Abstract
Research indicates that nonstudent emerging adults, as compared to their college-attending peers, are at higher risk for experiencing alcohol-related problems, including alcohol use disorders. The present study sought to extend the limited research on nonstudent drinking by (1) identifying sub-groups of nonstudent drinkers based on their drinking patterns and (2) determining the extent to which social-cognitive between-person factors related to drinking (i.e., social expectancies, perceived drinking norms, social drinking motivations) distinguish these sub-groups. Participants were 195 (65.1% men) nonstudent emerging adult heavy episodic drinkers recruited from the community. Mean age was 21.88 (SD = 2.08) years and 45.4% were unemployed. Latent profile analysis identified two classes based on drinking across 30 days. The “moderate drinkers” group (n = 143; 73.3%) reported consuming 10–11 drinks weekly and drinking two to three times per week, on average. The “heavy drinkers” class (n = 52; 26.7%) reported consuming 42–43 drinks weekly and drinking six to seven days per week. Both groups exhibited a cyclic pattern of drinking whereby weekday drinking was lower, with increases on the weekend; the heavy drinkers class had stronger weekend increases starting earlier. Heavy drinkers reported greater volume, frequency, and problematic drinking behaviors, as compared to the moderate drinkers. The heavy drinkers class also endorsed stronger social motives and perceived their peers to drink more. The present study offered unique insights into nonstudent emerging adult drinking patterns by identifying sub-populations of drinkers based on their past 30-day use. Knowledge gained from this study could aide in tailoring existing alcohol interventions to nonstudents to reduce alcohol-related harms.
Keywords: Alcohol, nonstudents, emerging adults, drinking patterns
Peak rates of drinking and risk for alcohol-related problems are observed among emerging adults (i.e., ages 18 to 25; Hingson, Zha, & Weitzman, 2009; Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2014). Most of the drinking literature among this age group has been conducted largely with college student-based samples. Inclusion of emerging adults who are noncollege-attenders is needed as they may be a more vulnerable population of drinkers. Compared to college students, nonstudents are less likely to mature out of heavy drinking (Muthén & Muthén, 2000; White, Labouvie, & Papadaratsakis, 2005) and are at a higher risk for alcohol-related problems (Barnett et al., 2003; Muthén & Muthén, 2000; White et al., 2005). The current study sought to extend previous research on nonstudents by examining variations in past 30-day alcohol consumption and to identify potential sub-groups of nonstudent drinkers.
Prior work on drinking patterns is limited, with most primarily focused on first-year college students (e.g., Beets et al., 2009; Del Boca, Darkes, Greenbaum, & Goldman, 2004; Maggs, Williams, & Lee, 2011; Tremblay, Colley, Saunders, Healy, & Owen, 2010). There have been a handful of studies on daily drinking patterns among nonstudents. Findings have shown that a greater portion of daily drinking variation is attributable to the intra-individual (within) versus inter-individual (between) level (Lau-Barraco, Braitman, Stamates, & Linden-Carmichael, in press). Nonstudent drinking tends to peak during holidays (Goldman, Greenbaum, Darkes, Brandon, & Del Boca, 2011; Kushnir & Cunningham, 2014) and on weekends (Kushnir & Cunningham, 2014), with weekday to weekend drinking increases being uniquely associated with social alcohol outcome expectancies (Lau-Barraco, Braitman, Linden-Carmichael, & Stamates, in press). Furthermore, in an effort to understand drinking patterns in nonstudent emerging adults, Cleveland and colleagues (2013) used latent class analysis to identify classes of alcohol users. Drinking was measured in the study using aggregate indicators, such as typical alcohol consumption and frequency. To our knowledge, research has yet to identify sub-groups of nonstudent drinkers by examining drinking patterns based on retrospective reports of drinking for the previous 30 days. Given the variable nature of alcohol use, the current approach would provide a more fine-grain, nuanced perspective of drinking in this vulnerable population.
Drinking patterns of nonstudent sub-groups may differentially relate to key social-cognitive variables. Specifically, theory (Maisto, Carey, & Bradizza, 1999) and empirical evidence assert that cognitively-based social factors, such as social expectancies, perceived drinking norms, and social drinking motives, are each uniquely related to alcohol use outcomes. For example, social expectancies (i.e., beliefs about drinking’s social effects) have distinguished latent classes of drinking trajectories (Greenbaum, Del Boca, Darkes, Wang, & Goldman, 2005) and accounted for increases in drinking on weekends (Lau-Barraco et al., in press). Perceived drinking norms (i.e., descriptive norms) also are influential in college student (e.g., see Borsari & Carey, 2003 for a review) and nonstudent (Lau-Barraco & Collins, 2011) drinking. The more one perceives others to drink, the greater their own drinking. Social motives (i.e., reasons for drinking; Cooper, 1994) are most commonly reported among college students (Arbeau, Kuiken, & Wild, 2011; LaBrie, Hummer, & Pedersen, 2007) and are predictive of drinking (see Kuntsche, Knibbe, Gmel, & Engels, 2005 for a review; Maggs et al., 2011). Research has yet to examine the association of these factors to latent classes of nonstudents based on their day-to-day drinking habits.
The present study contributes to the limited research on nonstudent drinking. We aimed to (1) identify sub-groups of nonstudent drinkers based on day-to-day drinking patterns, and (2) determine the extent to which social-cognitive between-person factors related to drinking (i.e., social expectancies, perceived drinking norms, social drinking motivations) distinguish sub-groups.
Method
Participants and Procedure
Participants were 195 (65.1% men; mean age = 21.88 [SD = 2.08]) individuals recruited from the community of a mid-size southeastern city in the U.S. via online advertisements and local newspaper listings for two separate studies (i.e., two phases of a larger study to develop a brief alcohol intervention). They were largely single/never married (64.1%), unemployed (45.4%), and African-American (52.9%). Study eligibility included being between ages 18–25 years, having no prior or current college attendance, consuming fewer than 40 drinks per week, engaging in at least two heavy drinking episodes (4+/5+ drinks for women/men) in the past month, and having no history of alcohol treatment. Eligible participants provided informed consent and completed a self-report questionnaire in-person. Participants were compensated $40 to $60, depending on the phase of the study. The study was approved by the university’s Institutional Review Board and followed the American Psychological Association (2010) guidelines.
Measures
Self-reported drinking for each day during the past 30 days was assessed using the Timeline Follow-back (TLFB; Sobell & Sobell, 1992). Alcohol-related problems were measured using the Brief Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Questionnaire (BYAACQ; Kahler, Strong, & Read, 2005). Alcohol use severity was assessed using the Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test (AUDIT; Babor, de la Fuente, Saunders, & Grant, 1992). Alcohol expectancies were evaluated using the sociability subscale of Comprehensive Effects of Alcohol questionnaire (CEOA; Fromme, Stroot, & Kaplan, 1993). Social drinking motives were assessed using the social subscale from the Drinking Motives Questionnaire (DMQ-R; Cooper, 1994). Descriptive norms were measured using the Descriptive Norms Rating Form (DNRF; Baer, Stacy, & Larimer, 1991).
Statistical Analyses
Latent profile analysis (LPA) was conducted using Mplus version 6.1 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2010). LPA was used to identify sub-populations based on drinking across 30 days (Aim 1). The number of standard drinks consumed on each day of the 30-day TLFB data served as the set of indicators. The square root of these values were used in the LPA to transform the skewed raw metric into a set of normally distributed variables. To match patterns across participants, the data for each participant was shifted so that Day 1 always started on a Sunday, resulting in 36 daily indicators with each participant contributing 30 days of data and having “missing” data for the other days. The best-fitting number of classes was determined by information criteria (AIC, BIC, aBIC), entropy values, the Lo-Mendell-Rubin likelihood ratio test, proportional class size, and interpretability of the identified classes. After finalizing the number of latent classes, class differences were explored for social-cognitive factors, alcohol-related problems, and general drinking levels using Wald tests based on posterior probability-based multiple imputations.
Results
The information criteria indicated that model fit improved as number of classes increased, whereas entropy indicated that the model with 2 classes had the highest certainty for classification (see Table 1). Nylund, Asparouhov, and Muthén (2007) concluded after extensive simulations that no one indicator is consistently accurate across all models, and that examining multiple indices is necessary to see the complete picture. Given that LMR likelihood ratio probabilities also indicate that higher numbers of classes do not significantly improve model fit, this supports the 2-class model. Finally, the proportion of participants in the smallest class indicates that 2 classes may represent the most meaningful proportions of the population.
Table 1.
Classes: | AIC | BIC | Adjusted BIC |
Relative Entropy |
LMR LRT p |
Proportion of smallest group |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | 18385.221 | 18620.877 | 18392.792 | -- | -- | -- |
2 | 16518.688 | 16875.445 | 16530.149 | .980 | .0220 | .268 |
3 | 15991.573 | 16469.431 | 16006.925 | .976 | .2321 | .097 |
4 | 15733.011 | 16331.970 | 15752.253 | .968 | .7614 | .086 |
Note. AIC = Akaike Information Criterion, BIC = Bayesian Information Criterion, LMR LRT = Lo-Mendell-Rubin likelihood ratio test. Entropy and LMR LRT are not available for models with only one class. Proportion of smallest group comes from estimated posterior probabilities rather than most likely class membership. Note that models with more than 4 classes were not estimated due to large number of parameters that would need to be estimated in comparison to the size of the sample.
Demographic characteristics varied across latent classes (see Table 2). Class 2 (described below) had a higher proportion of males, and single participants who never married and are not living with a partner. They also varied significantly by race. While there was a trend toward higher unemployment in Class 2, this difference failed to reach significance.
Table 2.
Latent Class 1 | Latent Class 2 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Indicator | M | SE | Squared M |
M | SE | Squared M |
Day 1 | 0.53 | 0.17 | 0.28 | 1.74 | 0.32 | 3.02 |
Day 2 | 0.35 | 0.08 | 0.12 | 1.94 | 0.24 | 3.76 |
Day 3 | 0.33 | 0.07 | 0.11 | 1.75 | 0.18 | 3.07 |
Day 4 | 0.39 | 0.08 | 0.15 | 1.92 | 0.20 | 3.69 |
Day 5 | 0.51 | 0.08 | 0.26 | 2.17 | 0.22 | 4.70 |
Day 6 | 1.28 | 0.11 | 1.65 | 2.89 | 0.17 | 8.36 |
Day 7 | 1.46 | 0.11 | 2.14 | 2.94 | 0.17 | 8.61 |
Day 8 | 0.50 | 0.07 | 0.25 | 1.83 | 0.20 | 3.36 |
Day 9 | 0.44 | 0.07 | 0.19 | 1.58 | 0.17 | 2.48 |
Day 10 | 0.31 | 0.06 | 0.10 | 1.69 | 0.19 | 2.85 |
Day 11 | 0.41 | 0.07 | 0.17 | 1.69 | 0.17 | 2.86 |
Day 12 | 0.46 | 0.08 | 0.21 | 2.11 | 0.16 | 4.46 |
Day 13 | 1.34 | 0.11 | 1.78 | 2.92 | 0.18 | 8.52 |
Day 14 | 1.36 | 0.11 | 1.85 | 2.96 | 0.19 | 8.74 |
Day 15 | 0.44 | 0.07 | 0.19 | 1.90 | 0.21 | 3.63 |
Day 16 | 0.29 | 0.06 | 0.08 | 1.69 | 0.16 | 2.86 |
Day 17 | 0.33 | 0.06 | 0.11 | 1.63 | 0.15 | 2.66 |
Day 18 | 0.31 | 0.06 | 0.10 | 1.79 | 0.19 | 3.19 |
Day 19 | 0.51 | 0.08 | 0.26 | 1.75 | 0.16 | 3.06 |
Day 20 | 1.27 | 0.11 | 1.62 | 2.83 | 0.18 | 8.02 |
Day 21 | 1.39 | 0.11 | 1.93 | 2.98 | 0.17 | 8.88 |
Day 22 | 0.52 | 0.08 | 0.27 | 1.95 | 0.19 | 3.79 |
Day 23 | 0.40 | 0.07 | 0.16 | 1.78 | 0.17 | 3.15 |
Day 24 | 0.30 | 0.06 | 0.09 | 1.81 | 0.18 | 3.27 |
Day 25 | 0.42 | 0.07 | 0.17 | 1.49 | 0.17 | 2.22 |
Day 26 | 0.58 | 0.08 | 0.34 | 1.87 | 0.16 | 3.51 |
Day 27 | 1.31 | 0.11 | 1.70 | 2.96 | 0.18 | 8.75 |
Day 28 | 1.44 | 0.11 | 2.08 | 2.95 | 0.18 | 8.73 |
Day 29 | 0.67 | 0.09 | 0.45 | 1.92 | 0.17 | 3.67 |
Day 30 | 0.40 | 0.07 | 0.16 | 1.86 | 0.17 | 3.46 |
Day 31 | 0.48 | 0.08 | 0.23 | 1.92 | 0.16 | 3.68 |
Day 32 | 0.31 | 0.09 | 0.10 | 1.74 | 0.23 | 3.01 |
Day 33 | 0.83 | 0.24 | 0.68 | 2.01 | 0.40 | 4.04 |
Day 34 | 1.92 | 0.22 | 3.68 | 2.66 | 0.41 | 7.05 |
Day 35 | 1.86 | 0.27 | 3.46 | 2.62 | 0.37 | 6.84 |
Day 36 | 0.50 | 0.19 | 0.25 | 1.34 | 0.51 | 1.79 |
Variable | M | SE | M | SE | χ2 | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Social Expectancies | 27.03 | (0.34) | 28.01 | (0.48) | 2.78 | .095 | |
Social Motives | 18.05 | (0.37) | 19.58 | (0.51) | 5.93 | .015 | |
Descriptive Norms | 4.62 | (0.24) | 8.05 | (0.55) | 32.81 | <.001 | |
AUDIT | 11.22 | (0.50) | 17.85 | (1.01) | 34.32 | <.001 | |
BYAACQ | 31.61 | (0.45) | 34.51 | (0.82) | 9.64 | .002 | |
TLFB quantity | 45.70 | (2.80) | 178.05 | (15.91) | 67.32 | <.001 | |
TLFB quantity per week | 10.88 | (0.67) | 42.39 | (3.79) | 67.32 | <.001 | |
TLFB frequency | 10.65 | (0.53) | 26.32 | (0.58) | 396.49 | <.001 | |
TLFB frequency per week | 2.54 | (0.13) | 6.27 | (0.14) | 396.49 | <.001 | |
TLFB peak | 8.09 | (0.40) | 14.65 | (1.29) | 23.77 | <.001 | |
TLFB number of HDD | 4.23 | (0.30) | 15.30 | (1.14) | 87.61 | <.001 | |
TLFB proportion of HDD | 0.44 | (0.03) | 0.61 | (0.05) | 9.93 | .002 | |
Prob. | Prob. | df | |||||
Gender | 1 | 3.95 | .047 | ||||
Male | .61 | .75 | |||||
Female | .39 | .25 | |||||
Race | 3 | 15.18 | .002 | ||||
Caucasian/White | .41 | .37 | |||||
Native American | .00 | .04 | |||||
African American/Black | .51 | .59 | |||||
Hispanic/Latino | .08 | .00 | |||||
Employment | 3 | 6.37 | .095 | ||||
Yes, part-time only | .26 | .25 | |||||
Yes, full and part-time | .11 | .10 | |||||
Yes, full-time only | .22 | .09 | |||||
No | .41 | .56 | |||||
Marital Status | 3 | 16.09 | .001 | ||||
Single/Never married | .69 | .87 | |||||
Living with partner | .17 | .11 | |||||
Married | .08 | .02 | |||||
Separated/Divorced | .06 | .00 |
Note. Means for day indicators reflect the square root of drinks consumed per day. Descriptive norms reflect estimated drinks per drinking day for a close friend. AUDIT = Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test, BYAACQ = Brief Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Questionnaire, TLFB = Timeline Follow-back, HDD = heavy drinking days (4+ drinks for women, 5+ drinks for men). Proportion of HDD reflects the proportion of drinking days where the participant engaged in heavy drinking (i.e., number of HDD / number of drinking days). “Per week” for TLFB reflects dividing by 4.2 to represent the average quantity consumed per week during the TLFB period. Prob. = probabilities across classes, equivalent to proportions for the current data. Unless otherwise specified, chi-square tests had one degree of freedom.
Drinking based on Class Membership
Mean drinks consumed each day is presented in Table 2 by latent class. For both groups, weekday drinking (Sunday [days 1, 8, 15, 22, 29] through Wednesday [days 4, 11, 18, 25, 32]) was lower, with increases on the weekend (Friday [days 6, 13, 20, 27, 34] and Saturday [days 7, 14, 21, 28, 35]). Patterns for Thursday (days 5, 12, 19, 26, 33) varied across groups. Mean drinks consumed stayed under or near 0.5 drinks for weekdays as well as Thursdays for class 1 (“moderate drinkers”; n = 143 [73.3%]), then between 1.5 and 2.5 drinks per day on Friday and Saturday (except the final weekend which was slightly higher). In contrast, class 2 (“heavy drinkers”; n = 52 [26.7%]) consumed more during the week (about three drinks per day), then increased on Thursdays with around four drinks, and peaked at eight or nine drinks per day on Friday and Saturday. In general, class 2 had higher base levels than class 1, as well as stronger weekend peaks starting earlier.
Class Differences on Drinking Constructs
Class 2, “heavy drinkers,” reported significantly higher social motives and descriptive norms. There was a trend toward higher social expectancies, although this trend was non-significant. In addition, class 2 reported drinking at significantly higher quantity during the TLFB period, drinking more frequently, having a higher number of peak drinks, more heavy drinking days, a higher proportion of heavy drinking days, and experiencing more problems (via AUDIT and BYAACQ scores; see Table 2).
Discussion
The current study sought to explore sub-populations of nonstudents based on retrospectively reported day-to-day drinking patterns as well as to investigate sub-group differences by examining social-cognitive between-person constructs relevant to the etiology and maintenance of drinking. In identifying homogenous sub-groups of drinkers based on drinking profiles, findings revealed two distinct classes of users. Class 1 (“moderate drinkers”) was most prevalent (73.3% of the sample) and characterized nonstudents who reported consuming 10–11 drinks weekly, drinking two to three times per week. Their weekly pattern consisted of typically consuming very low levels (0 to 0.5 drinks) during the weekday but increasing one to two additional drinks per day on Fridays and Saturdays. On the other hand, class 2 (“heavy drinkers”) characterized individuals who reported consuming 42–43 drinks weekly, drinking almost each day of the week. Class 2 drank about three drinks per day during weekdays but then showed a steady increase starting one day earlier on Thursday (to around four drinks), and peaked on Friday and Saturday (eight to nine drinks per day). Thus, across both groups, although at varying degrees, users exhibited a cyclic pattern of drinking whereby weekday drinking is lower, with increases on the weekends. This pattern of weekday to weekend drinking escalation is consistent with prior findings with college student and general population samples (e.g., Goldman et al., 2011; Hoeppner et al., 2012; Kushnir & Cunningham, 2014; Maggs et al., 2011).
In addition to drinking quantity differences between the two classes identified, the latent groups also exhibited differences in the intensity and frequency of heavy drinking as well as alcohol use severity. Class 2 consumed close to 15 drinks on the highest drinking occasion compared to eight drinks for class 1. Class 2 reported 15 heavy drinking days while class 1 reported four heavy days over the course of 30 days. The apparent severity of drinking exhibited by class 2 was verified by comparing the two groups on indicators of problem drinking. Class 2 experienced greater alcohol-related harms as measured by the BYAACQ. Though both classes scored above the AUDIT cut-off of eight (class 1 = 11.2; class 2 = 17.9), reflecting risk for hazardous and harmful alcohol use patterns, class 2 fell within a range that suggests a high level of alcohol problems and for whom more intensive interventions such as brief counseling and continued monitoring may be appropriate (Babor, Higgins-Biddle, Saunders, & Monteiro, 2001). Overall, in the present study, we were able to discern different temporal patterns of drinking based on daily cycles that corresponded with differential risk levels. We extended previous findings by identifying two groups of nonstudent drinkers that not only differed in drinking patterns, but also diverged in the volume, frequency, and intensity of drinking.
Our LPA findings are in line with previous research conducted among adolescent and emerging adult samples. For example, Cleveland et al. (2013) found that nonstudent emerging adults reporting high-risk drinking and daily alcohol use in general were at an increased risk for experiencing alcohol-related harms. Similarly, among underage drinkers, studies have also identified subgroups of drinkers, whereby more frequent, heavy drinkers are at a greater likelihood to report more alcohol-related harms (Diestelkamp et al., 2015; Reboussin, Song, Shrestha, Lohman, & Wolfson, 2006). Heterogeneous subgroups have also been identified among college students showing that heavy drinking groups are often characterized by more alcohol-related problems (e.g., Beseler, Taylor, Kraemer, & Leeman, 2012; Kuvass, Dvorak, Pearson, Lamis, & Sargent, 2014) and behavioral risks (e.g., Chiauzzi, DasMahapatra, & Black, 2013). However, it is worth noting that classes of drinkers with the greatest risk level based on college samples still exhibit relatively lower consumption than the high risk class found in the present study. For example, in an undergraduate college sample, three latent classes emerged based on their Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV substance use disorder criteria endorsement (Beseler et al., 2012). The most severe class endorsed all DSM-IV criteria with high probabilities and all met threshold for a dependence diagnosis. Their mean weekly consumption was 24.6 drinks per week. This is in contrast to the mean consumption of approximately 42 drinks weekly for participants found in the “heavy drinkers” class in our sample of nonstudents. This robust relative difference could suggest that our “heavy drinkers” class may represent a group of emerging adults who are especially at-risk for alcohol dependence and long-term alcohol-related impairment and should be specifically targeted for intervention efforts.
Our second aim was to examine the extent to which sub-groups differed on social-cognitive predictors (i.e., social drinking motives, descriptive drinking norms, social expectancies). We found support that normative drinking perceptions and social motivations distinguished classes of drinkers, such that “heavy drinkers” perceived their peers to drink more and they reported greater motivation to drink for social reasons. Both classes had similar levels of social alcohol expectancies. These findings suggest that, similar to college students (Maggs et al., 2011; O’Grady, Cullum, Tennen, & Armeli, 2011), nonstudent drinking patterns are associated with perceptions of peers’ drinking and motivations to drink to be social.
The current findings offer several implications that could guide efforts to enhance drinking reduction efforts for nonstudent heavy drinkers. Intervention strategies that target social-influence processes, such as correcting drinking norm misperceptions (e.g., Neighbors, Larimer, & Lewis, 2004) may be successfully tailored to meet the needs of nonstudent emerging adult populations. Another implication is that our results identified a particularly risky class of drinkers who drank almost daily, consistently engaged in binge drinking on weekends, and began their “weekend” drinking a day early (on Thursdays). In light of their vulnerability to experiencing alcohol-related harms, concerted efforts should be made to specifically reach this highly at-risk group. Further suggested by the results is that nonstudent drinkers are not a homogeneous group with similar alcohol use patterns. Because of the heterogeneity of drinking even among heavy drinking nonstudents, a universal standard approach may not be optimal but instead, tailored strategies may be necessary to meet varying needs based on drinker type.
Several study limitations should be noted. First, reports of drinking behavior were based on retrospective self-report data, which may be susceptible to recall or social desirability bias. Second, our findings may not generalize to other populations beyond heavy drinking nonstudent emerging adults. Conclusions beyond our sample should also be made with caution given that that 45% of our sample was unemployed and 60% of the sample identified as a minority. Third, other daily diary assessment methods (e.g., ecological momentary assessment) may be better suited for studying associations more in-depth. Finally, although all participants reported consuming fewer than 40 drinks per week at baseline screening for the study, class 2 reported consuming an average of more than 40 drinks per week. It is likely that participants underreported their alcohol use during study screening given they simply provided an overall estimate of their weekly drinking. A more detailed assessment may be needed at baseline to accurately screen for drinking inclusion/exclusion criteria.
Despite limitations, the present study offered unique insights into the drinking patterns of nonstudent emerging adults by identifying sub-populations of drinkers based on past 30-day use. Findings revealed that these sub-groups of nonstudents are distinguished based on normative perceptions of others’ drinking, social drinking motives, and alcohol outcomes. Knowledge gained from the current study could be used to help tailor existing alcohol interventions to nonstudents or guide for whom to direct intervention efforts as to reach those most in need.
Highlights.
Identified sub-groups of nonstudent drinkers based on daily drinking patterns.
Two groups were identified with each group exhibiting a cyclic pattern of drinking.
“Heavy drinkers” class reported greater volume, frequency, and problematic drinking behaviors, as compared to “moderate drinkers”.
“Heavy drinkers” class endorsed stronger social motives and perceived their peers to drink more.
Acknowledgments
Author Disclosures
Cathy Lau-Barraco is supported by a Career Development Award (K01-AA018383) from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA). Ashley N. Linden-Carmichael was supported by the NIAAA under a Ruth L. Kirschstein National Research Service Award (F31-AA023118). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and the NIAAA had no other role other than financial support.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- American Psychological Association. Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct. 2010 Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/ethics/code/principles.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- Arbeau KJ, Kuiken D, Wild TC. Drinking to enhance and to cope: A daily process study of motive specificity. Addictive Behaviors. 2011;36:1174–1183. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.07.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Auerbach KJ, Collins LM. A multidimensional developmental model of alcohol use during emerging adulthood. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2006;67:917–925. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2006.67.917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Babor TF, de la Fuente JR, Saunders J, Grant M. The alcohol use disorders identification test: Guidelines for use in primary care. Geneva: World Health Organization; 1992. WHO Publication No. 92.4. [Google Scholar]
- Babor TF, Higgins-Biddle JC, Saunders JB, Monteiro MG. AUDIT. The alcohol use disorders identification test: Guidelines for use in primary care. 2nd. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Baer JS, Stacy A, Larimer M. Biases in the perception of drinking norms among college students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1991;52:580–586. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1991.52.580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnett NP, Monti PM, Spirito A, Colby SM, Rohsenow DJ, Ruffolo L, Woolard R. Alcohol use and related harm among older adolescents treated in an emergency department: The importance of alcohol status and college status. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2003;64:342–349. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2003.64.342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beets MW, Flay BR, Vuchinich S, Li KK, Acock A, Snyder FJ Tobacco Etiology Research Network. Longitudinal patterns of binge drinking among first year college students with a history of tobacco use. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2009;103:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.12.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beseler CL, Taylor LA, Kraemer DT, Leeman RF. A latent class analysis of DSM-IV alcohol use disorder criteria and binge drinking in undergraduates. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2012;36:153–161. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01595.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borsari B, Carey KB. Descriptive and injunctive norms in college drinking: A meta-analytic integration. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2003;64:331–341. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2003.64.331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiauzzi E, DasMahapatra P, Black RA. Risk behaviors and drug use: A latent class analysis of heavy episodic drinking in first-year college students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2013;27:974–985. doi: 10.1037/a0031570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cleveland MJ, Mallett KA, White HR, Turrisi R, Favero S. Patterns of alcohol use and related consequences in non-college-attending emerging adults. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2013;74:84–93. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2013.74.84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper ML. Motivations for alcohol use among adolescents: Development and validation of a four-factor model. Psychological Assessment. 1994;6:117–128. [Google Scholar]
- Del Boca FK, Darkes J, Greenbaum PE, Goldman MS. Up close and personal: Temporal variability in the drinking of individual college students during their first year. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2004;72:155–164. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.2.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diestelkamp S, Kriston L, Arnaud N, Wartberg L, Sack PM, Härter M, Thomasius R. Drinking patterns of alcohol intoxicated adolescents in the emergency department: a latent class analysis. Addictive Behaviors. 2015;50:51–59. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.06.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fromme K, Stroot EA, Kaplan D. Comprehensive effects of alcohol: Development and psychometric assessment of a new expectancy questionnaire. Psychological Assessment. 1993;51:19–26. [Google Scholar]
- Goldman MS, Greenbaum PE, Darkes J, Brandon KO, Del Boca FK. How many versus how much: 52 weeks of alcohol consumption in emerging adults. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2011;25:16–27. doi: 10.1037/a0021744. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenbaum PE, Del Boca FK, Darkes J, Wang C, Goldman MS. Variation in the drinking trajectories of freshman college students. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2005;72:229–238. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.2.229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hingson RW, Zha W, Weitzman ER. Magnitude of and trends in alcohol-related mortality and morbidity among US college students ages 18–24, 1998–2005. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, Supplement. 2009;16:12–20. doi: 10.15288/jsads.2009.s16.12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoeppner BB, Barnett NP, Jackson KM, Colby SM, Kahler CW, Monti PM, Read J, Fingeret A. Daily college student drinking patterns across the first year of college. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2012;73:613–624. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2012.73.613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kahler CW, Strong DR, Read JP. Toward efficient and comprehensive measurement of the alcohol problems continuum in college students: The Brief Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Questionnaire. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2005;29:1180–1189. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000171940.95813.a5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuntsche E, Knibbe R, Gmel G, Engels R. Why do young people drink? A review of drinking motives. Clinical Psychology Review. 2005;25:841–861. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuvass NJ, Dvorak RD, Pearson MR, Lamis DA, Sargent EM. Self-regulation and alcohol use involvement: A latent class analysis. Addictive Behaviors. 2014;39:146–152. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.09.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kushnir V, Cunningham JA. Event-specific drinking in the general population. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2014;75:968–972. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2014.75.968. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LaBrie JW, Hummer JF, Pedersen ER. Reasons for drinking in the college student context: The differential role and risk of the social motivator. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2007;68:393–398. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2007.68.393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lau-Barraco C, Collins RL. Social networks and alcohol use among nonstudent emerging adults: A preliminary study. Addictive Behaviors. 2011;36:47–54. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.08.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lau-Barraco C, Braitman AL, Linden-Carmichael AN, Stamates AL. Differences in weekday versus weekend drinking among nonstudent emerging adults. doi: 10.1037/pha0000068. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lau-Barraco C, Braitman AL, Stamates AL, Linden-Carmichael AN. Alcohol use variability in a community-based sample of nonstudent emerging adult heavy drinkers. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse. doi: 10.1080/00952990.2016.1183671. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maggs JL, Rankin William L, Lee CM. Ups and downs of alcohol use among first-year college students: Number of drinks, heavy drinking, and stumble and pass out drinking days. Addictive Behaviors. 2011;36:197–202. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.10.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maisto SA, Carey KB, Bradizza CM. Social learning theory. In: Blane HT, Leonard KE, editors. Psychological theories of drinking and alcoholism. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1999. pp. 106–163. [Google Scholar]
- Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User’s Guide. Sixth. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 1998–2010. [Google Scholar]
- Muthén B, Muthén LK. Integrating person-centered and variable-centered analyses: Growth mixture modeling with latent trajectory classes. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2000;24:882–891. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neighbors C, Larimer ME, Lewis MA. Targeting misperceptions of descriptive drinking norms: Efficacy of a computer-delivered personalized normative feedback intervention. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2004;72:434–447. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.3.434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nylund KL, Asparouhov T, Muthén BO. Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A Monte Carlo simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling. 2007;14:535–569. [Google Scholar]
- O’Grady MA, Cullum J, Tennen H, Armeli S. Daily relationship between event-specific drinking norms and alcohol use: A four-year longitudinal study. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2011;72:633–641. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2011.72.633. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reboussin BA, Song EY, Shrestha A, Lohman KK, Wolfson M. A latent class analysis of underage problem drinking: Evidence from a community sample of 16–20 year olds. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2006;83:199–209. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.11.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sobell LC, Sobell MB. Measuring alcohol consumption. Humana Press; 1992. Timeline follow-back; pp. 41–72. [Google Scholar]
- Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. Results from the 2013 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: National Findings. 2014 (Office of Applied Studies, NSDUH Series H-30, DHHS Publication No. SMA 06-4194) [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tremblay MS, Colley RCC, Saunders TJ, Healy GN, Owen N. Physiological and health implications of a sedentary lifestyle. Applied Physiology, Nutrition, and Metabolism. 2010;35:725–740. doi: 10.1139/H10-079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White HR, Labouvie EW, Papadaratsakis V. Changes in substance use during the transition to adulthood: A comparison of college students and their noncollege age peers. Journal of Drug Issues. 2005;35:281–306. [Google Scholar]