Abstract
Resting behaviors are an essential component of animal welfare but have received little attention in zoological research. African savanna elephant (Loxodonta africana) and Asian elephant (Elephas maximus) rest includes recumbent postures, but no large-scale investigation of African and Asian zoo elephant recumbence has been previously conducted. We used anklets equipped with accelerometers to measure recumbence in 72 adult female African (n = 44) and Asian (n = 28) elephants housed in 40 North American zoos. We collected 344 days of data and determined associations between recumbence and social, housing, management, and demographic factors. African elephants were recumbent less (2.1 hours/day, S.D. = 1.1) than Asian elephants (3.2 hours/day, S.D. = 1.5; P < 0.001). Nearly one-third of elephants were non-recumbent on at least one night, suggesting this is a common behavior. Multi-variable regression models for each species showed that substrate, space, and social variables had the strongest associations with recumbence. In the African model, elephants who spent any amount of time housed on all-hard substrate were recumbent 0.6 hours less per day than those who were never on all-hard substrate, and elephants who experienced an additional acre of outdoor space at night increased their recumbence by 0.48 hours per day. In the Asian model, elephants who spent any amount of time housed on all-soft substrate were recumbent 1.1 hours more per day more than those who were never on all-soft substrate, and elephants who spent any amount of time housed alone were recumbent 0.77 hours more per day than elephants who were never housed alone. Our results draw attention to the significant interspecific difference in the amount of recumbent rest and in the factors affecting recumbence; however, in both species, the influence of flooring substrate is notably important to recumbent rest, and by extension, zoo elephant welfare.
Introduction
Obtaining adequate rest is essential for the good health and welfare of animals [1–2], yet few studies of zoo animal welfare focus on resting behaviors, perhaps due to the difficulty of measuring and interpreting these behaviors. For example, many species perform rest both while standing and recumbent; of these, the welfare implications of recumbent rest are better understood due to extensive research on cattle. Cattle are highly motivated to lie down [3], and cattle that have been deprived of opportunities for recumbence, feeding, and social contact will prioritize compensatory recumbence over other behaviors [4–5]. Reducing opportunities for cattle to lie down can also affect growth hormone levels [6] and result in various behavioral or physiological indications of stress [7]. In addition, cattle that spend more time standing are at a greater risk for lameness and hoof problems [8].
An important component of resting is sleep. Many species (e.g., cattle, horses, elephants; [2,9]) require recumbence for some types of sleep; in these species a lack of recumbence may lead to sleep deprivation. The health and welfare consequences of sleep deprivation have been well-studied in humans and laboratory animals. In a variety of mammalian species, sleep deprivation causes disruptions in vital biological processes including immune function [10–11], thermoregulation, energy conservation, tissue restoration, and higher cognitive function [12–16].
Like cattle and horses, the resting postures of African savanna elephants (Loxodonta africana) and Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) include both standing rest and recumbent rest. Standing rest often precedes recumbence [17], and recumbent elephants seem to quickly fall asleep: they are immediately motionless with their eyes closed [17], and display heavy, sometimes irregular respiration [18], twitching of the musculature and eyelids [17–18], snoring [19–20], and sometimes loud vocalizations [17]. Elephant sleep has not been described using EEG, so the exact nature of sleep occurring during recumbence is unknown. Regardless, recumbence is a natural resting behavior exhibited by elephants both in the wild [19–24] and in managed care [17–18,25–32].
In order to better understand recumbence in zoo elephants, we sought to quantify and describe recumbence, including its timing, patterns, and prevalence. We also sought to determine the potential associations between a variety of social, housing, management, and demographic factors and recumbence. As part of this process we tested three specific hypotheses. First, we hypothesized that zoo elephants who spent more time on hard substrates would be recumbent less. This intuitive relationship has been seen in cattle, where concrete floors result in significantly less time spent lying down [33]. If zoo elephants on concrete are experiencing a similar reduction in recumbence, they may be experiencing sleep deprivation or sleep disturbance, or be experiencing stress or frustration because of a reluctance to exhibit natural resting postures. Next, we hypothesized that the amount of space experienced would be positively correlated with recumbence. A relationship between space and recumbence is supported by research in cattle and horses, which has shown that recumbence increases when more stall space is provided [34–35]. Finally, we hypothesized that recumbence would decline with age based on previous research showing that adult elephants are recumbent less than infants, juveniles, and sub-adults [17,28,30,32,36].
Our study is the first large-scale investigation of African and Asian zoo elephant recumbence and was a component of the Using Science to Understand Zoo Elephant Welfare project, a multi-institutional collaborative effort to produce scientific data that will support decision making with regard to best practices in elephant management [37].
Methods
Ethics statement
This study was authorized by the management at each participating zoo and, where applicable, was reviewed and approved by zoo research committees. In addition, the study protocol was reviewed and approved by the Zoological Society of San Diego Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee N.I.H. Assurance A3675-01; Protocol 11–203. The study was non-invasive.
Subjects and facilities
Zoos that were accredited members of the Association of Zoos and Aquariums in 2012 were eligible for participation in this study provided that they managed only African savanna or Asian elephants in a non-mixed species herd, and their herd included at least two adult female elephants who were not pregnant or experiencing severe illness or injury. A total of 49 zoos participated in the study. We used simplified random sampling to select two adult females (age ≥ 12 years) as subjects from each zoo; however, 26 zoos only had two eligible subjects so there was no randomization. In one case there were four subjects from one zoo; this zoo housed African and Asian elephants in separate exhibits. Three subjects were removed from the dataset prior to analysis because they were transferred between zoos or died during the 2012 study year.
Data collection
All data were collected between May 2012 and November 2012. We used historical weather data [38] to select a one month data collection period at each location that minimized inter-zoo variation in predicted daily maximum temperature (range: 22.3 C to 34.1 C). We instructed zoos to collect five non-consecutive days of data (24 hours/day) from each subject within a one-month timeframe. Zoos could collect data from both subjects on the same day, or use an alternating schedule.
Leather anklets (Excelsior Leather, California, USA) [39–40] were custom-fit to elephants using measurements provided by participating zoos. The ends of the anklets had D-rings to which shackles and brummel hooks were attached. This hardware was used to secure the anklet in place without causing constriction. A pouch attached to each anklet contained a waterproof case (OtterBox Drybox OTR3-1000S, OtterBox, Colorado, USA) inside of which was a GPS data logger (used to collect data for a related study [40]) and an HOBO Pendant G Data Logger accelerometer (model UA-004-64, Onset Computer Corporation, Massachusetts, USA). Accelerometers are data loggers that can measure g-force and degree of tilt; we chose to evaluate recumbence using g-force measurement following previous studies of cattle recumbence using the same device [41–42]. The accelerometer was placed inside the anklet such that the x-axis was perpendicular to the ground pointing dorsally, and the y- and z-axes were parallel to the ground. We programmed the accelerometers to collect x-axis data at one-minute intervals. The total weight of the unit was approximately 1.2 kg depending on the anklet size and number of shackles used. We shipped the anklets to the zoos and elephant care staff attached the anklets to one of the front legs of each subject.
Data processing
Of the 49 original participating zoos, 40 zoos successfully collected data from 72 elephants. We downloaded the data using HOBOware Pro software (v. 3.2.0, Onset Computer Corporation) and exported it into Microsoft Excel (Microsoft Corporation, Washington, USA). We then followed established data processing methods [42] by adding a constant (3.2) to all g-force values (range: -3.2 to 3.2) to make them positive (range: 0 to 6.4), then coding values < 2.55 as standing and ≥ 2.55 as lying. All lying values indicate accelerometer tilt of ≥ 50°, a cutoff selected based on visual observations of recumbence in cattle [42]. Before the study began we validated these methods for elephants by outfitting two subjects with anklets and videotaping their behaviors over two nights (data in S1 Appendix).
We omitted standing and lying bouts that consisted of only a single reading (e.g., a one-minute interval of “standing” sandwiched between “lying” bouts) because these readings may represent subtle leg movements during a period of consistent orientation [41,43]. We summed all other lying time to calculate recumbence (hours) for each day of data, and we averaged these daily values to calculate mean daily recumbence (hours/day). In addition, we calculated the nighttime (20:00–07:00) mean bout frequency by averaging the number of nightly recumbence bouts for each elephant, then averaging across all elephants. Finally, we calculated the nighttime mean bout duration by averaging the duration of nightly recumbent bouts for each elephant, then averaging across all elephants; however, we excluded nights on which elephants did not lay down to avoid under-estimating bout duration.
Independent variables
Independent variables were selected based on hypotheses regarding their potential association with recumbence. Definitions for the variables selected for testing in this study are described in Table 1. Details on the collection and calculation of independent variables are presented in [40] and [44–47].
Table 1. Definitions of independent variables tested for correlation with mean daily recumbence.
Variable | Unit of Analysis | Unit | Time Scale | Description | Ref |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | Elephant | Age of elephant (years) | [47] | ||
Species | Elephant | African or Asian | [47] | ||
Origin | Elephant | Captive or wild born | [47] | ||
Space Experience | The average weighted (by percent time) size of all environments in which an elephant spent time | [44] | |||
Total | Elephant | 500 ft2 | Overall, Day, Night | For all environment types | [44] |
Indoor | Elephant | 500 ft2 | Overall, Day, Night | For indoor environments only | [44] |
In/Out Choice | Elephant | 500 ft2 | Overall, Day, Night | For environments where there is a choice of indoors or outdoors | [44] |
Outdoor | Elephant | 500 ft2 | Overall, Day, Night | For outdoor environments only | [44] |
Space Experience per Elephant | Elephant | 500 ft2 | Overall, Day, Night | The area of all environments in which an elephant spent time, divided by the number of elephants sharing each environment, weighted by the percent time spent in each environment and averaged. | [44] |
Percent Time | Sum of monthly percent time spent in category, averaged over time period | ||||
Indoor | Elephant | % | Overall, Day, Night | Time spent in indoor environments | [44] |
In/Out Choice | Elephant | % | Overall, Day, Night | Time spent in environments with an indoor/outdoor choice | [44] |
Outdoor | Elephant | % | Overall, Day, Night | Time spent in outdoor environments | [44] |
Soft Substrate | Elephant | % | Overall, Day, Night | Time spent in environment with 100% grass, sand, or rubber substrate | [44] |
Hard Substrate | Elephant | % | Overall, Day, Night | Time spent in environment with 100% concrete or stone aggregate substrate | [44] |
Housed Separately | Elephant | % | Overall, Day, Night | Time spent housed in a social group of one | [44] |
Juveniles (<7 years old) | Elephant | % | Overall, Day, Night | Time spent in social groups where an elephant 7 years or younger was present | [44] |
Social Experience | Elephant | Overall, Day, Night | The average weighted (by percent time) size of all social groups in which an elephant spent time | [44] | |
Animal Contact | Elephant | Overall, Day, Night | Max number of unique elephants focal animal is in contact with | [44] | |
Mean Daily Walking Distance | Elephant | Distance walked per day while outdoors, averaged over all days of data collection | [40] | ||
Herd Size | Zoo | Total number of elephants at zoo | [44] | ||
Temperature | Zoo | Average daily temperature at zoo, averaged over all days of data collection | [38] | ||
Enrichment Program | Zoo | Standardized factor score created using a polychoric PCA to examine the frequency of use of the different components of an enrichment program | [45] | ||
Enrichment Diversity | Zoo | Shannon diversity index score of enrichment activities types and frequencies conducted at zoo | [45] | ||
Exercise Diversity | Zoo | Shannon diversity index score of exercise types and frequencies conducted at zoo | [45] | ||
Foot Health | Zoo | Score of 0–12 indicating abnormalities on nails, pads, and interdigital space on any foot | [46] |
A novel variable called Space Experience warrants further attention. Space Experience was based on data from detailed facility surveys [44] and was calculated by first taking the size of each environment in which an elephant spent time, and then multiplying it by the percentage of time the elephant spent in that environment. These weighted environment sizes were then averaged to calculate a representative value for each elephant [for additional details, see [44]]. This allows us to account for the complex housing conditions of zoo elephants, in which they may be shifted between environments of different sizes for varying amounts of time, including at night.
We also created novel environment type and flooring substrate variables. We first defined each space in which elephants spent time as indoors, outdoors or mixed based on detailed facility surveys [44]. Mixed environments were areas where elephants had a choice to move freely between indoor and outdoor spaces. We then defined multiple classes of flooring substrate: grass, sand, rubber padding, stone aggregate, concrete and categorized the types of substrates into hard surface (concrete and stone aggregate) and soft surface (grass, sand, and rubber padding), and determined the percent coverage for each substrate type for each environment. We wanted to calculate the time that elephants spent in contact with each substrate type so to confirm this we determined which environments were comprised of 100% hard and 100% soft substrate and calculated the percent time each elephant spent in environments that met these criteria from detailed housing time budgets [44].
We checked all continuous independent variables for outliers and removed any values that were greater than three standard deviations away from the mean. We adjusted some variables from continuous to binary because of zero-values for a high number of subjects. Adjusted variables included two space variables (Space Experience In/Out Choice and Percent Time In/Out Choice), two flooring variables (Percent Time Hard Substrate and Percent Time Soft Substrate) and two social variables (Percent Time Housed Separately and Percent Time Juveniles). The Space Experience variables were adjusted to a value of “per 500 ft2” to aid in the interpretation of Beta values.
Statistical analysis
To determine whether there were interspecific differences in patterns of recumbence, we used two-sample Student’s t-tests assuming equal variances to test three null hypotheses: that both species had the same (1) mean daily recumbence; (2) mean number of nighttime recumbence bouts; and (3) mean duration of nighttime recumbence bouts. Upon discovering that many elephants were highly non-recumbent, or intermittently non-recumbent, we used Fisher's Exact Test to determine if the proportion of elephants displaying non-recumbence differed by species.
The regression models were fitted using generalized estimating equations (GEE), which allow for the individual elephant to be used as the unit of analysis, account for clustering of individuals within zoos, and support repeated measurement [48–49]. Zoos were treated as random effects and an independent correlation structure was specified [50]. Multi-variable regression models were built by first assessing individual predictors at the univariate level. During this univariate analysis we found a significant correlation between species and mean daily recumbence (β = 1.073, df = 71, P < 0.001), thus, we created separate models for African elephants and Asian elephants. We assessed individual predictors at the univariate level and then at the bivariate level with demographic variables (age and origin). Based on the fact that age and origin were likely to have an effect on both outcome and the tested input variable, these variables were tested as potential confounding variables [51–52]. Confounding variables (those that altered the beta values of input variables by more than 10% during bivariate analysis) were included in all models, and any variables correlated with recumbence (P < 0.15) following the univariate and bivariate assessments were retained for evaluation in the hierarchical model building process. The hierarchical selection was based on quasi-likelihood under the independence model criterion (QIC) values and parameter estimates of explanatory variables.
Models exhibiting multi-collinearity, as defined by a variance inflation factor (VIF) of greater than 10 and a Condition Index (CI) of greater than 30, were not considered for further analysis. The African elephant model used an autoregressive correlation matrix type, while the Asian elephant model specified an independent correlation structure. Statistical analyses were conducted by using SAS software, version 9.3 [PROC GENMOD, with options REPEATED, CORR = IND or AR, and DIST = NORMAL; SAS Institute, Inc., Cary, NC].
Results
Summary of recumbence data
Our final dataset included 344 days of data, collected between May 7, 2012 and November 1, 2012, from 72 elephants at 40 zoos. A full 24 hours of data were collected on 277 days; on 67 days anklets were removed before a full 24 hours of data were collected, resulting in an average of 31 minutes (range: 2 to 105) when recumbence data were not available. Any recumbence occurring during these times was not recorded, thus, the mean daily recumbence values may slightly underestimate actual values. For the majority of elephants (61/72) five days of data were collected, but in some cases the data were limited to four days (6/72) or three days (5/72). The 72 elephants included 44 African elephants (61.1%) and 28 Asian elephants (39.9%) (Fig 1). The mean age of African elephants was 32.6 years (range: 20 to 52); the mean age of Asian elephants was 40.0 years (range: 16 to 61).
Mean daily recumbence for all elephants was an average of 2.6 hours/day; the individual mean recumbence value and standard error for all subjects is available in S2 Appendix. African elephants had significantly lower recumbence (2.1 hours/day) than Asian elephants (3.2 hours/day) (t(70) = -3.48, P < 0.001, two-tailed) (Table 2). Mean nighttime bout frequencies in African and Asian elephants were not significantly different at 3.1 bouts/night for both species (t(70) = 0.05, P = 0.96, two-tailed) (Table 2). Africans and Asians were different in mean nighttime bout duration (t(70) = -4.95, P < 0.001) with African elephants recumbent an average of 39 minutes/bout and Asian elephants recumbent an average of 66 minutes/bout (Table 2). Variations in the standing and lying patterns of African and Asian elephants can be seen by comparing the behavior of representative individuals (Fig 2). African and Asian elephants showed similar mean daily recumbence profiles: recumbence rarely occurred during the day, started to increase at 20:00, and reached a peak at 04:00 before sharply dropping off (Fig 3).
Table 2. Summary of recumbence data for African and Asian zoo elephants.
Combined (n = 72) | African (n = 44) | Asian (n = 28) | P | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean Daily Recumbence (Hours/Day) | Mean | 2.6 | 2.1 | 3.2 | <0.01 | * |
Min. | 0.0 | 0.0 | 0.0 | |||
Max. | 7.9 | 3.7 | 7.9 | |||
S.D. | 1.4 | 1.1 | 1.5 | |||
Mean Nighttime Bout Frequency (Bouts/Night) | Mean | 3.1 | 3.1 | 3.1 | 0.96 | |
Min. | 0.0 | 0.0 | 0.0 | |||
Max. | 6.3 | 5.8 | 6.3 | |||
S.D. | 1.4 | 1.5 | 1.4 | |||
Mean Nighttime Bout Duration (Hours/Bout) | Mean | 0.8 | 0.7 | 1.1 | <0.001 | * |
Min. | 0.0 | 0.0 | 0.0 | |||
Max. | 2.0 | 1.4 | 2.0 | |||
S.D. | 0.4 | 0.3 | 0.4 |
A number of elephants showed some form of non-recumbence. Seven elephants (six African, one Asian) were classified as highly non-recumbent because they lay down for less than one hour total over five days of data collection. An additional 15 elephants (nine African, six Asian) were classified as intermittently non-recumbent because they lay down for less than ten minutes per day on three days of data collection (n = 1), two days (n = 1), or one day (n = 13). There was no significant difference in the number of highly non-recumbent elephants by species (Fisher's Exact Test, P = 0.252) or the number of intermittently non-recumbent elephants by species (Fisher's Exact Test, P = 1.00).
Univariate analyses
We evaluated a variety of demographic, housing, social, and management factors for association with recumbence (Tables 3 and 4). In the African elephant univariate tests (Table 3), mean daily recumbence was negatively correlated with Age and Percent Time Hard Substrate Overall, Day, and Night. Recumbence was also negatively correlated with Social Experience Day, and Percent Time Housed Separately Overall and at Night. There was a positive correlation between recumbence and the variables Space Experience Total Night, Space Experience Outdoor Night, and Space Experience per Elephant Night. We were unable to test for a correlation between recumbence and Origin due to the small number of African elephants who had been imported from a range country (1/44).
Table 3. Univariate correlations between mean daily recumbence and independent variables in African zoo elephants.
Overall | Day a | Nighta | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Variables | +/- b | Reference | n | Beta | P-value | n | Beta | P-value | n | Beta | P-value |
Demographics | |||||||||||
Age | - | 44 | -0.087 | <0.001* | |||||||
Space | |||||||||||
Space Experience Total (500 ft2) | + | 42 | 0.003 | 0.363 | 44 | -0.000 | 0.925 | 42 | 0.007 | <0.001* | |
Space Experience Indoor (500 ft2) | + | 43 | -0.069 | 0.259 | 44 | -0.046 | 0.414 | 43 | -0.063 | 0.321 | |
Space Experience Outdoor (500 ft2) | + | 43 | 0.001 | 0.830 | 44 | -0.001 | 0.663 | 42 | 0.007 | 0.002* | |
Space Experience In/Out Choice (500 ft2) | ref = 0% | 18 | 22 | 18 | |||||||
+ | >0% | 26 | 0.137 | 0.681 | 22 | -0.945 | 0.749 | 26 | 0.090 | 0.782 | |
Space Experience per Elephant | + | 44 | 0.005 | 0.379 | 44 | -0.003 | 0.631 | 42 | 0.028 | 0.013* | |
Percent Time Indoor | - | 44 | -0.006 | 0.456 | 44 | 0.001 | 0.944 | 44 | -0.007 | 0.191 | |
Percent Time Outdoor | + | 44 | 0.000 | 0.968 | 44 | -0.002 | 0.717 | 44 | 0.002 | 0.750 | |
Percent Time In/Out Choice | ref = 0% | 18 | 22 | 18 | |||||||
+ | >0% | 26 | 0.137 | 0.681 | 22 | -0.150 | 0.622 | 26 | 0.137 | 0.681 | |
Flooring | |||||||||||
Percent Time Hard Substrate | ref = 0% | 20 | 24 | 21 | |||||||
- | >0% | 24 | -0.673 | 0.018* | 20 | -0.647 | 0.029* | 23 | -0.749 | 0.007* | |
Percent Time Soft Substrate | ref = 0% | 22 | 26 | 25 | |||||||
+ | >0% | 22 | 0.040 | 0.901 | 18 | 0.057 | 0.860 | 19 | 0.146 | 0.667 | |
Social | |||||||||||
Herd Size | + | 44 | 0.022 | 0.711 | |||||||
Animal Contact | + | 42 | -0.136 | 0.192 | 42 | -0.136 | 0.192 | 42 | -0.055 | 0.767 | |
Social Experience | + | 42 | -0.297 | 0.221 | 42 | -0.192 | 0.146^ | 42 | 0.009 | 0.974 | |
Percent Time Juveniles (age <7) | ref = 0% | 33 | 33 | 34 | |||||||
+ | >0% | 11 | 0.017 | 0.963 | 11 | 0.017 | 0.963 | 10 | -0.094 | 0.823 | |
Percent Time Housed Separately | ref = 0% | 28 | 33 | 28 | |||||||
- | >0% | 15 | -0.601 | 0.098^ | 11 | -0.217 | 0.580 | 16 | -0.704 | 0.054^ | |
Management | |||||||||||
Enrichment Diversity | + | 42 | -0.833 | 0.269 | |||||||
Enrichment Program | + | 42 | -0.084 | 0.652 | |||||||
Exercise Diversity | + | 42 | 0.103 | 0.740 | |||||||
Other | |||||||||||
Foot Health | - | 39 | -0.046 | 0.614 | |||||||
Temperature | + | 44 | -0.021 | 0.268 | |||||||
Walking Distance | + | 32 | 0.023 | 0.829 |
^P value <0.15 utilized as threshold significant level for model building
*P value <0.05
a Day and night are defined as the number of hours in a 24 hour period considered daytime or nighttime according to management schedule.
b Hypothesized direction of relationship between mean daily recumbence and variable.
Table 4. Univariate correlations between mean daily recumbence and independent variables in Asian zoo elephants.
Overall | Day a | Night a | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Variables | +/- b | Reference | n | Beta | P-value | n | Beta | P-value | n | Beta | P-value |
Demographics | |||||||||||
Age | - | 28 | 0.016 | 0.236 | |||||||
Origin | refc = Wild | 21 | |||||||||
+ | Captive | 7 | -0.291 | 0.576 | |||||||
Space | |||||||||||
Space Experience Total (500 ft2) | + | 27 | 0.008 | 0.462 | 28 | -0.006 | 0.494 | 27 | 0.009 | 0.465 | |
Space Experience Indoor (500 ft2) | + | 27 | -0.081 | 0.553 | 28 | -0.136 | 0.028* | 28 | -0.103 | 0.267 | |
Space Experience Outdoor (500 ft2) | + | 27 | 0.003 | 0.647 | 27 | 0.002 | 0.718 | 27 | 0.001 | 0.910 | |
Space Experience In/Out Choice (500 ft2) | ref = 0% | 11 | 19 | 13 | |||||||
+ | >0% | 17 | 0.399 | 0.511 | 9 | 0.211 | 0.633 | 15 | 0.213 | 0.695 | |
Space Experience per Elephant | + | 28 | 0.038 | 0.314 | 28 | 0.021 | 0.435 | 28 | 0.026 | 0.403 | |
Percent Time Indoor | - | 27 | -0.012 | 0.432 | 27 | -0.029 | 0.038* | 28 | -0.011 | 0.261 | |
Percent Time Outdoor | + | 28 | 0.008 | 0.397 | 28 | 0.004 | 0.647 | 28 | 0.006 | 0.444 | |
Percent Time In/Out Choice | ref = 0% | 11 | 19 | 13 | |||||||
+ | >0% | 17 | 0.399 | 0.511 | 9 | 0.211 | 0.633 | 15 | 0.213 | 0.695 | |
Flooring | |||||||||||
Percent Time Hard Substrate | ref = 0% | 10 | 11 | 13 | |||||||
- | >0% | 17 | -0.049 | 0.919 | 16 | 0.151 | 0.770 | 15 | -0.815 | 0.079^ | |
Percent Time Soft Substrate | ref = 0% | 11 | 13 | 16 | |||||||
+ | >0% | 17 | 0.971 | 0.029* | 15 | 0.858 | 0.064^ | 12 | 0.793 | 0.123^ | |
Social | |||||||||||
Herd Size | + | 26 | -0.223 | 0.447 | |||||||
Animal Contact | + | 27 | -0.342 | 0.323 | 27 | -0.342 | 0.323 | 27 | -0.685 | 0.099^ | |
Social Experience | + | 28 | -0.760 | 0.130^ | 28 | -0.659 | 0.183 | 28 | -0.574 | 0.212 | |
Percent Time Juveniles (age <7) | ref = 0% | 24 | 24 | 24 | |||||||
+ | >0% | 4 | -0.348 | 0.614 | 3 | -0.761 | 0.150 | 3 | 0.136 | 0.775 | |
Percent Time Housed Separately | ref = 0% | 9 | 15 | 14 | |||||||
- | >0% | 19 | 1.051 | 0.029* | 13 | 0.998 | 0.050^ | 14 | 0.936 | 0.045* | |
Management | |||||||||||
Enrichment Diversity | + | 23 | 0.585 | 0.730 | |||||||
Enrichment Program | + | 23 | -0.052 | 0.751 | |||||||
Exercise Diversity | + | 22 | -0.220 | 0.773 | |||||||
Other | |||||||||||
Foot Health | - | 24 | 0.050 | 0.634 | |||||||
Temperature | + | 28 | 0.026 | 0.554 | |||||||
Walking Distance | + | 23 | -0.079 | 0.418 |
^P value <0.15 utilized as threshold significant level for model building
*P value <0.05
a Day and night are defined as the number of hours in a 24 hour period considered daytime or nighttime according to management schedule.
b Hypothesized direction of relationship between mean daily recumbence and variable.
c The reference value (ref =) was the baseline value used when calculating univariate correlations with these binary variables.
In the Asian elephant univariate tests (Table 4), mean daily recumbence was negatively correlated with Space Experience Indoor Day and Percent Time Indoor Day, as well as Percent Time Hard Substrate Night, Animal Contact Night, and Social Experience Overall. Recumbence was positively correlated with Percent Time Soft Substrate Overall, Day, and Night, and Percent Time Housed Separately Overall, Day, and Night.
The population level descriptive statistics for the variables that were significant in African and Asian elephant univariate analyses are shown in Table 5.
Table 5. Descriptive statistics for independent variables included in the multi-variable modeling process.
Variable | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Species | Variable | Mgmt. | Reference | n | Mean Age | Mean | SD | Min | Max | Median |
African | Age | 44 | - | 32.6 | 6.6 | 20 | 52 | 32 | ||
African | Space Experience Total (500 ft2) | Night | 42 | 33 | 44.0 | 51.1 | 0. 9 | 227.4 | 27.5 | |
African | Space Experience Outdoor (500 ft2) | Night | 43 | 33 | 57.3 | 59.7 | 0 | 244.0 | 37. 9 | |
African | Space Experience per Elephant (500 ft2) | Night | 42 | 33 | 14.2 | 11.5 | 0.8 | 45.0 | 5.9 | |
African | Percent Time Hard Substrate | Overall | ref = 0% | 20 | 31 | - | - | - | - | - |
>0% | 24 | 35 | 15.8 | 8.5 | 2.3 | 32.2 | 15.0 | |||
African | Percent Time Hard Substrate | Day | ref = 0% | 24 | 31 | - | - | - | - | - |
>0% | 20 | 35 | 10.5 | 7.8 | 1.1 | 24.2 | 11.3 | |||
African | Percent Time Hard Substrate | Night | ref = 0% | 21 | 31 | - | - | - | - | - |
>0% | 23 | 34 | 25.5 | 16.7 | 5.6 | 53.3 | 23.5 | |||
African | Social Experience | Day | 42 | 34 | 19.9 | 13.7 | 6.8 | 56.3 | 14.7 | |
African | Percent Time Housed Separately | Overall | ref = 0% | 28 | 31 | - | - | - | - | - |
>0% | 15 | 35 | 22.9 | 17.7 | 1.4 | 57.1 | 19.9 | |||
African | Percent Time Housed Separately | Night | ref = 0% | 28 | 31 | - | - | - | - | - |
>0% | 16 | 35 | 44.5 | 34.1 | 3.8 | 100. 0 | 37.6 | |||
Asian | Space Experience Indoor (500 ft2) | Day | 28 | 40 | 1.7 | 2.2 | 0 | 8.0 | 1.1 | |
Asian | Percent Time Indoor | Day | 27 | 40 | 12.3 | 11.8 | 0 | 36.3 | 10 | |
Asian | Percent Time Hard Substrate | Night | ref = 0% | 13 | 39 | - | - | - | - | - |
>0% | 15 | 41 | 20.2 | 19.3 | 2.2 | 53.4 | 8.6 | |||
Asian | Percent Time Soft Substrate | Overall | ref = 0% | 11 | 44 | - | - | - | - | - |
>0% | 17 | 37 | 15.8 | 11.1 | 0.4 | 31.4 | 18.4 | |||
Asian | Percent Time Soft Substrate | Day | ref = 0% | 13 | 46 | - | - | - | - | - |
>0% | 15 | 35 | 10.4 | 9.5 | 0.4 | 29.2 | 8.6 | |||
Asian | Percent Time Soft Substrate | Night | ref = 0% | 16 | 42 | - | - | - | - | - |
>0% | 12 | 37 | 27.0 | 9.3 | 14.7 | 48.5 | 26.3 | |||
Asian | Animal Contact | Night | 27 | 41 | 1.0 | 0.7 | 0 | 3.0 | 1. 0 | |
Asian | Social Experience | Overall | 28 | 40 | 18.9 | 10.3 | 0.7 | 45 | 15.7 | |
Asian | Percent Time Housed Separately | Overall | ref = 0% | 9 | 41 | - | - | - | - | - |
>0% | 19 | 40 | 38.7 | 38.2 | 1.8 | 100 | 25 | |||
Asian | Percent Time Housed Separately | Day | ref = 0% | 15 | 42 | - | - | - | - | - |
>0% | 13 | 37 | 39.2 | 39.8 | 5.3 | 100 | 19.5 | |||
Asian | Percent Time Housed Separately | Night | ref = 0% | 14 | 35 | - | - | - | - | - |
>0% | 14 | 45 | 68.5 | 37.8 | 8.1 | 100 | 86.1 |
African elephant multi-variable model
The African elephant multi-variable model (Table 6) includes Beta estimates for Percent Time Hard Substrate Overall and Space Experience Outdoor Night. Beta estimates reflect the magnitude of the effect of the independent variables on recumbence as described below, but it is important to note that this effect is conditional on the effects of the other independent variables in each model. The model demonstrates an association between flooring and recumbence such that elephants who spent any time on 100% concrete or stone aggregate substrate (“all-hard”) were recumbent 0.6 hours less per day than elephants who spent no time on all-hard substrate. The model also demonstrates a positive relationship between the amounts of outdoor space an elephant experiences at night and recumbence: elephants who experienced an additional 500 ft2 of outdoor space during the night were recumbent 0.006 hours more per day; this translates to a 0.5 hour increase per additional acre.
Table 6. African elephant mean daily recumbence multi-variable model (*P < 0.05)1.
Variable | Beta Estimate | Standard Error | Pr > |Z| |
---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 2.194 | 0.217 | <0.001 |
0% time hard substrate | - | - | - |
>0% time hard substrate | -0.600 | 0.245 | 0.014* |
Space experience outdoor night | 0.006 | 0.002 | 0.001* |
1 Variance Inflation Factor = 1.051; Maximum Condition Index = 3.755
Asian elephant multi-variable model
The Asian elephant multi-variable model (Table 7) includes Beta estimates for Percent Time Soft Substrate Overall and Percent Time Housed Separately Night. The model shows that elephants who spent any time on 100% grass, sand, or rubber substrate (“all-soft”) were recumbent 1.1 hours more per day than elephants who spent no time on all-soft substrate. The variable Age is included as a confounder of Percent Time Soft Substrate Overall indicating that age is a factor that is both related to being on hard surfaces and being recumbent; age is included in the model to control for its potential effects. The model also demonstrates a positive relationship between whether an elephant is housed alone at night and recumbence, such that elephants who were alone for any amount of time during the night were recumbent 0.77 hours more per day than elephants who were never housed alone during the night.
Table 7. Asian elephant mean daily recumbence multi-variable model (*P < 0.05)1.
Variable | Beta Estimate | Standard Error | Pr > |Z| |
---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 1.646 | 0.733 | 0.025 |
0% time soft substrate | - | - | - |
>0% time soft substrate | 1.056 | 0.352 | 0.003* |
0% time housed separately | - | - | - |
>0% time housed separately | 0.770 | 0.387 | 0.047* |
Age | 0.016 | 0.107 | 0.358 |
1 Variance Inflation Factor = 1.005; Maximum Condition Index = 4.112
Discussion
Recumbence patterns, timing and prevalence
We found that on average, African elephants lay down for just over two hours per day and Asian elephants lay down for just over three hours per day. Recumbence occurred almost exclusively at night. Our results correspond with other large studies of recumbence in elephants under managed care: adult female African elephants (n = 11) in European zoos lay down an average of 2.0 hours per night [32], while adult female Asian elephants (n = 8) at a zoo and circus lay down an average of 3.4 hours per night [17]. Our results also correspond with available data from wild African elephants (n = 4) that lay down for between one and two hours per night on average [19]; no data are available for nighttime recumbence in wild Asian elephants.
Sleep appears to be the primary function of recumbence, as elephants entering a recumbent posture appear to fall asleep almost immediately [17]. Thus, differences in recumbence between African and Asian elephants likely reflect interspecific variation in sleep requirements. Sleep patterns appear to be determined primarily by ecological variables [53]. For example, some species show a trade-off between time available for sleep and time available for foraging [54]. Wild Asian elephants may inhabit more resource-rich areas than wild African elephants, allowing them to fulfill their nutritional requirements in less time. Asian zoo elephants spend significantly less time feeding than African zoo elephants [45], but whether this is attributable to the species’ natural history or a difference in zoo feeding methods is not known. Another ecological variable that may explain interspecific variation in sleep is predation risk. Species that sleep more tend to use less exposed sleeping sites [54–55]. Asian elephants may be more likely than African (savanna) elephants to inhabit dense forested areas that conceal them from predators and allow for more sleep. Regardless of the cause of the difference in recumbence, our results suggest that animal welfare indices based on behavior should take into account the potential for significant differences between elephant species.
African and Asian elephants showed similar timing of recumbence behavior, being mainly recumbent between 01:00 and 05:00 with a peak at 04:00, in agreement with other studies [17, 19]. The timing of recumbence may have management implications. For example, zoos that have nighttime elephant care staff or automated feeders should plan management routines so as to minimize disturbances to sleeping elephants during peak recumbence hours; indeed, automated feeders have been shown to interrupt recumbence in zoo elephants [32].
Non-recumbence
We observed some form of non-recumbence in nearly one-third of elephants in our study. Age-related health problems (e.g., arthritis) could be limiting recumbence in some individuals [29], and although a related study [46] provided musculoskeletal health data for some of our subjects we were unable to test for an association with recumbence due to limited variability in joint health in the subjects. We also observed the behavior across a range of ages, so health problems are unlikely to be the sole cause of non-recumbence. We considered that non-recumbence may be a normal and adaptive behavior in elephants. For example, animals living in groups may use vigilance to increase the probability of predator detection [56]. Although wild African elephants are rarely vigilant during the day [54], vigilance may be more important at night when the majority of predation attempts on elephants occur [57–58]. Non-recumbence may also be a specific form of vigilance called sentinel behavior. Zoo elephants have been observed standing in close proximity to a recumbent elephant for extended periods of time [30] and “taking turns” being recumbent [25]. However, no studies of zoo elephants have closely examined vigilance or sentinel behavior. Finally, non-recumbence may be an abnormal behavioral consequence of the zoo environment, with no physiological or ecological function, and may be caused by stress, disturbance, or some other unmeasured variable. In this case, the welfare of non-recumbent elephants may be impacted by sleep deprivation. Whether zoo elephants are able to make up for lost sleep the night after exhibiting non-recumbence could not be tested with our dataset because we collected data on non-consecutive nights, however, rebound recumbence is an area of possible future research.
Substrate
Of all the independent variables we tested, substrate had the strongest association with recumbence, and a substrate variable was present in nearly every model during the model-building process. Our African model showed that elephants who spent time on all-hard substrate (concrete or stone aggregate) were recumbent 0.6 hours per day less than elephants who were never on all-hard substrate. Along the same lines, the Asian model showed an increase in recumbence of 1.1 hours per day for elephants who spent time on all-soft substrate (grass, sand, or rubber) when compared to elephants who were never on all-soft substrate.
Our results add to a growing body of evidence suggesting that hard substrate negatively impacts animal welfare. Concrete has been associated with higher rates of sole hemorrhages [59] and swollen knees [60] in cattle, and with incidents of foot and joint disease in zoo elephants [46]. Meanwhile, the reduction and removal of hard substrate from zoo elephant exhibits is already underway. A 2006 survey [61] following up on 1997 survey results [62] found that the proportion of concrete flooring in elephant barns had reduced 22% in the intervening years. In addition, nearly half of responding zoos planned to further reduce the proportion of concrete flooring in their indoor facilities over the next 10 years [61]. Despite these ongoing efforts, we found that 18 of 40 zoos in our study (45%) had elephants housed in environments with all-hard substrate at some time in 2012. This is in addition to the time these elephants spent in mixed substrate environments that included hard and soft substrate. The continued prevalence of hard substrate in zoo environments indicates that zoos must remain proactive in their attempts to incorporate soft substrate into both indoor and outdoor areas. Furthermore, we suggest continued research into soft substrate types (i.e., sand, grass, and rubber) in order to determine which are most effective at promoting health, welfare, and natural behaviors in zoo elephants. For example, research in horses has shown that despite straw and wood shavings both being arguably soft substrate, horses that are given a choice between the two preferentially spend time on straw [63] and exhibit more bedding-related activities [63] and lateral recumbence [64] on straw. Finally, our model suggests that substrate directly affects zoo elephant recumbence; that is, it is not a proxy for other related measurements such as time spent inside or outside.
Space
We found a positive correlation between outdoor Space Experience at night and recumbence in African elephants. For example, an African elephant that experienced an additional acre of outdoor space at night increased their recumbence by 0.48 hours in the final model–a potentially important contribution to the mean daily recumbence of an African elephant.
There are a variety of ways by which zoos can increase outdoor Space Experience at night. Providing access to a consistent amount of additional space is certainly one way. However, Space Experience allows for a flexible consideration of both space and time, so zoos can work within their own housing and management constraints to find ways to increase Space Experience by altering the amount of time elephants are housed in different exhibit configurations (for more details see [44]). Whether increases in Space Experience will eventually begin to reach a level of diminishing returns is an important area for future research.
Age
We found a negative correlation between age and recumbence in our African (but not Asian) elephant model: as adult African elephants got older, they were recumbent less and less. Whether this trend is related to age-related health problems or merely reflects changes in sleep requirements (or both), we cannot say as research on senescence and sleep has not been conducted with elephants. However, many age-related changes occur in rodent sleep patterns, including a reduction in total sleep time and reduction in the duration of uninterrupted bouts of sleep (i.e. sleep fragmentation) with increased age [65], so future research into age-related sleep changes in elephants is warranted.
Social
The initiation and termination of recumbence bouts is often synchronized amongst elephants in zoos [17, 26] and in the wild [19]. This suggests that recumbence is a highly social behavior. Although not one of our three focal hypotheses, we predicted that elephants who were never alone would show more recumbence, assuming that a more natural social environment would be more likely to result in the expression of natural behaviors. However, the Asian elephant model showed a positive correlation between recumbence and time housed separately. Why might Asian elephants who spend time alone be more recumbent? One possible explanation is that elephants housed alone do not experience overcrowding. Cattle, for example, were significantly less recumbent when the number of cows per stall increased by 50% [66]. However, we found no correlation between Asian elephant recumbence and social density, as measured by our Space Experience per Elephant variable. An alternate explanation is that being housed alone eliminates the possibility of physical disruption of rest by other members of a social group. In cattle, recumbence patters are related to social rank, and subordinate cattle are recumbent significantly less than middle-ranked or high-ranked cows [67], presumably because dominant cows physically disturb lower-ranking cows during resting periods. Future research will be needed to better understand the nocturnal social lives of elephants and their effect on rest and recumbence.
Our study used a standardized methodology to complete a large-scale investigation of African and Asian zoo elephant recumbence. This rare look into the resting behavior of zoo elephants resulted in a few notable conclusions. First, our finding that Asian elephants have significantly higher mean daily recumbence than African elephants suggests that animal welfare researchers should remain vigilant of possible species differences in zoo elephants when using behavioral indices as a tool to measure welfare. We also observed that nearly one-third of the elephants were non-recumbent on at least one night; this could indicate an important animal welfare concern and as such more research should be directed at determining the causes and effects of being non-recumbent. Finally, we established an association between recumbence and substrate, which supports continued efforts by zoos to replace hard substrate with soft substrate in order to provide zoo elephants with an environment where they can comfortably express recumbence behavior.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
This project was part of a large scale collaboration titled “Using Science to Understand Zoo Elephant Welfare”. The authors would like to acknowledge the significant efforts of the full project team: Christy Alligood, Anne Baker, Jeff Bolling, Mary Bonaparte-Saller, Janine Brown, Kathy Carlstead, Candice Dorsey, Brian Greco, Greg Guagnano, Mike Keele, Katherine Leighty, John Lehnhardt, Georgia Mason, Jill Mellen, Joy Mench, Michele Miller, Kari Morfeld, Steve Paris, Harry Peachey, Josh Plotnik, Beth Posta, Natalia Prado-Oviedo, Daniel Sneed, Nadja Wielebnowski, and James Witte.
In addition, special thanks to the AZA Elephant TAG and TAG Chair Martha Fischer for logistical support, Jackie Ogden for communications support, and Vistalogic, Inc. for technological support and software services.
We thank Debbie Ethell, research associate at the Oregon Zoo, for assistance with data management and processing, and Jean-Philippe Parent, Ph.D. candidate at Université de Montréal, for providing advice and expertise in accelerometer use.
Finally, sincere thanks to the people and elephants at each of the following zoos for incredible participation and support of the project:
Africam Safari, Albuquerque Biological Park, Audubon Institute, Birmingham Zoo, BREC's Baton Rouge Zoo, Buffalo Zoological Gardens, Busch Gardens, Buttonwood Park Zoo, Caldwell Zoo, Calgary Zoo, Cameron Park Zoo, Cheyenne Mountain Zoological Park, Cincinnati Zoo & Botanical Garden, Cleveland Metroparks Zoo, Columbus Zoo, Dallas Zoo, Denver Zoo, Dickerson Park Zoo, Disney's Animal Kingdom, El Paso Zoo, Fresno Chaffee Zoo, Greenville Zoo, Honolulu Zoo, Houston Zoological Gardens, Indianapolis Zoological Society, Inc., Jacksonville Zoological Gardens, Knoxville Zoological Gardens, Lee Richardson Zoo, Little Rock Zoological Garden, Los Angeles Zoo and Botanical Gardens, Louisville Zoological Garden, Lowry Park Zoological Garden, Maryland Zoo, Memphis Zoological Garden and Aquarium, Metropolitan Toronto Zoo, Milwaukee County Zoological Gardens, Montgomery Zoo, Nashville Zoo, National Zoo, Niabi Zoo, North Carolina Zoological Park, Oakland Zoo, Oklahoma City Zoological Park, Oregon Zoo, Parque Zoologico de Leon, Phoenix Zoo, Point Defiance Zoo and Aquarium, Reid Park Zoo, Riverbanks Zoological Park, Roger Williams Park Zoo, Rosamond Gifford Zoo at Burnet Park, San Antonio Zoological Gardens & Aquarium, San Diego Safari Park, San Diego Zoo, Santa Barbara Zoological Gardens, Sedgwick County Zoo, Seneca Park Zoo, Saint Louis Zoo, The Kansas City Zoo, Topeka Zoological Park, Tulsa Zoological Park, Utah's Hogle Zoo, Virginia Zoological Park, Wildlife Conservation Society—Bronx Zoo, Wildlife Safari, Woodland Park Zoo, Zoo Atlanta, Zoo de Granby, Zoo Miami.
Data Availability
For reasons relating to protection of the facilities and animals included in this study, access restrictions apply to the individual-level data underlying the findings. A data set of de-identified, population-level data is available at doi: 10.6084/m9.figshare.3383554.
Funding Statement
Funding for this work was provided by a National Leadership Grant to the Honolulu Zoological Society from the Institute of Museum and Library Services (www.imls.gov) grant number: LG-25-10-0033-10. This paper was completed while MRH was undertaking a Ph.D. in Biology at Portland State University and in receipt of funding from the Forbes-Lea Research Grant, Marie Brown Award, and Student Education Travel Grant. This study was also funded in part by a research grant from the Pittsburgh Zoo. Employers provided financial support in the form of authors' salaries as follows: Oregon Zoo (DS and MH); Disney’s Animal Kingdom (JS); Chicago Zoological Society-Brookfield Zoo (LM); Busch Gardens (JA). After the IMLS-funded period of performance (November 2010 – December 2013) AWARE Institute provided support in the form of salaries for author CM and JH. The specific roles of these authors are articulated in the ‘author contributions’ section. Neither the funders nor authors’ employers had any role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Botreau R, Veissier I, Butterworth A, Bracke MBM, Keeling LJ (2007) Definition of criteria for overall assessment of animal welfare. Anim Welf 16: 225–228. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Broom DM, Fraser AF (2007) Domestic Animal Behaviour and Welfare (4th edition). Wallingford: CAB International. 437 p. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Jensen MB, Pedersen LJ, Munksgaard L (2005) The effect of reward duration on demand functions for rest in dairy heifers and lying requirements as measured by demand functions. Appl Anim Behav Sci 90: 207–217. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Metz JHM (1985) The reaction of cows to short-term deprivation of lying. Appl Anim Behav Sci 13: 301–307. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Munksgaard L, Jensen MB, Pedersen LJ, Hansen SW, Matthews L (2005) Quantifying behavioural priorities—Effects of time constraints on behaviour of dairy cows, Bos taurus. Appl Anim Behav Sci 92: 3–14. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Munksgaard L, Løvendahl P (1993) Effects of social and physical stressors on growth hormone levels in dairy cows. Can J Anim Sci 73: 847–853. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Munksgaard L, Ingvartsen KL, Pedersen LJ, Nielsen VKM (1999) Deprivation of lying down affects behaviour and pituitary-adrenal axis responses in young bulls. Acta Agric Scand A 49: 172–178. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Bell NJ, Bell MJ, Knowles TG, Whay HR, Main DJ, Webster AJ (2009) The development, implementation and testing of a lameness control programme based on HACCP principles and designed for heifers on dairy farms. Vet J 180(2): 178–188. 10.1016/j.tvjl.2008.05.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Tobler I (1995) Is sleep fundamentally different between mammalian species? Behav Brain Res 69: 35–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Preston BT, Capellini I, McNamara P, Barton RA, Nunn CL (2009) Parasite resistance and the adaptive significance of sleep. BMC Evol Biol 9:7 10.1186/1471-2148-9-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Opp MR, Krueger JM (2015) Sleep and immunity: A growing field with clinical impact. Brain Behav Immun 47: 1–3. 10.1016/j.bbi.2015.03.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Everson CA, Bergmann BM, Rechtschaffen A (1989) Sleep deprivation in the rat: III. Total sleep deprivation. Sleep 12: 13–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Everson CA (1995) Functional consequences of sustained sleep deprivation in the rat. Behav Brain Res 69: 43–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Toth LA (1995) Sleep, sleep deprivation and infectious disease: Studies in animals. Adv Neuroimmunol 5: 79–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Rechtschaffen A, Bergmann BM (2002) Sleep deprivation in the rat: An update of the 1989 paper. Sleep 25: 18–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Brown RE, Basheer R, McKenna JT, Strecker RE, McCarley RW (2012) Control of sleep and wakefulness. Physiol Rev 92: 1087–1187. 10.1152/physrev.00032.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Tobler I (1992) Behavioral sleep in the Asian elephant in captivity. Sleep 15: 1–12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Hartmann E, Bernstein J, Wilson C (1968) Sleep and dreaming in the elephant [Abstract]. Psychophysiology 4: 389. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Wyatt JR, Eltringham SK (1974) The daily activity of the elephant in the Rwenzori National Park, Uganda. Afr J Ecol 12: 273–289. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Moss C (1975) Portraits in the wild: behavior studies of East African mammals Boston: Houghton Mifflin. 371 p. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Hendrichs H, Hendrichs U (1971) Dikdik und Elefanten: Ökologie und Soziologie zweier afrikanischer Huftiere. München: R. Piper. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Sikes SK (1971) The natural history of the African elephant London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson. 397 p. [Google Scholar]
- 23.McKay GM (1973) Behavior and ecology of the Asiatic elephant in southeastern Ceylon Smithsonian Contributions to Zoology (125) Washington D.C: Smithsonian Institution Press. 113 p. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Guy PR (1976) Diurnal activity patterns of elephant in the Sengwa Area, Rhodesia. Afr J Ecol 14: 285–295. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Adams J, Berg J (1980) Behavior of female African elephants (Loxodonta africana) in captivity. Appl Anim Ethol 6: 257–276. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Kühme W (1963) Ethology of the African Elephant (Loxodonta africana Blumenbach 1797) in Captivity. Int Zoo Yearb 4: 113–121. [Google Scholar]
- 27.McKnight BL (1995) Behavioural ecology of `hand-reared' African elephants (Loxodonta africana (Blumenbach)) in Tsavo East National Park, Kenya. Afr J Ecol 33: 242–256. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Brockett R, Stoinski T, Black J, Markowitz T, Maple T (1999) Nocturnal behavior in a group of unchained female African elephants. Zoo Biol 18: 101–109. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Weisz I, Wuestenhagen A, Schwammer H (June 2000) Research on nocturnal behaviour of African elephants at Schönbrunn Zoo. International Zoo News 47: 228–233. Available: http://www.izn.org.uk/Archive/301/IZN-301.htm. Accessed 12 August 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Wilson ML, Bashaw MJ, Fountain K, Kieschnick S, Maple TL (2006) Nocturnal behavior in a group of female African elephants. Zoo Biol 25: 173–186. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Meller CL, Croney CC, Shepherdson D (2007) Effects of rubberized flooring on Asian elephant behavior in captivity. Zoo Biol 26: 51–61. 10.1002/zoo.20119 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Kandler C (2010) Analyse des nächtlichen verhaltens von Afrikanischen elefanten (Loxodonta africana) in menschenobhut unter anwendung chronoethologischer methoden. Ph.D. Dissertation, Goethe University Frankfurt.
- 33.Haley DB, de Passillé AM, Rushen J (2001) Assessing cow comfort: Effects of two floor types and two tie stall designs on the behaviour of lactating dairy cows. Appl Anim Behav Sci 71: 105–117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Tucker CB, Weary DM, Fraser D (2004) Free-stall dimensions: Effects on preference and stall usage. J Dairy Sci 87:1208–1216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Raabymagle P, Ladewig J (2006) Lying behavior in horses in relation to box size. J Equine Vet Sci 26(1): 11–17. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Posta B, Huber R, Moore DE III (2013) The effects of housing on zoo elephant behavior: A quantitative case study of diurnal and seasonal variation. Int J Comp Psychol 26: 37–52. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Carlstead K, Mench JA, Meehan CL, Brown JL (2013) An epidemiological approach to welfare research in zoos: The Elephant Welfare Project. J Appl Anim Welf Sci 16(4): 319–37. 10.1080/10888705.2013.827915 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.NOAA (2011) Quality Controlled Local Climatological Data (QCLCD) provided by NOAA/NESDIS/NCDC, North Carolina, USA, from their website at http://cdo.ncdc.noaa.gov/qclcd/QCLCD.
- 39.Miller L, Andrews J, Anderson M (2012) Validating methods to determine walking rates of elephants within a zoological institution. Anim Welf 21 (4): 577. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Holdgate MR, Meehan CL, Hogan JN, Miller LJ, Soltis J. Walking behavior of zoo elephants: associations between GPS-measured daily walking distances and environmental factors, social factors and welfare indicators. PLoS ONE 2016. doi: pone.0150331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Ito K, Weary DM, von Keyserlingk MAG (2009) Lying behavior: Assessing within- and between-herd variation in free-stall-housed dairy cows. J Dairy Sci 92: 4412–4420. 10.3168/jds.2009-2235 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Ledgerwood DN, Winckler C, Tucker CB (2010) Evaluation of data loggers, sampling intervals, and editing techniques for measuring the lying behavior of dairy cattle. J Dairy Sci 93: 5129–5139. 10.3168/jds.2009-2945 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Endres M I, Barberg AE (2007). Behavior of dairy cows in an alternative bedded-pack housing systems. J Dairy Sci 90: 4192–4200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Meehan CL, Hogan JN, Bonaparte-Saller M, Mench JA. Housing and social environments of African (Loxodonta africana) and Asian (Elephas maximus) elephants in North American zoos. PLoS ONE 2016. doi: pone.0146703. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Greco BJ, Meehan CL, Miller LJ, Shepherdson DJ, Morfeld KA, Andrews J, Baker AM, Carlstead K, Mench JA. Elephant management in North American zoological facilities: environmental enrichment, feeding, exercise, and training. PLoS ONE 2016. doi:pone.0152490. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Miller MA, Hogan JN, Meehan CL. Housing and demographic risk factors impacting foot and musculoskeletal health in African elephants (Loxodonta africana) and Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) in North American zoos. PLoS ONE 2016. doi: pone.0155223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Prado-Oviedo NA, Bonaparte-Saller MK, Malloy E, Meehan CL, Mench JA, Brown JL. Evaluation of demographics and social life events of Asian (Elephas maximus) and African elephants (Loxodonta africana) in North American zoos. PloS One 2016. doi: pone.0154750 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Hanley JA. Statistical analysis of correlated data using generalized estimating equations: An orientation. Am J Epidemiol. 2003;157(4):364–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Hardin JW, Hilbe JM. Generalized Estimating Equations. 2 ed: Chapman and Hall / CRC; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Liang K, Zeger SL. Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models. Biometrika. 1986;73(1):13–22. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Stene LC, Magnus P, Lie RT, Søvik O, Joner G. Birth weight and childhood onset type 1 diabetes: population based cohort study. BMJ. 2001; 322: 889–92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Ozminkowski RJ, Ling D, Goetzel RZ, Bruno JA, Rutter KR, Isaac F, et al. Long-term impact of Johnson & Johnson's health and wellness program on health care utilization and expenditures. J Occup Environ Med. 2002; 44: 21–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Siegel J M (2009) Sleep viewed as a state of adaptive inactivity. Nat Rev Neurosci 10(10): 747–753. 10.1038/nrn2697 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Capellini I, Barton RA, McNamara P, Preston BT, Nunn CL (2008) Phylogenetic analysis of the ecology and evolution of mammalian sleep. Evolution 62: 1764–1776. 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00392.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Lesku JA, Roth TC, Amlaner CJ, Lima SL (2006) A phylogenetic analysis of sleep architecture in mammals: the integration of anatomy, physiology and ecology. Am Nat 168: 441–453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Burger J, Gochfeld M (1994) Vigilance in African mammals: Differences among mothers, other females, and males. Behaviour 131(3): 153–169. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Joubert D (2006) Hunting behaviour of lions (Panthera leo) on elephants (Loxodonta africana) in the Chobe National Park, Botswana. Afr J Ecol 44(2): 279–281. [Google Scholar]
- 58.Power RJ, Compion RXS (2009) Lion predation on elephants in the Savuti, Chobe National Park, Botswana. Afr Zool 44(1): 36–44. [Google Scholar]
- 59.Bergsten C, Frank B (1996) Sole haemorrhages in tied primiparous cows as an indicator of periparturient laminitis: effects of diet, flooring and season. Acta Vet Scand 37(4): 383–394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Rushen J, Haley D, de Passillé AM (2007). Effect of softer flooring in tie stalls on resting behavior and leg injuries of lactating cows. J Dairy Sci 90: 3647–3651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Lewis KD, Shepherdson DJ, Owens TM, Keele M (2010) A Survey of elephant husbandry and foot health in North American zoos. Zoo Biol 29: 221–236. 10.1002/zoo.20291 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Dimeo-Ediger N (2000) Appendix II: Results of a survey of elephant foot condition and care in North America In: Csuti B, Sargent EL, Bechert US, editors. The elephant’s foot. Ames: Iowa State University Press; p 153. [Google Scholar]
- 63.Mills DS, Eckley S, Cooper JJ (2000) Thoroughbred bedding preferences, associated behaviour differences and their implications for equine welfare. Anim Sci 70: 95–106. [Google Scholar]
- 64.Pedersen GR, Søndergaard E, Ladewig J (2004) The influence of bedding on the time horses spend recumbent. J Equine Vet Sci 24: 153–158. [Google Scholar]
- 65.Pandi-Perumal SR, Seils LK, Kayumov L, Ralph MR, Lowe A, Moller H, Swaab DF (2002) Senescence, sleep, and circadian rhythms. Ageing Res Rev 1(3): 559–604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Fregonesi JA, Tucker CB, Weary DM (2007) Overstocking reduces lying time in dairy cows. J Dairy Sci 90: 3349–3354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Galindo F, Broom DM (2000) The relationships between social behaviour of dairy cows and the occurrence of lameness in three herds. Res Vet Sci 69: 75–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
For reasons relating to protection of the facilities and animals included in this study, access restrictions apply to the individual-level data underlying the findings. A data set of de-identified, population-level data is available at doi: 10.6084/m9.figshare.3383554.