Abstract
Hierarchies in the correlated forms of power (resources) and status (prestige) are constants that organize human societies. This article reviews relevant social psychological literature and identifies several converging results concerning power and status. Whether rank is chronically possessed or temporarily embodied, higher ranks create psychological distance from others, allow agency by the higher ranked, and exact deference from the lower ranked. Beliefs that status entails competence are essentially universal. Interpersonal interactions create warmth-competence compensatory tradeoffs. Along with societal structures (enduring inequality), these tradeoffs reinforce status-competence beliefs. Race, class, and gender further illustrate these dynamics. Although status systems are resilient, they can shift, and understanding those change processes is an important direction for future research, as global demographic changes disrupt existing hierarchies.
Keywords: status, power, hierarchy, intergroup, impression, race, class, gender
“Hierarchy may have a psychological advantage over equality in that it is familiar, rehearsed, socially efficient” (1).
In this research example, when participants’ deliberate thinking was constrained—by increased blood alcohol, cognitive load, low-effort thought, ego depletion, or rapid responding—participants increased their endorsement of hierarchy.
Accumulating evidence such as this indicates that group power and status distinctions are relatively automatic and spontaneous, perhaps even the human default. Power and status ranking is not going away any time soon. This essay reviews relevant recent work, focusing on how intergroup status (including race, class, and gender as exemplars) gets enacted in interpersonal encounters. In light of shifting demographics—with migration, globalization, and changing populations—the article ends by considering potential shifts in intergroup status and power in the current era.
Defining Power and Status
Expert consensus is clear: Power is asymmetrical control over resources, and status is social prestige (2, 3). Inequality is not just about power (resources) but also about status (respect) (4). Their empirical independence appears in several findings: Power per se makes people less just, but status (especially without power) makes people more just (5). Power without status is particularly destructive, as it allows demeaning others (6). Men disproportionately prefer power, while women disproportionately prefer status (7); legitimacy affects the desirability of status but not power (7).
Nevertheless, in practice, power and status are often correlated. For example, people are attracted to the powerful (and attraction confers status/prestige)—but only if the powerful actually possess control and are recognized as such, enacting it by controlling the conversation and being legitimated by others (8). As another example, social class combines power and general hierarchical rank (9).
Granted, concepts of power differ by culture (10), for example as more self- or other-oriented. Power also differs by domain (e.g., workplace or domestic; 11). Still, hierarchy beliefs do generalize across contexts (12). Power and status are typically correlated features of the human condition, which is fundamentally ranked. Here we focus on the interpersonal enactment of intergroup rank.
Encounters across Power/Status
High power, and to some extent status, creates psychological distance from others (13). Power thus leads to higher cognitive construal level, allowing the powerful to follow their dispositions (14). Power allows people to act freely, power leading to approach motivation (15). Such approach mindsets reciprocally increase status (16). Power allows agenda setting in intergroup encounters, for example by postponing consequential negotiations that might shift power arrangements (17, 18). Thus, power, agency, and action are intertwined.
Power manifests physically as well. Individuals can dress for status, pose powerfully, emote dominantly, and act nonverbally like a leader (19, 20, 21, 22, 23, 24). Groups as a whole can enact embodiments parallel to those of individuals (e.g., when the powerful group expresses dominant emotions, such as anger, their violent actions seem more legitimate; 25). Such physical manisfestations encourage the powerful in risky decisions, even dishonest ones, but they do elicit deference (even to flawed decisions), encourage legitimation, and improve job-interview performance.
Compensation Effect as a Function of Status/Power
How do people perceive others who are higher or lower in social status and power, and how do those perceptions affect their interactions and broader group dynamics? Hierarchy operates at different levels, from individual to societal, and research at these different levels helps to explain the maintenance of unequal hierarchies, despite most Americans’ beliefs that society should ideally be less stratified (e.g., 26). Status hierarchies at organizational and societal levels affect how individuals perceive social groups and individual others. At more immediate levels, such perceptions reinforce unequal societal hierarchies.
Perceived social status informs judgments of others’ competence, with judgments of a group’s societal status and its trait competence positively correlated at an average of .90 across countries (27). Going beyond this apparently universal status-competence link (28), across 37 cross-national samples, people in countries with greater income inequality, as measured by the Gini coefficient, perceive outgroup members more ambivalently, with judgments of competence but not warmth influenced by targets’ status.
This tendency in more hierarchical societies to see groups more ambivalently carries weight in interpersonal interactions: Commenting on a person’s high competence while omitting information about the person’s interpersonal warmth (or commenting on high warmth while omitting competence information) actually implies negative standing on the omitted dimension, an ambivalent code understood by both speakers and listeners (29, 30). Relative power and status can be communicated as subtly as by referring to one group as the norm and another (otherwise equal) group as the target of comparison, with judgments of high agency and low communion tied to members of the normative (higher status) group (31). In this way, inequality and norms of communication perpetuate stereotypes of high- and low-status targets as respectively cold-but-competent and incompetent-but-warm.
Targets of these ambivalent stereotypes are not unaware of this coded language: In interpersonal interactions, high-status targets compensate for their stereotypic coldness by downplaying their own competence to appear warmer, while low-status targets pursue the opposite strategy, compensating for stereotypic incompetence by downplaying their own warmth to appear more competent (32, see also Yzerbyt, this issue). When status-based stereotypes have a positive side, such as strength in academics at a higher-status school or strength in athletics at a lower-status school, higher- and lower-status individuals stake their claim to these strengths, while judging their outgroup peers as weaker on the ingroup-favoring dimension (33, 34).
In addition to downplaying stereotypic incompetence and bolstering their comparative strengths, lower-status people show in-group favoritism in allocating resources to improve their group’s standing (35,36). Leaders recognize this threat posed by a unified lower-status group, and actually undermine their highly-skilled subordinates’ group cohesion to maintain the status quo (37). Dominance-oriented leaders threatened by competent underlings will restrict their subordinates’ communications with each other, physically sequester them, and discourage their bonding. The subordinates’ competence would be threatening because it runs against a stereotypic lower-status role.
Whether negative or positive, ambivalent status-based stereotypes are ingrained in hierarchies and perpetuated at the interpersonal level. Ironically, individuals’ need for order and control, and their system-justifying ideologies, can combine with status stereotypes to reinforce inequality as desirable and legitimate (38, 39). The resulting cycle in which inequality strengthens status stereotypes, and status stereotypes legitimize inequality, seems hard to break.
Race, Gender, and Class as Exemplars
Social group membership is often confounded with social status. Regarding race, both Black and White participants implicitly and explicitly associate Black targets with low-status positions and White targets with high-status positions (40). Such race-status associations predict Blacks’ status seeking and Whites’ status keeping. Recent work has explored how difficult it can be to cross this implied status gap. Even well intentioned Whites struggle to do so. Whites high in the desire to affiliate with racial minorities are nevertheless unable to accurately understand minority strangers or even roommates (41). Further, egalitarian Whites tend to present less competence to Black interaction partners than to White interaction partners, engaging in patronizing self-presentation, to appear warmer, despite endorsing socially liberal ideologies (42). White Democratic Presidential candidates also do this to minority audiences, more than do White Republicans ones (42). Perceptions of cross-race partners as dissimilar or anxious may hinder the ability to cross the implied status gap when interacting with racial outgroup members (43).
Likewise, a recent surge in social-class research has explored how social class is closely associated with social status. A rank-based perspective on social class (44) suggests that people use social class as a tool to compare their own social standing to others. Varying social-class environments socialize differently and produce different cultural experiences. This sociocultural perspective suggests that psychological and behavioral outcomes follow not only from perceived rank, but also from the resources, ideas, perspectives, and institutions encountered over time in different social classes (45). For example, members of the upper class tend to express more narcissistic behavior (as a function of entitlement) and act more selfishly than do those of lower social classes (46, 47). High status in general allows both adults and children to act less prosocially than do lower-status people (48).
Finally, gender also reflects perceptions of power and social status. The stereotypes applied to men as the dominant, higher-status group worldwide reflect cultural values in their specific manifestation. In the U.S., a nation that values individualism, men are seen as more individualistic than women; in South Korea, a nation that values collectivism, men are seen as more collectivistic than women (49). Work on gender in management and business reflects this implied status gap. For example, people in leadership positions are more willing to relinquish power to men than to women (50).
Shifting Power and Status
Power and status are not static features of social hierarchies, as evidenced by a growing body of research that has identified some of the catalysts involved in social change. Social change does not require merely decreasing unpleasant or awkward intergroup interactions. For example, asymmetries in fair access to resources leads disadvantaged groups to push for discussing inequality—while advantaged groups, although potentially well intentioned, try to avoid focusing on intergroup differences (18). These discordant strategies frequently maintain the status quo, as the powerful group fails to address issues of intergroup disparities for the sake of avoiding conflict. Such motivational discrepancies may be at play in intergroup negotiations, institutional change, and everyday interactions (for a review, see 18).
To be sure, status does shift for those individuals or groups who do move up or down the social hierarchy. For example, new majorities, especially those low in perceived control over their status, may abuse their power more than do established majorities, favoring their ingroup and derogating new minorities (51).
Even the potential for change in the hierarchy has wide-ranging consequences for intergroup relations. The salience of demographic information predicting that minorities will become the majority in the United States links to Whites’ greater support for conservative policies (20). And racial minorities’ social progress is associated with greater perceived anti-White bias (i.e., perceiving an interracial zero-sum game) by those Whites who see the hierarchy as legitimate (52). Both of these effects may operate through an increase in perceived threat, stronger at the physiological level (e.g., cardiovascular response) for members of powerful groups (53).
Future research will doubtless continue exploring these topics and grow to include the study of more subtle changes in social hierarchies. Promising areas of inquiry include research on the exclusion of ambiguous targets from high-status ingroups (e.g., multiracials; 54) and on the differential effects of subjective (i.e., context-dependent) and objective status on intergroup relations (e.g., 55).
Conclusions
Societal status and power hierarchies endure and have some apparently universal cross-rank dynamics: Power and status create psychological distance, conferring agency at the top and requiring deference at the bottom. Status conveys competence, although it tends to tradeoff against lower warmth. This warmth-competence compensation appears in encounters across status, race, gender, and class. Societies with more inequality are especially likely to position social groups as stereotypically high on one dimension and low on the other.
At the same time, demographic change also predicts shifts in which groups will occupy which status/power roles. The majority group will become merely one group among many groups, no longer dominating by their sheer numbers. With migration and differential birth rates, different ethnic groups and nationalities will become more or less numerically powerful, locally and globally. Gender role changes will continue to alter the societal ranking of men and women. The more we know about societal, group, and personal responses to the inevitable hierarchy upheavals, the fewer surprises our societies will encounter, and quite possibly, thoughtful planning for demographic change will reduce intergroup conflict and make us all the safer.
Highlights.
Hierarchy, a human constant, comprises power (resources) and status (prestige)
Rank, temporary or chronic, creates distance, agency, and deference
Individual and societal relations mutually reinforce status-competence beliefs
Race, class, and gender dynamics further illustrate status
Although resilient, status systems can shift, a future research direction
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1**.Van Berkel L, Crandall CS, Eidelman S, Blanchar JC. Hierarchy, dominance, and deliberation: Egalitarian values require mental effort. Pers Soc Psychol B. 2015;41:1207–1222. doi: 10.1177/0146167215591961. Exemplar of research showing the inevitability of human hierarchy. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Fiske ST. Interpersonal stratification: Status, power, and subordination. In: Fiske ST, Gilbert DT, Lindzey G, editors. Handbook of social psychology. Wiley; 2010. pp. 941–982. [Google Scholar]
- 3*.Galinsky AD, Rucker DD, Magee JC. Power: Past findings, present considerations, and future directions. In: Mikulincer M, Shaver PR, editors. APA Handbook of personality and social psychology. Vol. 33. American Psychological Association; 2015. pp. 421–460. Authoritative review of power. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Ridgeway CL. Why status matters for inequality. American Sociological Review. 2014;79:1–16. Argues for the importance of status, not just power, in societal ranking. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Blader SL, Chen Y-R. Differentiating the effects of status and power: A justice perspective. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2012;102:994–1014. doi: 10.1037/a0026651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Fast NJ, Halevy N, Galinsky AD. The destructive nature of power without status. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2012;48:391–394. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Hays NA. Fear and loving in social hierarchy: Sex differences in preferences for power versus status. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2013;49:1130–1136. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Eastwick PW, Wilkey BM, Finkel EJ, Lambert NM, Fitzsimons GM, Brown PC, Fincham FD. Act with authority: Romantic desire at the nexus of power possessed and power perceived. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2013;49:267–271. [Google Scholar]
- 9**.Kraus MW, Piff PK, Keltner D. Social class as culture: The convergence of resources and rank in the social realm. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2011;20:246–250. Reviews how perceived class rank influences reactions consistent with objective indicators of resource control. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Torelli CJ, Shavitt S. Culture and concepts of power. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2010;99:703–723. doi: 10.1037/a0019973. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Williams MJ, Chen S. When “mom’s the boss”: Control over domestic decision making reduces women’s interest in workplace power. Group Processes Intergroup Relations. 2014;17:436–452. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Kteily N, Ho AK, Sidanius J. Hierarchy in the mind: The predictive power of social dominance orientation across social contexts and domains. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2012;48:543–549. [Google Scholar]
- 13**.Magee JC, Smith PK. The social distance theory of power. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2013;17:158–186. doi: 10.1177/1088868312472732. Definitive statement of power’s effect on psychological and social distance, including construal level. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Guinote A, Weick M, Cai A. Does power magnify the expression of dispositions? Psychol Sci. 2012;23:475–482. doi: 10.1177/0956797611428472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15*.Keltner D, Gruenfeld DH, Anderson C. Power, approach, and inhibition. Psychol Rev. 2003;110:265–284. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.110.2.265. Foundational theory arguing for power as approach motivation. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kilduff GJ, Galinsky AD. From the ephemeral to the enduring: How approach-oriented mindsets lead to greater status. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2013;105:816–831. doi: 10.1037/a0033667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kteily N, Saguy T, Sidanius J, Taylor DM. Negotiating power: Agenda ordering and the willingness to negotiate in asymmetric intergroup conflicts. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2013;105:978–995. doi: 10.1037/a0034095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Saguy T, Kteily N. Power, negotiations, and the anticipation of intergroup encounters. Eur Rev Soc Psychol. 2014;25:107–141. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Carney DR, Cuddy AJC, Yap AJ. Power posing: Brief nonverbal displays affect neuroendocrine levels and risk tolerance. Psychol Sci. 2010;21:1363–1368. doi: 10.1177/0956797610383437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20**.Carney DR, Cuddy AJC, Yap AJ. Review of research on the embodied effects of expansive (vs. contractive) nonverbal displays. Psychol Sci. 2015;26:657–663. doi: 10.1177/0956797614566855. More than 30 studies show effects of expansive posture (power poses) on behavior such as risk-taking; the effects on hormones have been less often tested and remain open. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21*.Craig MA, Richeson JA. On the precipice of a “majority-minority” America: Perceived status threat from the racial demographic shift affects white Americans’ political ideology. Psychol Sci. 2014;25:1189–1197. doi: 10.1177/0956797614527113. Shows how perceptions of changing demographics create status threat for majorities anticipating a majority-minority future. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Cuddy AJC, Wilmuth CA, Yap AJ, Carney DR. Preparatory power posing affects nonverbal presence and job interview performance. J Applied Psychol. 2015;100:1286–1295. doi: 10.1037/a0038543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kraus MW, Mendes WB. Sartorial symbols of social class elicit class-consistent behavioral and physiological responses: A dyadic approach. J Exp Psychol: Gen. 2014;143:2330–2340. doi: 10.1037/xge0000023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Locke CC, Anderson C. The downside of looking like a leader: Power, nonverbal confidence, and participative decision-making. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2015;58:42–47. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Yap AJ, Wazlawek AS, Lucas BJ, Cuddy AJC, Carney DR. The ergonomics of dishonesty: The effect of incidental posture on stealing, cheating, and traffic violations. Psychol Sci. 2013;24:2281–2289. doi: 10.1177/0956797613492425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Kamans E, van Zomeren M, Gordijn EH, Postmes T. Communicating the right emotion makes violence seem less wrong: Power-congruent emotions lead outsiders to legitimize violence of powerless and powerful groups in intractable conflict. Group Processes Intergroup Relations. 2014;17:286–305. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Norton MI, Ariely D. Building a better America — One wealth quintile at a time. Perspectives Psychol Sci. 2011;6:9–12. doi: 10.1177/1745691610393524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Fiske ST, Durante F. Stereotype content across cultures: Variations on a few themes. In: Gelfand MJ, Chiu CY, Hong YY, editors. Advances in culture and psychology. Vol. 6 Oxford University Press; [Google Scholar]
- 29.Durante F, Fiske ST, Kervyn N, Cuddy AJC, Akande AD, Adetoun BE, Storari CC. Nations’ income inequality predicts ambivalence in stereotype content: How societies mind the gap. Brit J Soc Psychol. 2012;52:726–746. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Bergsieker HB, Leslie LM, Constantine VS, Fiske ST. Stereotyping by omission: Eliminate the negative, accentuate the positive. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2012;102:1214–38. doi: 10.1037/a0027717. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Kervyn N, Bergsieker HB, Fiske ST. The innuendo effect: Hearing the positive but inferring the negative. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2012;48:77–85. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Bruckmüller S, Abele AE. Comparison focus in intergroup comparisons: Who we compare to whom influences who we see as powerful and agentic. Pers Soc Psychol B. 2010;36:1424–1435. doi: 10.1177/0146167210383581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33*.Swencionis JK, Fiske ST. Promote up, ingratiate down: Status comparisons drive warmth-competence tradeoffs in impression management. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2016 in press. Exemplar of dysfunctional interpersonal interactions across status. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Oldmeadow JA, Fiske ST. Social status and the pursuit of positive social identity: Systematic domains of intergroup differentiation and discrimination for high- and low-status groups. Group Processes Intergroup Relations. 2010;13:425–444. doi: 10.1177/1368430209355650. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Ellemers N, Van Rijswijk W. Identity needs versus social opportunities: The use of group-level and individual-level identity management strategies. Soc Psychol Quarterly. 1997;60:52–65. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Branscombe NR, Spears R, Ellemers N, Doosje B. Intragroup and intergroup evaluation effects on group behavior. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2002;28:744–753. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Rubin M, Badea C, Jetten J. Low status groups show in-group favoritism to compensate for their low status and compete for higher status. Group Processes Intergroup Relations. 2014;17:563–576. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Case CR, Maner JK. Divide and conquer: When and why leaders undermine the cohesive fabric of their group. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2014;107:1033–1050. doi: 10.1037/a0038201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Friesen JP, Kay AC, Eibach RP, Galinsky AD. Seeking structure in social organization: Compensatory control and the psychological advantages of hierarchy. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2014;106:590–609. doi: 10.1037/a0035620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Oldmeadow JA, Fiske ST. System-justifying ideologies moderate status=competence stereotype: Roles for belief in a just world and social dominance orientation. Euro J Soc Psychol. 2007;37:1135–1148. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Dupree CH, Obioha OA, Fiske ST. Race-status associations predict Blacks’ and Whites’ occupational preferences for self and others. (under review) [Google Scholar]
- 42.Holoien DS, Bergsieker HB, Shelton JN, Alegre JM. Do you really understand? Achieving accuracy in interracial relationships. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2015;108:76–92. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Dupree CH, Fiske ST. Egalitarianism ironically predicts self-presentation in intergroup settings: The competence downshift by liberals. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000166. (under review) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.West TV, Dovidio JF, Pearson AR. Accuracy and bias in perceptions of relationship interest for intergroup and intragroup roommates. Soc Psychol Pers Sci. 2014;5:235–242. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Kraus MW, Tan JJX, Tannenbaum MB. The social ladder: A rank-based perspective on social class. Psychol Inquiry. 24:81–96. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Stephens NM, Townsend SSM. Rank is not enough: Why we need a sociocultural perspective to understand social class. Psychol Inquiry. 2013;24:126–130. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Dubois D, Rucker DD, Galinsky AD. Social class, power, and selfishness: When and why upper and lower class individuals behave unethically. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2015;108:436–449. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Piff PK. Wealth and the inflated self: Class, entitlement, and narcissism. Pers Soc Psychol B. 2014;40:34–43. doi: 10.1177/0146167213501699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49*.Guinote A, Cotzia I, Sandhu S, Siwa P. Social status modulates prosocial behavior and egalitarianism in preschool children and adults. P Natl Acad Sci. 2015;112:731–736. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1414550112. Exemplar showing how status undermines pro-social behavior. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Cuddy AJC, Wolf EB, Glick P, Crotty S, Chong J, Norton MI. Men as cultural ideals: Cultural values moderate gender stereotype content. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2015;109:622–635. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Ratcliff NJ, Vescio TK, Dahl JL. (Still) waiting in the wings: Group-based biases in leaders’ decisions about to whom power is relinquished. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2015;57:23–30. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Prislin R, Sawicki V, Williams K. New majorities’ abuse of power: Effects of perceived control and social support. Group Processes Intergroup Relations. 2011;14:489–504. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Wilkins CL, Kaiser CR. Racial progress as threat to the status hierarchy: Implications for perceptions of anti-White bias. Psychol Sci. 2014;25:439–446. doi: 10.1177/0956797613508412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Scheepers D, Röell C, Ellemers N. Unstable power threatens the powerful and challenges the powerless: Evidence from cardiovascular markers of motivation. Front Psychol. 2015;6:720. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00720. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Kteily N, Cotterill S, Sidanius J, Sheehy-Skeffington J, Bergh R. “Not one of us”: Predictors and consequences of denying ingroup characteristics to ambiguous targets. Pers Soc Psychol B. 2014;40:1231–1247. doi: 10.1177/0146167214539708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Brown-Iannuzzi JL, Lundberg KAB, Kay AC, Payne BK. Subjective status shapes political preferences. Psychol Sci. 2015;26:15–26. doi: 10.1177/0956797614553947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]