Skip to main content
Journal of Physical Therapy Science logoLink to Journal of Physical Therapy Science
. 2016 Jun 28;28(6):1795–1800. doi: 10.1589/jpts.28.1795

Self-selected intensity, ratings of perceived exertion, and affective responses in sedentary male subjects during resistance training

Hassan Mohamed Elsangedy 1,*, Kleverton Krinski 2, Daniel Gomes da Silva Machado 1,3, Pedro Moraes Dutra Agrícola 1, Alexandre Hideki Okano 1, Sergio Gregório da Silva 4
PMCID: PMC4932059  PMID: 27390418

Abstract

[Purpose] This study examined the exercise intensity and psychophysiological responses to a self-selected resistance training session in sedentary male subjects. [Subjects and Methods] Twelve sedentary male subjects (35.8 ± 5.8 years; 25.5 ± 2.6 kg·m2) underwent four sessions at 48-h intervals: familiarization; two sessions of one repetition maximum test and a resistance training session in which they were told to self-select a load to complete 3 sets of 10 repetitions of chest press, leg press, seated rows, knee extension, overhead press, biceps curl, and triceps pushdown exercises. During the latter, the percentage of one repetition maximum, affective responses (feeling scale), and rating of perceived exertion (OMNI-RES scale) were measured. [Results] The percentage of one repetition maximum for all exercises was >51% (14–31% variability), the rating of perceived exertion was 5–6 (7–11% variability), and the affective responses was 0–1 point with large variability. [Conclusion] Sedentary male subjects self-selected approximately 55% of one maximum repetition, which was above the intensity suggested to increase strength in sedentary individuals, but below the recommended intensity to improve strength in novice to intermediate exercisers. The rating of perceived exertion was indicative of moderate intensity and slightly positive affective responses.

Key words: Adherence, Guidelines, Pleasure/displeasure

INTRODUCTION

Resistance training (RT) is indicated to increase muscle fitness, improve athletic performance, and prevent and treat disease1, 2). To achieve the benefits derived from RT, the American College of Sports Medicine (ACSM) recommends that subjects perform RT at a specific intensity based on external load (percentage of one repetition maximum [1RM])1, 2). This model of prescription is based on two pillars, namely safety and effectiveness. However, Ekkekakis, Parfitt, and Petruzzello3) stated that a prescription might be effective and safe, but if very few people want to follow it, then its public-health relevance becomes questionable. In a broad review regarding aerobic exercise3), they found an increasing body of evidence linking affective responses to exercise and adherence. Therefore, the authors suggest that exercise prescription should not only rely upon effectiveness and safety but also consider the pleasure/displeasure people felt3).

In fact, the hedonic theory considers affective responses to behavior as determinants of future actions4); in other words, they influence decisions regarding whether an activity is repeated5). In this sense, hedonic theory has being supported by studies of aerobic exercise in which positive affective responses predict more time spent exercising6,7,8). In this regard, studies have suggested that individuals self-regulate their own exercise to a preferred intensity9, 10). An extensive body of literature with aerobic exercise has demonstrated that self-selected intensity reached or exceeded the minimum level recommended by the ACSM to improve health, particularly in sedentary individuals10,11,12,13,14). Additionally, this method may improve the exercise experience through increased perceived autonomy15) and pleasure, thus contributing to increasing individuals’ chances of repeating the activity4, 6,7,8, 16). In fact, these studies have shown that the pleasure responses are higher in self-selected than in imposed intensity11, 17), even if this imposed intensity is the same as that the subjects self-selected15) or a little higher (10%)10, 12).

However, the relationship between self-select intensity and the recommendations of the ACSM in RT as well as the affective responses to it have been poorly studied. The first report was by Glass and Stanton18), who found a self-selected load ranging from 42–57% of 1RM in five exercises in young male and female novice weightlifters. In addition, Focht compared self-selected and imposed intensity in young untrained female subjects and found that ratings of perceived exertion (RPE) and load were higher for imposed intensity (75% of 1RM) than for self-selected intensity (~56% of 1RM) in four exercises19). Furthermore, Ratamess et al. found that young female subjects who trained with a personal trainer self-selected ~51.4% of 1RM, with a higher RPE compared to ~42.3% for female subjects who trained without a personal trainer in four exercises20). Elsangedy et al. recently showed that older sedentary females self-selected an intensity of 33–51.4% of 1RM in eight exercises21). Two recent studies showed that active male subjects and trained female subjects self-selected an intensity of 37–58% and 57% of 1RM in four exercise sessions, respectively22, 23).

However, except for one study21), all of these studies18,19,20, 22, 23) have assessed active or trained samples during self-selected intensity in RT, and responses of sedentary individuals during RT using this approach is lacking. Additionally, only two studies have assessed the affective responses during self-selected intensity RT22, 23). Given that, one of the principal purposes of self-selection is to increase perceived autonomy and control to generate positive affective responses and therefore increase adherence, and this information would be appreciable to guide exercise prescriptions for sedentary individuals. Additionally, the last ACSM position shows little information regarding RT prescriptions for sedentary individuals, stating only that 40–50% of 1RM may be beneficial for this population2). Therefore, it is important to be aware of the intensity at which sedentary individuals exercise as well as the affective responses to self-selecting the intensity in RT. Furthermore, it is not clear how other sedentary populations respond to this training approach. These data may provide valuable insight regarding the prescription of RT and adherence for sedentary individuals. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to evaluate whether sedentary male subjects self-select a sufficient RT load to meet ACSM recommendations for RT and describe its affective and perceived exertion responses.

SUBJECTS AND METHODS

Twelve sedentary male subjects (age, 35.8 ± 5.8 years; height, 1.78 ± 0.07 m; body mass, 81.2 ± 8.7 kg; body mass index [BMI], 25.5 ± 2.6 kg·m−2) volunteered to participate in this study. All participants were healthy, classified as sedentary (≤1 h/week of physical activity during the previous 6 months), not taking any medication known to influence metabolic or cognitive function, stable body mass (<2.5 kg net change over the previous three months), and were nonsmokers. Exclusion criteria included one or more positive responses on the Physical Activity Readiness questionnaire; BMI<18.5 kg·m2 or >29.9 kg·m2; diagnosis of cardiovascular, metabolic, and orthopedic disease; or any other contraindications for physical activity as determined by medical history from the previous 12 months. Subjects were recruited via e-mails sent to large community university and fliers displayed in public places. The Institutional Review Board of Ethics Committee of Federal University of Paraná approved this study (CEP/SD: 1087.012.11.03-CAAE: 0014.0.091.000-11); and after a detailed explanation of the experimental procedures, benefits, and possible risks, written informed consent was obtained from all volunteers.

To describe the affective responses and RPE of acute resistance exercise at a self-selected intensity, sedentary male volunteers completed four experimental sessions on different days. During the first session, individuals underwent medical screening, anthropometric measurements, and familiarization with the experimental procedures. The second and third sessions involved a 1RM testing session for test and re-test interclass correlation assessment at a 48-h interval. During the fourth experimental trial, participants performed three sets of 10 repetitions with a self-selected load for each set and exercise. RPE, affective responses, and the training load of each set and exercise were recorded as the outcome variables.

All the sessions were conducted between 8 and 11 AM under similar environmental conditions (21°C and 55% relative humidity). The participants were instructed to refrain from exercise and avoid caffeine-containing products during the 24 hours prior to the exercise trials.

RPE was obtained immediately after each set during the resistance exercise sessions by using the OMNI-RES scale24), which includes exercise mode–specific pictorial and verbal descriptors distributed along a comparatively narrow decimal numerical range from 0, corresponding to “extremely easy,” to 10, corresponding to “extremely hard,” which facilitates the reporting of exercise intensity during dynamic exercise. During the familiarization session, standard instructions and anchoring procedures were explained. RPE has been conceptualized as a psychobiological configuration of effort sense and has been shown to be valid for use during acute resistance exercise25, 26).

Basic affect (pleasure/displeasure) was assessed using the feeling scale27), an 11-point, single-item, bipolar rating scale that is commonly used to assess affective responses during exercise. The scale ranges from −5 to +5. Anchors are provided at zero (“neutral”) and at all odd integers, ranging from “very good” (+5) to “very bad” (−5)27). The positive numbers represent pleasure and negative numbers represent displeasure. Subjects were familiarized with the scale using a standard procedure27). The participants were informed and subsequently reminded that the scale measured the affective or emotional component of exercise, that is, whether the sensation of effort during RT felt pleasant or unpleasant rather than the actual level of physical effort or strain.

During the first session, all subjects underwent anthropometric measurements and a familiarization session with the experimental procedures. The familiarization process included teaching the participants adequate lifting techniques for each exercise. Subjects were also familiarized with the process of selecting a preferred load. Each participant was asked to self-select the load to complete three sets of 10 repetitions20). Additionally, the participants received instructions regard the utilization of the RPE OMNI-RES scale and the feeling scale. In the second session, the 1RM was performed for seven different exercises by using machines in the following order: 1) chest press, 2) leg press, 3) seated rows, 4) knee extension, 5) overhead press, 6) biceps curl, and 7) triceps pushdown.

The 1RM was determined using a previously validated testing procedure28). First, the participants received instructions regarding the adequate lifting technique for each exercise before starting the 1RM tests. The subjects then warmed up by performing a few repetitions with lighter loads. The first attempt was approximately 50% of the participant’s estimated 1RM load. Subsequently, the participant was given 5 minutes of passive recovery, and another attempt at applying progressively heavier loads followed by recovery was performed until the subject could not complete a repetition through the full range of motion. The increase in load was based on the ease with which the previous trial was performed. The determination of 1RM for each exercise tested never took more than five attempts, not including the warm-up. These stipulations concerning the amount of attempts executed were necessary to prevent any fatigue effect on the result. If participants perceived that they could perform 10 repetitions with the designated load, the trial attempt was quickly ended. This procedure ensured that the participants would spare themselves from any unnecessary muscular fatigue, allowing a most accurate 1RM determination.

The participants performed two 1RM test sessions for all exercises at 48-h intervals to determine the interclass correlation coefficients. Test-retest reliabilities for the 1RM assessment were 0.97, 0.84, 0.99, 0.94, 0.96, 0.97, and 0.98, respectively; no significant differences were found (p>0.05) between the test and retest values for any of the measurements. Muscular strength for the 1RM was chest press=68.5 ± 16.6 kg; leg press=202.5 ± 37.4 kg; seated row=72.8 ± 21.6 kg; knee extension=100.4 ± 11.6 kg; overhead press=44.0 ± 14.1 kg; biceps curl=48.3 ± 12.3 kg; and triceps pushdown=64.2 ± 10.4 kg.

At least 48 hours after the end of the 1RM session, the participants underwent a self-selected bout of resistance exercise. For each exercise, the participants performed three sets of 10 repetitions with 2 minutes of passive recovery between each set.

Participants were asked to self-select the load to complete three sets of 10 repetitions20). Before initiating the first set, the participants selected a weight and performed a few repetitions to gauge the load. The individuals were permitted to choose the load lifted for each set during the self-selected bout. The investigators provided no additional information that could have created bias in the weight selection. RPE, affective response, amount of resistance (kg), and number of repetitions completed were recorded after each set.

The reliability of the 1RM test was analyzed as described by Hopkins29) using intra-class correlation coefficients. Descriptive statistics was used. The values are expressed as means and standard deviation or individual data. Values of %1RM, RPE, and affective responses are displayed as the mean of the 3 sets for each exercise. We also calculated the coefficient of variation of the %1RM, RPE, and affective responses as the ratio between the standard deviation and the mean to better describe the self-selected intensity.

RESULTS

As shown in Table 1, the absolute load did not change over the three exercise sets, although the individuals were allowed to change the load selected at each set. Similarly, the participants used the same relative load for the three sets (data not shown).

Table 1. Absolute load (kg) used during the resistance training session at a self-selected intensity (mean ± SD).

Exercise First set Second set Third set Mean ± SD
Chest press 37.2 ± 18.4 37.2 ± 18.4 37.2 ± 18.4 37.2 ± 18.4
Leg press 116.7 ± 31.7 115.0 ± 33.2 115.0 ± 33.2 115.6 ± 32.6
Seated rows 42.0 ± 17.3 42.0 ± 17.3 42.0 ± 17.3 42.0 ± 17.3
Knee extension 52.5 ± 13.7 51.7 ± 12.9 51.7 ± 12.9 51.9 ± 13.1
Overhead press 23.5 ± 9.2 23.8 ± 9.4 23.8 ± 9.4 23.7 ± 9.3
Biceps curl 28.4 ± 8.7 28.4 ± 8.7 28.2 ± 9.2 28.3 ± 8.9
Triceps pushdown 36.7 ± 9.6 36.7 ± 9.6 36.7 ± 9.6 36.7 ± 9.6

The mean %1RM and coefficient of variation for the exercises were: chest press=51.2 ± 15.8% (CV=0.309); leg press=57.5 ± 12.7% (CV=0.221); seated row=56.6 ± 12.1% (CV=0.214); knee extension=51.6 ± 10.9% (CV=0.211); overhead press=53.3 ± 8.8% (CV=0.165); biceps curl=58.5 ± 8.5% (CV=0.145); and triceps pushdown=56.7 ± 9.6% (CV=0.169). The mean percentage of 1RM for all exercises was >51%, although there was quite large inter-subject variability of 14–31%.

Mean RPE and coefficient of variation were chest press=5.3 ± 0.5 (CV=0.094); leg press=6.2 ± 0.6 (CV=0.097); seated row=5.7 ± 0.5 (CV=0.088); knee extension=6.2 ± 0.7 (CV=0.113); overhead press=6.5 ± 0.7 (CV=0.108); biceps curl=6.4 ± 0.7 (CV=0.109); and triceps pushdown=5.8 ± 0.4% (CV=0.069). The mean RPE for all exercises was 5–7 with an inter-subject variability of 7–11%.

Mean affect and coefficient of variation were chest press=1.1 ± 0.5 (CV=0.455); leg press=0.4 ± 0.4 (CV=1.00); seated row=1.1 ± 0.6 (CV=0.545); knee extension=0.3 ± 0.3 (CV=1.00); overhead press=0.3 ± 0.6 (CV=2.00); biceps curl=0.1 ± 0.7 (CV=7.00); and triceps pushdown=1.1 ± 0.6% (CV=0.545). The mean affective responses for all exercises was 0–1 point, but there was large inter-subject variability (>45%). The huge variability is likely due to fact that in some exercises, such as the overhead press and biceps curl, three and two individuals showed negative affective responses, respectively.

DISCUSSION

This study aimed to evaluate whether sedentary male subjects would self-select a load in RT that meets the ACSM’s recommendations and describe its affective and perceived exertion responses. Our results showed that when sedentary male subjects are allowed to self-select the load during RT, they choose a load approximately 55% of 1RM, which is below the recommended intensity to improve strength in novice to intermediate exercisers, but above the intensity suggested to increase strength in sedentary male subjects. RPE was indicative of moderate intensity and slightly positive affective responses. However, it should be noted that there was large inter-subject variability for self-selected %1RM and affective responses. Investigations regarding the influence of self-selected and imposed exercise intensity (recommended) on physiological, perceptual, and affective responses during aerobic exercise have increased in recent years10). However, although some researchers have investigated physiological and perceptual responses during resistance exercise18,19,20,21,22,23), this is the first study to describe perceived exertion and affective responses during a self-selected bout of acute resistance exercise in sedentary male subjects.

The ACSM guidelines recommend an intensity of 60–70% of 1RM (moderate to high intensity) in novice to intermediate exercisers and that 40–50% of 1RM (very light to light intensity) for sedentary subjects may be beneficial1, 2). In our study, sedentary male subjects self-selected approximately 55% of 1RM, which met the recommended intensity for sedentary male subjects. In other words, even if we allow sedentary subjects to self-select the load that they want to use to perform the resistance exercise, they will choose a load recommended for improving strength. Additionally, it has been suggested that moderate loading (50–60% of 1RM or less) can be used initially since learning proper form and technique is paramount for novice individuals1). The %1RM self-selected by sedentary male subjects in this study (51–58%) was similar to that found in novice weightlifters (~42–57%)18), untrained female subjects (~56%)19), trained female subjects (~42–51% and 57%)20, 23), and active male subjects (37–58%)22). However, it should be noted that only Elsangedy et al. assessed a sample of sedentary older females (33–51.4%)21). Therefore, the relative load self-selected in RT may be sufficient to improve strength in sedentary male subjects and seems consistent across different samples.

The ACSM also recommends a load of 60–70% of 1RM, which corresponds to an RPE of moderate to high intensity2). In this study, the mean RPE responses were 5–6 points (with low variability) on a 0–10 scale, which is considered moderate, while the self-selected load was 51–58% of 1RM. Similarly, for a self-selection of 56% of 1RM, untrained females reported an RPE just above “fairly hard”19). Additionally, it was shown that RPE for imposed intensity is higher than self-selected intensity, probably due to decreased perceived autonomy30), and there is a linear function between RPE responses and relative intensity (%1RM)24, 26, 31). Therefore, if a higher load was applied as suggested by the ACSM, sedentary individuals would perceive this intensity as being harder than they should, consequently increasing exercise attrition and impairing adherence19, 32).

Regarding affective responses, the values on the feeling scale were generally from zero (“neutral”) to positive (“fairly good”). However, there was large inter-subject variability. It is important to note that in two exercises (overhead press and biceps curl), three and two subjects exhibited negative affective responses. To date, only two studies with other samples have shown positive affective responses to self-selected intensity in RT22, 23). In this regard, positive affective responses during exercise could contribute to an individual’s adherence to exercise programs6,7,8). Aerobic exercise results have shown that psychological responses in self-selected intensity are more pleasant than imposed intensity11, 33, 34), even when this imposition is equal15) or a little higher (10%) than the self-selected intensity10, 12). Moreover, there is a tendency for participants to diverge from the imposed intensity levels in the direction of the preferred levels35, 36). Therefore, based on the results of the negative psychological effects of imposed exercise intensity, we suggest that the same negative emotional responses may be expected after imposed resistance exercise intensity.

From a practical perspective, given that ≥50% of subjects that begin an exercise program desist within the first six months37), the present results may be applied to sedentary individuals beginning an RT program. A recent meta-analysis concluded that loads <60% of 1RM promote substantial increases in muscular strength and hypertrophy in untrained individuals38). Additionally, considering that beginners may become easily discouraged from an exercise routine that elicits negative affect, the results of this study indicate that a self-selected resistance exercise bout may be more appropriate when adherence is the primary goal since imposed exercise intensity induces less positive or negative affective responses during exercise. After this period, the current ACSM recommendations may be fully applied2). Some limitations of this study warrant mention. Since we included only sedentary male adults, the use of these findings for other populations (e.g., physically active subjects, the elderly, the obese, individuals with chronic diseases) should be made with caution. As this study used a self-selection protocol, the execution speed of motion during RT was not controlled. Additionally, future research should measure the rate of perceived autonomy as well as other psychological responses (e.g., self-efficacy, extroversion, neuroticism, behavioral inhibition) regarding load control during self-selected resistance exercise.

In conclusion, sedentary male subjects self-selected the load during RT of approximately 55% of 1RM, which was above the intensity suggested to increase strength in sedentary individuals but was below the recommended intensity to improve strength in novice to intermediate exercisers. RPE was indicative of moderate intensity and slightly positive affective responses. These findings have a practical significance, demonstrating that the self-selection approach is an interesting method for use at the beginning of a RT program when the primary goal is adherence. Therefore, exercise professionals should consider this strategy to prescribe and/or encourage the RT practice by sedentary subjects.

Acknowledgments

We thank the volunteers who contributed their time and energy to participate in this study. We also thank CNPq and CAPES for providing scholarships to our students. The authors report no conflicts of interest. This study was not financed by any grant.

REFERENCES

  • 1.American College of Sports Medicine: American College of Sports Medicine position stand. Progression models in resistance training for healthy adults. Med Sci Sports Exerc, 2009, 41: 687–708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Garber CE, Blissmer B, Deschenes MR, et al. American College of Sports Medicine: American College of Sports Medicine position stand. Quantity and quality of exercise for developing and maintaining cardiorespiratory, musculoskeletal, and neuromotor fitness in apparently healthy adults: guidance for prescribing exercise. Med Sci Sports Exerc, 2011, 43: 1334–1359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Ekkekakis P, Parfitt G, Petruzzello SJ: The pleasure and displeasure people feel when they exercise at different intensities: decennial update and progress towards a tripartite rationale for exercise intensity prescription. Sports Med, 2011, 41: 641–671. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Kahneman D: Objective happiness. In: Well-being: the foundation of hedonic psychology. New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 1999, pp 3–25. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kahneman D, Fredrickson BL, Schreiber CA, et al.: When more pain is preferred to less: adding a better end. Psychol Sci, 1993, 4: 401–405. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Williams DM, Dunsiger S, Ciccolo JT, et al. : Acute affective response to a moderate-intensity exercise stimulus predicts physical activity participation 6 and 12 months later. Psychol Sport Exerc, 2008, 9: 231–245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kwan BM, Bryan A: In-task and post-task affective response to exercise: translating exercise intentions into behaviour. Br J Health Psychol, 2010, 15: 115–131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Schneider M, Dunn A, Cooper D: Affect, exercise, and physical activity among healthy adolescents. J Sport Exerc Psychol, 2009, 31: 706–723. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Ekkekakis P, Hargreaves EA, Parfitt G: Invited Guest Editorial: Envisioning the next fifty years of research on the exercise–affect relationship. Psychol Sport Exerc, 2013, 14: 751–758. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Ekkekakis P: Let them roam free? Physiological and psychological evidence for the potential of self-selected exercise intensity in public health. Sports Med, 2009, 39: 857–888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Parfitt G, Rose EA, Burgess WM: The psychological and physiological responses of sedentary individuals to prescribed and preferred intensity exercise. Br J Health Psychol, 2006, 11: 39–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Lind E, Ekkekakis P, Vazou S: The affective impact of exercise intensity that slightly exceeds the preferred level: ‘pain’ for no additional ‘gain’. J Health Psychol, 2008, 13: 464–468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Freitas LA, Ferreira SS, Freitas RQ, et al. : Effect of a 12-week aerobic training program on perceptual and affective responses in obese women. J Phys Ther Sci, 2015, 27: 2221–2224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Almeida FA, Nunes RF, Ferreira SS, et al. : Effects of musical tempo on physiological, affective, and perceptual variables and performance of self-selected walking pace. J Phys Ther Sci, 2015, 27: 1709–1712. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Vazou-Ekkekakis S, Ekkekakis P: Affective consequences of imposing the intensity of physical activity: does the loss of perceived autonomy matter? Hell J Psychol, 2009, 6: 125–144. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Emmons RA, Diener E: A goal-effect analysis of everyday situational choices. J Res Pers, 1986, 20: 309–326. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Sheppard KE, Parfitt G: Acute affective responses to prescribed and self-selected exercise intensities in young adolescent boys and girls. Pediatr Exerc Sci, 2008, 20: 129–141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Glass SC, Stanton DR: Self-selected resistance training intensity in novice weightlifters. J Strength Cond Res, 2004, 18: 324–327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Focht BC: Perceived exertion and training load during self-selected and imposed-intensity resistance exercise in untrained women. J Strength Cond Res, 2007, 21: 183–187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Ratamess NA, Faigenbaum AD, Hoffman JR, et al. : Self-selected resistance training intensity in healthy women: the influence of a personal trainer. J Strength Cond Res, 2008, 22: 103–111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Elsangedy HM, Krause MP, Krinski K, et al. : Is the self-selected resistance exercise intensity by older women consistent with the American College of Sports Medicine guidelines to improve muscular fitness? J Strength Cond Res, 2013, 27: 1877–1884. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Portugal EM, Lattari E, Santos TM, et al. : Affective responses to prescribed and self-selected strength training intensities. Percept Mot Skills, 2015, 121: 465–481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Focht BC, Garver MJ, Cotter JA, et al. : Affective responses to acute resistance exercise performed at self-selected and imposed loads in trained women. J Strength Cond Res, 2015, 29: 3067–3074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Robertson RJ, Goss FL, Rutkowski J, et al. : Concurrent validation of the OMNI perceived exertion scale for resistance exercise. Med Sci Sports Exerc, 2003, 35: 333–341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Gearhart RE, Goss FL, Lagally KM, et al. : Standardized scaling procedures for rating perceived exertion during resistance exercise. J Strength Cond Res, 2001, 15: 320–325. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Gearhart RF, Jr, Goss FL, Lagally KM, et al. : Ratings of perceived exertion in active muscle during high-intensity and low-intensity resistance exercise. J Strength Cond Res, 2002, 16: 87–91. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Hardy CJ, Rejeski WJ: Not what, but how one feels: the measurement of affect during exercise. J Sport Excercise Psychol, 1989, 11: 204–317. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Semenick DM: Testing protocols and procedures. In: Essentials of strength training and conditioning. Champaign: Human Kinetics, 1994, pp 258–273. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Hopkins WG: Measures of reliability in sports medicine and science. Sports Med, 2000, 30: 1–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Rose EA, Parfitt G: A quantitative analysis and qualitative explanation of the individual differences in affective responses to prescribed and self-selected exercise intensities. J Sport Exerc Psychol, 2007, 29: 281–309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Suminski RR, Robertson RJ, Arslanian S, et al. : Perception of effort during resistance exercise. J Strength Cond Res, 1997, 11: 261–265. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bibeau WS, Moore JB, Mitchell NG, et al. : Effects of acute resistance training of different intensities and rest periods on anxiety and affect. J Strength Cond Res, 2010, 24: 2184–2191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Ekkekakis P, Lind E: Exercise does not feel the same when you are overweight: the impact of self-selected and imposed intensity on affect and exertion. Int J Obes, 2006, 30: 652–660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Ekkekakis P, Hall EE, Petruzzello SJ: The relationship between exercise intensity and affective responses demystified: to crack the 40-year-old nut, replace the 40-year-old nutcracker! Ann Behav Med, 2008, 35: 136–149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Cox KL, Burke V, Gorely TJ, et al. : Controlled comparison of retention and adherence in home- vs center-initiated exercise interventions in women ages 40–65 years: The S.W.E.A.T. Study (Sedentary Women Exercise Adherence Trial). Prev Med, 2003, 36: 17–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Dishman RK, Farquhar RP, Cureton KJ: Responses to preferred intensities of exertion in men differing in activity levels. Med Sci Sports Exerc, 1994, 26: 783–790. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Dishman RK: Compliance/adherence in health-related exercise. Health Psychol, 1982, 1: 237–267. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Schoenfeld BJ, Wilson JM, Lowery RP, et al. : Muscular adaptations in low- versus high-load resistance training: a meta-analysis. Eur J Sport Sci, 2016, 16: 1–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Physical Therapy Science are provided here courtesy of Society of Physical Therapy Science

RESOURCES