Skip to main content
American Journal of Hypertension logoLink to American Journal of Hypertension
. 2016 Mar 10;29(8):913–924. doi: 10.1093/ajh/hpw013

Psychosocial Factors Are Associated With Blood Pressure Progression Among African Americans in the Jackson Heart Study

Cassandra D Ford 1,, Mario Sims 2, John C Higginbotham 3, Martha R Crowther 4, Sharon B Wyatt 2,5,, Solomon K Musani 2, Thomas J Payne 6, Ervin R Fox 2, Jason M Parton 7
PMCID: PMC4941592  PMID: 26964661

Abstract

BACKGROUND

Research that examines the associations of psychosocial factors with incident hypertension among African Americans (AA) is limited. Using Jackson Heart Study (JHS) data, we examined associations of negative affect and stress with incident hypertension and blood pressure (BP) progression among AA.

METHODS

Our sample consisted of 1,656 normotensive participants at baseline (2000–2004) (mean age 47±12; 61% women). We investigated associations of negative affect (cynical distrust, anger-in, anger-out, and depressive symptoms) and stress (perceived stress, weekly stress inventory (WSI)-event, WSI-impact, and major life events) with BP progression (an increase by one BP stage as defined by JNC VII) and incident hypertension by examination 2 (2005–2008). Poisson regression analysis was utilized to examine the prevalence ratios (PRs; 95% confidence interval (CI)) of BP tracking and incident hypertension with psychosocial factors, adjusting for baseline age, sex, socioeconomic status (SES), and hypertension risk factors.

RESULTS

Fifty-six percentage of the sample (922 cases) had BP progression from 2005 to 2008. After adjustment for age, sex, and SES, a high anger-out score was associated with a 20% increased risk of BP progression compared to a low anger-out score (PR 1.20; 95% CI 1.05–1.36). High depressive symptoms score was associated with BP progression in the age, sex, and SES-adjusted model (PR 1.14; 95% CI 1.00–1.30). High WSI-event scores were associated with BP progression in the fully adjusted model (PR 1.21; 95% CI 1.04–1.40). We did not observe significant associations with any of the psychosocial measures and incident hypertension.

CONCLUSIONS

Psychosocial factors were associated with BP progression, with the strongest evidence for number of stressful events that occurred.

Keywords: African Americans, blood pressure, health disparities, hypertension, Jackson Heart Study, psychosocial.


Hypertension is a risk factor for cardiovascular disease (CVD), affecting 77.9 million adults in the United States.1,2 While the prevalence among men and women is nearly equivalent (33.6% and 32.3%, respectively),3 previous studies report a higher prevalence of among African Americans (AA) than other groups.4,5 Although rates of control have improved among AA, disparities persist6,7 and continue to increase.3 Studies also report a higher incidence of hypertension in AA than Whites.8–10

While hypertension is attributed to known risk factors (i.e., age, diet, family history),5,11,12 psychosocial factors may also contribute to hypertension risk and related CVD risk factors.13–16 Yan et al. 17 found that greater hostility and time urgency–impatience were associated with a higher risk of hypertension among AA and White young adults. Cohen and colleagues18 discussed the impact of mental health factors (i.e., depression, anxiety, and stress) and the extent to which they are associated with CVD and related risk factors. Research has reported that greater levels of depressive symptoms were associated with incident hypertension,19,20 and that increased negative affect was associated with hypertension risk among AA and Whites.21

There is limited research on psychosocial factors and blood pressure (BP) risk in large AA samples. Previous studies have examined few psychosocial measures, which limits the ability to fully capture the extent to which psychological ill-being predicts BP outcomes. Since research has primarily examined psychosocial factors and incident hypertension as an outcome, it is important to examine the extent to which psychosocial factors are associated outcomes other than incident hypertension (such as BP progression and changes in systolic and diastolic BP). Using Jackson Heart Study (JHS) data, we hypothesized that higher levels of negative affect (cynical distrust, anger-in, anger-out, and depressive symptoms) and stress (global stress, weekly stress, and major life events (MLEs)) are associated with BP progression, incident hypertension, and changes in systolic and diastolic BP among AA between 2000–2004 and 2005–2008.

METHODS

Study population

The JHS, an observational study of the etiology of CVD among AA, includes 5,301 men and women (21–95 years old) at the baseline examination (2000–2004) living in the tri-county area of Jackson, MS. Examination 2 (2005–2008) included 4,203 participants. Each examination included a physical examination, blood/urine analyses, and interview questions including psychosocial measures. Further details about data collection are described elsewhere.22,23 The study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the 3 participating universities and participants provided informed consent.

BP measures

Standardized protocols were used for examinations 1 and 2. Systolic BP (SBP) and diastolic BP (DBP) were measured twice in the right arm of participants using the random-zero BP sphygmomanometer (Hawksley and Sons Limited, Sussex, England). The first BP measurement was taken after the participant rested for 5 minutes in a seated position; the second BP was taken after waiting an additional minute. The average of the 2 measurements comprised the BP measure for the examination.

For the primary analysis, we examined BP progression (increase) by at least one category according to the Joint National Committee (JNC VII) on Prevention, Detection, Evaluation, and Treatment of High Blood Pressure. We also performed the analysis using JNC VI criteria; however, since results varied little, we present only results for JNC VII criteria. We also examined incident hypertension and longitudinal changes in SBP and DBP. The categories utilized for BP progression included normal BP (<120/80mm Hg), prehypertension (120–139/80–89mm Hg), stage 1 (140–159/90–99mm Hg) and stage 2 (≥160/≥100) hypertension. Categories for BP progression were classified according to JNC VII guidelines which showed the level of progression and how much SBP and DBP progressed. Incident hypertension was defined as new cases of hypertension from examination 1 to 2 and was derived from a BP of 140/90mm Hg or higher, reported taking antihypertensive medications, pill bottle review, or being told by a provider that one has hypertension. Longitudinal changes in SBP and DBP were measured as continuous changes between examination 1 and 2.

Psychosocial factors

Baseline psychosocial measures included cynical distrust, anger-in and -out, depressive symptoms, global stress, weekly stress inventory (WSI)-event, WSI-impact, and MLE. Cynical distrust was measured as a component of the Cook–Medley Hostility Scale,24 where participants were asked to answer “true or false” on such questions as “….It is safer to trust nobody.” We calculated a cynical distrust score where the total average of all “true” responses was divided by 13 (range 0–13). Cronbach’s alpha was 0.76.

Anger was measured using the Spielberger trait anger scale, a 16-item scale that assesses anger-in (8 items) and anger-out (8 items).25,26 Participants were asked to rate how often they reacted to: “I keep things in”; “I express my anger.” Each item is rated from never (0) to almost always (5). The average of both was derived and ranged from 0 to 23 (anger-in) and 0 to 22 (anger-out). In general, internal consistency coefficients for the scale were 0.86–0.95, and the range for test–retest reliability coefficients was 0.65–0.75.27 For this specific study, internal consistency was good for the scale: total anger (α = 0.81), anger-in (α = 0.77), and anger-out (α = 0.77).

Depressive symptoms were measured using the 20-item Centers for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) that measures frequency of depressive symptoms.28 Participants were asked about their mood over the past week, responding to each item (e.g., “I was bothered by things that usually don’t bother me”). Item ratings ranged from 0 (“rarely/none of the time”) to 3 (“most/all of the time”). This scale ranged from 0 to 60 with higher scores reflecting greater levels of depressive symptoms. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.82.

Global perceived stress was created for the JHS29 and adapted from standardized stress scales.30,31 Participants rated the extent of chronic stress from 8 domains (e.g., employment, relationships) over a 12-month period. Choices ranged from “not stressful” (=1) to “very stressful” (=3); summing items yields a total score ranging from 0 to 24. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.72.

The WSI, developed by Jones and Brantley,32,33 is an 87-item questionnaire that measured occurrence of minor irritants (e.g., work tasks, financial challenge) during the past week and the extent of stressfulness based on a 7-point Likert-type scale (1 = not stressful; 7 = extremely stressful). We measured WSI-event, the number of stressful events endorsed (range 0–87), and WSI-impact, the sum of the subjective ratings assigned to endorsed events (range 0–493).33 Cronbach’s alpha was 0.97.

MLEs were assessed using an 11-item life events inventory.34 The MLE, administered at annual follow-up, measured whether participants experienced events in the last 12 months by answering “yes” or “no” to select items (experiencing illness, being a victim of assault). The total score was calculated by summing the “yes” (=1) responses where the range was 1–11. Since the MLE is an index rather than a true scale, Cronbach’s alpha was not calculated.

We constructed cumulative scores for negative affect and stress measures.16 Cumulative negative affect was constructed by assigning a separate score of 1 (referent) to 3 to tertiles of the cynical distrust, anger-in and -out, and depression measures separately and summing across the 4 measures (range 4–12). Cumulative stress was constructed by assigning a separate score of 1 (referent) to 3 to tertiles of the global stress, WSI-event, WSI-impact, and MLE measures separately and summing across the 4 measures (range 4–12).

Covariates

Covariates included baseline age (continuous), sex (men/women (referent)), education, cigarette smoking, physical activity, diet, body mass index, type 2 diabetes, total cholesterol, SBP, and DBP. Education was classified as less than high school (referent), high school graduate to some college, or college graduate and above. Cigarette smoking status was classified as current, former, and never (referent). Physical activity was measured as a continuous sum of the 4 index scores (Active Living, Work/Occupational, Home Life, and Sport) from the JHS physical activity instrument.35 Diet was assessed using a validated 158-item questionnaire.36 Measures related to the Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension trial37 (percent calories from fat, sodium (mg/1,000 kcal), potassium (mg/1,000 kcal), calcium (mg/kcal), and total fiber (g/1,000 kcal)) were examined. Body mass index was calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared. Type 2 diabetes was defined according to American Diabetes Association (ADA) 2004 criteria as fasting glucose ≥ 126mg/dl, or confirmed medication usage from the medication inventory, or self-reported use of antidiabetic medications within the past 2 weeks of the examination, or self-reported diabetes diagnosis. Total cholesterol was assayed by the cholesterol oxidase method supplied by Boehringer Mannheim Diagnostics on a Roche COBAS Fara analyzer (Indianapolis, IN). Baseline hypertension was defined as a SBP ≥ 140mm Hg or DBP ≥ 90mm Hg and/or antihypertensive medication.

Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics were presented, in that we characterized the sample by BP progression status. Differences were tested using chi-square or analysis of variance tests for categorical and continuous variables, respectively. Next, associations of psychosocial measures with each BP end point were estimated. Multivariable Poisson regression analysis estimated the associations of psychosocial factors with BP progression with robust SEs, where prevalence (risk) ratios (PRs; 95% confidence interval (CI)) estimated the prevalence of BP progression. Psychosocial factors were measured in tertiles in order to assess threshold effects and as continuous variables in SD units. P for trend, estimated for tertiles of each psychosocial measure, represented the linear trend across categories. A sensitivity test was conducted to examine associations of psychosocial factors with BP progression from normotensive status only to stage 1 and 2 hypertension, and associations with progression from prehypertensive status only to stage 1 and 2 hypertension. Poisson regression was used to estimate these models, and fully adjusted predicted values for the 2 outcomes were graphically displayed for select psychosocial variables.

The associations of psychosocial measures (SD units) with incident hypertension were estimated using multivariable Poisson regression. Poisson regression was used for this and the BP-progression analyses because prevalence of BP progression was 56%, and incident hypertension was 37%.38 The associations of psychosocial factors with changes in SBP and DBP between examination 1 and 2 were examined using multivariable linear regression to estimate mean differences (SEs) in changes in SBP and DBP. For participants taking antihypertensive medications, BP values were imputed by adding 10mm Hg to the SBP and 5mm Hg to the DBP in each of the analyses. We also performed imputations by adding 15mm Hg to the SBP and 10mm Hg to the DBP.39,40 Since results were similar, we reported those for the first imputation.

For each BP end point, models were estimated sequentially. Model 1 adjusted for age, sex, and psychosocial factors; model 2 adjusted for model 1 plus education; model 3 adjusted for model 2 plus smoking, sodium intake, calories from fat, total fiber, physical activity, body mass index, type 2 diabetes, and total cholesterol. In the BP progression and incident hypertension analyses, we added model 4 that adjusted for model 3 plus baseline SBP and DBP. In order to avoid multicolinearity, each psychosocial measure was analyzed in separate models. A test for interactions between sex and psychosocial measures was calculated in fully adjusted models to examine potential effect modification by sex. All reported P values correspond to 2-tailed tests and were significant at the 0.05 level. Due to performing multiple test scenarios that puts us at an increased risk for type I error, we have implemented a Bonferroni method of correction, for all comparisons and presented the adjusted P values for multiple testing in Tables 24. The following correction for multiple testing was used for the 3 negative affect measures: 0.05/3 = 0.0167 or 0.02. The following correction was used for the 3 stress measures: 0.05/3 = 0.0167 or 0.02. Both unadjusted and adjusted P values will be considered in our analysis. All analyses were performed using STATA 13.0 (STATA Corp, College Station, TX).

Table 2.

PRs of blood pressure progression (JNC VII criteria) with psychosocial factors among African Americans, Jackson Heart Study, 2000–2004 and 2005–2008a

Cases/people at risk Model 1 PR (95% CI) P value Model 2 PR (95% CI) P value Model 3 PR (95% CI) P value
Cynical distrust
 Low (N = 351; 4.0±1.6) 180/351 Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (N = 464; 7.0±1.1) 255/464 1.07 (0.94–1.22) 0.31 1.03 (0.90–1.18) 0.64 0.93 (0.81–1.08) 0.36
 High (N = 222; 11.0±0.9) 125/222 1.10 (0.94–1.28) 0.25 1.05 (0.90–1.23) 0.54 0.99 (0.84–1.17) 0.92
P for trend 0.45 0.82 0.59
 Cynical distrust (SD units) 1.03 (0.97–1.09) 0.31 1.01 (0.96–1.07) 0.67 0.98 (0.92–1.04) 0.52
Anger-in
 Low (N = 432; 3.0±1.3) 232/432 Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (N = 248; 5.0±0.5) 134/248 1.01 (0.87–1.16) 0.93 1.01 (0.88–1.17) 0.87 0.96 (0.82–1.12) 0.62
 High (N = 377; 9.0±2.8) 208/377 1.03 (0.90–1.17) 0.67 1.10 (0.90–1.16) 0.77 0.96 (0.83–1.10) 0.54
P for trend 0.91 0.96 0.46
 Anger-in (SD units) 1.02 (0.97–1.07) 0.50 1.01 (0.96–1.07) 0.57 0.99 (0.93–1.05) 0.64
Anger-out
 Low (N = 435; 2.0±1.0) 216/435 Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (N = 270; 5.0±0.5) 151/270 1.13 (0.98–1.30) 0.08 1.13 (0.98–1.30) 0.08 1.10 (0.94–1.28) 0.23
 High (N = 369; 8.0±2.2) 218/369 1.20 (1.05–1.36) 0.01b 1.20 (1.05–1.36) 0.01b 1.13 (0.98–1.31) 0.09
P for trend 0.02 0.02 0.22
 Anger-out (SD units) 1.06 (1.01–1.12) 0.03b 1.06 (1.01–1.12) 0.03b 1.02 (0.96–1.09) 0.51
Depressive symptoms
 Low (N = 375; 3.0±2.0) 191/375 Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (N = 336; 9.0±1.6) 173/336 1.01 (0.87–1.17) 0.89 0.99 (0.85–1.14) 0.84 1.01 (0.86–1.18) 0.91
 High (N = 333; 17.0±7.1) 201/333 1.18 (1.04–1.35) 0.01b 1.14 (1.00–1.30) 0.05b 1.12 (0.96–1.30) 0.16
P for trend 0.02 0.06 0.28
 Depression (SD units) 1.08 (1.03–1.14) 0.003b 1.06 (1.01–1.12) 0.02b 1.05 (0.99–1.11) 0.13
Perceived stress
 Low (N = 469; 1.0±0.8) 271/469 Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (N = 550; 4.0±1.1) 301/550 0.95 (0.85–1.06) 0.39 0.96 (0.86–1.07) 0.49 0.98 (0.87–1.11) 0.73
 High (N = 628; 10.0±3.1) 347/628 0.96 (0.87–1.07) 0.51 0.96 (0.86–1.07) 0.50 0.95 (0.84–1.07) 0.40
P for trend 0.67 0.74 0.69
 Perceived stress (SD units) 1.00 (0.96–1.04) 0.99 1.00 (0.96–1.04) 0.90 0.98 (0.94–1.03) 0.51
WSI-event
 Low (N = 317; 12.0±6.0) 156/317 Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (N = 413; 28.0±5.1) 234/413 1.16 (1.01–1.34) 0.04b 1.17 (1.01–1.34) 0.03b 1.20 (1.03–1.41) 0.02b
 High (N = 452; 58.0±15.8) 258/452 1.18 (1.02–1.36) 0.03b 1.17 (1.02–1.35) 0.03b 1.24 (1.06–1.45) 0.01b
P for trend 0.06 0.06 0.02
 WSI-event (SD units) 1.05 (1.00–1.10) 0.06 1.05 (1.00–1.10) 0.08 1.05 (1.00–1.11) 0.07
WSI-impact
 Low (N = 299; 20.0±10.6) 146/299 Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (N = 335; 56.0±14.5) 192/335 1.18 (1.02–1.38) 0.03b 1.19 (1.02–1.38) 0.03b 1.18 (1.00–1.38) 0.05b
 High (N = 381; 151.0±80.3) 213/381 1.16 (1.00–1.35) 0.05b 1.15 (0.99–1.34) 0.06 1.13 (0.96–1.33) 0.14
P for trend 0.07 0.07 0.14
 WSI-impact (SD units) 1.04 (0.99–1.10) 0.11 1.04 (0.99–1.10) 0.13 1.04 (0.99–1.11) 0.14
Major life events
 Low (N = 879; 2.0±0.5) 456/879 Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (N = 360; 4.0±0.0) 198/360 1.05 (0.94–1.18) 0.40 1.06 (0.94–1.19) 0.32 1.05 (0.93–1.19) 0.43
 High (N = 271; 5.0±1.0) 160/271 1.13 (1.00–1.27) 0.04b 1.13 (1.00–1.27) 0.05b 1.07 (0.94–1.22) 0.31
P for trend 0.12 0.13 0.52
  Major life events (SD units) 1.04 (0.99–1.09) 0.09 1.04 (0.99–1.09) 0.11 1.02 (0.97–1.07) 0.41
Cumulative negative affect
 Low (N = 316; 5.0±0.7) 157/316 Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (N = 390; 8.0±0.8) 214/390 1.10 (0.96–1.27) 0.18 1.09 (0.94–1.25) 0.26 1.08 (0.92–1.26) 0.34
 High (N = 215; 10.0±0.7) 120/215 1.12 (0.95–1.32) 0.17 1.09 (0.92–1.28) 0.33 1.02 (0.84–1.23) 0.85
P for trend 0.30 0.48 0.59
 Cumulative negative affect (SD units) 1.06 (1.00–1.13) 0.05b 1.05 (0.99–1.11) 0.14 1.02 (0.96–1.10) 0.50
Cumulative stress
 Low (N = 268; 5.0±0.8) 127/268 Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (N = 423; 8.0±0.8) 221/423 1.12 (0.95–1.31) 0.17 1.12 (0.96–1.31) 0.16 1.05 (0.89–1.24) 059
 High (N = 236; 10.0±0.8) 142/236 1.30 (1.10–1.55) 0.002b 1.30 (1.10–1.55) 0.002b 1.24 (1.04–1.48) 0.02b
P for trend 0.01 0.01 0.03
 Cumulative stress (SD units) 1.11 (1.04–1.18) 0.002b 1.11 (1.04–1.18) 0.002b 1.10 (1.03–1.18) 0.01b

Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; BP, blood pressure; CI, confidence interval; JNC, Joint National Committee; PR, prevalence ratio; SES, socioeconomic status; WSI, weekly stress inventory.

aLow, medium, and high represent the first, second, and third tertile of each psychosocial factor at baseline. Sample size, N, and median ± SD are noted in parenthesis for tertile of psychosocial factor; cases represent participants who had progression of BP by at least one category of BP; PR estimates for blood progression represent the prevalence (or risk) of progressing upward by at least one category of BP based on JNC VII criteria. Standardized regression coefficients are presented for blood progression per 1-SD increment of each psychosocial factor. Model 1 is adjusted for age, sex, and psychosocial factor; model 2 is adjusted for model 1 + education; model 3 is adjusted for model 2 + current smoking, sodium intake, calories from fat, total fiber, physical activity, BMI, type 2 diabetes, and total cholesterol. P for trend represents the linear trend across categories for each psychosocial factor. Regression models for each psychosocial measure were restricted to the following analytic samples: cynical distrust (1,037), anger-in (1,057), anger-out (1,074), depressive symptoms (1,044), global perceived stress (1,647), WSI-event (1,182), WSI-impact (1,015), major life events (1,510), cumulative negative affect (921), and cumulative stress (927). Cumulative negative affect is the combination of standardized cynical distrust, anger-in, anger-out, and depressive symptoms. Cumulative stress is the combination of standardized global perceived stress, WSI-event, WS-impact, and major life events stress.

bBonferroni-adjusted P value for multiple testing: 0.02 (negative affect measures) and 0.02 (stress measures).

Table 4.

Mean differences in changes in SBP and DBP (SE) with psychosocial factors among African Americans, Jackson Heart Study, 2000–2004 and 2005–2008 (first imputation)

Psychosocial measures Changes in SBP (SE) Changes in DBP (SE)
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
Cynical distrust
 Low (n = 351) Referent Referent Referent Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (n = 464) 2.48 (2.02) 2.07 (2.04) 1.34 (2.14) 1.64 (1.25) 1.48 (1.27) 0.57 (1.32)
 High (n = 222) 0.41 (2.43) −0.21 (2.48) −1.07 (2.62) 1.92 (1.51) 1.78 (1.54) 1.27 (1.61)
P for trend 0.43 0.49 0.60 0.32 0.40 0.73
 Cynical distrust (SD units) 0.79 (0.88) 0.57 (0.90) 0.18 (0.95) 0.65 (0.55) 0.57 (0.56) 0.32 (0.59)
Anger-in
 Low (n = 432) Referent Referent Referent Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (n = 248) 1.33 (2.13) 1.39 (2.32) 1.59 (2.42) 1.19 (1.43) 1.13 (1.43) 1.06 (1.47)
 High (n = 377) 0.44 (2.04) 0.37 (2.04) −0.58 (2.13) 0.65 (1.26) 0.63 (1.26) 0.64 (1.29)
P for trend 0.85 0.83 0.67 0.70 0.72 0.75
 Anger-in (SD units) −0.03 (0.88) −0.05 (0.88) −0.45 (0.91) 0.69 (0.54) 0.72 (0.54) 0.68 (0.55)
Anger-out
 Low (n = 435) Referent Referent Referent Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (n = 270) −0.56 (2.24) −0.54 (2.24) −0.80 (2.33) 0.40 (1.36) 0.46 (1.36) 0.75 (1.40)
 High (n = 369) 0.79 (2.10) 0.87 (2.10) 0.91 (2.19) 2.22 (1.28) 2.30(1.28) 2.73 (1.31)*,a
P for trend 0.84 0.83 0.78 0.20 0.18 0.11
 Anger-out (SD units) −0.53 (0.93) −0.53 (0.93) −0.71 (0.97) 0.81 (0.57) 0.82 (0.57) 0.84 (0.58)
Depressive symptoms
 Low (n = 375) Referent Referent Referent Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (n = 336) −1.54 (2.15) −1.89 (2.18) −1.47 (2.26) −0.27 (1.33) −0.29 (1.35) 0.13 (1.38)
 High (n = 333) 0.40 (2.20) 0.00 (2.24) −0.17 (2.34) 1.21 (1.36) 1.18 (1.38) 1.27 (1.43)
P for trend 0.65 0.61 0.78 0.53 0.54 0.63
 Depressive symptoms (SD units) −0.31 (0.96) −0.49 (0.97) −0.79 (1.01) 0.53 (0.59) 0.53 (0.60) 0.50 (0.62)
Perceived stress
 Low (n = 469) Referent Referent Referent Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (n = 550) −1.95 (1.86) −1.83 (1.86) −1.55 (1.94) −1.29 (1.11) −1.32 (1.12) −1.11 (1.15)
 High (n = 628) −1.53 (1.83) −1.55 (1.83) −1.29 (1.94) −0.54 (1.10) −0.57 (1.10) −0.63 (1.15)
P for trend 0.55 0.58 0.71 0.50 0.49 0.63
 Perceived stress (SD units) −0.77 (0.74) −0.80 (0.74) −0.93 (0.77) −0.24 (0.44) −0.25 (0.44) −0.36 (0.46)
WSI-event
 Low (n = 317) Referent Referent Referent Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (n = 413) 0.35 (2.18) 0.57 (2.19) 1.08 (2.28) 2.33 (1.34) 2.38 (1.34) 2.63 (1.38)
 High (n = 452) 1.29 (2.20) 1.30 (2.20) 2.21 (2.31) 1.65 (1.35) 1.58 (1.35) 2.53 (1.40)
P for trend 0.82 0.84 0.63 0.21 0.20 0.11
 WSI-event (SD units) 0.72 (0.87) 0.69 (0.87) 0.75 (0.91) 0.54 (0.53) 0.49 (0.54) 0.64 (0.55)
WSI-impact
 Low (n = 299) Referent Referent Referent Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (n = 335) 0.67 (2.30) 0.74 (2.30) 1.38 (2.39) 0.77 (1.42) 0.68 (1.43) 1.46 (1.46)
 High (n = 381) 1.93 (2.28) 1.92 (2.29) 2.02 (2.38) 1.35 (1.41) 1.24 (1.41) 1.75 (1.45)
P for trend 0.69 0.69 0.69 0.64 0.68 0.45
 WSI-impact (SD units) 1.13 (0.90) 1.12 (0.91) 1.19 (0.95) 0.88 (0.56) 0.86 (0.56) 1.04 (0.58)
Major life events
 Low (n = 879) Referent Referent Referent Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (n = 360) 0.54 (1.76) 0.63 (1.76) 0.72 (1.83) 1.71 (1.06) 1.70 (1.06) 1.40 (1.09)
 High (n = 271) −0.92 (1.98) −0.95 (1.98) −1.53 (2.04) 1.04 (1.19) 1.05 (1.19) 1.00 (1.22)
P for trend 0.81 0.79 0.63 0.24 0.25 0.39
 Major life events (SD units) −0.40 (0.76) −0.40 (0.76) −0.56 (0.78) 0.41 (0.46) 0.40 (0.46) 0.42 (0.47)
Cumulative negative affect
 Low (n = 316) Referent Referent Referent Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (n = 390) 2.27 (2.16) 2.13 (2.17) 2.61 (2.27) 1.50 (1.32) 1.46 (1.33) 1.93 (1.37)
 High (n = 215) 0.78 (2.54) 0.43 (2.56) −0.13 (2.69) 1.74 (1.55) 1.69 (1.57) 2.27 (1.63)
P for trend 0.57 0.59 0.42 0.42 0.45 0.26
 Cumulative negative affect (SD units) 0.19 (0.95) 0.05 (0.96) −0.07 (1.01) 0.69 (0.58) 0.67 (0.58) 0.92 (0.61)
Cumulative stress
 Low (n = 268) Referent Referent Referent Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (n = 423) −0.11 (2.19) −0.12 (2.19) 0.03 (2.30) 2.59 (1.36) 2.45 (1.36) 2.38 (1.41)
 High (n = 236) 2.94 (2.57) 2.95 (2.58) 2.96 (2.70) 3.52 (1.60)*,a 3.37 (1.60)*,a 3.67 (1.65)*,a
P for trend 0.37 0.37 0.42 0.06 0.08 0.08
 Cumulative stress (SD units) 0.96 (0.98) 0.96 (0.98) 1.10 (1.03) 1.07 (0.61) 1.02 (0.61) 1.21 (0.63)

PR represents prevalence ratio; low, medium, and high represent the first, second, and third tertile of each psychosocial factor at baseline. Model 1 is adjusted for age, gender, and psychosocial factor; model 2 is adjusted for model 1 + education; model 3 is adjusted for model 2 + current smoking, sodium intake, calories from fat, total fiber, physical activity, BMI, type 2 diabetes, and total cholesterol. P for trend represents the linear trend across categories for each psychosocial factor. First imputation—for participants taking antihypertensive medications, BP values were imputed by adding 10mm Hg to the SBP and 5mm Hg to the DBP.

Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; BP, blood pressure; DBP, diastolic blood pressure; PR, prevalence ratio; SBP, systolic blood pressure; WSI, weekly stress inventory.

aBonferroni-adjusted P value for multiple testing: 0.02 (negative affect measures) and 0.02 (stress measures).

P ≤ 0.1; *P ≤ 0.05.

RESULTS

Sample characteristics

Fifty-three (53) participants with missing data on hypertension and 3,301 hypertensives at baseline were excluded. Two hundred ninety-one (291) participants had missing data on education, smoking, physical activity, and diet variables. This left 1,656 participants free of hypertension at baseline in the final sample (mean age 47±12; 61% women). The median follow-up period was 5.0±0.8 years. Fifty-six percentage (56%) of the sample progressed at least one BP stage from 2000–2004 to 2005–2008 (Table 1). Incident hypertension occurred among 37.4% of normotensives at baseline, and mean increase in SBP and DBP were 6.5 and 3.7mm Hg, respectively.

Table 1.

Characteristics of study population by evidence of blood pressure progression (between baseline and examination 2), Jackson Heart Study, 2000–2004 and 2005–2008

BP progression JNC VII criteria
Without BP progression; 44% (n = 734) With BP progression; 56% (n = 922) P value
Demographic and SES factors
 Mean age (SD) 47.47 (12.28) 48.02 (12.66) 0.37
 Education (%)
  <High school 12.41 14.33 0.01
  High school 4−College 3 39.02 44.73
  College 4+ 48.57 40.93
Psychosocial factors (mean, SD)
 Cynical distrust 6.63 (3.18) 6.84 (3.11) 0.29
 Anger-in 5.70 (3.51) 5.84 (3.73) 0.52
 Anger-out 4.44 (3.05) 4.83 (3.06) 0.04
 Depressive symptoms 9.73 (7.47) 11.14 (8.44) 0.00
 Perceived stress 5.70 (4.37) 5.66 (4.54) 0.85
 WSI-event 34.62 (23.76) 36.93 (22.87) 0.09
 WSI-impact 85.93 (83.42) 93.88 (85.24) 0.14
 Major life events 3.33 (1.37) 3.46 (1.36) 0.07
 Cumulative negative affect 7.53 (2.17) 7.81 (2.16) 0.05
 Cumulative stress 7.64 (2.15) 8.05 (2.19) 0.00
Risk factors
 Cigarette smoking (%)
  Never smoked 74.34 70.87 0.17
  Former smokers 12.97 13.25
  Current smoker 12.69 15.88
 Physical activity score (mean, SD) 9.17 (2.44) 8.91 (2.48) 0.04
 Calories from fat (%) 36.25 35.03 0.00
 Sodium in diet (mg) (mean, SD) 4,506.59 (2,623.95) 4,625.96 (2,693.73) 0.37
 Potassium (mg) (mean, SD) 2,918.43 (1,589.30) 2,976.44 (1,544.93) 0.46
 Calcium (mg) (mean, SD) 932.69 (579.80) 892.59 (532.46) 0.15
 Total dietary fiber (g) (mean, SD) 17.07 (9.76) 16.92 (8.67) 0.75
 BMI (m, SD) 29.99 (6.60) 30.57 (7.33) 0.10
 Diabetes (%) 7.39 4.49 0.02
 Total cholesterol (mg/dl) (mean, SD) 197.06 196.24 0.69
 SBP (mean, SD) 126.12 (18.48) 125.83 (17.40) 0.75
 DBP (mean, SD) 79.35 (11.18) 78.95 (9.76) 0.46
 JNC VII BP category (%)
  Normal 57.90 54.87 0.22
  Prehypertensive 42.10 45.13

Sample sizes for each psychosocial factor: cynical distrust score, n = 1,037; anger-in score, n = 1,057; anger-out score, n = 1,074; depression score, n = 1,044; perceived stress score, n = 1,647; WSI-event score, n = 1,182; WSI-impact score, n = 1,015; major life events, n = 1,510; cumulative negative affect, n = 921; cumulative stress score, n = 927. P value based on chi-square and ANOVA tests. Normotensive status is defined as SBP < 120mm Hg; DBP < 80mm Hg. Prehypertensive is defined as SBP 120–139mm Hg; DBP 80–89mm Hg. Cumulative negative affect is the combination of standardized cynical distrust, anger-in, anger-out, and depressive symptoms. Cumulative stress is the combination of standardized global perceived stress, WSI-event, WSI-impact, and major life events stress.

Abbreviations: ANOVA, analysis of variance; BMI, body mass index; BP, blood pressure; DBP, diastolic blood pressure; JNC, Joint National Committee; SBP, systolic blood pressure; SES, socioeconomic status; WSI, weekly stress inventory.

BP progression

Table 2 presents associations of each psychosocial factor with BP progression in sex-pooled models. There were moderate correlations between cynical distrust and anger-in (Pearson’s coefficient: 0.24; P < 0.001), depressive symptoms and cynical distrust (0.32; P < 0.001), and depressive symptoms and anger-in (0.45; P < 0.001). There were also weak-to-moderate correlations between the stress measures: MLE and WSI-event (0.08; P < 0.01), global stress and WSI-impact (0.39; P < 0.001).

A high anger-out score was associated with a 20% increased risk of BP progression compared to a low anger-out score (PR 1.20; 95% CI 1.05–1.36) (model 1). The association remained significant after adjustment for socioeconomic status (SES). There was a graded association of anger-out with BP progression (P trend = 0.02). The high anger-out values in models 1 and 2 were also significant after adjustment for multiple testing. The standardized regression analysis in model 2 confirmed this association; the risk of BP progression increased by 6% for each 1-SD increase in anger-out (PR 1.06; 95% CI 1.01–1.12). The association attenuated after adjustment for risk factors in model 3. Participants with high depressive symptoms scores had a 14% increased risk of BP progression compared to those with low scores (PR 1.14; 95% CI 1.00–1.30) after adjustment for age, sex, and SES. This association weakened after adjustment for risk factors. After adjustment for multiple testing, high depressive symptoms in model 2 was not significant; however, the continuous depressive symptoms value remained significant (P = 0.02). WSI-event analyses revealed that a greater number of minor stressors was associated with higher risk of BP progression in the 3 models (model 3, P trend = 0.04). WSI-impact was inversely associated with BP progression in each model, high (vs. low) MLE scores were associated with a 13% increased prevalence of BP progression, but estimates attenuated in the fully adjusted model. Participants with high cumulative stress scores had a 24% increased risk of BP progression than those with low scores (PR 1.24; 95% CI 1.04–1.48) after full adjustment. Associations were graded, and standardized regression coefficients confirmed this association. Associations did not change when full models were adjusted for baseline SBP and DBP. Findings remained significant when considering adjusted P values for multiple testing. There was no evidence of effect modification by sex in fully adjusted models, as indicated by the nonsignificant P values for interaction (data not shown).

Select psychosocial factors were marginally associated with BP progression from normotensive status only to stage 1 and stage 2 hypertension (P < 0.1). In fully adjusted models, anger-out, depressive symptoms, WSI-event, and cumulative stress were positively associated with risk of BP progression from prehypertensive status to stage 1 and 2 hypertension (P < 0.05) (Supplementary Figure).

Incident hypertension

We did not observe a significant association with any of the psychosocial factors and incident hypertension (Table 3). There was attenuation in the PRs from models 1 to 3 for most measures, but no significant associations.

Table 3.

PRs of incident hypertension with psychosocial factors among African Americans, Jackson Heart Study, 2000–2004 and 2005–2008

Cases/people at risk Model 1 PR (95% CI) P value Model 2 PR (95% CI) P value Model 3 PR (95% CI) P value
Cynical distrust
 Low (N = 351; 4.0±1.6) 119/351 Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (N = 464; 7.0±1.1) 166/464 1.01 (0.83–1.22) 0.93 0.96 (0.79–1.16) 0.69 0.88 (0.72–1.07) 0.19
 High (N = 222; 11.0±0.9) 80/222 1.04 (0.83–1.30) 0.76 0.98 (0.78–1.23) 0.84 0.93 (0.73–1.17) 0.52
P for trend 0.95 0.92 0.42
 Cynical distrust (SD units) 1.02 (0.93–1.11) 0.71 0.99 (0.91–1.08) 0.90 0.96 (0.87–1.05) 0.37
Anger-in
 Low (N = 432; 3.0±1.3) 155/432 Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (N = 248; 5.0±0.5) 92/248 1.01 (0.83–1.24) 0.91 1.02 (0.84–1.25) 0.83 1.01 (0.82–1.24) 0.95
 High (N = 377; 9.0±2.8) 130/377 0.93 (0.77–1.12) 0.42 0.91 (0.76–1.10) 0.35 0.84 (0.69–1.02) 0.08
P for trend 0.64 0.52 0.14
 Anger-in (SD units) 0.99 (0.91–1.07) 0.71 0.98 (0.91–1.06) 0.63 0.93 (0.86–1.01) 0.10
Anger-out
 Low (N = 435; 2.0±1.0) 152/435 Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (N = 270; 5.0±0.5) 85/270 0.87 (0.70–1.08) 0.21 0.87 (0.70–1.08) 0.20 0.83 (0.66–1.04) 0.11
 High (N = 369; 8.0±2.2) 145/369 1.10 (0.91–1.31) 0.32 1.09 (0.91–1.31) 0.35 1.04 (0.86–1.25) 0.71
P for trend 0.11 0.12 0.14
 Anger-out (SD units) 1.05 (0.96–1.13) 0.28 1.04 (0.96–1.13) 0.31 1.00 (0.92–1.09) 0.94
Depressive symptoms
 Low (N = 375; 3.0±2.0) 129/375 Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (N = 336; 9.0±1.6) 112/336 0.96 (0.78–1.18) 0.69 0.92 (0.75–1.13) 0.42 0.93 (0.75–1.16) 0.53
 High (N = 333; 17.0±7.1) 132/333 1.13 (0.93–1.37) 0.21 1.06 (0.87–1.29) 0.55 1.04 (0.84–1.27) 0.74
P for trend 0.23 0.37 0.61
 Depression (SD units) 1.06 (0.98–1.15) 0.12 1.04 (0.96–1.12) 0.38 1.03 (0.95–1.12) 0.52
WSI-event
 Low (N = 317; 12.0±6.0) 106/317 Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (N = 413; 28.0±5.1) 151/413 1.06 (0.87–1.30) 0.58 1.07 (0.87–1.31) 0.51 1.03 (0.84–1.28) 0.76
 High (N = 452; 58.0±15.8) 169/452 1.03 (0.84–1.26) 0.77 1.03 (0.84–1.26) 0.76 1.02 (0.82–1.26) 0.88
P for trend 0.85 0.79 0.95
 WSI-event (SD units) 1.00 (0.92–1.07) 0.91 0.99 (0.92–1.07) 0.85 0.99 (0.92–1.07) 0.85
WSI-impact
 Low (N = 299; 20.0±10.6) 99/299 Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (N = 335; 56.0±14.5) 124/335 1.08 (0.87–1.33) 0.50 1.09 (0.88–1.35) 0.44 1.15 (0.92–1.44) 0.22
 High (N = 381; 151.0±80.3) 131/381 0.96 (0.77–1.19) 0.71 0.96 (0.77–1.19) 0.69 0.95 (0.75–1.19) 0.64
P for trend 0.51 0.42 0.15
 WSI-impact (SD units) 0.98 (0.90–1.07) 0.67 0.98 (0.90–1.06) 0.60 0.97 (0.89–1.05) 0.44
Major life events
 Low (N = 879; 2.0±0.5) 294/879 Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (N = 360; 4.0±0.0) 120/360 1.02 (0.86–1.21) 0.80 1.03 (0.87–1.23) 0.73 0.98 (0.82–1.18) 0.84
 High (N = 271; 5.0±1.0) 101/271 1.13 (0.95–1.35) 0.18 1.12 (0.94–1.34) 0.20 1.05 (0.87–1.27) 0.58
P for trend 0.40 0.44 0.81
  Major life events (SD units) 1.04 (0.97–1.12) 0.22 1.04 (0.97–1.12) 0.24 1.02 (0.95–1.10) 0.57
Cumulative negative affect
 Low (N = 316; 5.0±0.7) 107/316 Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (N = 390; 8.0±0.8) 141/390 1.05 (0.86–1.28) 0.64 1.03 (0.84–1.26) 0.76 1.02 (0.83–1.27) 0.83
 High (N = 215; 10.0±0.7) 78/215 1.03 (0.81–1.30) 0.83 0.98 (0.77–1.25) 0.89 0.91 (0.70–1.17) 0.46
P for trend 0.90 0.90 0.59
 Cumulative negative affect (SD units) 1.03 (0.94–1.12) 0.56 1.01 (0.92–1.10) 0.83 0.98 (0.89–1.08) 0.65
Cumulative stress
 Low (N = 268; 5.0±0.8) 86/268 Referent Referent Referent
 Medium (N = 423; 8.0±0.8) 134/423 0.96 (0.77–1.20) 0.72 0.97 (0.77–1.21) 0.76 0.97 (0.77–1.22) 0.78
 High (N = 236; 10.0±0.8) 86/236 1.05 (0.82–1.35) 0.70 1.06 (0.82–1.35) 0.67 1.05 (0.81–1.35) 0.73
P for trend 0.73 0.73 0.80
 Cumulative stress (SD units) 0.99 (0.90–1.09) 0.86 0.99 (0.90–1.10) 0.88 1.00 (0.90–1.10) 0.88

Standardized regression coefficients for incident hypertension per 1-SD increment of each psychosocial factor. PR estimates for incident hypertension represent the risk of developing hypertension based on blood pressure and/or interview in examination 2 after being normotensive at baseline. Model 1 is adjusted for age, gender, and psychosocial factor; model 2 is adjusted for model 1 + education; model 3 is adjusted for model 2 + current smoking, sodium intake, calories from fat, total fiber, physical activity, BMI, type 2 diabetes, and total cholesterol. Cumulative negative affect is the combination of standardized cynical distrust, anger-in, anger-out, and depressive symptoms. Cumulative stress is the combination of standardized global perceived stress, WSI-event, WS-impact, and major life events stress.

Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; CI, confidence interval; PR, prevalence ratio; WSI, weekly stress inventory.

Longitudinal changes

While changes in SBP and DBP were examined, psychosocial factors were only associated with changes in DBP (Table 4). In the fully adjusted model, anger-out was associated with changes in DBP (mean difference in DBP: 2.73, P < 0.05). After full adjustment, high (vs. low) cumulative stress was associated with changes in DBP (mean difference in DBP: 3.67, P < 0.05). This association was graded from low to high cumulative stress (P for trend = 0.08) and was not significant according to the P value (0.02) for multiple testing.

DISCUSSION

This study investigated associations of psychosocial factors with BP end points among AA. To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine multiple psychosocial factors and BP end points in a large sample of AA. We found that high anger-out, depressive symptoms, WSI-event, MLE, and cumulative stress scores were associated with risk of BP progression. Associations of psychosocial factors with BP outcomes did not vary by sex. Psychosocial factors were not associated with incident hypertension. We did not observe a significant association of any of the psychosocial factors with incident hypertension. Anger-out and cumulative stress were significantly associated with changes in DBP.

High WSI-event and cumulative stress scores significantly predicted the risk of BP progression, which suggests that the number and accumulation of stressors are important for understanding BP progression. One study found that anger was associated with progression from prehypertension to hypertension over a period of 3–6 years among AA.41 Similarly, we found an association of anger-out with progression from prehypertension to hypertension after full adjustment, which suggests that the relationship is not fully explained by risk factors.

We did not observe significant interactions by sex between psychosocial factors and BP end points, although research has found that anger and depression significantly predicted incident hypertension and changes in SBP among women.20 Räikkönen et al. 20 also reported that an increase in psychological distress over time can affect individuals physiologically, which may be indicated in our pooled analysis. Previous research has also found that stress is associated with CVD risk primarily in men,42 which was not found in our study.

Previous studies have reported associations of psychosocial measures with incident hypertension.20–22 One study of normotensive, middle-aged AA and White women (n = 541) found high levels of anger and anxiety were associated with hypertension risk.20 Conversely, we did not observe any significant associations of any of the psychosocial factors with incident hypertension, which is consistent with one study that found no associations of depressive symptoms with incident hypertension among AA.43 One potential reason we did not observe any significant associations may have been due to not accounting for time to diagnosis of events as previous studies have used.

Our study also examined longitudinal changes in SBP and DBP, which has been examined by a few studies. One multiethnic study found associations of depressive symptoms and slight changes in SBP (+2.4mm Hg) and DBP (+0.8mm Hg) over a 2-year period.43 We found associations of anger-out and cumulative stress and changes in DBP, which supports the association of stress on BP.

One novel feature of our study included the examination of multiple psychosocial measures. Recent studies of psychosocial factors and BP have examined few negative affect measures.41,43 We, however, examined 4 dimensions of negative affect and stress among AA. The examination of more than one dimension is important for representing the broad array of psychological ill-being which is important for predicting BP disparities. The cumulative exposure of these factors (vs. one at a time) may be a truer depiction of how multiple stressors predict chronic diseases among AA, a highly stressed population. Cumulative negative affect did not predict BP outcomes, which is similar to another study that found no associations between cumulative negativity and subclinical disease in a multiethnic sample.16

The findings of this study should be evaluated in the context of several limitations. The study was conducted in a single metropolitan area in the Southeastern United States, possibly limiting its generalizability to other AA populations. The study sample was restricted to individuals that did not have hypertension at baseline. We recognize this may result in a survivor bias; however, the focus of the study was to identify normotensive individuals that developed hypertension over time by way of hypertension incidence and BP progression. We were unable to estimate incident hypertension using time to diagnosis. Participants that developed hypertension between examination 1 and 2 were likely placed on antihypertensive medications upon diagnosis. Although this was accounted for in our imputations that considered those taking antihypertensive medication, this may have affected our longitudinal analysis. The strengths of this study include utilizing JHS, the largest study of CVD in AA, to examine multiple dimensions of negative affect and stress and BP progression. The use of multiple BP end points is another strength. The use of BP progression longitudinally enabled us to draw causal inferences about psychosocial factors and BP progression unlike cross-sectional studies.

In summary, psychosocial factors were associated with BP progression, with the strongest evidence for number of stressful events and accumulation of stressors that occurred, possibly contributing to the clustering of stressors among this AA population. Considering the high levels of stress and affective states reported by AA may be important for preventing the development of hypertension, and the extent to which coping strategies (e.g., family networks) might mitigate the association of psychosocial factors with BP end points among AA should be considered.

Supplementary materials are available at American Journal of Hypertension (http://ajh.oxfordjournals.org).

DISCLOSURE

The authors declared no conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Data

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank the Jackson Heart Study team (University of Mississippi Medical Center, Jackson State University, and Tougaloo College) and participants for their long-term commitment that continues to improve our understanding of the epidemiology of cardiovascular and other chronic diseases. The Jackson Heart Study is supported by contracts HHSN268201300046C, HHSN268201300047C, HHSN268201300048C, HHSN268201300049C, and HHSN268201300050C from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (NHLBI) and the National Institute on Minority Health and Health Disparities (NIMHD). Dr Sims is supported by the grants P60MD002249 and U54MD008176 from the NIMHD. C.D.F. and M.S. should be regarded as joint first authors.

REFERENCES

  • 1. Egan BM, Zhao Y, Axon RN. US trends in prevalence, awareness, treatment, and control of hypertension, 1988-2008. JAMA 2010; 303:2043–2050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Go AS, Mozaffarian D, Roger VL, Benjamin EJ, Berry JD, Blaha MJ, Dai S, Ford ES, Fox CS, Franco S, Fullerton HJ, Gillespie C, Hailpern SM, Heit JA, Howard VJ, Huffman MD, Judd SE, Kissela BM, Kittner SJ, Lackland DT, Lichtman JH, Lisabeth LD, Mackey RH, Magid DJ, Marcus GM, Marelli A, Matchar DB, McGuire DK, Mohler ER, III, Moy CS, Mussolino ME, Neumar RW, Nichol G, Pandey DK, Paynter NP, Reeves MJ, Sorlie PD, Stein J, Towfighi A, Turan TN, Virani SS, Wong ND, Woo D, Turner MB; American Heart Association Statistics Committee and Stroke Statistics Subcommittee Heart disease and stroke statistics–2014 update: a report from the American Heart Association. Circulation 2014; 129:e28–e292. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Go AS, Mozaffarian D, Roger VL, Benjamin EJ, Berry JD, Borden WB, Bravata DM, Dai S, Ford ES, Fox CS, Franco S, Fullerton HJ, Gillespie C, Hailpern SM, Heit JA, Howard VJ, Huffman MD, Kissela BM, Kittner SJ, Lackland DT, Lichtman JH, Lisabeth LD, Magid D, Marcus GM, Marelli A, Matchar DB, McGuire DK, Mohler ER, Moy CS, Mussolino ME, Nichol G, Paynter NP, Schreiner PJ, Sorlie PD, Stein J, Turan TN, Virani SS, Wong ND, Woo D, Turner MB; American Heart Association Statistics Committee and Stroke Statistics Subcommittee Heart disease and stroke statistics–2013 update: a report from the American Heart Association. Circulation 2013; 127:e6–e245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Mujahid MS, Diez Roux AV, Cooper RC, Shea S, Williams DR. Neighborhood stressors and race/ethnic differences in hypertension prevalence (the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis). Am J Hypertens 2011; 24:187–193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Wong ND, Lopez VA, L’Italien G, Chen R, Kline SE, Franklin SS. Inadequate control of hypertension in US adults with cardiovascular disease comorbidities in 2003-2004. Arch Intern Med 2007; 167:2431–2436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Hertz RP, Unger AN, Cornell JA, Saunders E. Racial disparities in hypertension prevalence, awareness, and management. Arch Intern Med 2005; 165:2098–2104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Wyatt SB, Akylbekova EL, Wofford MR, Coady SA, Walker ER, Andrew ME, Keahey WJ, Taylor HA, Jones DW. Prevalence, awareness, treatment, and control of hypertension in the Jackson Heart Study. Hypertension 2008; 51:650–656. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Dyer AR, Liu K, Walsh M, Kiefe C, Jacobs DR, Jr, Bild DE. Ten-year incidence of elevated blood pressure and its predictors: the CARDIA study. Coronary Artery Risk Development in (Young) Adults. J Hum Hypertens 1999; 13:13–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Hajjar I, Kotchen JM, Kotchen TA. Hypertension: trends in prevalence, incidence, and control. Annu Rev Public Health 2006; 27:465–490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Carson AP, Howard G, Burke GL, Shea S, Levitan EB, Muntner P. Ethnic differences in hypertension incidence among middle-aged and older adults: the multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis. Hypertension 2011; 57:1101–1107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Lloyd-Jones DM, Evans JC, Levy D. Hypertension in adults across the age spectrum: current outcomes and control in the community. JAMA 2005; 294:466–472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Forman JP, Stampfer MJ, Curhan GC. Diet and lifestyle risk factors associated with incident hypertension in women. JAMA 2009; 302:401–411. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Everson-Rose SA, Lewis TT. Psychosocial factors and cardiovascular diseases. Annu Rev Public Health 2005; 26:469–500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Sims M, Diez-Roux AV, Dudley A, Gebreab S, Wyatt SB, Bruce MA, James SA, Robinson JC, Williams DR, Taylor HA. Perceived discrimination and hypertension among African Americans in the Jackson Heart Study. Am J Public Health 2012; 102 Suppl 2:S258–S265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Ranjit N, Diez-Roux AV, Shea S, Cushman M, Seeman T, Jackson SA, Ni H. Psychosocial factors and inflammation in the multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis. Arch Intern Med 2007; 167:174–181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Diez Roux AV, Ranjit N, Powell L, Jackson S, Lewis TT, Shea S, Wu C. Psychosocial factors and coronary calcium in adults without clinical cardiovascular disease. Ann Intern Med 2006; 144:822–831. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Yan LL, Liu K, Matthews KA, Daviglus ML, Ferguson TF, Kiefe CI. Psychosocial factors and risk of hypertension: the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) study. JAMA 2003; 290:2138–2148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Cohen BE, Edmondson D, Kronish IM. State of the art review: depression, stress, anxiety, and cardiovascular disease. Am J Hypertens 2015; 28:1295–1302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Davidson K, Jonas BS, Dixon KE, Markovitz JH. Do depression symptoms predict early hypertension incidence in young adults in the CARDIA study? Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults. Arch Intern Med 2000; 160:1495–1500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Räikkönen K, Matthews KA, Kuller LH. Trajectory of psychological risk and incident hypertension in middle-aged women. Hypertension 2001; 38:798–802. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Jonas BS, Lando JF. Negative affect as a prospective risk factor for hypertension. Psychosom Med 2000; 62:188–196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Taylor HA, Jr, Wilson JG, Jones DW, Sarpong DF, Srinivasan A, Garrison RJ, Nelson C, Wyatt SB. Toward resolution of cardiovascular health disparities in African Americans: design and methods of the Jackson Heart Study. Ethn Dis 2005; 15(4 Suppl 6):S6-4–17. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Fuqua SR, Wyatt SB, Andrew ME, Sarpong DF, Henderson FR, Cunningham MF, Taylor HA., Jr Recruiting African-American research participation in the Jackson Heart Study: methods, response rates, and sample description. Ethn Dis 2005; 15(4 Suppl 6):S6-18–29. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Cook WW, Medley DM. Proposed hostility and pharisaic-virtue scales for the MMPI. J Appl Psychol. 1954; 38:414–418. [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Spielberger CD, Krasner S, Solomon E. The experience, expression, and control of anger. In Janisse MP. (ed), Health Psychology: Individual Differences and Stress. Springer Verlag: New York, 1988, pp. 89–108. [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene R, Vagg PR, Jacobs GA. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press: Palo Alto, CA, 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 27. American Psycological Association. The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) http://www.apa.org/pi/about/publications/caregivers/practice-settings/assessment/tools/trait-state.aspx Accessed 15 November 2015.
  • 28. Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas 1977; 1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Payne TJ, Wyatt SB, Mosley TH, Dubbert PM, Guiterrez-Mohammed ML, Calvin RL, Taylor HA, Jr, Williams DR. Sociocultural methods in the Jackson Heart Study: conceptual and descriptive overview. Ethn Dis 2005; 15(4 Suppl 6):S6-38–48. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. LePore SJ. Measurement of chronic stressors. In Cohen S, Kessler RC, Gordon LU. (eds), Measuring Stress: A Guide for Health and Social Scientists. Oxford University Press: New York, 1995, pp. 102–120. [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav 1983; 24:385–396. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Brantley PJ, Waggoner CD, Jones GN, Rappaport NB. A Daily Stress Inventory: development, reliability, and validity. J Behav Med 1987; 10:61–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Mosley TH, Jr, Payne TJ, Plaud JJ, Johnson CA, Wittrock DA, Seville JL, Penzien DB, Rodriguez G. Psychometric properties of the Weekly Stress Inventory (WSI): extension to a patient sample with coronary heart disease. J Behav Med 1996; 19:273–287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Schulz AJ, Israel BA, Parker EA, Lockett M, Hill Y, Wills R. The East Side Village Health Worker Partnership: integrating research with action to reduce health disparities. Public Health Rep 2001; 116:548–557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Dubbert PM, Carithers T, Ainsworth BE, Taylor HA, Jr, Wilson G, Wyatt SB. Physical activity assessment methods in the Jackson Heart Study. Ethn Dis 2005; 15(4 Suppl 6):S6-56–61. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Carithers T, Dubbert PM, Crook E, Davy B, Wyatt SB, Bogle ML, Taylor HA, Jr, Tucker KL. Dietary assessment in African Americans: methods used in the Jackson Heart Study. Ethn Dis 2005; 15 (4 Suppl 6):S6-49–55. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Mellen PB, Gao SK, Vitolins MZ, Goff DC., Jr Deteriorating dietary habits among adults with hypertension: DASH dietary accordance, NHANES 1988-1994 and 1999-2004. Arch Intern Med 2008; 168:308–314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Spiegelman D, Hertzmark E. Easy SAS calculations for risk or prevalence ratios and differences. Am J Epidemiol 2005; 162:199–200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Tobin MD, Sheehan NA, Scurrah KJ, Burton PR. Adjusting for treatment effects in studies of quantitative traits: antihypertensive therapy and systolic blood pressure. Stat Med 2005; 24:2911–2935. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Fox ER, Musani SK, Singh P, Bidulescu A, Nagarajarao HS, Samdarshi TE, Steffes MW, Wang TJ, Taylor HA, Vasan RS. Association of plasma B-type natriuretic peptide concentrations with longitudinal blood pressure tracking in African Americans: findings from the Jackson Heart Study. Hypertension 2013; 61:48–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Player MS, King DE, Mainous AG, III, Geesey ME. Psychosocial factors and progression from prehypertension to hypertension or coronary heart disease. Ann Fam Med 2007; 5:403–411. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Cohen S, Janicki-Deverts D, Miller GE. Psychological stress and disease. JAMA 2007; 298:1685–1687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Delaney JA, Oddson BE, Kramer H, Shea S, Psaty BM, McClelland RL. Baseline depressive symptoms are not associated with clinically important levels of incident hypertension during two years of follow-up: the multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis. Hypertension 2010; 55:408–414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Data

Articles from American Journal of Hypertension are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES