Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2016 Apr 6;15(4):361–366. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12286

The genetics of neuroticism and human values

George Zacharopoulos 1,, Thomas M Lancaster 1,2,3, Gregory R Maio 1, David E J Linden 1,2,3
PMCID: PMC4950013  PMID: 26915771

Abstract

Human values and personality have been shown to share genetic variance in twin studies. However, there is a lack of evidence about the genetic components of this association. This study examined the interplay between genes, values and personality in the case of neuroticism, because polygenic scores were available for this personality trait. First, we replicated prior evidence of a positive association between the polygenic neuroticism score (PNS) and neuroticism. Second, we found that the PNS was significantly associated with the whole human value space in a sinusoidal waveform that was consistent with Schwartz's circular model of human values. These results suggest that it is useful to consider human values in the analyses of genetic contributions to personality traits. They also pave the way for an investigation of the biological mechanisms contributing to human value orientations.

Keywords: Genetics, human values, neuroticism, personality, polygenic score


The beliefs people have about ideals that are important in life, their ‘values’, are reliably associated with certain personality traits (Parks‐Leduc et al. 2015; Rim 1984). Extending this connection, studies of twins have found that the shared variance between human values and personality has a significant heritable component (Schermer et al. 2008, 2011).

Schermer et al.'s (2008, 2011) analyses of shared genetic variance between traits and values utilized Schwartz's (1992) circular model of values. This model is supported by data from over 70 nations with a range of cross‐sectional, longitudinal and experimental methods (Maio 2010; Schwartz et al. 2012). The model posits the existence of 10 types of social values (Fig. 1a), with each expressing specific motives. These motives are organized along two dimensions. One dimension contrasts motives to promote the self (self‐enhancement) against motives that transcend personal interests (self‐transcendence), whereas the other dimension contrasts motives to follow the status quo (conservation) against motives to pursue personal intellectual and emotional interests in uncertain directions (openness). One important characteristic of this circumplex model is that it makes specific predictions about sinusoidal associations between social values and external variables. As shown in Fig. 1b, this sinusoidal waveform becomes evident if the values are ordered according to their positions along the value circle: an external variable that is most positively related to a particular value should manifest less positive and progressively more negative correlations until reaching the opposing value type. This prediction has received support in many studies finding that values at opposite ends of the circular model exhibit opposing relations to other judgements and behaviour (see Schwartz 1996) and in one study observing a sinusoidal pattern in relations between values and personality traits (Parks‐Leduc et al. 2015). This sinusoidal waveform supports the model's assumptions about latent motivational conflicts between values.

Figure 1.

GBB-12286-FIG-0001-b

(a) The circumplex structure of personal values. (b) Plot of hypothesized relationships between three external variables (A, B and C) and the 10 values (SD, self‐direction; ST, stimulation; HE, hedonism; AC, achievement; PO, power; SE, security; CO, conformity; TR, tradition; BE, benevolence; UN, universalism). Each dot point could represent a correlation coefficient (modified from Schwartz 1992).

However, the exact genetic loci driving this association between values from Schwartz's model and personality have remained obscure. This association can be investigated by utilizing a growing body of knowledge on personality genetics. As complex psychological dispositions, human values and personality traits are both likely to be affected by numerous genes simultaneously (in addition to strong environmental influences). To capture the genetic influence of complex traits and values, it is therefore useful to focus on genetic indices that reflect the contribution of a great number of single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs), such as polygenic scores derived from Genome‐Wide Association Studies (GWAS).

Thus far, a polygenic score has been identified only for one trait: neuroticism. A polygenic neuroticism score (PNS) is available through a recent meta‐analysis of GWAS of personality traits (N = 63 661) (Genetics of Personality Consortium et al. 2015). Neuroticism is a personality factor ranging from emotional stability to high nervousness, tension and moodiness. In the meta‐analysis, a neuroticism score (NS) was derived from a number of measures including the NEO Personality Inventory, the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire, the International Personality Item Pool inventory, harm avoidance scores in Cloninger's Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire and negative emotionality scores in the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. The meta‐analysis showed that 0.6% of the variance in this NS was explained by the PNS. Although this low percentage suggests only a small genetic component, it was reliable and potentially important, making it a relevant candidate for studying genetic contributions to neuroticism and other individual differences related to neuroticism.

The shared genetic associations between personality traits and human values provide a foundation for expecting that the polygenic association with neuroticism may also relate to value orientations. Human values are particularly interesting in connection to neuroticism. A recent meta‐analysis of the relations between human values and the big five traits found reliable trait–value associations, except when looking at neuroticism (Parks‐Leduc et al. 2015). The authors explained this non‐association using Cloninger's (1994) proposition that neuroticism is more appropriately described as a temperament (i.e. an automatic associative response to emotional stimuli) than as a character trait (i.e. a self‐aware volitional concept related to behavioural intentions). This indicates a stronger biological component to neuroticism than to other traits, which, like human values, may be amenable to higher levels of cognitive processing and control. Thus, from this perspective, neuroticism may manifest a genetic component, but little association with human values.

However, a different possibility emerges if we consider relevant research examining links between neuroticism and relevant affective states and attitudes. Neuroticism is associated with a higher likelihood of anxiety and depression, which are two hallmarks of emotional instability that lead people to withdraw from the world around them (Angst et al. 2003; Thompson et al. 2011). This pattern suggests that emotional instability may cause people to be less open to new experiences, ideas and feelings, because of the potential threats to their fragile emotional state. Convergent with these observations, lower levels of neuroticism are associated with more liberal, curious and open‐minded attitudes (Carney et al. 2008; Van Hiel & Mervielde 2004). Strong links between such attitudes and Schwartz's openness value type (Ashton et al. 2005) suggest that an inverse relation between openness values (see Fig. 1a) and neuroticism is viable.

The present research was therefore motivated by the shared genetic variance between human values and personality, the existence of a polygenic score for neuroticism, and the ambiguity about neuroticism value relations. We sought to test whether the potential genetic contribution to neuroticism has similar patterns of the association with human values and the trait on a phenotypic level. To be clear, we were not predicting that values mediate the link between genes and traits or that traits mediate the link between genes and values. In theory, values and traits should reciprocally influence each other, as stable individual differences over time, leading to an association that is bidirectional. Our principal aim was to test whether associations with genes emerged for both the trait and values. Moreover, we wished to detect whether any observed associations arose in a sinusoidal pattern congruent with Schwartz's circumplex model of values.

Materials and methods

Subjects

A total of 81 right‐handed Caucasian university students aged between 19 and 42 (50 females; mean ± SD age = 23.85 ± 3.71) participated in the study, which was approved by the Ethics Committee in the School of Psychology, Cardiff University. Participants were informed that the study examined the connection between value–morality judgements and biological indices. They took part individually in the laboratory, wherein they completed the measures of human values and personality, provided a saliva sample, and were then debriefed. The sample used consisted of an existing sample collected for behavioural analysis. In this study we included all the participants from the existing sample for which the human value score, personality score and the genetic score were available.

Human values

Participants completed the 56‐item Schwartz Value Survey (Schwartz 1992). Participants rated the importance of each of the 56 values as a guiding principle in their lives, using a quasi‐bipolar 9‐point scale ranging from −1 (opposed to my values), 0 (not important), 4 (important) to 7 (of supreme importance). Examples of Schwartz Value Survey items are as follows: ‘equality: equal opportunity for all’ (universalism); ‘pleasure: gratification of desires’ (hedonism); ‘obedient: dutiful meeting obligations’ (conformity). The average score across the 56 items was then calculated and subtracted from each of the 56 initial raw scores. Schwartz recommends this procedure to help control for superfluous individual variations in rating styles (Schwartz 1992). The individual centred item scores were then averaged to form scores for each type of value examined in Schwartz's model (see Fig. 1a). The internal consistency of these indices was moderate to good (see Table 1).

Table 1.

Cronbach's α for each of the 10 values

Value Number of items Cronbach's α
Universalism 7 0.76
Benevolence 9 0.76
Tradition 6 0.63
Conformity 4 0.63
Security 6 0.68
Power 5 0.79
Achievement 6 0.67
Hedonism 2 0.74
Stimulation 3 0.79
Self‐direction 6 0.65

Personality measure

We quantified NS using the 100‐item self‐reported version of the HEXACO Personality Inventory‐Revised (HEXACO‐PI‐R) (Lee & Ashton 2004). In the HEXACO‐PI‐R, NS is termed emotionality, and it features subscales for fearfulness, anxiety, dependence and sentimentality. These subscales are combined together as the total emotionality score (α = 0.64). Furthermore, many influential research programmes have interpreted and labelled neuroticism from the big five as emotional stability (De Raad et al. 2010; Goldberg 1990; Saucier 1994). It was previously shown that the HEXACO emotionality represents an alternative rotation of big five neuroticism (Ashton et al. 2014) and that they are similar constructs (Ashton et al. 2014; Romero et al. 2015). Furthermore, the emotionality score provides a particularly interesting and important rendition of neuroticism in this context because of its relative emphasis on emotional instability, which leads people to withdraw from the world around them (Angst et al. 2003; Thompson et al. 2011), and HEXACO's emotionality dimension is well suited to detecting the links with values (Pozzebon & Ashton 2009).

DNA extraction and genotyping

Genomic DNA was obtained from saliva using Oragene OG‐500 saliva kits. Genotyping was performed using custom genotyping arrays (Illumina HumanCoreExome‐24 BeadChip), which contain 570 038 genetic variants (Illumina, Inc., San Diego, CA, USA). Quality control was implemented in plink (Purcell et al. 2007) to ensure that the genotypes did not display ambiguous sex, cryptic relatedness (up to third‐degree relatives by the identity of descent), genotyping completeness <97% and non‐European ethnicity admixture (detected as outliers in iterative eigenstrat analyses of an LD‐pruned data set) (Price et al. 2006). The SNPs were excluded where the minor allele frequency was <1%, if the call rate <98% or if the χ 2‐test for Hardy–Weinberg Equilibrium had a P‐value <1 e‐04. Individuals' genotypes were imputed using the pre‐phasing/imputation stepwise approach implemented in impute2/shapeit (Delaneau et al. 2012; Howie et al. 2009) and 1000Genomes (December 2013, release 1000 Genome haplotypes Phase I integrated variant set) as the reference data set.

Generation of risk profile scores

The PNS was calculated using the method described by the International Schizophrenia Consortium (International Schizophrenia Consortium et al. 2009). The PNS was estimated using publicly available data from the international GWAS (Genetics of Personality Consortium et al. 2015). The SNPs were subsequently pruned for linkage disequilibrium (r 2 < 0.2). This method ensured that all SNPs included in the PNS model were fairly independent. The PNSs were calculated using the ‘score’ command in plink, which averages the number of risk alleles for each index SNP, weighted by the natural logarithm of the SNP's odds ratio extracted from the GWAS results (Genetics of Personality Consortium et al. 2015). From the 6 949 612 SNPs, a total of 206 516 quasi‐independent SNPs were considered in the PNS (PT < 0.5). We calculated PNS at the liberal P‐threshold (PT < 0.5), because it best predicted NS in the GWAS reference data (Genetics of Personality Consortium et al. 2015). There were no outliers in the PNSs, and the scores were normally distributed (Shapiro–Wilk: P > 0.3).

Sinusoidal relationship analysis

To test for a sinusoidal pattern of association between values, NS and PNS, we calculated the correlation coefficients of the 10 value types with NS and PNS. The fit of the sinusoidal function presented below (eqn (1)) was calculated using the programme r.

y=fx=a+b×sinc×x+d, (1)

where x is a vector containing the correlation coefficients of the 10 values with either PNS or NS.

Firstly, all four of the parameters (a, b, c and d) of the sinusoidal function were optimized with the r command optim. The parameter a, the y‐offset, which moves the function up and down along the ordinate, was restricted to between −1 and 1, as were the correlation coefficients. The same restrictions were applied to parameter b, which determines the differences between the turning points of the sinusoidal function (amplitude).

The parameter c, the period of the sine wave, was allowed to range from 85% to 95% of a full sine wave. This restriction was based on the circular model's assumption that ‘the distances between the values around the circle may not be equal’ (Schwartz et al. 2012). Given that the first value type was plotted at x = 1, the parameter d (x‐offset), which moves the sinusoidal function along the abscissa, was set to the interval [1 + k/2, 1 − k/2]. Therefore, parameter d was unrestricted because there was no hypothesis regarding the exact starting point of the sine wave for each of the two measures, PNS and NS. To define a lower and upper bound given these constraints, a method developed by Byrd et al. (1995) was used.

We calculated the sum of the squared residuals divided by the variance to estimate the model fit indices for the sinusoidal function. This fit is called the Sinusoidal Fit Index (SFI) (Hanel et al. 2016) and is presented below (eqn (2)).

SFI=1K1k=1Kyky^k21K1k=1Kykyk2 (2)

In this eqn (2), K represents the number of correlation coefficients, yk represents the correlation coefficients, yk represents the estimated correlation coefficient through the optimization function and yk represents the mean of the correlation coefficients. The denominator is the formula for the variance.

Hanel et al. (2016) tested the number of false‐positive results for the SFI, using three simulations of m = 100 000 samples each in r. To simulate a random pattern of correlation coefficients, they relied upon two assumptions about the distribution of the correlation coefficients. First, they sampled 10 numbers (i.e. number of human values) between −0.5 and 0.5, with k being the number of correlation coefficients (k = 10), assuming a uniform distribution. The numbers −0.5 to 0.5 represent the interval in which most correlation coefficients of values with external variables usually fall. Second, they sampled 10 numbers from a normal distribution with ∼N(0, 0.1) and ∼N(0, 0.3). Numbers >|1| were restricted to −1 or 1, respectively. For the obtained values of SFI <0.20, the percentages of false positives were below 1% for all the three simulations of 100 000 samples. The percentage of false positives for an SFI <0.20 was 0.49% (i.e. less than five false‐positive results per 1000 comparisons) assuming a normal distribution and 0.76% assuming a uniform distribution. Similarly, assuming normal and uniform distributions, respectively, the false positives were 0.20% and 0.30% for SFI <0.15. For SFI <0.10, the false positives were 0.05% and 0.08%, and for SFI <0.05, the false positives were 0.005% and 0.007%.

Results

Replicating the link between PNS and NS

Our first aim was to provide further evidence on the association between emotionality (NS from HEXACO‐PI‐R) and PNS. As expected, we obtained a positive association between these variables, r 79 = 0.22, P = 0.048 (Fig. 2), replicating the findings of the personality GWAS (Genetics of Personality Consortium et al. 2015).

Figure 2.

GBB-12286-FIG-0002-b

Scatter‐plot depicting the positive association between NS and PNS. Both NS (derived from the HEXACO‐PI‐R, see Material and methods) and PNS were standardized with a z‐score transformation. Each dot represents a participant.

Structure of values

Before testing for a sinusoidal waveform in the pattern of associations between values and NS, and PNS, we validated Schwartz's hypothesized circular structure in our sample. This test used two multi‐dimensional scaling (MDS) as recommended by Schwartz (Bilsky et al. 2011). The first analysis plotted the 56‐value items, and the second analysis plotted the 10 higher order values. Both the analyses use the respective correlation matrix to plot the values in a two‐dimensional space. The first analysis yielded S‐Stress = 0.167 and Stress I = 0.274, whereas the second analysis yielded S‐Stress = 0.032 and a Stress‐I = 0.115. The stress value is an index of how well the data fit the hypothesized configuration; higher stress values signify a poorer configuration. The stress values and the patterns in the MDS (see Table 1) were consistent with the structure hypothesized by Schwartz (1992). In addition, the openness values, self‐direction and stimulation, were significantly negatively related to NS, but these associations did not reach significance when related to the PNS.

Fitting the sinusoidal model to the NS and PNS

Given our replication of Schwartz's circular structure in the MDS analyses, we turned to testing whether there are sinusoidal patterns of association between values and NS and PNS. To address this question, we plotted the correlation coefficients between NS and PNS on the y‐axis and each of the 10 lower order values on the x‐axis in an order that follows their circular structure. The patterns are shown in Fig. 3. A pattern of sinusoidal association was found between human values and PNS, particularly near the inflection points (Fig. 3a), which was significant, SFI = 0.19; false positives = 0.6%. Similarly, our analysis of NS show a sinusoidal association of a similar form (Fig. 3b) but the fit to the sine wave was not reliable, SFI = 29; P > 0.05. Visual inspection of Fig. 3a shows that the correlation between NS and the human value benevolence deviated from the sine wave; running the sinusoidal test while excluding benevolence yields a significant SFI = 0.08 (Fig. S1). Overall, NS and PNS map onto the human value space in similar, sinusoidal waveforms. Furthermore, in addition to testing the patterns of correlations using the SFI method, we replicated the PNS and NS findings using two previously established methods, with even more robust results (Boer & Fischer 2013; Roccas et al. 2002) (Appendix S1).

Figure 3.

GBB-12286-FIG-0003-b

Correlation coefficients between the 10 value types (x‐axis, conformity, tradition, benevolence, universalism, self‐direction, stimulation, hedonism, achievement, power and security) and PNS (a) and NS (b).

Discussion

The present research investigated the genetic components connected to the relations between human values and an important dimension of personality, neuroticism. We used empirically robust measures of human values, neuroticism and genetic neuroticism. The results replicated the association between NS and PNS despite using a different measure of neuroticism than in prior research (i.e. emotionality from HEXACO‐PI‐R). This result adds to the evidence that the PNS derived by GWAS helps to explain individual variation in neuroticism (Genetics of Personality Consortium et al. 2015). Moreover, it laid the foundation for testing whether human values are linked to both NS and PNS. Results indicated that human values were indeed associated with NS and PNS, following the sinusoidal pattern predicted by Schwartz et al. (2012) cross‐cultural model.

These findings fundamentally extend the understanding of human values. Previous twin studies (Keller et al. 1992; Schermer et al. 2008, 2011; Waller et al. 1990) have documented that human values may have a genetic component, but this has occurred without simultaneously pinpointing relevant patterns of genes, the pattern of associations with the values and the nature of the common association to the behavioural phenotype for personality. Here, we document a novel sinusoidal relationship between human values and a specific genetic marker, the PNS – a relationship that was very similar to that found between NS and values.

Furthermore, as expected, Fig. 3 shows that the sinusoidal waveforms were anchored at one end by negative relations between values promoting stimulation or self‐direction on one hand and NS or PNS on the other hand. This pattern fits links between neuroticism and anxiety and depression. As noted earlier, anxiety and depression lead people to withdraw from the world around them (Angst et al. 2003; Thompson et al. 2011). In addition, higher levels of neuroticism are associated with less liberal, curious and open‐minded attitudes (Carney et al. 2008; Van Hiel & Mervielde 2004). Neuroticism may contribute to lower openness to new experiences, ideas and feelings because of the threats posed by novelty. At the same time, the pattern of withdrawal elicited by lower stimulation and self‐direction values may contribute to emotional instability by increasing rumination, perseveration in an isolated environment and self‐absorption. Further evidence is needed to explore these possibilities.

Two other aspects of our results merit further discussion. First, it is informative to contrast the sinusoidal pattern, which is a test of association across all values, with the strength of the correlations with specific values. This is interesting in part because most of the correlations between specific values and PNS or NS were weak and unreliable, aside from the significant theoretically congruent correlations discussed above (see Table S1). Nonetheless, the sinusoidal fit shows a crucial pattern that is missing from univariate tests that focus on one value at a time. It is possible for individual relations to be weak at the same time as their combined pattern is meaningful and reliable. In the analyses of values, this difference between individual correlations and the net pattern is crucial, because the relative roles of different values are psychologically more important and meaningful than the roles of any single value type in isolation, because of the competing implications between values (Rokeach 1973; Schwartz 1992).

Second, the NS variance explained from the PNS was much higher in this study (4%) than in the initial discovery sample (0.6%). A number of factors may account for the larger relation in our study. First, this study measured neuroticism using a single scale in a single homogeneous cohort, whereas the meta‐analytic study assessed neuroticism from multiple instruments (even in the same cohort). Second, this study used a single measure of neuroticism with subscales (fearfulness, anxiety, dependence and sentimentality) that are different and more emotional in focus than in the replication cohort in the meta‐analytic study (NEO‐FFI's neuroticism: anxiety, hostility, depression, self‐consciousness, impulsiveness, vulnerability to stress and Amsterdam Biographical Questionnaire). Third, the power of this study merely allows the detection of a moderate effect, and future replication studies may yield a smaller effect; therefore, future research should interpret the current effect size with caution. Despite these possibilities, the current replication of the NS–PNS relation is promising for future research attempting to learn more about this relation and its implications.

In summary, the present research (1) replicated the prior evidence of a polygenic contribution to neuroticism using a novel measure of the trait, (2) showed an association between specific genetic components and human values for the first time and (3) found a pattern of associations with values that is congruent with Schwartz's (1992) and Schwartz et al.'s (2012) circular model of values. Together, these results show that it is useful to include value orientations as relevant individual differences in polygenic contributions to neuroticism‐related traits, suggesting that future research should consider values in investigations of polygenic contributions to other traits.

Supporting information

Table S1: Pearson correlation coefficients between PNS and NS with the 10 human values (conformity, benevolence, tradition, universalism, self‐direction, stimulation, hedonism, achievement, power and security).

Figure S1: Correlation coefficients between the nine value types (x‐axis, conformity, tradition, universalism, self‐direction, stimulation, hedonism, achievement, power and security) and NS.

Appendix S1: Replication of the sinusoidal findings of PNS and NS using two previously established methods.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by the National Centre for Mental Health (NCMH) at Cardiff University, with funds from the National Institute for Social Care and Health Research (NISCHR), Welsh Government, Wales (grant number BR09) and by grant MR/K004360/1 from the Medical Research Council (MRC) and by the MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics (G0800509). We are grateful to all professionals, patients and volunteers involved with the National Centre for Mental Health (NCMH).

References

  1. Angst, J. , Gamma, A. & Endrass, J. (2003) Risk factors for the bipolar and depression spectra. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl, 418, 15–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ashton, M.C. , Danso, H.A. , Maio, G.R. , Esses, V.M. , Bond, M.H. & Keung, D.K.Y. (2005) Two dimensions of political attitudes and their individual difference correlates: a cross‐cultural perspective. Ont Symp P 10, 1–29. [Google Scholar]
  3. Ashton, M.C. , Lee, K. & de Vries, R.E. (2014) The HEXACO honesty‐humility, agreeableness, and emotionality factors: a review of research and theory. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 18, 139–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bilsky, W. , Janik, M. & Schwartz, S.H. (2011) The structural organization of human values‐evidence from three rounds of the European social survey (ESS). J Cross‐Cult Psychol 42, 759–776. [Google Scholar]
  5. Boer, D. & Fischer, R. (2013) How and when do personal values guide our attitudes and sociality? explaining cross‐cultural variability in attitude‐value linkages. Psychol Bull 139, 1113–1147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Byrd, R.H. , Lu, P.H. , Nocedal, J. & Zhu, C.Y. (1995) A limited memory algorithm for bound constrained optimization. Siam J Sci Comput 16, 1190–1208. [Google Scholar]
  7. Carney, D.R. , Jost, J.T. , Gosling, S.D. & Potter, J. (2008) The secret lives of liberals and conservatives: personality profiles, interaction styles, and the things they leave behind. Polit Psychol 29, 807–840. [Google Scholar]
  8. Cloninger, C.R. (1994) Temperament and personality. Curr Opin Neurobiol 4, 266–273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. De Raad, B. , Barelds, D.P. , Levert, E. , Ostendorf, F. , Mlacic, B. , Di Blas, L. , Hrebickova, M. , Szirmak, Z. , Szarota, P. , Perugini, M. , Church, A.T. & Katigbak, M.S. (2010) Only three factors of personality description are fully replicable across languages: a comparison of 14 trait taxonomies. J Pers Soc Psychol 98, 160–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Delaneau, O. , Marchini, J. & Zagury, J.F. (2012) A linear complexity phasing method for thousands of genomes. Nat Methods 9, 179–181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Genetics of Personality Consortium , de Moor, M.H. , van den Berg, S.M. et al. (2015) Meta‐analysis of genome‐wide association studies for neuroticism, and the polygenic association with major depressive disorder. JAMA Psychiatry 72, 642–650. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Goldberg, L.R. (1990) An alternative "description of personality": the big‐five factor structure. J Pers Soc Psychol 59, 1216–1229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Hanel, P.H.P. , Zacharopoulos, G. , Mégardon, G. , & Maio, G.R. (2016) Detecting sinusoidal patterns of prediction from circumplex models of psychological constructs.
  14. Howie, B.N. , Donnelly, P. & Marchini, J. (2009) A flexible and accurate genotype imputation method for the next generation of genome‐wide association studies. PLoS Genet 5, e1000529. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. International Schizophrenia Consortium , Purcell, S.M. , Wray, N.R. , Stone, J.L. , Visscher, P.M. , O'Donovan, M.C. , Sullivan, P.F. & Sklar, P. (2009) Common polygenic variation contributes to risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nature 460, 748–752. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Keller, L.M. , Arvey, R.D. , Dawis, R.V. , Bouchard, T.J. & Segal, N.L. (1992) Work values ‐ genetic and environmental‐influences. J Appl Psychol 77, 79–88. [Google Scholar]
  17. Lee, K. & Ashton, M.C. (2004) Psychometric properties of the HEXACO Personality Inventory. Multivar Behav Res 39, 329–358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Maio, G.R. (2010) Mental representations of social values. Adv Exp Soc Psychol 42, 1–43. [Google Scholar]
  19. Parks‐Leduc, L. , Feldman, G. & Bardi, A. (2015) Personality traits and personal values: a meta‐analysis. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 19, 3–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Pozzebon, J.A. & Ashton, M.C. (2009) Personality and values as predictors of self‐ and peer‐reported behavior. J Individ Differ 30, 122–129. [Google Scholar]
  21. Price, A.L. , Patterson, N.J. , Plenge, R.M. , Weinblatt, M.E. , Shadick, N.A. & Reich, D. (2006) Principal components analysis corrects for stratification in genome‐wide association studies. Nat Genet 38, 904–909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Purcell, S. , Neale, B. , Todd‐Brown, K. , Thomas, L. , Ferreira, M.A. , Bender, D. , Maller, J. , Sklar, P. , de Bakker, P.I. , Daly, M.J. & Sham, P.C. (2007) PLINK: a tool set for whole‐genome association and population‐based linkage analyses. Am J Hum Genet 81, 559–575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Rim, Y. (1984) Importance of values according to personality, intelligence and sex. Pers Individ Differ 5, 245–246. [Google Scholar]
  24. Roccas, S. , Sagiv, L. , Schwartz, S.H. & Knafo, A. (2002) The big five personality factors and personal values. Pers Soc Psychol B 28, 789–801. [Google Scholar]
  25. Rokeach, M. (1973) The Nature of Human Values. Free Press Collier‐Macmillan, New York and London. [Google Scholar]
  26. Romero, E. , Villar, P. & Lopez‐Romero, L. (2015) Assessing six factors in Spain: validation of the HEXACO‐100 in relation to the five factor model and other conceptually relevant criteria. Pers Individ Differ 76, 75–81. [Google Scholar]
  27. Saucier, G. (1994) Mini‐markers: a brief version of Goldberg's unipolar big‐five markers. J Pers Assess 63, 506–516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Schermer, J.A. , Feather, N.T. , Zhu, G. & Martin, N.G. (2008) Phenotypic, genetic, and environmental properties of the portrait values questionnaire. Twin Res Hum Genet 11, 531–537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Schermer, J.A. , Vernon, P.A. , Maio, G.R. & Jang, K.L. (2011) A behavior genetic study of the connection between social values and personality. Twin Res Hum Genet 14, 233–239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Schwartz, S.H. (1992) Universals in the content and structure of values ‐ theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. Adv Exp Soc Psychol 25, 1–65. [Google Scholar]
  31. Schwartz, S. (1996) Value priorities and behavior: applying a theory of integrated value systems. In Seligman C., Olson J. M., & Zanna M. P. (eds), Psychology of Values: The Ontario Symposium, Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc., Mahwah, NJ. Vol. 8, pp. 1–24.
  32. Schwartz, S.H. , Cieciuch, J. , Vecchione, M. , Davidov, E. , Fischer, R. , Beierlein, C. , Ramos, A. , Verkasalo, M. , Lonnqvist, J.E. , Demirutku, K. , Dirilen‐Gumus, O. & Konty, M. (2012) Refining the theory of basic individual values. J Pers Soc Psychol 103, 663–688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Thompson, R.J. , Berenbaum, H. & Bredemeier, K. (2011) Cross‐sectional and longitudinal relations between affective instability and depression. J Affect Disord 130, 53–59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Van Hiel, A. & Mervielde, I. (2004) Openness to experience and boundaries in the mind: relationships with cultural and economic conservative beliefs. J Pers 72, 659–686. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Waller, N.G. , Kojetin, B.A. , Bouchard, T.J. , Lykken, D.T. & Tellegen, A. (1990) Genetic and environmental‐influences on religious interests, attitudes, and values ‐ a study of twins reared apart and together. Psychol Sci 1, 138–142. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Table S1: Pearson correlation coefficients between PNS and NS with the 10 human values (conformity, benevolence, tradition, universalism, self‐direction, stimulation, hedonism, achievement, power and security).

Figure S1: Correlation coefficients between the nine value types (x‐axis, conformity, tradition, universalism, self‐direction, stimulation, hedonism, achievement, power and security) and NS.

Appendix S1: Replication of the sinusoidal findings of PNS and NS using two previously established methods.


Articles from Genes, Brain, and Behavior are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES