Abstract
Observations by Alfred Wallace and Jared Diamond of plumage similarities between co-occurring orioles (Oriolus) and friarbirds (Philemon) in the Malay archipelago led them to conclude that the former represent visual mimics of the latter. Here, we use molecular phylogenies and plumage reflectance measurements to test several key predictions of the mimicry hypothesis. We show that friarbirds originated before brown orioles, that the two groups did not co-speciate, although there is one plausible instance of co-speciation among species on the neighbouring Moluccan islands of Buru and Seram. Furthermore, we show that greater size disparity between model and mimic and a longer history of co-occurrence have resulted in a stronger plumage similarity (mimicry). This suggests that resemblance between orioles and friarbirds represents mimicry and that colonization of islands by brown orioles has been facilitated by their ability to mimic the aggressive friarbirds.
Keywords: Australo-Papua, coexistence, community assembly, competition, island biogeography, molecular phylogeny
1. Introduction
Species may resemble each other in appearance for various reasons. For example, closely related species may not have had sufficient time to diverge phenotypically, or convergence may lead to a similar appearance across distantly related species. This could have developed in the course of millions of years either by random chance or because a given design (shape or colour) is favoured in a particular habitat or environment. Alternatively, mimicry may increase the resemblance of one species (mimic) to another (the model), thereby leading to an advantage for the mimic or for both species [1,2].
Interspecific social dominance mimicry (ISDM) is a proposed form of social parasitism in which a subordinate species evolves to mimic and deceive a dominant ecological competitor to avoid attack by the dominant model species. Among the roughly 10 000 species of birds, there may be about 50 phylogenetically independent origins of visual ISDM (reviewed by Prum [3]), and game-theoretic models have shown the plausibility of this hypothesis [4]. However, few of these putative cases have been investigated in detail.
Wallace [5] was the first, to the best of our knowledge, to describe visual mimicry between orioles (Oriolus, Oriolidae) and friarbirds (Philemon, Meliphagidae) in the Moluccan islands. He suggested that orioles could avoid attack by hawks by mimicking the aggressive friarbirds. Stresemann [6] disputed the mimicry claim, which he thought was simply the result of convergent evolution (“Resultat unabhängiger Convergenz der Entwicklungsrichtungen”). More recently, Diamond [7] provided a detailed account of the various explanations of the mimicry between orioles and friarbirds and based on field observations suggested that orioles do mimic the friarbirds to avoid attack from the friarbirds when feeding in the same fruiting trees. Diamond [7] also noted that the similarities between orioles and friarbirds must represent a case of mimicry and not convergence, because field evidence suggested that the smaller orioles benefit from mimicking the larger pugnacious friarbirds. Consequently, Wallace and Diamond both believed that orioles mimic friarbirds but provided two different explanations for the origin.
The oriole–friarbird study system is remarkable because it includes multiple species pairs, which show different degrees of plumage similarity [7]. This provides an excellent setting for studying the evolution of visual mimicry. While both Wallace and Diamond spent years in the field to understand the mimicry of orioles and friarbirds, they could only base their conclusions on observations, distributional data and anecdotal evidence but had no tools to investigate when and where the two species groups evolved relative to each other. Molecular phylogenetic data provide the means to address these questions. Recent studies have suggested that brown orioles originated in Australo-Papua and subsequently dispersed to the Wallacean archipelago [8]. Friarbirds also appear to have originated in the Australo-Papuan region [9–11] with subsequent colonizations of the Wallacean and Pacific archipelagos. However, taxon sampling for friarbirds has been incomplete, and here we present a comprehensive, dated phylogenetic framework including both groups. Because human and avian perceptions of colours differ [12], we also quantify the patterns of colour similarity between orioles and friarbirds based on spectrometric measurements of reflectance of museum specimens and analyse these measurements using psychophysical models of avian colour vision [13].
In this study, we integrate molecular data, morphological data and plumage similarity for orioles (Oriolus) and friarbirds (Philemon) to address questions pertaining to the evolution of mimicry. We explicitly assess the predictions of the ISDM model set forth by Prum [3]. We also assess how coexistence times of models and mimics have affected mimicry, expecting that mimicry improves the longer orioles and friarbirds have coexisted. Finally, we assess the diversification of orioles and friarbirds in space and time to ask when the mimetic group originated relative to the model group and also specifically tested if brown orioles and friarbirds co-speciated.
2. Material and methods
(a). Taxon sampling
We included the molecular data from 53 individuals of 30 out of 32 taxa (all 18 species) of the genus Philemon (missing P. buceroides ammitophilus from interior Australia and P. novaeguineae trivialis from the north coast of southeast New Guinea) and 40 individuals of all 17 taxa (eight species) of brown orioles (Oriolus) following the International Ornithological Congress World Bird List v. 5.1 [14] (electronic supplementary material, table S1). To root the trees, we also included four additional outgroup taxa for Oriolus following Jønsson et al. [8] and four outgroup taxa for Philemon following Andersen et al. [11]. We sequenced the mitochondrial gene ND2 for all individuals and included two nuclear markers (GAPDH and ODC) for three species of brown orioles and one nuclear marker (Fib-5) for 11 species of friarbirds.
(b). Phylogenetic and dating analyses
DNA sequences were aligned for each gene individually using MAFFT [15] and subsequently checked in SEAVIEW [16]. Each of the four gene partitions (ND2, GAPDH, ODC and Fib-5) were then analysed separately in BEAST [17] applying the most appropriate model of nucleotide evolution as determined by Modeltest v. 3.7 [18] following the Akaike information criterion (AIC). Analyses were run for 20 million generations using a relaxed uncorrelated lognormal distribution for the molecular clock model and assuming a Yule speciation process for the tree prior.
We then analysed the concatenated datasets for Philemon (ND2 and Fib-5) and Oriolus (ND2, GAPDH and ODC) independently. These analyses were run for 100 million generations (sampling every 1000 generations) using a relaxed uncorrelated lognormal distribution for the molecular clock model and assuming a birth–death speciation process for the tree prior. We used two nucleotide substitution partitions for Oriolus and three nucleotide substitution partitions for Philemon applying the most appropriate model of nucleotide evolution as determined by Modeltest v. 3.7 [18] following the AIC. Finally, we analysed ND2 for both Philemon and Oriolus combined using a relaxed uncorrelated lognormal distribution for the molecular clock model and assuming a birth–death speciation process for the tree prior. To obtain absolute dates, we applied to our data a rate of 0.0145 substitutions per site per lineage (2.9%) per Myr [19]. We used default prior distributions for all other parameters and ran Markov chain Monte Carlo chains for 100 million generations sampling every 1000th generation to produce a posterior distribution of 100 000 trees.
All analyses were repeated multiple times, and convergence diagnostics were assessed using tracer [20] checking that effective sample size values for all parameters were higher than 200, suggesting little autocorrelation between samples. Output trees were summarized using TreeAnnotator [21] as a maximum clade credibility (MCC) tree after discarding 25% of generations as burn-in.
(c). Ancestral state reconstruction
We used LAGRANGE [22,23] to estimate ancestral areas within Oriolus and Philemon. We assigned three geographical areas for the LAGRANGE analysis: (i) Australo-Papua, (ii) Wallacea (islands west of New Guinea), and (iii) the Pacific (islands east of New Guinea). We randomly selected 1000 trees from the posterior distribution of the BEAST analysis of the concatenated dataset and ran LAGRANGE on each of these trees. The frequency of the most likely ancestral areas for clades was plotted as marginal distributions on the MCC tree derived from the BEAST analysis, recording the area (max. areas = 2) with the highest relative probability for each node.
(d). Friarbird–brown oriole co-speciation
We tested the strength of evolutionary associations between co-occurring friarbirds and orioles by using the function ‘ParaFit’ [24] implemented in the R package ‘ape’ [25] in R v. 3.1.2 [26]. The null hypothesis is that the two phylogenies and the set of co-occurrence links have diversified independently.
(e). Colour resemblance between orioles and friarbirds
We quantified plumage similarity between orioles and friarbirds using spectrometric measurements from museum specimens and visual models. Reflectance spectra were collected using a Avaspec 2048 spectrometer connected to a AvaLight-XE pulsed xenon light source (Avantes, Eerbeek, The Netherlands) through a bifurcated fibre-optic cable. The measuring probe was fitted with a black plastic cylinder (6 mm inner diameter) to standardize measuring distance (5 mm) and exclude ambient light, which was held perpendicular to the plumage (coincidental normal, [27]). We collected the reflectance spectra from 18 standardized plumage patches: front, crown, dorsal neck, upper back, lower back, rump, dorsal tail proximal, dorsal tail distal, lores, cheek, wing scapulars, wing primaries, throat, ventral neck, upper breast, lower breast, belly and vent (electronic supplementary material, figure S1). Specimens were measured at the British Museum of Natural History in Tring, UK. We measured one to six specimens per species (average = 3.8, s.e. = 0.26; see electronic supplementary material, table S2 for a list of specimens) for 137 specimens and 38 taxa at subspecies level (14 orioles and 24 friarbirds, see electronic supplementary material), totalling 2457 reflectance spectra. Reflectance spectra (300–700 nm, the visual sensitivity range of most birds, [12]) were expressed relative to a WS-2 white standard (Avantes). For the analyses, we pooled the data from male and female specimens.
Differences in coloration between orioles and friarbirds were assessed using psychophysical models of avian vision [13], computing chromatic and achromatic contrast between homologous plumage patches for each oriole–friarbird pair (for further details on visual models, see electronic supplementary material, figure S2). Chromatic contrasts quantify the differences in hue and saturation between homologous patches while achromatic contrasts quantify differences in brightness (light to dark). For each sympatric oriole–friarbird pair, we averaged all chromatic contrasts, and all achromatic contrasts computed between homologous plumage patches and combined these into a new variable (colour contrast) following [28] (for more details, see the electronic supplementary material).
Chromatic and achromatic contrasts were computed following Vorobyev et al. [13] and Siddiqi et al. [29], respectively. Given that oriole mimicry is thought to be aimed at deceiving friarbirds, we used visual sensitivity functions for V-type birds [30], the most likely visual system for friarbirds [31]. Similarly, to compute noise-to-signal ratios (using a Weber fraction of 0.05 and formula 9 in [13], we used the average cone proportions (0.41 : 0.77 : 1.01 : 1; VS : S : M : L) of the only two species in the family for which there are data (Entomyzon cyanotis and Manorina melanocephala [32]). Sensitivity functions for double cones are not known to vary between species, and we used the sensitivity spectrum of Leiothrix lutea and a Weber fraction of 0.05 [29]. Finally, we used the spectrum of standard daylight (D65) as illuminant [13]. Visual models were run in the R environment [26], using the scripts in Delhey et al. [33] (for further details, see the electronic supplementary material).
(f). Statistical analyses
Diamond [7] suggested that the greater the size difference between the model and the mimic, the better the plumage mimicry. Another aspect that appears relevant is the time of co-occurrence. If orioles mimic friarbirds, then one would hypothesize that the mimicry improves the longer orioles and friarbirds have coexisted. Size differences were computed as the ratio between oriole and friarbird tarsus length. K.A.J. measured the length of the tarsus for one to eight specimens (in total, 65 specimens) of 24 out of 32 taxa of Philemon and one to five specimens (in total, 36 specimens) of 15 out of 17 taxa of brown orioles (electronic supplementary material, table S3). Coexistence time was taken as the youngest divergence time to the nearest sister taxon of any given co-occurring oriole–friarbird pair (e.g. for Philemon moluccensis and Oriolus bouroensis that coexist on the island of Buru, the age of the oriole is 2.13 Myr as this is the estimated age of the node of common ancestry between this taxon and its sister taxon. Similarly, the age of the friarbird is 2.67 Myr. Thus, the time of coexistence is set at the youngest of the two time estimates at 2.13 Myr). We used generalized-linear models to assess the potential effects of size difference and coexistence time on colour contrast. In all cases, we fitted three models: one for each independent variable in separate models and one for both variables together including the interaction term. These analyses were first carried out, using all sympatric oriole–friarbird pairs as independent data points (n = 33 for coexisting time and n = 30 for size difference owing to missing morphological data on Philemon c. corniculatus). We also used generalized-linear models to assess whether colour contrast, size differences and coexistence times varied geographically (between the three regions in which orioles and friarbirds co-occur: Wallacea, New Guinea and Australia).
These analyses, using all oriole–friarbird pairs, have two drawbacks: some oriole taxa contribute more than one data point to the analyses, and phylogenetic relatedness is not accounted for. Hence, for those oriole taxa with multiple sympatric friarbirds, we averaged colour contrast values to obtain a single taxon-specific value (n = 14 oriole taxa). We then fitted phylogenetic generalized-linear models with function ‘pgls’ as implemented in the R package ‘caper’ [34], using the oriole phylogeny depicted in figure 1. These models estimate Pagel's lambda, a parameter that indicates the degree of phylogenetic correlation in the data and statistically accounts for it using suitable branch length transformations [36]. As mentioned above, these models included those testing for the separate effects of size difference and coexistence time and for their effects in interaction. We did not test for regional differences using this approach owing to low number of oriole taxa in some areas (New Guinea).
Figure 1.
Distribution of coexisting friarbirds and brown orioles with the phylogeny of brown orioles oriented to match the phylogeny of friarbirds (see also electronic supplementary material, figures S3–S5 for additional phylogenetic information). Species names are coloured according to island distributions. To the right of taxon names are indicated distributions (Au, Australia; NG , New Guinea) followed by a number which indicates coexistence. For example, Philemon brassi (NG, 1) occurs in New Guinea and is sympatric with Oriolus szalayi (NG, 1,5,6,9). Timescales (in million years) are indicated below the phylogenies. Illustrations from Handbook of the Birds of the World [35]. Asterisks at nodes indicate posterior probabilities of 0.99–1.00.
The values of colour contrast computed provide us an indication of the absolute levels of contrast between orioles and friarbirds. If oriole plumage has evolved to match that of sympatric friarbirds, then we expect that values of colour contrast between orioles and sympatric friarbirds should be lower compared with colour contrast values between orioles and allopatric friarbirds. To quantify this, we computed for each oriole taxon (n = 14) the average colour contrast with sympatric and allopatric friarbirds and compared them using a phylogenetic paired t-test [37] as implemented in the R package ‘phytools’ [38] using the oriole phylogeny depicted in figure 1. Negative values of paired differences would indicate that orioles are better mimics of sympatric than allopatric friarbirds. Similar to the phylogenetic generalized-linear models mentioned above, this test estimates lambda and adjusts results accordingly.
3. Results
(a). Phylogenetic relationships and taxonomy
Analysis of the mitochondrial ND2 gene produced a phylogeny for brown orioles and friarbirds (figure 1 and electronic supplementary material, S3), which is almost fully congruent with the phylogenies for friarbirds and orioles based on both mitochondrial and nuclear data (electronic supplementary material, figures S4–S5), with only minor differences for poorly supported nodes. The two subspecies of O. melanotis (O. m. melanotis and O. m. finschi) differ by 6.4% in mitochondrial DNA, which is similar to differences between several other species of brown orioles. Also, O. decipiens represents a distinct lineage, which is not closely related to O. bouroensis as previously suggested [35,39]. The two species of brown orioles of Australo-Papua with five (O. sagittatus) and six (O. flavocinctus) subspecies are both monophyletic and subspecies are largely indistinguishable in the molecular analysis. Three species of friarbirds (P. novaeguineae, P. buceroides and P. citreogularis) are not monophyletic and may include multiple valid species.
(b). Timing, origin and dispersal of friarbirds and orioles
Ancestral area analyses are not able to unambiguously identify a particular region as the origin for brown orioles and friarbirds (figure 1). Other analyses with a broader taxon sampling have suggested that the origin is likely Australo-Papuan [8–11]. While colonization of Wallacea by brown orioles may be the result of a single dispersal event with back-colonization to Australo-Papua, the ancestral area reconstruction analysis suggests that friarbirds colonized both the Pacific and the Wallacean archipelagos multiple times (figure 1). The dating analysis suggests that friarbirds originated 9.9 Ma (95% HPD = 8.3–11.5 Ma), whereas brown orioles originated 5.0 Ma (95% HPD = 4.2–5.9 Ma). Despite the difference in time of origin, most friarbirds colonized Wallacea and the Pacific within the last 5 Myr.
To test for the possible coevolutionary radiation between brown orioles and friarbirds, we evaluated the null hypothesis that events of speciation of coexisting friarbirds and brown orioles have taken place independently of each other. The time of speciation among all friarbird–oriole pairs was evaluated against the association from 500 randomly assigned matches of species. The timing of co-speciation among coexisting lineages was not different from the co-speciation of randomly assigned species pairs (ParaFitGlobal = 120 580.7, p = 0.235). Thus, there is no evidence of co-speciation among coexisting friarbirds and orioles.
(c). Colour similarity between friarbirds and orioles
Across all sympatric oriole–friarbird pairs, both size disparity and time of co-occurrence separately correlated with better mimicry across regions (table 1 and electronic supplementary material, S4 and S5). Colour contrast was lower for pairs that had coexisted longer and for pairs where friarbirds were relatively larger than orioles. In addition, we found a significant positive interaction between both variables (table 1 and electronic supplementary material, S4 and S5) indicating that the effects of time and size together are not completely additive. Indeed, the poorest cases of mimicry were found in species pairs that had short coexistence times and where orioles were nearly as large as friarbirds (figure 3). These effects could explain why plumage colour resemblance between orioles and friarbirds is most marked in the Wallacean islands and in New Guinea compared with the Australian mainland (figure 2a, F2,30 = 8.79, p < 0.001). Both size differences (F2,27 = 4.81, p = 0.016) and coexistence times (F2,30 = 7.9, p = 0.001) vary between regions being lowest in Australia (figure 3). Thus, it appears that better mimicry in Wallacea and New Guinea can be explained largely by the combination of co-occurrence times and size differences.
Table 1.
Results from linear models assessing the effects of coexistence time and size difference between sympatric oriole–friarbird pairs and their interaction on colour differences between orioles and friarbirds. All sympatric oriole–friarbird pairs are included as independent data points in these analyses. Sample size: 30 oriole–friarbird pairs for the interaction and size difference models and 33 pairs for the coexistence time model. Sample size difference owing to missing morphological data on Philemon corniculatus corniculatus.
interaction model |
coexistence time model |
size difference model |
||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
estimate | s.e. | t | p | estimate | s.e. | t | p | estimate | s.e. | t | p | |
intercept | −8.90 | 5.99 | −1.49 | 0.149 | 9.72 | 0.75 | 12.95 | 0.000 | −4.81 | 4.881 | −0.985 | 0.3329 |
coexistence time (millions of years) | 6.82 | 3.94 | 1.73 | 0.095 | −1.68 | 0.60 | −2.79 | 0.009 | — | — | — | — |
relative size difference (tarsus length) | 23.53 | 7.48 | 3.14 | 0.004 | — | — | — | — | 16.617 | 6.153 | 2.701 | 0.0116 |
coexistence time × size difference | −10.99 | 5.24 | −2.10 | 0.046 | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — |
adjusted R2 = 0.36 | adjusted R2 = 0.17 | adjusted R2 = 0.17 |
Figure 3.
Average coexistence time and relative size difference correlate with the level of colour contrast between orioles and coexisting friarbirds. Red colours depict high and green colours low values of colour contrast. (Online version in colour.)
Figure 2.
Colour contrast between orioles and sympatric friarbirds varies across the study region (a) as does coexistence time (b) and size difference measured as the ratio between oriole and friarbird tarsus length (c).
Phylogenetic generalized-linear models confirm the effects of size differences and coexistence time presented above. At the taxon level, (n = 14) brown orioles that have lower colour contrast against sympatric friarbirds are those that have coexisted with relatively larger friarbirds for longer (table 2 and electronic supplementary material, S6 and S7, figures 2 and 3). Finally, orioles tend to resemble sympatric friarbirds more than allopatric ones, but this difference is not significant (phylogenetic paired t-test, mean paired colour contrast [sympatric–allopatric] = −2.04, s.e. = 1.29, t = −1.58, p = 0.14, λ = 0.66).
Table 2.
Results from phylogenetic linear models assessing the effects of coexistence time and size difference between sympatric oriole–friarbird pairs and their interaction on colour differences between orioles and friarbirds. Phylogenetic relatedness was accounted for by using the phylogeny of orioles depicted in figure 1. For orioles that are sympatric with more than one species of friarbird, values of colour distance, coexistence time and size difference were averaged prior to the analysis so that each oriole taxon entered the analysis once. Sample size: 14 oriole taxa.
interaction model |
coexistence time model |
size difference model |
||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
estimate | s.e. | t | p | estimate | s.e. | t | p | estimate | s.e. | t | p | |
intercept | −13.16 | 9.21 | −1.43 | 0.184 | 8.97 | 1.62 | 5.55 | <0.001 | −8.72 | 7.01 | −1.24 | 0.238 |
coexistence time (millions of years) | 15.09 | 7.02 | 2.15 | 0.057 | −1.50 | 0.82 | −1.82 | 0.093 | — | — | — | — |
relative size difference (tarsus length) | 28.08 | 11.31 | 2.48 | 0.032 | — | — | — | — | 20.64 | 9.29 | 2.22 | 0.046 |
coexistence time × size difference | −22.94 | 10.25 | −2.24 | 0.049 | — | — | — | — | — | — | — | — |
adjusted R2 = 0.39 lambda = 0.53 |
adjusted R2 = 0.15 lambda = 0.30 |
adjusted R2 = 0.23 lambda = 0.54 |
4. Discussion
Recently, Prum [3,40] summarized the current knowledge of ISDM in birds and set forth predictions of the ISDM model. First, ‘Visual deception should be physiologically plausible at ecologically relevant visual distances between individuals’. This is certainly the case for this study system as both the models and mimics are medium-sized passerine birds that often feed (and compete for resources) in the same fruiting trees [7].
Second, ‘Model species are larger in body mass than mimic species, and socially dominant over them’. Our morphological data demonstrate that members of Philemon and Oriolus overlap in size. However, while comparing coexisting orioles and friarbirds, it is clear that there are notable size differences between the models (larger) and the mimics, particularly in the Indonesian islands. Diamond [7] suggested that a larger difference in size between co-occurring friarbirds and brown orioles led to better mimicry. He hypothesized that if mimicry had evolved because it enabled orioles to feed alongside aggressive friarbirds, there would be a stronger selection on small orioles to mimic friarbirds as they would be more vulnerable to attacks. Based on a large-scale survey, Prum [3] found that linear dimensions of mimics are on average 82% of the model, which is in good agreement with our study (the tarsus length of orioles was on average 76% of that of sympatric friarbirds, n = 14). However, the relative size differences between the model and the mimic varied greatly within the study system (the linear dimensions of mimics ranged between 62% and 92%), and this variation was correlated with colour contrast (figures 2 and 3). Orioles that are substantially smaller than co-occurring friarbirds show lower levels of colour contrast to the model and are therefore better mimics.
Third, ‘Model and mimic species are not closest relatives, and are each more closely related to other species that differ in their appearance’. It is well established that friarbirds and brown orioles belong to different clades (Meliphagidae and Oriolidae) that are well separated in the passerine tree [41]. Our phylogenetic analyses of the two groups show that the origin of friarbirds dates back to the Miocene (9.9 Ma) with the majority of taxa having evolved within the last 5 Myr. This probably reflects the emergence of an extensive archipelago to the west of New Guinea [42], which was colonized multiple times by friarbirds (figure 1 and electronic supplementary material, figure S3). Brown orioles originated later (5.0 Ma), and the number of colonizations of the Wallacean archipelagos remains unclear. While the biogeographic origins of the clades are ambiguous, the results suggest that they both colonized and diversified within Wallacea at roughly the same time during the Late Miocene and the Early Pliocene. Consequently, some friarbirds and brown orioles may have coexisted for millions of years on these islands. Our results show that friarbirds and brown orioles do not represent a case of co-speciation but that the sequential order of island colonization across the Wallacean islands took place in different ways for the two groups. However, there is one plausible instance of co-speciation among species on the neighbouring Moluccan islands of Buru (Philemon moluccensis mimicked by Oriolus bouroensis) and Seram (Philemon subcorniculatus mimicked by Oriolus forsteni; figure 1).
Finally, ‘SDM can create a coevolutionary arms race in visual appearance. If geographic isolation or speciation occurs subsequent to the origin of mimicry, independent populations of models and mimics may produce coevolutionary radiation in visual appearance among clades’. Diamond [7] pointed out several convincing reasons that brown orioles mimic friarbirds and not vice versa. However, the mimicking brown orioles are not a monophyletic assemblage of species (figure 1 and electronic supplementary material, figure S3). Our analyses suggest that different lineages of brown orioles currently present in the Moluccan islands (O. phaeochromus, O. bouroensis and O. forsteni) and in the lesser Sunda islands (O. decipiens and O. melanotis) have evolved improved mimicry compared with their relatives in Australia. It is uncertain if the brown oriole clade adapted to mimic friarbirds early on or if being brown simply provided a selective advantage that could be further honed once brown orioles came into contact with friarbirds in depauperate island ecosystems. Following Diamond's [7] observations, we found a negative relationship between levels of plumage divergence and size divergence, thereby confirming that the closest plumage similarity (i.e. the strongest mimicry) is found in the coexisting species pairs that differ most in size. In addition, we found that the duration of coexistence between friarbirds and orioles also affected the degree of mimicry. Oriole–friarbird pairs that have presumably coexisted for longer times had lower levels of colour contrast (figures 2b and 3). Taken together, these results suggest that both the strength of selection to avoid aggression, which should be more intense the larger the size difference, and the opportunity for phenotypic change to happen afforded by longer periods of coexistence, have shaped the evolution of mimicry in brown orioles.
5. Conclusion
We present a detailed analysis of a classic example of visual mimicry in birds in Australo-Papua and the surrounding archipelagos, using nearly complete dated molecular subspecies-level phylogenies, and drawing on several lines of evidence to determine how brown orioles (the mimics) evolved in relation to friarbirds (the model). Consistent with the mimicry hypothesis, our analyses show that friarbirds evolved before brown orioles and that they speciated and dispersed largely independently of each other in Australo-Papua and the surrounding archipelagos. In the depauperate island environments, improved mimicry probably facilitated the persistence of brown orioles. Our data also show that orioles tend to better match the colour of sympatric friarbirds than allopatric friarbirds. Finally, a longer history of co-occurrence and a larger size difference between model and mimic lead to better mimicry, ultimately allowing for the existence of both friarbirds and orioles on Wallacean islands.
Supplementary Material
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We thank the following institutions for providing tissue and/or footpad samples for this study: American Museum of Natural History, New York, NY, USA (Paul Sweet); Australian National Wildlife Collection, Canberra, Australia (Leo Joseph); British Museum of Natural History, Tring, UK (Robert Prŷs-Jones, Hein van Grouw and Mark Adams); Naturalis Biodiversity Center, Leiden, The Netherlands (Steven van der Mije and René Dekker); Swedish Museum of Natural History, Stockholm, Sweden (Ulf Johansson); Zoological Museum, University of Copenhagen, Denmark (Jon Fjeldså and Jan Bolding Kristensen). We are grateful for valuable comments and insights provided by Jared Diamond, Richard Prum, Jordan Price, an anonymous reviewer and associate editor Daniel Costa. We also thank Lynx Edicions for giving us permission to use illustrations from the Handbook of the Birds of the World.
Data accessibility
Genetic sequences are uploaded on GenBank (KX272488–KX272597, electronic supplementary material, table S1). Plumage coloration data are available from the electronic supplementary material.
Authors' contributions
K.A.J., G.S. and M.I. conceived the study and carried out the sequencing. K.A.J. collected the morphological data and K.D. collected the plumage coloration data. K.A.J. and K.D. performed the analyses and interpreted the analyses. K.A.J. wrote the manuscript with input from all other authors.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Funding
Fieldwork in the Moluccas was supported by a National Geographic Research and Exploration grant (no. 8853-10), and we are grateful to a number of Indonesian institutions that facilitated our fieldwork: the State Ministry of Research and Technology (RISTEK), the Ministry of Forestry, Republic of Indonesia, the Research Center for Biology, Indonesian Institute of Sciences (RCB-LIPI) and the Museum Zoologicum Bogoriense (MZB). From the MZB, we are particular indebted to Tri Haryoko, Mohammed Irham and Sri Sulandari. K.A.J. acknowledges the Danish National Research Foundation for funding to the Center for Macroecology, Evolution and Climate (grant no. DNRF96). K.A.J. further acknowledges support from the People Programme (Marie Curie Actions) of the European Union's Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007–2013) under REA grant agreement no. PIEF-GA-2011-300924. K.D. was supported by the Australian Research Council (DECRA, DE120102323), G.S. was supported by a grant from Mark Constantine, M.I. was supported by the Swedish Research Council (grant no. 621-2014-5113), and P.E. was supported by the Swedish Research Council (grant no. 621-2013-561).
References
- 1.Wickler W. 1968. Mimicry in plants and animals. New York, NY: McGraw Hill. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Ruxton GD, Sherrat JA, Speed MP. 2004. Avoiding attack: the evolutionary ecology of crypsis, warning signals, and mimicry. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Prum RO. 2014. Interspecific social dominance mimicry in birds Zool. J. Linn. Soc. 172, 910–941. ( 10.1111/zoj.12192) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Prum RO, Samuelson L. 2016. Mimicry cycles, traps, and chains: the coevolution of toucan and kiskadee mimicry. Am. Nat. 187, 753–764. ( 10.1086/686093) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Wallace AR. 1869. The Malay archipelago. New York, NY: Dover Publication. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Stresemann E. 1914. Beiträge zur Kenntnis der Avifauna von Buru. Novit. Zool. 21, 358–400. ( 10.5962/bhl.part.16332) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Diamond JM. 1982. Mimicry of friarbirds by orioles. Auk 99, 187–196. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Jønsson KA, Bowie RCK, Moyle RG, Irestedt M, Christidis L, Norman JA, Fjeldså J. 2010. Phylogeny and biogeography of Oriolidae (Aves: Passeriformes). Ecography 33, 1–10. ( 10.1111/j.1600-0587.2009.06412.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Driskell AC, Christidis L. 2004. Phylogeny and evolution of the Australo-Papuan honeyeaters (Passeriformes, Meliphagidae). Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 31, 943–960. ( 10.1016/j.ympev.2003.10.017) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Gardner JL, Trueman JWH, Ebert D, Joseph L, Magrath RD. 2010. Phylogeny and evolution of the Meliphagoidea, the largest radiation of Australasian songbirds. Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 55, 1087–1102. ( 10.1016/j.ympev.2010.02.005) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Andersen MJ, Naikatini A, Moyle RG. 2014. A molecular phylogeny of Pacific honeyeaters (Aves: Meliphagidae) reveals extensive paraphyly and an isolated Polynesian radiation. Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 71, 308–315. ( 10.1016/j.ympev.2013.11.014) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Cuthill IC. 2006. Color perception. In Bird coloration (eds Hill GE, McGraw K), pp. 3–40. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Vorobyev M, Osorio D, Bennett AT, Marshall NJ, Cuthill IC. 1998. Tetrachromacy, oil droplets and bird plumage colours. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 183, 621–633. ( 10.1007/s003590050286) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Gill F, Donsker D (eds). 2015. IOC world bird list (v 5.1). See http://www.worldbirdnames.org/ (accessed July 2015). [Google Scholar]
- 15.Katoh K, Misawa K, Kuma K, Miyata T. 2002. MAFFT: a novel method for rapid multiple sequence alignment based on fast Fourier transform. Nucl. Acids Res. 30, 3059–3066. ( 10.1093/nar/gkf436) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Gouy M, Guindon S, Gascuel O. 2010. SeaView version 4: a multiplatform graphical user interface for sequence alignment and phylogenetic tree building. Mol. Biol. Evol. 27, 221–224. ( 10.1093/molbev/msp259) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Drummond AJ, Suchard MA, Xie D, Rambaut A. 2012. Bayesian phylogenetics with BEAUti and the BEAST 1.7. Mol. Biol. Evol. 29, 1969–1973. ( 10.1093/molbev/mss075) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Posada D, Crandall KA. 1998. Modeltest: testing the model of DNA substitution. Bioinformatics 14, 817–818. ( 10.1093/bioinformatics/14.9.817) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Lerner HRL, Meyer M, James HF, Hofreiter M, Fleischer RC. 2011. Multilocus resolution of phylogeny and timescale in the extant adaptive radiation of Hawaiian honeycreepers. Curr. Biol. 21, 1838–1844. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2011.09.039) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Rambaut A, Drummond AJ. 2007. TRACER v1.4. See http://beast.bio.ed.ac.uk/Tracer.
- 21.Drummond AJ, Rambaut A. 2007. BEAST: Bayesian evolutionary analysis by sampling trees. BMC Evol. Biol. 7, 214 ( 10.1186/1471-2148-7-214) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Ree RH, Moore BR, Webb CO, Donoghue MJ. 2005. A likelihood framework for inferring the evolution of geographic range on phylogenetic trees. Evolution 59, 2299–2311. ( 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2005.tb00940.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Ree RH, Smith SA. 2008. Maximum likelihood inference of geographic range evolution by dispersal, local extinction, and cladogenesis. Syst. Biol. 57, 4–14. ( 10.1080/10635150701883881) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Legendre P, Desdevises Y, Bazin E. 2002. A statistical test for host–parasite coevolution. Syst. Biol. 51, 217–234. ( 10.1080/10635150252899734) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Paradis E, Claude J, Strimmer K. 2004. APE: analyses of phylogenetics and evolution in R language. Bioinformatics 20, 289–290. ( 10.1093/bioinformatics/btg412) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.R Development Core Team. 2014. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Andersson S, Prager M. 2006. Quantifying colors. In Bird coloration, vol. 1 (eds Hill GE, McGraw K), pp. 41–89. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Darst CR, Cummings ME, Cannatella DC. 2006. A mechanism for diversity in warning signals: conspicuousness versus toxicity in poison frogs. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 103, 5852–5857. ( 10.1073/pnas.0600625103) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Siddiqi A, Cronin TW, Loew ER, Vorobyev M, Summers K. 2004. Interspecific and intraspecific views of color signals in the strawberry poison frog Dendrobates pumilio. J. Exp. Biol. 207, 2471–2485. ( 10.1242/jeb.01047) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Endler JA, Mielke PW. 2005. Comparing entire colour patterns as birds see them. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 86, 405–431. ( 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2005.00540.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Ödeen A, Håstad O, Alström P. 2011. Evolution of ultraviolet vision in the largest avian radiation—the passerines. BMC Evol. Biol. 11, 313 ( 10.1186/1471-2148-11-313) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Hart N. 2001. Variations in cone photoreceptor abundance and the visual ecology of birds. J. Comp. Physiol. A 187, 685–697. ( 10.1007/s00359-001-0240-3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Delhey K, Delhey V, Kempenaers B, Peters A. 2015. A practical framework to analyze variation in animal colors using visual models. Behav. Ecol. 26, 367–375. ( 10.1093/beheco/aru198) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Orme D, Freckleton R, Thomas G, Petzoldt T, Fritz S, Isaac N, Pearse W. 2013. Caper: comparative analyses of phylogenetics and evolution in R. R package version 0.5.2. See http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=caper.
- 35.del Hoyo J, Elliott A, Christie DA. 2008. Handbook of the birds of the world, vol. 13 Barcelona, Spain: Lynx Edicions. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Freckleton RP, Harvey PH, Pagel M. 2002. Phylogenetic analysis and comparative data: a test and review of evidence. Am. Nat. 160, 712–726. ( 10.1086/343873) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Lindenfors P, Revell LJ, Nunn CL. 2010. Sexual dimorphism in primate aerobic capacity: a phylogenetic test. J. Evol. Biol. 23, 1183–1194. ( 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2010.01983.x) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Revell LJ. 2012. Phytools: an R package for phylogenetic comparative biology (and other things). Methods Ecol. Evol. 3, 217–223. ( 10.1111/j.2041-210X.2011.00169.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Dickinson EC, Christidis L. 2014. The Howard and Moore complete checklist of the birds of the world, 4th edn, vol. 2 Eastbourne, UK: Aves Press. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Prum RO, Samuelson L. 2012. Evolution of interspecific social dominance mimicry modeled by the ‘Hairy-Downy’ game. J. Theor. Biol. 313, 42–60. ( 10.1016/j.jtbi.2012.07.019) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Barker FK, Cibois A, Schikler P, Feinstein J, Cracraft J. 2004. Phylogeny and diversification of the largest avian radiation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci USA 101, 11 040–11 045. ( 10.1073/pnas.0401892101) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Hall R. 2009. Southeast Asia's changing palaeogeography. Blumea 54, 148–161. ( 10.3767/000651909X475941) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Genetic sequences are uploaded on GenBank (KX272488–KX272597, electronic supplementary material, table S1). Plumage coloration data are available from the electronic supplementary material.