals with serious mental illnesses, addictive disorders, or developmental disabilities. Such research could inform departmental training policies and procedures based on the needs of the jurisdictions served.

Keywords: census tract, law enforcement, mental illness, neighborhood disadvantage, police encounters

1. Introduction

The over-representation of persons with serious mental illnesses in the criminal justice system in recent decades is a prominent concern in mental health, advocacy, and criminal justice communities. Incarceration of persons with mental illnesses complicates their long-term psychosocial functioning and contributes to overcrowding and resource burdens in detention settings. Overall, serious mental illnesses affect about 5% of the United States (US) adult population (US Department of Health and Human Services, 2008), whereas roughly 16% of inmates in US federal and state prisons, and city and county jails, are thought to be struggling with a serious mental illness (US Department of Justice, 1999). A majority of these inmates are charged with misdemeanors or minor felonies, sometimes due to police officers’ or others’ misunderstanding of symptomatic behaviors linked to untreated psychotic, mood, or substance use disorders (Lamb, Weinberger, and DeCuir, 2002).

This overrepresentation of persons with mental illnesses has been termed the “criminalization” of mental illnesses. It is thought to partly result from deinstitutionalization, more rigid criteria for civil commitment, stricter drug and zero-tolerance nuisance enforcement, lack of adequate community support for persons with mental illnesses, and offenders with mental illnesses having difficulty gaining access to community treatment, in addition to the attitudes of police officers and society (Lamb et al., 2002). Because police officers are most often the first responders to crises or incidents involving individuals with serious mental illnesses, they have been labeled as “gatekeepers” to the mental health system and even “street-corner psychiatrists” (Teplin and Pruitt, 1992; Borum, 1998). Responding to situations involving someone with a serious mental illness necessitates a great deal of discretionary decision-making, leaving officers to make choices about whether one will enter the mental health or criminal justice system. Yet, police are often unequipped, lacking the knowledge and specialized skills required to effectively manage a situation with a person suspected of having a serious mental illness. These realizations have triggered an initiative to implement jail diversion programs in various layers of the criminal justice system nationwide (Dupont and Cochran, 2000).

One of the most frequently used pre-booking jail diversion models (facilitating entry into mental health services in lieu of arrest and incarceration when appropriate) is the Crisis Intervention Team (CIT) program, based on the original Memphis model (Dupont and Cochran, 2000). The CIT model is a widely implemented, nationally recognized pre-booking jail diversion program formed through collaboration between the law enforcement, mental health, and advocacy communities. Goals of CIT are to increase knowledge about mental illnesses, reduce stigma, improve safety of both officers and the individuals with whom they interact, and ultimately effect pre-booking jail diversion while facilitating access to mental health services (Dupont and Cochran, 2000; Oliva, Haynes, Covington, Lushbaugh, and Compton, 2006). One facet of CIT is a week-long training program for self-selected patrol officers. Like the CIT curriculum in other cities, counties, and states, Georgia's 40-hour curriculum consists of didactic lectures presented by mental health professionals, site visits to area psychiatric facilities, and de-escalation role-playing (Oliva and Compton, 2008). Research on CIT has shown the model's effectiveness for increasing trained officers' knowledge and confidence in responding to individuals with mental illnesses, reducing stigma, and positively altering preferred use of force (Compton, Broussard, Munetz, Oliva, and Watson, 2011).

In addition to the role of training in improving outcomes of police interactions with persons with mental illnesses, it is important to consider other situational and contextual variables that might influence officers’ behaviors. Police officers’ orientations towards subjects and their responses to crime and deviance may differ by the characteristics of the neighborhoods in which they work (Klinger, 1997; Smith, 1987). (Of note, for brevity and readability, and to be consistent with common policing terminology, we use the term “subject” to denote the individual with whom the officer interacts.) There is a sparse body of research showing that police officers’ decisions and behaviors are impacted by individual characteristics, situational variables such as subjects’ characteristics, the nature of the encounter, organizational variables like the police department’s culture and characteristics, and community-level variables such as features of the neighborhood in which the encounter takes place (Riksheim, 1993).

Smith (1986) examined how coercion, arrest, or reporting of subjects, for example, are influenced by the neighborhood in which police-subject encounters occur. Results revealed that more assistance (operationalized as police-initiated contacts with persons in need of assistance, or about whom police may be concerned), is offered in racially heterogeneous neighborhoods, as well as those with more single-parent households (Smith, 1986). These findings, which highlight the importance of variables pertaining to neighborhood disadvantage, may be due to the fact that police officers initiate more contacts, and are generally more active in disadvantaged neighborhoods. Additionally, police officers are three times more likely to arrest subjects in lower-status neighborhoods as compared to higher-status neighborhoods (Smith 1986). Police officers are also more likely to use increasing levels of force in high-crime and disadvantaged neighborhoods, where higher levels of police force are used in interactions with minority subjects because they are disproportionately encountered in these neighborhoods (Terrill and Reisig, 2003).

Some neighborhood-context studies include variables related to the mental state of subjects (Terrill and Reisig, 2003), and their influence on police encounters. In disorganized neighborhoods, residents tend to have fewer ties to formal and informal social networks (Bursik & Grasmick, 1993; Sampson & Groves, 1989) to facilitate early intervention for individuals exhibiting overt symptoms of mental illness. As a result of delayed intervention, situations involving these individuals may escalate to a crisis requiring police response. Thus, officers working in disorganized neighborhoods may have more contacts with persons experiencing a mental health-related crisis due to lack of earlier intervention and insufficient treatment services. Currently, there is an obvious lack of neighborhood-related research on police encounters specifically with persons with mental illnesses, addictive disorders, or developmental disabilities. Of concern are the effects that neighborhood characteristics might exert on such encounters and officers’ decisions related to disposition in this marginalized population.

The neighborhood in which one resides is a commonly used indicator of socioeconomic status, and neighborhood characteristics have been widely studied by varied approaches and sources of data. In the US, the decennial census data is considered to be a valid representation of the population distribution and the related social, economic, and demographic makeup of predetermined geographical units within US states. The US Decennial Census datasets for the year 2000 present extensive data on numerous population and housing variables for 115.9 million housing units and 281.4 million people across the country (US Census Bureau, 2000). Due to a lack of specific measures of neighborhood deprivation in the US, methods such as principal components analysis using socioeconomic variables of interest have been used to derive “indices of deprivation” for geographically defined areas such as census tracts (Singh, 2003; Eibner and Sturm, 2006). Census tracts are small statistical subdivisions of counties within a state, averaging about 4,000 persons per tract. They generally have stable boundaries, and are considered to have relatively homogeneous demographic characteristics (US Census Bureau, 2000). For example, the state of Georgia in the US (population of 9,815,210) has 159 counties. Fulton County (population of 949,599), an example of an urban county within the Atlanta metropolitan area, contains 247 census tracts within 526.64 square miles. Appling County (population of 18,420) is an example of a rural county in southern Georgia that contains 5 census tracts within its 507.08 square miles (US Census Bureau, 2000).

Census tract data have been used to derive a single composite census-based socioeconomic index or “areal index,” to study mortality rate differentials based on area deprivation (Singh, 2003). Others measure deprivation using individual factor scores derived from census data, examining the relationship of each score with outcomes of interest (Eibner and Sturm, 2006). Using the factor score approach, Eibner and Sturm (2006) found that different components of deprivation have varying effects on physical and mental health outcomes. Some studies use standardized census variable data (z-scores) in factor analysis to derive linear indices of deprivation (McManus, Robert, Albanese, Sadek-Badawi and Palta, 2011), whereas others create scales (e.g., the Psychosocial Hazard Scale) from census-derived data designed to assess social disorganization, physical disorder, public safety, and economic deprivation (Lee, Glass, James, Bandeen-Roche and Schwartz, 2011).

In this study, we addressed the lack of research on neighborhood characteristics and police encounters with persons with mental illnesses, addictive disorders, or developmental disabilities. We replicated the aforementioned factor score analysis methods using data on geographical location of actual police encounters with individuals suspected by the officer to have one or more of these behavioral health disorders. The objective was to examine area-specific socioeconomic data derived from the US census, and to determine whether corresponding neighborhood characteristics influence police officers’ encounters with these individuals.

2. Material and methods

This study was conducted in collaboration with six police departments located in metropolitan areas in the state of Georgia, in the US. The overarching parent study assessed the effects of Crisis Intervention Team (CIT) training on police officers’ attitudes, behaviors, and referral decisions (Compton et al., 2014a). For the overall study, both CIT-trained and non-CIT-trained officers were administered a cross-sectional survey in order to examine group differences. All officers participating in the parent study (n=586) were invited to take part in a second study, in which they would provide a brief record of encounters with individuals who they suspected to have a serious mental illness, addictive disorder, or developmental disability on a paper form (which we refer to as the “encounter form,” shown in APPENDIX A), during a period of six weeks. These encounter forms were retrieved from the police officers after the six weeks, and officers were provided monetary compensation for their efforts if allowed based on their departmental policies. The study protocols and all study materials were approved by Emory University’s Institutional Review Board, and all participants provided written informed consent prior to study participation.

The encounter form, which was designed to be easy to complete, was given to officers in a spiral-bound notebook to document details of circumstances, events, and outcomes of each encounter. Each notebook consisted of 30 individual double-sided forms that captured detailed information on the encounter from source of initiation to outcome/disposition. The officers were instructed to complete each form immediately after each relevant encounter to ensure recall accuracy. The encounter form included a space for the officer to write down the address of the encounter.

Starting with the source of initiation of the call (e.g., whether self-initiated or dispatch initiated), the form goes on to include documentation of the time and location of the incident, as well as time spent on the scene and in transport. Demographic information about the subject, as well as the officer’s assessment of his or her mental state and reported symptoms and behaviors, were recorded. In addition, characteristics of the encounter were captured with items such as level of resistance displayed by the subject and the ensuing use of force employed by the officer. The form also gave officers an opportunity to identify any de-escalation techniques or equipment used to handle the situation, as well as the resulting disposition (i.e., resolution at the scene, referral to services, transport to a treatment facility, or arrest), as shown in APPENDIX A.

The address for each encounter was validated by verifying the location recorded by the police officers in Google Maps (www.maps.google.com). The BatchGeo mapping tool (www.batchgeo.com) was used to geocode encounter addresses, and create area maps using Google Maps (Figures 1 and 2). For each valid address, a corresponding census tract number and county was obtained from the US Census website: http://factfinder2.census.gov. The two data sources used from the 2000 US Census were: (a) Summary File 1 (SF1) (or short form), administered to all citizens, which presents data for variables such as household relationship, sex, race, ethnicity, tenure (whether the home is owned or rented), age, and vacancy characteristics; and (b) Summary File 3 (SF3): Sample Characteristics (or long form), which is administered to 1 in 6 residents, and presents more detailed population and housing characteristic data for the census tract (U.S Census Bureau, 2000). Data for the variables of interest were derived from the SF1 and SF3 for each census tract associated with the police encounters.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

BatchGeo View of Encounter Locations in Northern Georgia. For example, the 48 encounters taking place in the Rome/Floyd County jurisdiction are shown in the upper left. This figure includes all study jurisdictions except Savannah/Chatham County in Southeast Georgia, which is shown in Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

BatchGeo View of Encounter Locations in Southeast Georgia. This figure includes the study jurisdiction of Savannah/Chatham County; the other five jurisdictions are shown in Figure 1.

3. Objectives

The primary goals of this study were to: (a) identify a methodology guided by past literature to obtain valid measures of neighborhood disadvantage from census tract variables, and (b) determine the relationship between indicators of neighborhood disadvantage and encounter-level and subject-level variables with regard to police encounters with persons suspected to have a mental illness, addictive disorder, or developmental disability. Given the largely exploratory nature of this study, we examined potential associations between indicators of neighborhood disadvantage and a number of encounter- and subject-level variables. Yet, we also had three specific research questions. Specifically, guided by findings from previous literature, which is mostly related to encounter characteristics such as disposition and use of force, the following research questions were posed:

  • Disposition-related research questions: (1) Do police officers offer less assistance (operationalized as a lower likelihood of referral to services or transport to a treatment facility) in more disadvantaged neighborhoods? (2) Is the likelihood of arrest higher in more disadvantaged neighborhoods?

  • Use of force-related research question: (3) Do police officers tend to use a greater level of force in more disadvantaged neighborhoods?

3.1 Variables used

3.1.1 Census variables

Guided by previous studies, 17 socioeconomic indicator variables representing population and housing characteristics were selected from the US census datasets.

3.1.2 Encounter-level variables

From the encounter form, 10 variables related to encounter characteristic were used: dispatch-initiated origin of the call, occurring during business hours, encounter taking place in a private home, subject self-report of a mental health or substance abuse diagnosis, subject self-report of current psychiatric medications, risks identified by the officer, level of force used by the officer, disposition of resolution at the scene, disposition of referral or transport to treatment services, and disposition of arrest.

3.1.3 Subject-level variables

From the encounter form, eight subject-level variables were used: suspected mental illness, suspected alcohol problem, suspected drug problem, suspected developmental disability, subject gender, subject age, subject race, and level of resistance.

3.2 Data analysis

SPSS (Version 18) was used to examine distributional properties of variables, create recoded and transformed variables from raw data, and conduct descriptive analyses. Given that encounters were nested within officers, MPlus (Version 6.1)—a statistical software package that can accommodate multi-level models including those with binary outcomes—was used to test the statistical significance of the effects of the neighborhood factors on encounter- and subject-level variables. Due to the number of analyses, we use p<.01 as our criterion for statistical significance and refer to effects significant at the .05 but not the .01 level as “marginal.”

Factor analysis was conducted using 17 census-tract variables. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin statistic was .83, suggesting that these variables were excellent candidates for factor analysis (values >.60 are regarded as good). A scree plot indicated three factors (the fourth had an eigenvalue of 1.13 and the remaining had egienvalues < 1). A principal-components extraction followed by varimax rotation indicated that all variables but one (% unemployed civilians) loaded > |.50| on one of the three factors. The names assigned to the three factors and the 16 variables associated with them are shown in Table 1.

Table 1.

Sixteen Variables Loading onto Three Factors

Factor and variable Definition
“Low-income neighborhood”
Rent (negative) Median rent value (dollars)
Income (negative) Median household income (dollars)
Mortgage (negative) Median mortgage value (dollars)
SSI-PA % receiving Supplemental Security Income + % receiving public assistance
Poverty % families below poverty level
Low education % education <9th grade
Single parent % single parent households
Race % African American
No telephone % housing units with no telephone service
“Neighborhood stability”
Unmarried % never married
Renters % renter-occupied housing units
Vacant units % vacant housing units
Home-years (negative) % living in same house ≥ 5yrs
“Immigrant neighborhood”
Limited English % who speak English not very well
Immigrant % foreign born
Housing crowded % >1.5 occupants/room (of occupied housing units)

Note for Table 1: (a) Low-income medians were converted to a 0–100 index. Factor scores were computed as the mean of the items. Higher income neighborhood index = 100 − low-income index. (b) More stable neighborhood index = 100 − transitory neighborhood index. (c) Non-immigrant neighborhood index = 1 if immigrant neighborhood index < 4 (68% of the sample), 0 otherwise.

All but three variables were percentages and all but one had positive loadings (the loading for % living in same house ≥ 5 years was negative); before computing factor scores its values were subtracted from 100. The other three variables were in dollars and had negative loadings. Before computing factor scores, these three were converted into percentages of their maximum value and subtracted from 100 so that higher percentages indicated lower income. Factor scores were then computed as the average percentage for the variables assigned to the factor. Three distinct factor scores were derived based on the variables loading on each factor:

  1. Neighborhood income. Higher scores indicate higher-income neighborhoods. This factor score was formed by subtracting the low-income neighborhood index from 100. Its mean and median for the 163 census tracts are both 69 and its range is 39–58 (25th and 75th percentiles of 61 and 77).

  2. Neighborhood stability. A higher score means more stable neighborhoods (same reasoning as above). This factor score was formed by subtracting the neighborhood stability index from 100. Its mean and median for the 163 census tracts are both 64 and its range is 33–84 (25th and 75th percentiles of 56 and 75).

  3. Immigrant neighborhood. This was a binary variable, as 68% of the immigrant neighborhood index scores fell in a well-defined non-immigrant hump below 4%. Consequently, this variable was coded 1 (<4%) for non-immigrant and 0 (≥4%) for immigrant neighborhood.

4. Results

The analysis included data from 916 encounters (30 encounters without disposition information and 152 with no address or an invalid address were excluded). The resulting 916 encounters occurred in 163 different census tracts, and the median number of encounters per census tract was 3. As shown in Table 2, 26% of the 163 census tracts corresponded to only one encounter, 25% to 2–3 encounters, 19% to 4–6 encounters, etc. These 163 census tracts were located in six jurisdictions within the state of Georgia (Table 3). The percentage of encounters across the six jurisdictions varied prominently, with three providing very few encounters and one (Savannah/Chatham County) providing just over half of the total numbers of encounters. Figures 1 and 2 show maps for the areas of occurrence of the encounters. A total of 943 encounters are geographically represented on the maps, including 27 encounters without disposition information.

Table 2.

Number of Encounters Taking Place in the 163 Represented Census Tracts

#
encounters
#
census tracts
%
census tracts
1 43 26
2–3 40 25
4–6 31 19
7–10 25 15
11–20 18 11
21–39 6 4

total 163 100

Table 3.

Participating Jurisdictions, and Numbers of Encounters and Census Tracts Represented*

Jurisdiction # census tracts # encounters % encounters
Atlanta 40 127 14
Henry County 11 39 4
Cherokee County 11 25 3
Rome/Floyd County 12 48 5
Clayton County 33 203 22
Savannah/Chatham County 56 474 52

total 163 916 100
*

Refer to Figures 1 and 2 for geographic mapping of encounters by jurisdiction.

A total of 166 officers were involved in the 916 encounters. The present study focuses on analyzing the effects of neighborhood characteristics derived for the 163 census tracts on those encounters; effects of officers’ characteristics on those encounters is the focus of a different report (Compton et al., 2014b), and so are not discussed here.

4.1 Results pertaining to the neighborhood income index

As shown in Table 4, encounters taking place in higher-income tracts were more likely to be dispatch initiated, less likely to occur during normal business hours, and more likely to take place in a private home. Subjects were more likely to be suspected of having a mental illness (marginal), and less likely to be African American in higher-income census tracts. Regarding our three specific research questions (pertaining to the effects of neighborhood characteristics on referral to services or transport to a treatment facility, arrest, and use of force), the neighborhood income index was unrelated to these three outcomes.

Table 4.

Effect of Neighborhood Income on Encounter- and Subject-Level Variables

Variable % for variable at
given income percentile
OR p
25pct 50pct 75pct
Encounter
  Dispatch initiated
50 58 65 1.46 .002
  Occurred during business hours 50 45 40 0.78 .009
  Took place in a private home 28 39 50 1.79 <.001
  Officer aware of a psychiatric diagnosis 48 48 48 1.00 .990
  Officer aware of psychiatric medication 21 20 19 0.93 .541
  One or more risks identified 50 50 50 0.99 .859
  Force level 5 or greater 27 26 26 0.96 .731
  Resolved on the scene 51 48 45 0.86 .176
  Referred or transported to services 25 27 30 1.16 .060
  Arrest executed 22 23 24 1.07 .645
Subject
  Suspected of having a mental illness
43 47 52 1.25 .028
  Suspected of having alcohol problems 31 28 25 0.82 .062
  Suspected of having drug problems 22 20 18 0.85 .150
  Suspected of having a developmental disability 12 12 13 1.09 .389
  Male subject 59 57 55 0.91 .295
  Age 40 or above 55 52 50 0.88 .154
  African American (vs. Caucasian) 80 65 46 0.38 <.001
  Resistance displayed by the subject 17 16 16 0.94 .537

Note for Table 4: The first three columns are encounter variable percentages estimated by a multilevel model for income index values of 61%, 69%, and 77% (the 25th, 50th, and 75th percentile values). Odds ratios indicate the effect of the income index, a Level 2 or census-tract variable, on each of the Level 1 encounter variables (as computed by MPlus). Odds ratios greater than one indicate that encounter variable percentages were higher in higher-income census tracts, whereas values less than one indicate that encounter variable probabilities were higher in lower-income census tracts. The income index was centered at the census tract median (69%) and scaled in increments of 10%.

4.2 Results pertaining to the neighborhood stability index

As shown in Table 5, encounters taking place in higher-stability census tracts were more likely to be dispatch initiated and to occur in a private home. Subjects were more likely to be suspected of having a mental illness, less likely to be suspected of alcohol problems (marginal), and less likely to be male or African American in more stable census tracts. Regarding our three specific research questions, the neighborhood stability index was unrelated to the three outcomes of most interest: referral to services or transport to a treatment facility, arrest, and use of force.

Table 5.

Effect of Neighborhood Stability on Encounter- and Subject-Level Variables

Variable % for variable at
given stability percentile
OR p
25pct 50pct 75pct
Encounter
  Dispatch initiated
51 57 64 1.34 .003
  Occurred during business hours 48 46 43 0.89 .142
  Took place in a private home 23 37 59 2.25 <.001
  Officer aware of a psychiatric diagnosis 47 49 51 1.09 .322
  Officer aware of psychiatric medication 19 20 22 1.07 .395
  One or more risks identified 49 50 52 1.06 .351
  Force level 5 or greater 27 26 24 0.92 .395
  Resolved on the scene 48 49 50 1.04 .654
  Referred or transported to services 26 26 26 0.99 .828
  Arrest executed 23 22 21 0.96 .704
Subject
  Suspected of having a mental illness
42 47 52 1.23 .010
  Suspected of having alcohol problems 31 28 25 0.87 .050
  Suspected of having drug problems 21 20 19 0.94 .486
  Suspected of having a developmental disability 12 12 11 0.95 .489
  Male subject 61 57 51 0.80 .002
  Age 40 or above 54 53 51 0.93 .274
  African American (vs. Caucasian) 78 69 54 0.56 <.001
  Resistance displayed by the subject 17 17 16 0.95 .521

Note for Table 5: The first three columns are encounter variable percentages estimated by a multilevel model for stability index values of 56%, 64%, and 75% (the 25th, 50th, and 75th percentile values). Odds ratios indicate the effect of the stability index, a Level 2 or census-tract variable, on each of the Level 1 encounter variables (as computed by MPlus). Odds ratios greater than one indicate that encounter variable percentages were higher in more stable census tracts, whereas values less than one indicate that encounter variable probabilities were higher in less stable census tracts. The stability index was centered at the census tract median (64%) and scaled in increments of 10%.

4.3 Results pertaining to the immigrant neighborhood index

As shown in Table 6, encounters in non-immigrant neighborhoods (i.e., those census tracts in which <4% of residents are immigrants) were more likely to occur during business hours (marginal), and were more likely to involve subjects suspected of drug problems. Encounters occurring in immigrant neighborhoods were more likely to involve subjects suspected of having a developmental disability (marginal). Regarding our three specific research questions, the non-immigrant neighborhood factor was unrelated to arrest or use of force; however, encounters that involved referral or transport were more likely to occur in immigrant neighborhoods, though this was a marginal effect.

Table 6.

Effect of Non-Immigrant Neighborhood on Encounter- and Subject-Level Variables

Variable % for variable by
neighborhood
OR p
Immigr
ant
Non-
immigrant
Encounter
  Dispatch initiated
62 55 0.83 .520
  Occurred during business hours 43 54 1.57 .041
  Took place in a private home 41 38 1.08 .803
  Officer aware of a psychiatric diagnosis 57 59 1.22 .351
  Officer aware of psychiatric medication 39 35 0.94 .746
  One or more risks identified 56 53 0.93 .692
  Force level 5 or greater 25 24 1.06 .840
  Resolved on the scene 40 51 1.26 .267
  Referred or transported to services 43 31 0.64 .015
  Arrest executed 17 18 1.24 .406
Subject
  Suspected of having a mental illness
54 47 0.77 .137
  Suspected of having alcohol problems 27 31 1.18 .388
  Suspected of having drug problems 14 29 2.26 .001
  Suspected of having a developmental disability 15 10 0.64 .019
  Male subject 59 61 1.04 .797
  Age 40 or above 45 54 1.35 .123
  African American (vs. Caucasian) 68 65 0.59 .114
  Resistance displayed by the subject 31 31 1.00 .989

Note for Table 6: The first two columns are encounter variable percentages computed for encounters in immigrant and non-immigrant neighborhoods. Odds ratios indicate the effect of the immigrant versus non-immigrant neighborhood, a Level 2 or census-tract variable, on each of the Level 1 encounter variables (as computed by MPlus). Odds ratios greater than one indicate that encounter variable percentages were higher in non-immigrant census tracts, whereas values less than one indicate that encounter variable probabilities were higher in immigrant census tracts.

5. Discussion

Contrary to what we had expected, neighborhood characteristics did not appear to have an effect on the likelihood of: (1) referral to services or transport to a treatment facility, (2) arrest, or (3) use of force in these encounters. In this vulnerable population, these preliminary findings can be seen as encouraging. Though not part of the original hypotheses, the findings of this study show that neighborhood characteristics have potentially interesting associations with some encounter-level variables (e.g., the encounter being dispatch initiated, occurring during business hours, and taking place in a private home) and with some subject-level variables (e.g., individuals being suspected of having a mental illness or suspected of having an alcohol problem).

The findings that encounters occurring in higher income and higher stability neighborhoods were more likely to be dispatch initiated, and more likely to take place in a private home, are likely explained by the fact that officers are less likely to informally come across persons in crisis situations “in the streets” in such areas. Additionally, higher income and more stable areas are likely to have more private residences; hence encounters are more likely to take place in private homes, in response to residents’ emergency phone calls. The increased likelihood of suspected mental illness in higher income/more stable tracts is probably because officers are more likely to attribute subjects’ behaviors to mental illness in these areas, and less likely to attribute such behaviors to alcohol or drugs. On the contrary, officers may be more likely to suspect substance abuse in lower income areas, where they are more likely to encounter vagrant behavior, or persons using alcohol or other substances.

The increased likelihood of encounters with African American subjects in low-income and less stable neighborhoods is consistent with previous findings (Terrill and Reisig, 2003), and due to higher minority populations in these neighborhoods. The relatively high number of encounters with African American subjects in this study is a function of the racial makeup in these areas, as most of the jurisdictions included in this study had sizeable African American populations (US Census Bureau, 2000).

Officers may provide more referral or transport to services in immigrant neighborhoods due to a lack of access to mental health services in these communities. It has been found that neighborhood disadvantage related to racial composition, language barriers, and household crowding is associated with worse mental health outcomes (Eibner and Sturm, 2006). The mental health care needs of racial and ethnic minorities are largely unmet, and there may be a greater burden of untreated or undiagnosed mental illnesses in minority communities due to stigma and barriers to treatment-seeking (Center for Mental Health Services, 1999). For more impoverished areas, it has been found that African Americans and Hispanics are more likely than Caucasians to be referred by law enforcement officials, possibly due to lower tolerance of minorities with mental illnesses and higher visibility of minorities in these areas (Chow, Jafee, Snowden, 2003).

There are a few limitations to be considered when interpreting results of this study. At the time of census data retrieval for this study (in April 2011), the most current and complete decennial census data available was for the year 2000. Because encounters took place between April 2010 and December 2010, 2000 decennial census tract data might not perfectly represent the characteristics of the neighborhoods involved. About 152 addresses for which census tracts could not be located, and about 30 encounters with no disposition information were excluded from this study; however, there is no reason to believe that this relatively sparse missing data would bias results. Also to be noted is the fact that unless an encounter occurred in a private home, it does not necessarily reflect the residence of the subject; addresses and their corresponding census tracts pertain to the neighborhood in which the encounter occurred, rather than the subject’s home address. Finally, all address and encounter information was self-reported by police officers and based on their knowledge of the encounter and perceptions of subjects involved. It is also important to acknowledge that because the study was based on voluntary participation, there may have been some selection bias involved based on the characteristics of participating officers.

Considering that officers’ use of force is related to the subject’s level of resistance, it is indeed encouraging to find that the neighborhood context does not appear to have an effect on police officers’ use of force when dealing with psychiatric populations. From this, we can conclude that police officers do not unnecessarily use excessive force simply because they encounter persons in disadvantaged neighborhoods, but instead use discretion with actions related to their use of force. However, based on our aforementioned findings related to encounter-level and subject-level variables, neighborhood characteristics may be an important aspect to consider in research on police interactions with persons with mental illnesses, addictive disorders, and developmental disabilities.

Supplementary Material

01

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

REFERENCES

  1. BatchGeo LLC. 2012 URL: www.batchgeo.com. [Google Scholar]
  2. Borum R. Police perspectives on responding to mentally ill people in crisis. Behavioral Sciences and the Law. 1998;16:393–405. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-0798(199823)16:4<393::aid-bsl317>3.0.co;2-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bursik RJ, Grasmick HG. Neighborhoods and Crime: The Dimensions of Effective Community Control. New York: Macmillan; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  4. Center for Mental Health Services. Mental Health: A Report of the Surgeon General. Rockville, MD: US Department of Health and Human Services, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration; 1999. Retrieved from: http://137.187.25.243/library/mentalhealth/home.html. [Google Scholar]
  5. Chow JC, Jafee K, Snowden L. Racial/ethnic disparities in the use of mental health services in poverty areas. American Journal of Public Health. 2003;93(5):792–797. doi: 10.2105/ajph.93.5.792. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Compton MT, Bakeman R, Broussard B, Hankerson-Dyson D, Husbands L, Krishan S, Stewart-Hutto T, D’Orio BM, Oliva JR, Thompson NJ, Watson AC. The police-based Crisis Intervention Team (CIT) model: I effects on officers’ knowledge, attitudes, and skills. Psychiatric Services. 2014a doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.201300107. [Epub ahead of print.] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Compton MT, Bakeman R, Broussard B, Hankerson-Dyson D, Husbands L, Krishan S, Stewart-Hutto T, D’Orio BM, Oliva JR, Thompson NJ, Watson AC. The police-based Crisis Intervention Team (CIT) model: II. effects on level of force and resolution, referral, and arrest. Psychiatric Services. 2014b doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.201300108. [Epub ahead of print.] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Compton MT, Broussard B, Munetz M, Oliva JR, Watson AC. The Crisis Intervention Team (CIT) Model of Collaboration between Law Enforcement and Mental Health. New York: Nova Science Publishers, Inc.; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  9. Dupont R, Cochran S. Police response to mental health emergencies: barriers to change. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law. 2000;28:338–344. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Eibner C, Sturm R. US-based indices of area-level deprivation: results from HealthCare for Communities. Social Science and Medicine. 2006;62:348–359. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.06.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Google Maps. 2011 URL: www.maps.google.com. [Google Scholar]
  12. Klinger DA. Negotiating order in patrol work: an ecological theory of police response to deviance. Criminology. 1997;35(2):277–306. [Google Scholar]
  13. Lamb HR, Weinberger LE, DeCuir WJ. The police and mental health. Psychiatric Services. 2002;53(10):1266–1271. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.53.10.1266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Lee BK, Glass TA, James BD, Bandeen-Roche K, Schwartz BS. Neighborhood psychosocial environment, apolipoprotein E genotype, and cognitive function in older adults. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2011;68(3):314–321. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. McManus BM, Robert SA, Albanese A, Sadek-Badawi M, Palta M. Relationship between neighborhood disadvantage and social function of Wisconsin 2- and 3-year-olds born at very low birth weight. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine. 2011;165(2):119–125. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2010.185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Oliva JR, Compton MT. A statewide Crisis Intervention Team (CIT) initiative: evolution of the Georgia CIT program. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law. 2008;36:38–46. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Oliva JR, Haynes N, Covington DW, Lushbaugh DJ, Compton MT. Crisis Intervention Team (CIT) programs. In: Compton MT, Kotwicki RJ, editors. Responding to Individuals with Mental Illnesses. Sudbury, Massachusetts: Jones and Bartlett Publishers, Inc.; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  18. Riksheim EC. Causes of police behavior. Journal of Criminal Justice. 1993;21:353–382. [Google Scholar]
  19. Sampson RJ, Groves WB. Community structure and crime: testing social-disorganization theory. American Journal of Sociology. 1989;94(4):774–802. [Google Scholar]
  20. Singh G. Area deprivation and widening inequalities in US Mortality, 1969–1998. American Journal of Public Health. 2003;93(7):1137–1143. doi: 10.2105/ajph.93.7.1137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Smith DA. The Neighborhood Context of Police Behavior. The University of Chicago Press; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  22. Smith DA. Police response to interpersonal violence: defining the parameters of legal control. Social Forces. 1987;65(3):767–782. [Google Scholar]
  23. Teplin L, Pruett N. Police as street-corner psychiatrists: managing the mentally ill. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry. 1992;15:139–156. doi: 10.1016/0160-2527(92)90010-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Terrill W, Reisig MD. Neighborhood context and police use of force. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency. 2003;40(3):291–321. [Google Scholar]
  25. U. S. Census Bureau. American Fact Finder. 2000 Retrieved from: http://factfinder2.census.gov/faces/nav/jsf/pages/index.xhtml.
  26. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. Prevalence of Serious Mental Illness among US Adults by Sex, Age and Race in 2008. National Survey on Drug Use and Health; 2008. 2008. [Google Scholar]
  27. U.S. Department of Justice, Bureau of Justice Statistics. Mental Health and Treatment of Inmates and Probationers: Special Report. Washington, DC: U S Government Printing; 1999. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

01

RESOURCES