Abstract
Both real and hypothetical monetary rewards are widely used as reinforcers in risk taking and decision making studies. However, whether real and hypothetical monetary rewards modulate risk taking and decision making in the same manner remains controversial. In this study, we used event-related potentials (ERP) with a balloon analogue risk task (BART) paradigm to examine the effects of real and hypothetical monetary rewards on risk taking in the brain. Behavioral data showed reduced risk taking after negative feedback (money loss) during the BART with real rewards compared to those with hypothetical rewards, suggesting increased loss aversion with real monetary rewards. The ERP data demonstrated a larger feedback-related negativity (FRN) in response to money loss during risk taking with real rewards compared to those with hypothetical rewards, which may reflect greater prediction error or regret emotion after real monetary losses. These findings demonstrate differential effects of real versus hypothetical monetary rewards on risk taking behavior and brain activity, suggesting a caution when drawing conclusions about real choices from hypothetical studies of intended behavior, especially when large rewards are used. The results have implications for future utility of real and hypothetical monetary rewards in studies of risk taking and decision making.
Understanding how people take risks and make decisions is a challenge for many disciplines such as economics, psychology, and neuroscience. Individual decisions are often motivated by the simulation of rewards. As a secondary but strong incentive, monetary reward is widely used in the real-world situations as well as in laboratory studies. In the real-world, companies typically use real money to retain and motivate their workers1. However, in laboratory studies, monetary rewards are often hypothetical due to the impractical expensive cost and/or unethical issues associated with real money incentives. In order to draw conclusions about real choices from hypothetical reports of intended behavior, it is necessary to validate that real and hypothetical rewards have a similar effect for guiding people’s action choice.
Various cognitive and economical paradigms have been used to compare the effects of real versus hypothetical monetary rewards on risk taking and decision making behavior, including the discounting task (DDT)2,3,4,5,6, probability discounting task (PDT)7,8,9, social discounting task10, Iowa Gambling Task (IGT)11, and economical games like the sharing game, the ultimatum game, and the dictator game12. However, the findings from these studies are inconsistent. While some studies showed similar effects2,3,4,5,7,10,11, other studies reported significant differential effects of real versus hypothetical rewards on decision behavior6,8,9,12. Moreover, people may overstate hypothetical valuations and plans compared to real choices, suggesting a “hypothetical bias” during decision making13,14,15,16,17. Using function magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), neuroimaging studies have also reported inconsistent findings regarding the effects of real versus hypothetical monetary rewards on brain activation patterns during decision making. No differences in brain activation were observed between real versus hypothetical monetary rewards during discounting tasks in one study18; however, another study found stronger activity in brain regions in responses to the value signals during a real choice condition compared to hypothetical choice condition19.
The balloon analogue risk task (BART) has also been used to examine the effects of real and hypothetical monetary rewards on risk taking behavior20. The BART is laboratory-based cognitive task originally developed by Lejuez and colleagues21 to provide an ecologically valid model for the assessment of risk taking propensity and behavior. During this task, participants are repeatedly given the option to continue or discontinue inflating a virtual balloon that could growing larger or explode (see Fig. 1). A larger balloon is associated with an increased probability of explosion but also with the potential for greater reward. Unlike other paradigms such as the IGT in which risk is defined and manipulated by arbitrary connections between stimuli and outcomes, risk in the BART is more directly and ecologically defined as the probability of explosion for each balloon. The average number of inflation pumps participants made for the balloons provides an objective assessment of risk taking propensity20,21. When comparing real versus hypothetical monetary rewards using the BART, previous studies have shown that subjects stop inflating balloons earlier and are more risk averse for a balloon if they received a negative outcome (i.e., money loss) during the previous balloon trial in the real reward monetary condition20. However, the neural mechanisms underlying the differential effects of real versus hypothetical monetary rewards on risk taking and decision making behavior remain unknown.
In the present study, we used electroencephalography (EEG) to measure time-locked event-related potential (ERP) responses to risky decision making during the BART, in order to compare the effects of real and hypothetical rewards on risk taking behavior and brain activity. ERP is a non-invasive electrophysiological measure of brain response consisting of a series of positive and negative voltage deflections that is the direct result of a specific sensory, motor, or cognitive process22. Previous decision making studies have used EEG and identified a specific ERP component, the feedback-related negativity (FRN), which is sensitive to feedback valence and typically more pronounced for negative outcomes during decision making23,24,25. We hypothesized that BART risk taking would show larger FRN in response to negative feedback (i.e., money loss due to balloon explosion) during real monetary reward condition as compared to hypothetical reward condition.
Results
Behavior Results
When averaging across all balloon trials, subjects tended to pump the balloon more times in the hypothetical reward condition than in the real reward condition (mean ± SD, 7.70 ± 1.40 vs. 7.18 ± 1.13, t(18) = 1.89, p = 0.07, Cohen’s d = 0.41; Fig. 2a). Further analysis showed that, during the trials immediately following a negative feedback, subjects pumped the balloon significantly more times in the hypothetical reward condition than in the real reward condition (7.72 ± 1.52 vs. 7.08 ± 1.35, t(18) = 2.65, p = 0.016, Cohen’s d = 0.45; Fig. 2b left). However, during the trials immediately following a positive feedback, the number of balloon pumps did not differ between the hypothetical and real reward conditions (7.74 ± 1.46 vs. 7.35 ± 1.20, t(18) = 0.99, p > 0.3, Fig. 2b right).
ERP Results
In line with previous ERP studies of the BART26,27,28, an obvious FRN component was observed at the Cz electrode site (Fig. 3a,c). Therefore, we analyzed the FRN using the mean amplitudes of ERP responses to different trials in the 180–240 ms time window post-onset of feedback from the Cz location. There were significantly more negative ERP responses to balloon loss (negative feedback) during the real reward condition compared to the hypothetical reward condition (5.12 ± 5.03 μV vs. 8.44 ± 6.88 μV, t(18) = −3.58, p = 0.002, Cohen’s d = 0.56; Fig. 3b). However, no differences were found between FRN amplitudes in response to balloon win (positive feedback) in the real reward condition compared to those in the hypothetical reward condition (5.99 ± 3.47 μV vs. 6.61 ± 3.13 μV, t(18) = −0.85, p > 0.4, Fig. 3b). A 2 (valence: positive/negative) × 2 (reward type: real/hypothetical) repeated measures ANOVA analysis on the FRN amplitudes (Fig. 2b) revealed a significant interaction between reward type and feedback valence (F(1, 18) = 5.41, p = 0.032, η2 = 0.23), a significant main effect of the reward type (F(1, 18) = 10.86, p = 0.004, η2 = 0.38), and no main effect of feedback valence (F(1, 18) = 0.26, p > 0.60, η2 = 0.01). The different waveforms between the negative feedback and positive feedback are displayed in Fig. 3c, which show a greater FRN component for the real reward condition than the hypothetical reward condition. However, no correlations were observed between the FRN amplitudes and risk taking behavior during the BART for both real and hypothetical reward conditions (all p > 0.05).
Discussion
In order to determine whether real and hypothetical monetary rewards modulate risk taking behavior and brain function in a similar manner, the present study used ERP and examined both behavioral and brain responses to positive and negative outcomes from increased risk taking in a BART paradigm. Behavioral data showed that subjects tended to inflate the balloon fewer times in the real monetary reward condition compared to the hypothetical monetary reward condition, especially after negative feedback of balloon burst (money loss). Furthermore, ERP data demonstrated a larger FRN response after a loss trial (balloon explosion and money loss) in the real reward condition compared to the hypothetical reward condition. These findings suggest that real and hypothetical monetary rewards have differential effects on risk taking behavior and brain activity.
The behavioral data in this study are consistent with findings from our previous study showing greater risk aversion during balloon inflation with real monetary rewards20. The results are also in line with previous studies reporting less impulsive choices associated with real monetary rewards8,9. However, these findings are inconsistent with those studies that failed to detect differences in choices behavior using discounting and gambling paradigms2,3,4,5,6,7,8. Several factors, such as different length of delays, payoff size, and payoff probability, may contribute to the inconsistent findings from previous studies. Hypothetical decisions have usually been associated with large payoffs while real decisions are typically made with small payoffs8. Moreover, for most previous DDT studies, real monetary rewards are not awarded in full; usually only one or a few selected responses are awarded as opposed to every trial. For example, participants were awarded about 1 reinforcer for every 90 choices in one study2, and were awarded 1 for every 412 and 1 for every 15 in two experiments in another study4. In the real reward condition of the present study, participants were awarded the exact amount of monetary rewards they won for the balloons, thus eliminating confounding factors associated with payoff size or probability.
In addition to behavioral differences, ERP data further revealed differential brain responses to negative feedback during the BART with real monetary rewards compared to hypothetical rewards. Specifically, a larger FRN was observed after balloon explosion during risk taking with real rewards. This finding may be interpreted by the prediction error in reinforcement learning theory. Numerous ERP studies on decision-making have consistently shown that the FRN is typically elicited by feedback stimuli associated with unfavorable or unexpected outcomes23,24,25,26,29,30,31,32,33,34. Larger FRN was also reported when strong expectancy was violated35. Compared to hypothetical monetary rewards which have no utility in the real world, real monetary rewards are directly related to individual wealth, status, economic interests, power and happiness. Consequently, real monetary rewards may enhance participants’ sensitivity to recent feedback, increase effort to formulate expectancy, and heighten the desire to win. Therefore, when subjects were confronted with a negative outcome (balloon explosion and money loss) during the real monetary reward condition, they might experience a greater sense of expectancy violation and prediction error during the real monetary condition compared to the hypothetical monetary condition, which led to a larger FRN.
Our findings are also consistent with the view that the FRN may reflect the evaluation of motivational and emotional consequences of decision outcomes36,37. FRN appears to be modulated by socio-emotional states and the degree of taking personal responsibility for performance38,39. Previous studies have shown that arousal level is increased after a loss compared to a gain of the same magnitude40, and that regret emotion is induced when individuals are aware that the outcome they obtained would have been better if they had chosen differently41. Greater regret has been reported when participants actually experienced a narrow loss compared to those who only imagined a narrow loss42. Thus, subjects in our study may have exhibited higher arousal and regret after loss trials during the real monetary condition compared to the hypothetical monetary condition, which elicited a larger FRN.
There are several limitations in our study. First, we did not examine the potential effects of different magnitudes of monetary rewards, which may influence differences in brain and behavior between real and hypothetical monetary rewards. Indeed, the previous BART study20 examined risk taking behavior during real and hypothetical monetary rewards in two conditions, a small reward (1 cent per pump) and a large reward (25 cents per pump). Participants exhibited significantly less risk taking behavior during the large real monetary reward condition than the small monetary reward condition. However, there were no differences in risk taking behavior when the rewards were hypothetical20. In the real monetary reward condition only, participants showed more risk averse behavior during the real monetary reward condition than the hypothetical monetary reward condition. This finding is consistent with behavioral results from the current study, in which we used a modified BART paradigm with a large reward (reward doubled after each pump, from 1 cent for the first pump to 2048 cent for the 12th pump) and observed differences in risk taking behavior. Future studies are needed to further examine the effects of both small and large real and hypothetical monetary rewards on risk taking and decision making in the brain.
Second, we did not observe a relationship between FRN amplitudes and risk taking behavior on the BART. This null finding may be due to the limitations of our modified BART paradigm. Due to time constraints in our study, we decreased the maximal inflation capacity of each balloon trial from 128 pumps (original BART) to 12 pumps (our modified BART); thus we may have reduced the sensitivity of our task for measuring individual differences in risk taking performance. Our null correlation was consistent with another study which also shortened the number of inflation capacity to 20 pumps26,27. Future studies are needed to replicate the current study while maintaining the original BART parameters.
In summary, this study used ERP with the BART paradigm and demonstrated differential effects of real versus hypothetical monetary rewards on risk taking behavior and brain responses. These findings suggest a caution to decision researchers when drawing conclusions about real choices from hypothetical studies of intended behavior, especially when large monetary rewards are used. The results have important implications for future utility of real and hypothetical monetary rewards to modulate risk taking and decision making.
Methods
Subjects
Nighteen healthy undergraduate students (mean age = 19.6 ± 1.0 years, 10 males) participated in this study. All participants reported no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders or head trauma. We excluded participants if they reported that they had drunk alcohol or smoked tobacco during the two weeks prior to the test. The present study was approved by the Sun Yat-sen University ethics committee. The entire experimental protocol was carried out according to the approved guidelines, which were in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All subjects provided written informed consent before enrollment and were compensated for participating in the study. They also received additional payment which was commensurate with earnings from the real monetary reward condition, but not the hypothetical monetary reward condition. Every participant ended up with earnings from 45 to 65 Chinese Yuan (about US $7 to $10).
Tasks and Procedure
Participants sat comfortably in a chair and completed the BART with both real money and hypothetical rewards. Before the study, participants were instructed to maximize the amount of monetary reward on all tasks. They were told that only the earnings from the real monetary reward condition, but not the earnings from the hypothetical monetary reward condition, would be compensated to them after the study. With the exception of the facticity of monetary rewards, the two versions of the task were identical in all other respects.
The BART paradigm (Fig. 1) was modified from previous studies27,43. During the task, subjects were asked to inflate a balloon that could either grow larger or explode. The balloon stimuli were blue spheres with radii that increased proportionally to the amount of money added. The visual angle of the first balloon is 4.5°, which increased by 0.3–0.54° after each inflation. Participants were repeatedly given two options: to press a button to continue inflating the balloon or to press another button to discontinue inflation and collect the reward for the current balloon. If participants chose to stop inflation, they won the reward for the current balloon, which was added to their cumulative earnings. If they chose to continue inflation and the balloon exploded, a silent balloon burst graphic with text indicating the loss of money was presented, and participants lost the reward for the current balloon, which was subtracted from the cumulative earnings as a penalty. The delay between the subject’s button press and feedback was randomized from 1 to 1.2 s.
In order to encourage participants to make multiple inflation attempts for a balloon, both real and hypothetical monetary rewards increased with the balloon size and the probability of explosion. The maximum number of inflation participants could make for each balloon was 12. The probability of the balloon bursting after first inflation was 1/11, after the second inflation was 1/10, and so on, until the 12th pump. The monetary reward was 1 cent for the first balloon and doubled after each pump. The reward value corresponding to each balloon size and the cumulative earning for the task were explicitly displayed on the screen, whereas the exact probability of explosion associated with a given inflation were unknown to participants. Prior to the formal experiment, participants practiced several balloon trials for each condition and experienced both loss and win for the balloons. Each subject completed 100 balloon trials for the real monetary reward condition and 100 balloon trials for the hypothetical monetary reward condition. The order of the two conditions was counter-balanced between subjects. Similar to previous studies21,43,44, risk-taking behavior on the BART was measured by calculating the mean adjusted number of inflation pumps on the trials when the balloon did not explode.
EEG Recording and Analysis
EEG was recorded from an array of 57 electrodes of the 10/10 system. Horizontal and vertical electroαoculogram (EOG) was also recorded. A common average reference was used for online EEG recording and EEG signal was off-line algebraically re-referenced to the average of the left and right mastoids. Electrode impedance was kept below 3 k Ω. EEG was amplified with a band pass of 0.1–100 Hz, digitized online at a sampling rate of 512 Hz, and then off-line filtered with a digital low-pass of 30 Hz. Each epoch of EEG was from 100 ms of pre-stimulus to 1000 ms of post-stimulus. Trials contaminated by eye blinks, eye movement or muscle potentials exceeding ±100 μv at any electrode were excluded before averaging. ERP were then averaged for all stimuli within each session. The baseline for ERP measurements was the mean voltage over the 100 ms pre-stimulus interval.
Based on the FRN literature26,27,28 and visual inspection of our ERP waveforms, we analyzed the FRN effects using the mean amplitudes of ERP responses to different trials in the 180–240 ms time window from the Cz location. A repeated measures ANOVA was used to examine the effects of reward type and feedback valence on FRN amplitudes. Greenhouse-Geisser correction for repeated measures was applied for statistical analysis.
Additional Information
How to cite this article: Xu, S. et al. Real and hypothetical monetary rewards modulate risk taking in the brain. Sci. Rep. 6, 29520; doi: 10.1038/srep29520 (2016).
Acknowledgments
This research was supported in part by the Program for Professors of Special Appointment (Eastern Scholar) at Shanghai Institutions of Higher Learning (TP2015031, TP2016020), National Natural Science Foundation of China (31070984, 31400872, 31271190, 81200962), Guangdong Natural Science Foundation (2015A03031 3631), New Century Excellent Talents in University (NCET-13-0685), and National Institutes of Health grants (R01 HL102119, R21 DA032022).
Footnotes
Author Contributions H.R., Z.Q. and S.X. designed the study and supervised the research. S.X., Y.P., Y.W., Z.Q. and H.R. carried out the research, collected the data and contributed to data analyses. S.X., Y.P., A.M.S. and H.R. prepared the Figures and wrote the manuscript. All authors discussed the results and commented on the manuscript.
References
- Tang T. L.-P., Kim J. K. & Tang D. S.-H. Does attitude toward money moderate the relationship between intrinsic job satisfaction and voluntary turnover? Hum Relat 53, 213–245 (2000). [Google Scholar]
- Johnson M. W. & Bickel W. K. Within-subject comparison of real and hypothetical money rewards in delay discounting. J Exp Anal Behav 77, 129 (2002). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Madden G. J. et al. Delay discounting of potentially real and hypothetical rewards: II. Between-and within-subject comparisons. Exp Clin Psychopharm 12, 251 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Madden G. J., Begotka A. M., Raiff B. R. & Kastern L. L. Delay discounting of real and hypothetical rewards. Exp Clin Psychopharm 11, 139 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lagorio C. H. & Madden G. J. Delay discounting of real and hypothetical rewards III: steady-state assessments, forced-choice trials, and all real rewards. Behav Process 69, 173–187 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lane S. D., Cherek D. R., Pietras C. J. & Tcheremissine O. V. Measurement of delay discounting using trial-by-trial consequences. Behav Process 64, 287–303 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matusiewicz A. K., Carter A. E., Landes R. D. & Yi R. Statistical equivalence and test–retest reliability of delay and probability discounting using real and hypothetical rewards. Behav Process 100, 116–122 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jikko Y. & Okouchi H. Real and hypothetical rewards in probability discounting. Jpn J Psychol 78, 269–276 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hinvest N. S. & Anderson I. M. The effects of real versus hypothetical reward on delay and probability discounting. Q J Exp Physiol 63, 1072–1084 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Locey M. L., Jones B. A. & Rachlin H. Real and hypothetical rewards. Judgm Decis Mak 6, 552 (2011). [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowman C. H. & Turnbull O. H. Real versus facsimile reinforcers on the Iowa Gambling Task. Brain Cognition 53, 207–210 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fantino E., Gaitan S., Kennelly A. & Stolarz-Fantino S. How reinforcer type affects choice in economic games. Behav Process 75, 107–114 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cummings R. G., Harrison G. W. & Rutström E. E. Homegrown values and hypothetical surveys: is the dichotomous choice approach incentive-compatible? Am Econ Rev 260–266 (1995). [Google Scholar]
- Johannesson M., Liljas B. & Johansson P.-O. An experimental comparison of dichotomous choice contingent valuation questions and real purchase decisions. Appl Econ 30, 643–647 (1998). [Google Scholar]
- Blumenschein K., Blomquist G. C., Johannesson M., Horn N. & Freeman P. Eliciting Willingness to Pay Without Bias: Evidence from a Field Experiment*. Econ J 118, 114–137 (2008). [Google Scholar]
- O’Donoghue T. & Rabin M. Procrastination on long-term projects. J Econ Behav Organ 66, 161–175 (2008). [Google Scholar]
- Tanner R. J. & Carlson K. A. Unrealistically optimistic consumers: A selective hypothesis testing account for optimism in predictions of future behavior. J Consum Res 35, 810–822 (2009). [Google Scholar]
- Bickel W. K., Pitcock J. A., Yi R. & Angtuaco E. J. Congruence of BOLD response across intertemporal choice conditions: fictive and real money gains and losses. J Neurosci 29, 8839–8846 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kang M. J., Rangel A., Camus M. & Camerer C. F. Hypothetical and real choice differentially activate common valuation areas. J Neurosci 31, 461–468 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu S., Fang Z. & Rao H. Real or Hypothetical Monetary Rewards Modulates Risk Taking Behavior. Acta Psychol Sinica 45, 874–886 (2013). [Google Scholar]
- Lejuez C. W. et al. Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). J Exp Psychol-Appl 8, 75 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hillyard S. A. & Anllo-Vento L. Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95, 781–787 (1998). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holroyd C. B. & Coles M. G. The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychol Rev 109, 679 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nieuwenhuis S., Yeung N., Holroyd C. B., Schurger A. & Cohen J. D. Sensitivity of electrophysiological activity from medial frontal cortex to utilitarian and performance feedback. Cereb Cortex 14, 741–747 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen M. X. & Ranganath C. Reinforcement learning signals predict future decisions. J Neurosci 27, 371–378 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Euser A. S., Van Meel C. S., Snelleman M. & Franken I. H. Acute effects of alcohol on feedback processing and outcome evaluation during risky decision-making: an ERP study. Psychopharmacology 217, 111–125 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fein G. & Chang M. Smaller feedback ERN amplitudes during the BART are associated with a greater family history density of alcohol problems in treatment-naive alcoholics. Drug Alcohol Depen 92, 141–148 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kóbor A. et al. Different strategies underlying uncertain decision making: Higher executive performance is associated with enhanced feedback‐related negativity. Psychophysiology 52, 367–377 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu Y. & Zhou X. The P300 and reward valence, magnitude, and expectancy in outcome evaluation. Brain Res 1286, 114–122 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou Z., Yu R. & Zhou X. To do or not to do? Action enlarges the FRN and P300 effects in outcome evaluation. Neuropsychologia 48, 3606–3613 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baker T. E. & Holroyd C. B. Dissociated roles of the anterior cingulate cortex in reward and conflict processing as revealed by the feedback error-related negativity and N200. Biol Psychol 87, 25–34 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foti D., Weinberg A., Dien J. & Hajcak G. Event‐related potential activity in the basal ganglia differentiates rewards from nonrewards: Temporospatial principal components analysis and source localization of the feedback negativity. Hum Brain Mapp 32, 2207–2216 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Becker M. P., Nitsch A. M., Miltner W. H. & Straube T. A single-trial estimation of the feedback-related negativity and its relation to BOLD responses in a time-estimation task. J Neurosci 34, 3005–3012 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ullsperger M., Fischer A. G., Nigbur R. & Endrass T. Neural mechanisms and temporal dynamics of performance monitoring. Trends Cogn Sci 18, 259–267(2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holroyd C. B. & Krigolson O. E. Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task. Psychophysiology 44, 913–917 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gehring W. J. & Willoughby A. R. The medial frontal cortex and the rapid processing of monetary gains and losses. Science 295, 2279–2282 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masaki H., Takeuchi S., Gehring W. J., Takasawa N. & Yamazaki K. Affective-motivational influences on feedback-related ERPs in a gambling task. Brain Res 1105, 110–121 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leng Y. & Zhou X. Modulation of the brain activity in outcome evaluation by interpersonal relationship: an ERP study. Neuropsychologia 48, 448–455 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li P. et al. The influence of the diffusion of responsibility effect on outcome evaluations: Electrophysiological evidence from an ERP study. Neuroimage 52, 1727–1733 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tversky A. & Kahneman D. The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science 211, 453–458 (1981). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raeva D., Mittone L. & Schwarzbach J. Regret now, take it now: On the role of experienced regret on intertemporal choice. J Econ Psychol 31, 634–642 (2010). [Google Scholar]
- Fernandez-Duque D. & Landers J. Feeling more regret than I would have imagined”: Self-report and behavioral evidence. Judgm Decis Mak 3, 449–456 (2008). [Google Scholar]
- Rao H., Korczykowski M., Pluta J., Hoang A. & Detre J. A. Neural correlates of voluntary and involuntary risk taking in the human brain: an fMRI Study of the Balloon Analog Risk Task (BART). Neuroimage 42, 902–910 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu S., Korczykowski M., Zhu S. & Rao H. Assessment of risk-taking and impulsive behaviors: A comparison between three tasks. Soc Behav Personal 41, 477 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]