Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Aug 1.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2016 Aug;28(3):743–756. doi: 10.1017/S0954579416000286

Transactional relations between caregiving stress, executive functioning, and problem behavior from early childhood to early adolescence

Linda L LaGasse a,b,*, Elisabeth Conradt c,*, Sarah L Karalunas d, Lynne M Dansereau b, Jonathan E Butner c, Seetha Shankaran e, Henrietta Bada f, Charles R Bauer g, Toni M Whitaker h, Barry M Lester a,b
PMCID: PMC4955944  NIHMSID: NIHMS802362  PMID: 27427803

Abstract

Developmental psychopathologists face the difficult task of identifying the environmental conditions that may contribute to early childhood behavior problems. Highly stressed caregivers can exacerbate behavior problems, while children with behavior problems may make parenting more difficult and increase caregiver stress. Unknown is: (1) how these transactions originate, (2) whether they persist over time to contribute to the development of problem behavior and (3) what role resilience factors, such as child executive functioning, may play in mitigating the development of problem behavior. In the present study, transactional relations between caregiving stress, executive functioning, and behavior problems were examined in a sample of 1,388 children with prenatal drug exposures at three developmental time points: early childhood (birth-age 5), middle childhood (ages 6 to 9), and early adolescence (ages 10 to 13). Transactional relations differed between caregiving stress and internalizing versus externalizing behavior. Targeting executive functioning in evidence-based interventions for children with prenatal substance exposure who present with internalizing problems and treating caregiving psychopathology, depression, and parenting stress in early childhood may be particularly important for children presenting with internalizing behavior.

Introduction

One of the most difficult problems developmental psychopathologists face is to identify the environmental conditions that may contribute to the development of behavior problems originating in early childhood. Behavior problems are common in young children (Sahker, McCabe & Arndt, 2015), developing during the preschool period (Calkins, Blandon, Williford, Kean,2007; Combs-Ronto, Olson, Lunkenheimer, & Sameroff, 2009; Loe, Feldman & Huffman, 2014) and for some children persisting into middle childhood, (Choe, Olson & Sameroff, 2013; Sulik et al., 2015) preadolescence (Romer et al., 2009) and adolescence. (Martel et al., 2007) Further, behavior problems that first emerge early in life can lead to mental health problems in childhood and adolescence (Fanti & Henrich, 2010) and psychopathology during adulthood (Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group, 1992). Unclear at this point is how these processes may unfold, though longitudinal, transactional models of change may help to uncover pathways leading to the emergence of psychopathology in early adolescence.

In this study we take a developmental psychopathology approach to understanding how internalizing and externalizing behavior in early adolescence evolved from experiences in early and middle childhood. This approach helps us to answer the questions related to how particular disorders develop from earlier manifestations. Of course not all children will follow the same pathway to disorder; an earlier risk factor can lead to multiple outcomes, or the same outcome may be due to multiple earlier pathways. Consistent with the developmental psychopathology approach is a targeted analysis of how multiple outcomes can arise from similar earlier risk factors, also known as multifinality.

We hypothesize that one way in which trajectories may be altered is through resilience factors. Even less is known about resilience processes that may alter developmental pathways leading to psychopathology. Resilience is thought of as the development of competency in the face of environmental risk or adversity (Luthar, Cicchetti, & Becker, 2000; Masten, 2001). The study of how resilience processes may alter developmental trajectories, particularly those stemming from early risk factors, is a core tenet of the developmental psychopathology perspective, and one that has been minimally applied to this study of children with prenatal substance exposure (though, for an exception, see Sheinkopf et al., 2007). The sample in this study comes from the Maternal Lifestyle Study (MLS) of high risk families with significant prenatal and postnatal maternal substance use including cocaine and opiates, and stressors impacting caregivers include poverty, depression, psychopathology, and domestic violence (LaGasse et al., 1999).

Caregiving Stress and Behavior Problems

One of the most important contextual factors that exacerbate behavior problems is caregiving stress (Calkins, Blandon, Williford, & Keane, 2007; Choe, Olson, & Sameroff, 2013). According to Patterson’s coercion theory (Dishion, Patterson, & Kavanagh, 1992; Granic & Patterson, 2006), children with externalizing behaviors can evoke negativity, anger, frustration, more physical discipline, and excessive parental control in highly stressed caregivers, which in turn provokes increased behavior problems (Combs-Ronto, Olson, Lunkenheimer, & Sameroff, 2009). The cycle continues whereby increased disciplinary action by the caretaker serves to maintain and enhance the negative behavioral cycle. Further, although internalizing behavior may seem less demanding of a caregiver, in fact, a similar parental pattern emerges in part because children with internalizing behaviors may be highly susceptible to the caregiving environment due to a low threshold for anxiety, depression, emotional reactivity, and tendency to withdraw (Wagner, Propper, Gueron-Sela, & Mills-Koonce, 2015).

Thus, the dyadic process between parent and child is a series of transactions whereby each partner reacts to the other. Negativity in the parent is the currency for development of disruptive behaviors in the child while disruptive behaviors in the child evoke stress and increased negativity in the parent. In children transitioning from preschool (ages 3.5 to 5.3 years), disruptive behavior showed bidirectional patterns with negative parenting (Combs-Ronto, Olson, Lunkenheimer, & Sameroff, 2009).

Further exacerbating caregiving stress, many children live in poverty with a single parent, whereby parental stress is high, but positive support for the child is limited (Bagner et al., 2009). In the MLS study, caretaker depression, impoverished home environment, violence and tobacco use all predicted behavior problems in the child at age 7 years through 13 years. While it is known that caregiving stress and psychopathology are bidirectional, very little research compares these factors across multiple sensitive developmental periods. This study adds to the existing research by determining the intra- and interrelations of behavior problems and caregiving stress across multiple developmental periods.

Role of Executive Functioning

Within-child factors, such as executive functioning, are also relevant to the transactional relationships between caregiving stress and behavior problems. Executive functioning encompasses domains such as attention, inhibitory control, working memory and set-shifting (Miyake et al., 2000) and is of particular interest in relation to developmental psychopathology because executive functioning abilities support self-regulation of behavior and emotion (Derryberry, 2002; Ochsner & Gross, 2005; Ochsner, Silvers, & Buhle, 2012; Rothbart, Sheese, Rueda, & Posner, 2011). Executive functioning also shows protracted development throughout infancy, childhood, and adolescence (Anderson et al., 2001; Williams et al., 1999; Zelazo, 2004; Zelazo, Muller, Frye, & Marcovitch, 2003), suggesting it sets the stage for child’s regulatory capacity, but may also be influenced by intervening environmental events that either facilitate or interfere with cognitive development.

The substantial change in executive functioning that occurs throughout childhood and adolescence complicates its measurement across a broad development range; however, models in both child and adult populations suggest that executive functioning abilities reflect a combination of domain general and domain specific abilities (Lehto, Juujärvi, Kooistra, & Pulkkinen 2003; Miyake et al., 2000), with increasing contributions of domain specific effects as children age (Lee, Bull, & Ho, 2013; Miyake et al., 2000; Shing et al., 2010).

Within the infancy and early childhood period, directed attention abilities (i.e., the ability to control the focus of attention) are most relevant and set the stage for later maturing executive functioning skills (Garon, Bryson, & Smith, 2008; Petersen & Posner, 2012; Rothbart, Sheese, Rueda & Posner, 2011; Rueda, Posner, & Rothbart, 2005). In early childhood, inhibitory control plays an increasing role (Carlson, 2005; Carlson & Moses, 2001; Kochanska et al., 1996; Kochanska, Murray, & Harlan, 2000; Rhoades, Greenberg, & Domitrovich, 2009), allowing children to inhibit preferred or prepotent responses in order to enact responses that facilitate longer-term goals. During middle childhood and adolescence, directed attention and inhibitory control remain critical, while further development in working memory and set-shifting (Cragg & Nation, 2009; Crone et al., 2006; Kalkut et al., 2009; Luciana, Conklin, Hooper, & Yarger, 2005) allow children to solve increasingly complex problems and improve the regulation of behavior and emotions (Note that the NIH Toolbox; http://www.nihtookbox,org/WhatAndWhy/Cognition/Pages/defaulty.as0x; provides measures of many of these constructs across a wide development range. However, these tools were not available when data were being collected for the MLS study).

Not surprisingly, because the development of executive functioning is closely related to self-regulation, executive functioning deficits are associated with clinical impairment in a variety of internalizing and externalizing pathologies (Eigsti et al., 2006; Hughes & Ensor, 2011; Schoemaker, Mulder, Dekovic, & Matthys, 2013; Sulik et al., 2015). Further, executive functioning impairment may mediate trait negative affect in early childhood (Bridgett, et al., 2013), as well as irritability and depression symptoms in later adulthood (Joormann & Gotlib, 2008; Williams, Suchy, & Kraybill, 2010). It is also predictive of functional outcomes, such as social competence, academic achievement, and job attainment (Biederman et al., 2004; Miller & Hinshaw, 2010; Thorell, 2007). Together, results highlight the potential for development of executive functioning to either mitigate or exacerbate emerging psychopathology during childhood and adolescence.

Although research within developmental psychopathology has often emphasized executive functioning abilities as trait-like markers of risk for psychopathology, the development of executive functioning is also affected by the child’s environment. In a study from this sample, we found that early adversity was associated with executive functioning difficulties at ages 8/9 years (Fisher et al., 2011). Parenting practices also effect development of executive functioning, and studies in early childhood have found that these effects are bidirectional such that a child’s executive functioning abilities both influence and are influenced by parenting practices. However, whether these relationships extend into later developmental periods has not been examined. Here we build on prior studies to examine the relationships between child executive functioning, caregiving stress (which may both directly and indirectly influence parenting practice), and child outcomes. Consistent with prior research in earlier developmental periods, we hypothesize that caregiving stress will predict concurrent and prospective child executive functioning, but that child executive functioning will also predict concurrent and prospective caregiving stress. Both caregiving stress and child executive functioning will prospectively predict child internalizing and externalizing symptoms.

Caregiving Stress, Executive Functioning and Behavior Problems

The purpose of this study is to determine the transactional or bidirectional relationships among caregiving stress, executive functioning, and behavior problems across early childhood (age 5), middle childhood (age 9), and early adolescence (age 13). To our knowledge, related studies focus on two of the three components and few studies include changes across more than two developmental periods. We conduct a structural equation model analysis of internalizing and externalizing behaviors in the context of caregiving stress and executive functioning to specify the unique patterns of each within and across ages 5, 9, and 13 years. We hypothesize that the relationship between caregiving stress and behavior problems will have a bidirectional association. Consistent with coercion theory, children’s behavior problems may increase caregiving stress and reciprocally caregiving stress may be predictive of increased behavior problems. These transactions are expected to occur within and across developmental periods. The inclusion of executive functioning is unique and important because it may uncover different resilience pathways leading from caregiving stress to psychopathology across developmental periods. For instance, in a low-income sample similar to our own, Blair and colleagues have found consistent, strong associations between the quality of early parenting and executive functioning in early childhood (Blair et al., 2011). As reviewed above, executive functioning is also a strong predictor of problem behavior. We hypothesize that executive functioning mediates the relation between early caregiving stress (or problem behavior) on problem behavior in adolescence. In other words, executive functioning may be a mechanism by which caregiving stress or problem behavior, experienced in early childhood, is related to problem behavior in adolescence.

Method

Participants

This study is based on data from the MLS. Subjects were recruited postpartum at 4 participating hospital sites from 1993 to 1995. The study was approved by the institutional review board at each site. Complete study details and eligibility requirements are reported elsewhere (Bauer et al., 2005; Bauer et al., 2002; Lester et al., 2014). Briefly, the study was conducted in two phases, an acute phase at birth and a longitudinal phase which began at the 1 month visit. Maternal exclusion criteria included: age<18 years; identified psychosis or history of institutionalization for retardation or emotional problems; or language barriers that prevented giving informed consent or understanding the study. Infant exclusion criteria included: not born at one of the participating hospitals; multiple gestation; birth weight<501 g; gestational age>42 weeks; or if the attending physician concluded that the infant was unlikely to survive.

There were 1,388 mother/infant dyads enrolled in the longitudinal study at the 1 month visit. The 1-month visit included neurobehavioral, medical and physical status measures of the infant; social and demographic questionnaires; and the Maternal Interview of Substance Use (MISU). The MISU provides information about the frequency and quantity of substance use for each trimester during this pregnancy and was administered only to the biological mothers who brought their infant to the 1-month visit (n=1255). At subsequent visits, behavioral and executive functioning measures were collected on children and psychosocial risk factors including SES, psychiatric status, parenting stress, abuse potential, and ongoing drug use were collected on caregivers. This study includes data from follow-up visits at 4, 8, 10, 12, 18, 24, 30, and 36 months and 4, 4.5, 5, 5.5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12 and 13 years.

The racial/ethnic distribution of the sample was: African American (76.6%), Caucasian (15.9%), Hispanic (6.3%), and children whose parents identified other racial and ethnic backgrounds (1.2%). 52.4% of the children were male. Average maternal age was 28.3 years. Most of the mothers were single (80.6%), 39.3% did not complete high school and 22.3% had low SES. Prenatal substance exposures included cocaine (43.7%), opiates (8.4%), alcohol (59.4%), tobacco (53.9%), and marijuana (23.6%) (Table 1). The retention rates based on the number of subjects with complete CBCL data for the 3 developmental periods are: early childhood (59.2%), middle childhood (73.0%), and early adolescence (71.1%). Little’s Missing Completely at Random (MCAR) test was performed (Little, 1998) and, as expected, showed that missing values were MCAR (χ2 =14.5, df=18, P=.696).

Table 1.

Maternal and Infant characteristics at birth

Mother-Infant Dyads (n=1388)
Maternal characteristics
 Race
  Black 1063 (76.6%)
  White 220 (15.9%)
  Hispanic 88 (6.3%)
  Other (non-Hispanic) 17 (1.2%)
 Education <12 years 545 (39.3%)
 Marital Status: single 1119 (80.6%)
 Low SES 309 (22.3%)
 Maternal age (yrs), mean (SD) 28.3 (5.8)
 Prenatal substance exposure
  Cocaine 606 (43.7%)
  Opiate 117 (8.4%)
  Alcohol 825 (59.4%)
  Tobacco 748 (53.9%)
  Marijuana 327 (23.3%)
Infant characteristics
 Child gender: Male 727 (52.4%)
 Gestational age (wks.), mean (SD) 36.3 (4.0)
 Birth Weight (g), mean (SD) 2630 (819)

Measures

Behavior problems

Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL)

Internalizing and externalizing behavior were measured using the CBCL (Achenbach, 1991) (αinternalizing = .90, αexternalizing =.94). The CBCL was administered to the child’s parent or caretaker every 2 years from ages 3 to 15 years). For this study, CBCL assessments at 5, 9, and 13 years old were included to represent three developmental periods: early childhood (Age 5 internalizing and externalizing) middle childhood (Age 9 internalizing and externalizing), and early adolescence (Age 13 internalizing and externalizing). A trained and certified interviewer administered the CBCL verbally during the annual clinic visit to assure uniform administration across all four sites. The CBCL T-scores for externalizing and internalizing behavior were used to provide a profile of the child’s social and behavioral functioning relative to children of the same age and gender.

Caregiving stress

Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI)

Caregiver psychological distress was assessed using the Brief Symptom Inventory (Derogatis, 1993) (α = .96). The BSI is a 53-item questionnaire administered by interview to the caregiver designed to reflect the psychological symptom patterns over the previous 7 days of psychiatric and medical patients as well as community non-patient respondents. The caregiver rated each item on a 5-point scale of distress ranging from “not at all” to “extremely/a whole lot”. There are 2 global scores: Global Severity Index and Positive Symptom Total.

Beck Depression Inventory (BDI)

Caregiver symptoms of depression were assessed using the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck, Steer & Brown, 1996) (α = .88). The BDI is a 21 item instrument designed to assess the severity of depression in adolescents and adults. For each item, the caregiver selected 1 of 4 statements that best describes how he/she has felt over the past week. A BDI total score provided an estimate of overall severity of depression.

Parenting Stress Index (PSI)

Caregiver stress was assessed using the Parenting Stress Index (Abidin, 1983) (α = .91). The PSI is a 36-item questionnaire, scored on a 5-point scale, administered to the caregiver that measured stress arising from parenting. The instrument provided a summary index of total stress.

Caretaker Inventory of Substance Use (CISU)

Caregiver history of abuse was assessed using the Caretaker Inventory of Substance Use (Shankaran et al., 1996). The CISU is a caregiver-report questionnaire of current substance use including tobacco, marijuana, alcohol, cocaine/crack/heroin, and methadone and domestic violence sustained by the caregiver. The caregiver indicated whether or not he/she had experienced emotional, physical or sexual abuse (0 = no, 1 = yes).

Caregiving stress summary score

Caregiving stress was calculated at each developmental period: early childhood, middle childhood, and early adolescence. For early childhood caregiving stress, the BSI global severity index was averaged from the 4 month and 30 month visits, the BDI total score was averaged from the 4 month, 30 month, and 4 year visits, the PSI total stress index was averaged from the 4 month and 30 month visits, and a sum of caregiver indicated abuse was calculated from the CISU reported at the 4 month, 8 month, 12 month, 24 month, 36 month, 4 year, and 5 year visits. An early childhood caregiving stress summary score (Age 5 caregiving stress) was calculated from the four individual measures.

For middle childhood caregiving stress, the BSI global severity index was averaged from the 5.5 year and 9 year visits, the BDI total score was averaged from the 5.5 year, 7 year, and 9 year visits, the PSI total stress index was averaged from the 5.5 year and 8 year visits, and a sum of caregiver indicated abuse was calculated from the CISU reported at the 6 year, 7 year, 8 year and 9 year visits. A middle childhood caregiving stress summary score (Age 9 caregiving stress) was calculated from the four individual measures.

For early adolescence caregiving stress, the BSI global severity index was measured at the 11 year visit, the BDI total score was averaged from the 11 year and 13 year visits, the PSI total stress index was measured at the 10 year visit, and a sum of caregiver indicated abuse was calculated from the CISU reported at the 10 year, 11 year, 12 year and 13 year visits. An early adolescence childhood caregiving stress summary score (Age 13 caregiving stress) was calculated from the four individual measures.

Correlations between caregiving stress summary scores were significant and ranged from 0.44 to 0.67.

Executive functioning

Age 5 years: Delay of Gratification Task

In the Delay of Gratification Task (Kochanska, 1997) a wrapped “present” was given to the child with an instruction not to touch the present until the examiner returned. A maximum of 3 minutes was allowed to see if the child violated the prohibition. The latency to contact the gift was the primary outcome variable. Maximum action taken was scored as follows: 0 - No action taken by the child to get or look at toy; 1 - Child peeked in the bag only; 2 - Child touched the bag but didn’t touch tissue; 3 - Child moved the tissue but didn’t get the toy; 4 - Child obtained the toy.

Age 5 years: Prohibition Task

In the Prohibition Task (Kochanska, 1997) the child was presented with an inviting display of toys by a “stranger” with instruction not to play with them until the “stranger” returned. The examiner stayed in the room but was reading and not paying attention to the child. If the child started to play with one of the toys the examiner reminded the child 1–2 times not to play with the toys. If the child did not touch the toys for 1–2 minutes the examiner tempted the child 1–2 times to play with the toys. Total touches are the total number of times the child purposefully touched the prohibited toys. One additional touch was counted for each 5 seconds of continuous play, if the child played with the toys.

Age 5 years: Test of Variables of Attention (TOVA)

The TOVA (Leark et al., 1996) is a computerized visual continuous performance test. It is a vigilance test that challenges the child’s ability to sustain attention. The test we used has been modified for children and contained geometric shapes as targets. The test consisted of 4 blocks of 325 trials in which a child pressed a key when the correct target appeared. The summary scores included Errors of Omission (%) (failure to press key when the correct target appeared) and Errors of Commission (%) (pressed the key for the incorrect target).

Age 9 and 13 years: Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB)

The CANTAB is an automated computerized, normed test battery (De Luca et a., 2003; Fray, Robbins & Sahakian, 1996; Ornstein et al., 2000) which captures executive functioning factors present in middle childhood (Lehto, Juujärvi, Kooistra, & Pulkkinen, L., 2003) through adulthood (Miyake et al., 2000). The tests are non-verbal, visually-presented using a touch screen for subjects’ responses. The CANTAB has been used with adult substance abusers and with high-risk children.

Executive functioning summary scores

One concern in studies of executive functioning in childhood are the modest reliabilities of cognitive tasks, which reduces statistical power to detect associations between cognitive processes and other variables of interest (Green et al., 2004; Kendler & Neale, 2010; Kuntsi et al., 2006). Composite scores are one method for improving measurement reliability. Further, some evidence suggests that composite measures that capture the complex coordination of executive functioning may be most associated with clinically-relevant outcomes (Miller, Loya, & Hinshaw, 2013). Thus, we also used a single composite score to capture executive functioning in early and middle childhood and early adolescence to maximize measurement reliability and clinical prediction. Executive functioning was calculated at each developmental period; early childhood, middle childhood, and early adolescence. The executive functioning summary score in early childhood (Age 5 executive functioning) was constructed using the delay of gratification task (maximum action taken), the prohibition task (total touches), and the TOVA (errors of omission and errors of commission).

At ages 9 and 13 (Age 9 and Age 13 executive functioning) measures from the CANTAB were used. The summary score at each age point included four individual subtests: Spatial Span Task (the longest sequence successfully recalled by the subject), Spatial Working Memory Task (number of times a subject revisits a box already found to be empty), Intradimensional/Extradimensional Set-Shifting Task (measure of subject’s efficiency in adapting to changing criteria), and Stockings of Cambridge (number of occasions upon which the subject had successfully completed a test problem in the minimum possible number of moves).

Correlations between executive functioning summary scores were significant and ranged from to 0.14 to 0.48.

Prenatal substance exposure

Prenatal substance exposure was measured as a summative index ranging from 0–5 for use of cocaine or opiates by self-report or by meconium toxicology using gas chromatography–mass spectrometry confirmation of presumptive positive meconium screens for cocaine or opiate metabolites and self-reported use of marijuana, alcohol, and tobacco during pregnancy (Fisher et al., 2011; Messinger & Lester, 2005). One point was assigned for each substance used.

SES

SES was derived using the Hollingshead Index of Social Position [ISP] (Hollingshead, 1975). The ISP was calculated at each follow-up visit and was based on the average of the weighted sum of education and occupation of the primary caregiver and another adult who was contributing to the household in which the child lived. For early childhood (Age 5 SES), the ISP was averaged from the 1 month, 12 month, 24 month, 36 month, 4 year, and 5 year visits. For middle childhood (Age 9 SES), the ISP was averaged from the 6 year, 7 year, 8 year, and 9 year visits. For early adolescence (Age 13 SES), the ISP was averaged from the 10 year, 11 year, 12 year, and 13 year visits. Lower ISP summary scores indicate low SES.

Statistical analyses

We used structural equation modeling to examine associations between caregiving stress, executive functioning, and internalizing and externalizing behavior at ages 5, 9, and 13 years. Percentage of missing data varied across indicators and ranged from 8.3% to 40.8% with a mean of 26.6%. Full-information maximum likelihood (FIML) was used to handle missing data, which uses all available information and provides more statistically reliable standard errors than mean-imputation, pair-wise or list-wise models (Brown et al., 2008). Model fit was determined using the standardized root-mean-square-residual (SRMR) and comparative fit index (CFI). Fit was considered acceptable if the SRMR was less than .08 and CFI was greater than .90 (McDonald & Ho, 2002). Mplus version 7.0 (Muthen & Muthen, 2012) was used to conduct these analyses.

Results

Descriptive statistics

Table 2 includes the correlations, means, and standard deviations between our core predictor and outcome variables. Correlations between the three caregiving stress and SES summary scores were all significant and positive, within each time point and across time. Correlations between the executive functioning summary scores were also moderate within time, and there was one significant association across time. Greater internalizing and externalizing behavior at age 5 was associated with greater caregiving stress and lower SES at ages 5, 9, and 13. Internalizing behavior at age 5 was not associated with the executive functioning variables at ages 9 and 13. Greater externalizing behavior at age 5 was associated with poorer executive functioning at age 13. Greater internalizing and externalizing behavior at age 9 was associated with greater caregiving stress at ages 5, 9, and 13. Greater externalizing was associated with lower SES at ages 5, 9, and 13. Greater externalizing behavior at age 9 was associated with poorer executive functioning at ages 9 and 13. Greater internalizing and externalizing behavior at age 13 was associated with greater caregiving stress and lower SES at ages 5, 9, and 13. Greater externalizing behavior at age 13 was associated with poorer executive functioning at ages 9 and 13.

Table 2.

Bivariate correlations and descriptive statistics for problem behavior, caregiving stress, executive functioning, and SES, ages 5 to 13

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15
 1. Internalizing, 5 y -
 2. Externalizing, 5 y .67*** -
 3. Caregiving Stress, 5 y .32*** .33*** -
 4. Executive Functioning, 5 y −.01 −.00 .04 -
 5. SES, 5 y −.12*** −.14*** −.33*** −.10* -
 6. Internalizing, 9 y .46*** .45*** .20*** −.05 −.03 -
 7. Externalizing, 9 y .38*** .57*** .23*** .05 −.12*** .72*** -
 8. Caregiving Stress, 9 y .39*** .38*** .58*** .07 −.23*** .36*** .38*** -
 9. Executive Functioning, 9 y .00 −.03 −.04 −.04 .14*** −.05 −.07* −.04 -
 10. SES, 9 y −.10** −.10* −.27*** −.08 .73*** .02 −.07* −.25*** .11*** -
 11. Internalizing, 13 y .41*** .38*** .21*** .00 −.10*** .57*** .44*** .37*** −.07 −.07* -
 12. Externalizing, 13 y .36*** .51*** .20*** .04 −.16*** .46*** .64*** .34*** −.09* −.14*** .63*** -
 13. Caregiving Stress, 13 y .30*** .33*** .44*** .07 −.17*** .40*** .44*** .67*** −.00 −.17*** .44*** .43*** -
 14. Executive Functioning, 13 y .00 −.06 −.04 −.14*** .19*** −.03 −.14*** −.10** .29*** .19*** −.09** −.15** −.11** -
 15. SES, 13 y −.09* −.07* −.26*** −.10** .70*** .01 −.09** −.24*** .09** .80*** −.11*** −.17*** −.23*** .21*** -
Mean 50.0 55.6 20.9 0.83 28.1 49.8 52.2 20.3 −26.1 29.2 49.8 53.5 20.6 −19.4 30.1
SD 9.5 11.0 5.3 1.3 9.3 11.1 12.2 5.6 8.4 9.7 10.2 10.6 6.0 8.4 9.8

Note:

*

p <.05,

**

p<.01,

***

p<.001

Covariates

We tested whether sex and race or ethnicity varied by caregiving stress, executive functioning, and internalizing and externalizing behavior. Sex was associated with internalizing behavior at ages 5 and 9, with girls exhibiting higher internalizing behavior than boys (p’s <.02). Race was related to caregiving stress at age 5 (p <.001), with non-African-American mothers exhibiting the lowest levels of caregiving stress and African-American mothers the highest levels. There were also main effects of ethnicity on externalizing (p <.001) and internalizing behavior at age 9 (p = .04). Hispanic children had significantly lower externalizing behavior compared to Caucasian and African American children, who did not differ from each other. Caucasian children had higher internalizing behavior than African American children who had higher internalizing behavior than Hispanic children. Differences between ethnic groups also emerged on externalizing (p = .05) and internalizing (p = .01) behavior at age 13. Caucasian adolescents had significantly higher externalizing behavior compared to Hispanic adolescents, and significantly higher internalizing behavior compared to African American adolescents.

Finally, ethnic/race differences were found with respect to executive functioning (summary score) at age 13 (p<.001). African Americans performed significantly worse on executive functioning tasks compared to Caucasians and Hispanics, who did not differ from each other. Some of these associations became non-significant when entered into our full structural equation model, and therefore are not included in our final models (Figure 1, below).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Transactional model predicting internalizing and externalizing behavior, caregiving stress, and executive functioning from ages 5–13 years. Note: Paths are standardized beta coefficients. Χ2 (67) = 353.51; comparative fit index (CFI) = .95; standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) =.06. Only significant paths are modeled due to ease of presentation.

*p<.05; **p<.01; ***p <.001.

aAge 5 caregiving stress was calculated from data collected at ages 4 months–5 years.

bAge 9 caregiving stress was calculated from data collected at ages 5.5 years–9 years.

cAge 13 caregiving stress and SES was calculated from data collected at ages 10 years–13 years

dAge 5 SES was calculated from data collected at ages 1 month–5 years.

eAge 9 SES was calculated from data collected at ages 6 years–9 years.

Structural Equation Modeling

Figure 1 shows a structural equation model that includes internalizing and externalizing behaviors at 5, 9, and 13 years in order to account for the significant co-occurrence of these symptoms in childhood and adolescence. There were significant, strong, positive associations between internalizing and externalizing behavior within each age (ranging from .54–.67). There was also significant stability in internalizing and externalizing behavior across time, ranging from .40–.49 for internalizing behavior and .45–.58 for externalizing behavior. The model below includes the specific contribution of caregiving stress and executive functioning to each type of behavior problem in the context of SES, prenatal drug exposure, race/ethnicity, and sex. Predictors and mediators of internalizing and externalizing behavior are presented separately below.

Internalizing behavior

Higher levels of prenatal drug exposure were related to greater internalizing behavior at age 5 and 9, but not at age 13. Females had higher internalizing behavior at age 5, but there were no significant sex effects at age 9 or 13. African American children experienced greater caregiving stress and lower internalizing behavior at age 5 compared to non African American children. Greater caregiving stress and lower socio-economic status (SES) at age 5 was associated with higher internalizing behavior at age 5. Lower SES was also related to higher caregiving stress and lower levels of executive functioning at age 5. Executive functioning at age 5 was not related to internalizing behavior at age 5.

Greater caregiving stress at age 5 and 9, and lower SES at age 9, was predictive of greater internalizing behavior at age 9, controlling for internalizing behavior at age 5. Greater caregiving stress and lower executive functioning at age 5 was predictive of lower executive functioning at age 9. Lower executive functioning at age 9 in turn was related to lower levels of internalizing behavior at age 9. Greater internalizing behavior at age 5 was predictive of greater caregiving stress at age 9 and lower SES at age 9 was related to higher caregiving stress at age 9. Not surprisingly, greater caregiving stress at age 5 was associated with greater caregiving stress at age 9, and higher SES at age 5 was predictive of higher SES at age 9.

We next turn to our age 13 outcomes. African-Americans, compared to non African Americans, experienced greater internalizing behavior at age 13. Greater caregiving stress and lower levels of executive functioning at age 9 were related to greater internalizing behavior at age 13. Greater caregiving stress at age 9 was related to greater caregiving stress at age 13. Unexpectedly, greater internalizing behavior at age 9 were associated with greater executive functioning at age 13 years. Greater internalizing behavior at age 13 was related to higher caregiving stress at age 13 and lower SES at age 13. Lower SES at age 13 was related to greater caregiving stress and lower executive functioning at age 13. High SES at age 9 was predictive of high SES at age 13, and high executive functioning at age 9 was predictive of high executive functioning at age 13.

Given our interest in how resilience processes may alter pathways leading to problem behavior, we tested whether executive functioning may mediate effects of caregiving stress and/or internalizing behavior on the expression of internalizing behavior at 13. Since higher caregiving stress at age 5 was related to lower executive functioning at age 9, and lower executive functioning at age 9 was predictive of greater internalizing behavior at age 13, we tested whether executive functioning at age 9 mediated the relation between caregiving stress at age 5 on internalizing behavior age 13 using the sobel test in Mplus. The indirect effect was not significant (p = .10).

Externalizing behavior

As with internalizing behavior, higher levels of prenatal drug exposure were related to greater externalizing behavior at age 5 and 9, but not at age 13. Higher caregiving stress and lower SES at age 5 was related to greater externalizing behavior at age 5.

African Americans experienced significantly greater externalizing behavior at age 9 compared to non African-Americans. Greater externalizing behavior at age 5 was predictive of greater caregiving stress at age 9. Consistent with our hypotheses, greater caregiving stress at ages 5 and 9 were related to greater externalizing behavior at age 9. Greater executive functioning at age 9 was associated with lower externalizing behavior at age 9.

Higher caregiving stress at age 9 was predictive of greater externalizing behavior at age 13. Higher externalizing behavior at age 9 was also predictive of higher caregiving stress and lower executive functioning at age 13. Lower SES and higher caregiving stress at age 13 was associated with greater externalizing behavior at age 13. None of the models tested with executive functioning at age 9 as a mediator were significant.

Discussion

Our goal was to evaluate transactions between caregiving stress and executive functioning across three developmental periods to highlight pathways leading to internalizing and externalizing behavior in early adolescence. We capitalized on a rich dataset of 1,388 children with prenatal substance exposure to model our constructs across three sensitive developmental periods: early childhood (birth to age 5), middle childhood (age 6 to age 9), and early adolescence (age 10 to age 13). Our findings are strengthened by our inclusion of internalizing and externalizing behavior at earlier developmental periods so that we can better understand what contributes to psychopathology in early adolescence, above and beyond the expression of symptoms in early childhood. Our study is unique in its inclusion of executive functioning, which is gaining traction as a possible resilience factor for the emergence of psychopathology, but which is rarely included in longitudinal, transactional models of development. We highlight these results in the context of a development and psychopathology theoretical framework and suggest directions for future research below.

Exposure to substances prenatally was predictive of internalizing and externalizing behavior at age 5 and 9, but not at age 13. It may be by age 13, children can compensate for the effects of prenatal substance exposure, which tend to target neurobiological systems of reactivity and self-regulation, (Conradt et al., 2013) through behavioral self-regulation, and/or improved cognitive control, given that prenatal drug exposure was not predictive of executive functioning at age 5, 9, or 13. It could also be that individual differences in pubertal timing, which may be influenced by prenatal drug exposure, is a stronger predictor of problem behavior in adolescence than is prenatal drug exposure (Fried, James, & Watkinson, 2001). In other words the effects of prenatal drug exposure on internalizing and externalizing behavior could depend on pubertal development, a hypothesis to be explored in future studies.

Transactional relations differed between caregiving stress and internalizing, versus externalizing behavior. Consistent with hypotheses and with Patterson’s coercion theory, within each age, greater caregiving stress was associated with greater internalizing and externalizing behavior. These associations were also consistent longitudinally, as age 5 externalizing behavior and caregiving stress predicted age 9 caregiving stress and externalizing behavior, which both predicted age 13 externalizing behavior and caregiving stress. In short, it is impossible, in the absence of intervention studies, to disentangle the directional effects of caregiving stress and externalizing behavior; they appear bidirectional with each one impacting the other across time. These results are not surprising, given that both child internalizing and externalizing behavior may elicit negative parenting responses and increase parenting stress, (Reitz, Dekovic & Meijer, 2006) and that psychopathology and parenting stress can lead to increases in internalizing and externalizing behavior (Eisenberg et al., 1999; Stormshak, Bierman, McMahon, & Lengua, 2000). This finding suggests it is important to target parental stress and psychopathology as well as child externalizing behavior, with the ultimate goal of reducing internalizing behavior in children.

Different associations emerged over time for internalizing behavior and caregiving stress. While higher internalizing behavior was related to higher caregiving stress within each age, internalizing behavior at age 9 was not predictive of caregiving stress at age 13, though caregiving stress at age 9 was predictive of greater internalizing behavior at age 13. This finding was unexpected and suggests that caregiver psychopathology in middle childhood may have a stronger association with the expression of internalizing behavior in adolescence than internalizing behavior in middle childhood has on the expression of caregiver psychopathology. Children with higher levels of internalizing behavior in middle childhood may present as more withdrawn and less likely to elicit stressful caregiving interactions, which in turn may make it less likely that caregivers experience stress in the form of elevated psychopathology in the future. Indeed the strongest evidence for coercive child-parent interactions is the presence of an externalizing or oppositional behavior (Stoolmiller, 2001), though more research is needed to tease apart whether coercive processes differ for children who present with primarily internalizing, as opposed to externalizing, behavior.

Unique to this study was our longitudinal examination of transactions between caregiving stress, executive functioning, and internalizing and externalizing behavior. We first turn to associations between caregiving stress and executive functioning. Within each age, caregiving stress and executive functioning were not associated with each other. However, caregiving stress at age 5prospectively predicted executive functioning at age 9, whereas child executive functioning did not prospectively predict caregiving stress. In contrast to the hypothesized bidirectional effects, these findings suggest there may be a direction of effect from caregiving stress to executive functioning. These results support earlier work indicating that the quality of the parenting environment may shape neural networks involved in executive functioning (Blair, Raver, Berry, & Family Life Project Investigators, 2014). However, in the absence of an intervention study we cannot claim causality.

Regarding the effects of executive functioning on problem behavior outcomes, at age 9 there was an association between executive functioning and concurrent internalizing and externalizing behavior. However, these associations were only found at age 9, and not at age 5 or 13. It is unclear why executive functioning would be more strongly associated with problem behavior in middle childhood, as opposed to early childhood or adolescence. A recent review indicates that there is a critical period for the development of cognitive flexibility, goal setting, and information processing between the ages of 7–9 years, with maturation in these areas by age 12 (Anderson, 2010). Perhaps during this putative sensitive period, cognitive systems are more susceptible to the extreme emotional experiences that accompany the presentation of internalizing and externalizing behavior.

Higher internalizing, but lower externalizing symptoms at age 9 predicted higher executive functioning skills at age 13. This finding with internalizing symptoms was unexpected given that executive functioning impairments are implicated in anxiety and depression and related to rumination and difficulties with cognitive reappraisal (Chambers, Lo, & Allen, 2008; Whitmer & Banich, 2007; Hofmann et al., 2011); however, concurrent internalizing symptoms at age 9 remained negatively related to executive functioning as expected. Some prior longitudinal research suggests that improvement in internalizing symptoms with age is associated with improvement in executive functioning (Eisenberg et al., 2009), and the current results may be explained by a group of children showing this pattern of co-occurring improvement in cognition and symptoms over time. Future studies could apply parallel process or cross-lag models, which can be applied to infer causality, to test this hypothesis.

Alternatively, internalizing symptoms (particularly when differentiated from any co-occurring externalizing symptoms) may be associated with higher levels of self-restraint and compliance (Friedman, Miyake, Robinson, & Hewitt, 2011; Murray & Kochanska, 2002) and attentional control theory predicts improved cognitive performance for individuals with anxiety under some conditions due to greater effort and utilization of processing resources (Eysenck, Derakshan, Santos, & Calvo, 2007), which may also account for the observed relationships. Additional longitudinal studies examining these complex transactional relationships will be required to confirm and extend our findings.

The remaining relationships between executive functioning and internalizing behavior at age 9 were as expected. At age 9, lower executive functioning was associated with higher concurrent internalizing symptoms and higher internalizing symptoms at age 13. Internalizing disorders have been associated with low effortful control (Nigg, 2006; Bridgett et al., 2013; Joormann & Gotlib, 2008; Williams et al., 1999), an aspect of temperament strongly associated with executive functioning (Zhou, Chen & Main, 2012). It is hypothesized that poor executive functioning may make it difficult for children prone to internalizing problems to inhibit stimuli that may make them anxious or fearful (Nigg, 2006), thus exacerbating their internalizing symptoms. Support for this hypothesis comes from our findings linking caregiving stress, executive functioning, and internalizing behavior. High caregiving stress at age 5 was predictive of both greater internalizing behavior and poorer executive functioning at age 9, though the indirect effect of executive functioning was not significant.

A growing literature suggests that the quality of the caregiving environment is related to executive functioning, particularly in early childhood (Blair, Raver, Berry, & Family Life Project Investigators, 2014). The quality of the caregiver-child relationship may be compromised when caregivers experience stress and psychopathology (Goodman & Gotlib, 1999). Indeed, children whose mothers have major depressive disorder exhibit poorer executive functioning as adolescents (Klimes-Dougan, Lee, Ronsaville, & Martinez, 2008). It may be that caregivers’ own stress related to parenting or psychopathology may make it difficult to act as an external source of regulation for their young children. As a result, these children may find it more difficult to control impulses or exhibit cognitive flexibility, or even inhibit attention to threatening or fearful stimuli, an attention bias they may be more prone to by virtue of having a caregiver with psychopathology (Bridgett, Burt, Laake, & Oddi, 2013; Nigg, 2006).

Our interests were in caregiving stress as a primary predictor of executive functioning, internalizing, and externalizing behavior, controlling for SES, since SES is not directly causal. Rather, it is a proxy for unmeasured variables that exert direct effects such as parental mental health, postnatal drug exposure, and neighborhood violence and criminality (Brooks-Gunn & Duncan, 1997). Given the nature of our sample, we controlled for SES in our models. It was not surprising that low SES was related to poorer executive functioning at age 5 and 13 (Kishiyama et al., 2009). Research designed to model the impact of SES on executive functioning across development is needed to understand why such an association did not emerge at age 9, particularly since both internalizing and externalizing behavior were related to executive functioning, but only at age 9.

Limitations and implications for the development of evidence-based interventions

We recognize that we discuss some of these processes in causal language given that we predict internalizing and externalizing behavior, over and above previous symptom levels. However, we cannot infer direction of effect with these data in the absence of intervention studies, which could help disentangle cause-and-effect. Due to the developmental nature of this study, we were also not able to employ the same executive functioning tasks at each age; our age 5 tasks differed from our age 9 and 13 tasks (which were the same). However, all tasks are well-validated and chosen based on theory and available evidence indicating they should be prognostic of later internalizing and externalizing behavior. There is also a substantial body of evidence suggesting that some children may be more susceptible to the effects of caregiving stress than other children due to their psychophysiological make-up. It will be important to include physiological indicators as possible moderators of links between caregiving stress, executive functioning, and psychopathology.

We also examined the effects of multiple drugs on internalizing and externalizing behavior rather than a single drug in isolation. Polydrug use during pregnancy is more common than the use of one specific substance and the cumulative effects of multiple drugs could have a more substantial effect than that of a single substance (Lester et al., 2009). At the same time we recognize that some substances are more harmful than others, and the mechanism of action of these drugs differ. This is a very complex issue involving many factors, such as when during pregnancy substances were used, how frequent was the use, and how large of a dose was taken, to name a few.

Finally, all children in this sample are high-risk due to prenatal substance exposure and poverty. Pathways from caregiving stress to executive functioning and psychopathology may differ from children with less stress exposure in their neighborhood or schools. It will also be important in future work to include additional measures of executive functioning that may have more external validity, such as parent-report measures of everyday behavior that is associated with problems in executive functioning. Given that effect sizes for executive functioning associations were low in the internalizing model and there were few associations with executive functioning in the externalizing model, it will be important to incorporate more externally valid markers of executive functioning, as these measures may reveal additional pathways to psychopathology.

More research investigating the utility of targeting executive functioning in evidence-based interventions for children with prenatal substance exposure is needed. This process may be particularly important for children presenting with internalizing behavior in early childhood, since executive functioning at age 5 was predictive of executive functioning at age 9, which in turn predicted internalizing behavior in early adolescence. Further research investigating the efficacy of treating caregiving psychopathology, depression, and parenting stress in early childhood (before age 5) may also prove fruitful, as this variable predicted both executive functioning at age 9 and later internalizing behavior at age 13. Given that current interventions for children presenting with internalizing behavior focus less on caregiving stress, tailoring interventions with drug exposed children and youth to focus on the family, rather than the individual, may be warranted.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by NIH grants: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) Neonatal Research Network and an inter-institute agreement with the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) through cooperative agreements: U10-DA-024117-01 and U10-HD-21385 (to S.S.), I10-DA-024128-06 and U10-HD-2786 (to H.S.B.), U10-DA-024119-01 and U10-HD-27904 (to B.M.L.), and U10-DA-024118-01 and U10-HD-21397 (to C.R.B.); and NICHD contract N01-HD-2-3159 (Dr. Lester).

References

  1. Abidin R. The Parenting Stress Index Manual. Charlottesville, VA: Pediatric Psychology Press; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  2. Achenbach T. Integrative guide for the 1991 CBCL/4-18, YSR, and TRF Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  3. Anderson VA, Anderson P, Northam E, Jacobs R, Catroppa C. Development of executive functions through late childhood and adolescence in an Australian sample. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2001;20(1):385–406. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN2001_5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bagner DM, Sheinkopf SJ, Miller-Loncar C, LaGasse LL, Lester BM, Liu J, Bauer CR, Shankaran S, Bada H, Das A. The effect of parenting stress on child behavior problems in high-risk children with prenatal drug exposure. Child Psychiatry & Human Development. 2009;40(1):73–84. doi: 10.1007/s10578-008-0109-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bauer CR, Langer JC, Shankaran S, Bada HS, Lester B, Wright LL, Krause-Steinrauf H, Smeriglio VL, Finnegan LP, Maza PL, Verter J. Acute neonatal effects of cocaine exposure during pregnancy. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine. 2005;159(9):824–34. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.159.9.824. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bauer CR, Shankaran S, Bada HS, Lester B, Wright LL, Krause-Steinrauf H, Smeriglio VL, Finnegan LP, Maza PL, Verter J. The Maternal Lifestyle Study: drug exposure during pregnancy and short-term maternal outcomes. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology. 2002;186(3):487–95. doi: 10.1067/mob.2002.121073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Beck A, Steer R, Brown G. Beck Depression Inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  8. Biederman J, Monuteaux MS, Doyble AE, Seidman LJ, Wilens TE, Ferrero F, Morgan CL, Faraone SV. Impact of executive function deficits and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on academic outcomes in children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2004;72(5):757–66. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.5.757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Blair C, Raver CC, Berry DJ Family Life Project Investigators. Two approaches to estimating the effect of parenting on the development of executive function in early childhood. Developmental Psychology. 2014;50(2):554–65. doi: 10.1037/a0033647. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bridgett DJ, Burt NM, Laake LM, Oddi KB. Maternal self-regulation, relationship adjustment, and home chaos; contributions to infant negative emotionality. Infant Behavior and Development. 2013;36(4):534–47. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bridgett DJ, Oddi KB, Laake LM, Murdock KW, Bachmann MN. Integrating and differentiating aspects of self-regulation: effortful control, executive functioning, and links to negative affectivity. Emotion. 2013;13(1):47–63. doi: 10.1037/a0029536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Brooks-Gunn J, Duncan GJ. The effects of poverty on children. Future Child. 1997;7(2):55–71. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Brown CH, Wang W, Kellam SG, Methen BO, Petras H, Toyinbo P, Poduska J, Ialongo N, Wyman PA, Chamberlain P, Slobada Z, Mackinnon DP, Windham A Prevention Science and Methodology Group. Methods for testing theory and evaluating impact in randomized field trials: intent-to-treat analyses for integrating the perspectives of person, place and time. Drug and Alchol Dependence. 2008;95(Supplement 1):S74–S104. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.11.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Calkins SD, Blandon AY, Williford AP, Keane SP. Biological, behavioral, and relational levels of resilience in the context of risk for early childhood behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology. 2007;19(3):675–700. doi: 10.1017/S095457940700034X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Carlson SM. Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2005;28(2):595–616. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Carlson SM, Moses LJ. Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development. 2001;72(4):1032–53. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Chambers R, Lo BCY, Allen NB. The impact of intensive mindfulness training on attentional control, cognitive style, and affect. Cognitive therapy and research. 2008;32(3):303–22. [Google Scholar]
  18. Choe DE, Olson SL, Sameroff AJ. Effects of early maternal distress and parenting on the development of children’s self-regulation and externalizing behavior. Development and Psychopathology. 2013;25(2):437–53. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412001162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Combs-Ronto LA, Olson SL, Lunkenheimer ES, Sameroff AJ. Interactions between maternal parenting and children’s early disruptive behavior: bidirectional associations across the transition from preschool to school entry. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2009;37(8):1151–63. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9332-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. A developmental and clinical model for the prevention of conduct disorder: The FAST Track Program. Development and Psychopathology. 1992;4(4):509–27. [Google Scholar]
  21. Conradt E, Sheinkopf SJ, Lester BM, Tronick E, LaGasse LL, Shankaran S, Bada H, Bauer CR, Whitaker TM, Hammond JA Maternal Lifestyle S. Prenatal substance exposure: neurobiologic organization at 1 month. The Journal of Pediatrics. 2013;163(4):989–94. e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2013.04.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Cragg L, Nation K. Shifting development in mid-childhood: The influence of between-task interference. Developmental Psychology. 2009;45(5):1465–79. doi: 10.1037/a0015360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Crone EA, Wendelken C, Donohue S, van Leijenhorst L, Bunge SA. Neurocognitive development of the ability to manipulate information in working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2006;103(24):9315–20. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0510088103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. De Luca CR, Wood SJ, Anderson V, Buchanan JA, Proffitt TM, Mahony K, Pantelis C. Normative data from the CANTAB. I: development of executive function over the lifespan. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2003;25(2):242–54. doi: 10.1076/jcen.25.2.242.13639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Derogatis L. Brief Symptom Inventory(BSI): Administration, Scoring and Procedures Manual, Third Edition. Minneapolis, MN: National Computer Systems, Inc; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  26. Derryberry D. Attention and voluntary self-control. Self and Identity. 2002;1(2):105–111. [Google Scholar]
  27. Dishion TJ, Brennan LM, Shaw DS, McEachern AD, Wilson MN, Jo B. Prevention of problem behavior through annual family check-ups in early childhood: intervention effects from home to early elementary school. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2014;42(3):343–54. doi: 10.1007/s10802-013-9768-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Dishion TJ, Patterson GR, Kavanagh K. An experimental test of the coercion model: Linking theory, measurement, and intervention. In: McCord J, Tremblay R, editors. Preventing antisocial behavior: Interventions from birth through adolescence. New York, NY: Guilford; 1992. pp. 253–282. [Google Scholar]
  29. Eigsti IM, Zayas V, Mischel W, Shoda Y, Ayduk O, Dadlani MB, Davidson MC, Lawrence Aber J, Casey BJ. Predicting cognitive control from preschool to late adolescence and young adulthood. Psychological Science. 2006;17(6):478–84. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01732.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Eisenberg N, Fabes RA, Shepard SA, Guthrie IK, Murphy BC, Reiser M. Parental reactions to children’s negative emotions: longitudinal relations to quality of children’s social functioning. Child Development. 1999;70(2):513–34. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Eisenberg N, Valiente C, Spinrad TL, Liew J, Zhou Q, Losoya SH, Reiser M, Cumberland A. Longitudinal relations of children’s effortful control, impulsivity, and negative emotionality to their externalizing, internalizing, and co-occurring behavior problems. Developmental Psychology. 2009;45(4):988–1008. doi: 10.1037/a0016213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Eysenck MW, Derakshan N, Santos R, Calvo MG. Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion. 2007;7(2):336–53. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Fanti KA, Henrich CC. Trajectories of pure and co-occurring internalizing and externalizing problems from age 2 to age 12: findings from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care. Developmental Psychology. 2010;46(5):1159–75. doi: 10.1037/a0020659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Fisher PA, Lester BM, DeGarmo DS, Lagasse LL, Lin H, Shankaran S, Bada HS, Bauer CR, Hammond J, Whitaker T, Higgins R. The combined effects of prenatal drug exposure and early adversity on neurobehavioral disinhibition in childhood and adolescence. Development and Psychopathology. 2011;23(3):777–88. doi: 10.1017/S0954579411000290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Fray P, Robbins T, Sahakian B. Neuorpsychiatyric applications of CANTAB. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 1996;11:329–36. [Google Scholar]
  36. Friedman NP, Miyake A, Robinson JL, Hewitt JK. Developmental trajectories in toddlers’ self-restraint predict individual differences in executive functions 14 years later: a behavioral genetic analysis. Developmental Psychology. 2011;47(5):1410–30. doi: 10.1037/a0023750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Garon N, Bryson SE, Smith IM. Executive function in preschoolers: a review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin. 2008;134(1):31–60. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Goodman SH, Gotlib IH. Risk for psychopathology in the children of depressed mothers: a developmental model for understanding mechanisms of transmission. Psychological Review. 1999;106(3):458–90. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.106.3.458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Granic I, Patterson GR. Toward a comprehensive model of antisocial development: A dynamic systems approach. Psychological Review. 2006;113(1):101–131. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.113.1.101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Green MF, Nuechterlein KH, Gold JM, Barch DM, Cohen J, Essock S, Fenton WS, Frese F, Goldberg TE, Heaton RKL, Kerefe RS, Kern RS, Kraemer H, Stover E, Weinberger DR, Zalcman S, Marder SR. Approaching a consensus cognitive battery for clinical trials in schizophrneia: the NIMH-Matrics conferences to select cognitive domains and test criteria. Biological Psychiatry. 2004;56(5):201–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.06.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Hofmann W, Friese M, Schmeichel BJ, Baddeley AD. Working memory and self-regulation. In: Vohs KD, Baumeister RF, editors. Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications. 2. New York, NY, US: Guilford Press; 2011. pp. 204–225. [Google Scholar]
  42. Hollingshead AB. Four Factor Index of Social Status. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press; 1975. [Google Scholar]
  43. Hughes C, Ensor R. Individual differences in growth in executive function across the transition to school predict externalizing and internalizing behaviors and self-perceived academic success at 6 years of age. Journal of Experimental & Child Psychology. 2011;108(3):663–76. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Joormann J, Gotlib IH. Updating the contents of working memory in depression: interference from irrelevant negative material. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2008;117(1):182–92. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.117.1.182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Kalkut EL, Han SD, Lansing AE, Holdnack JA, Delis DC. Development of set-shifting ability from late childhood through early adulthood. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2009;24(6):565–74. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acp048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kendler KS, Neale MC. Endophenotype: a conceptual analysis. Molecular Psychiatry. 2010;15(8):789–97. doi: 10.1038/mp.2010.8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kishiyama MM, Boyce WT, Jimenez AM, Perry LM, Knight RT. Socioeconomic disparities affect prefrontal function in children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009;21(6):1106–15. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Klimes-Dougan B, Lee CY, Ronsaville D, Martinez P. Suicidal risk in young adult offspring of mothers with bipolar or major depressive disorder: a longitudinal family risk study. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2008;64(4):531–40. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Kochanska G. Mutually responsive orientation between mothers and their young children: implications for early socialization. Child Development. 1997;68(1):94–112. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Kochanska G, Murray K, Jacques TY, Koenig AL, Vandegeest KA. Inhibitory control in young children and its role in emerging internalization. Child Development. 1996;67(2):490–507. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Kochanska G, Murray KT, Harlan ET. Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology. 2000;36(2):220–32. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Kuntsi J, Neale BM, Chen W, Faraone SV, Asherson P. The IMAGE project:methodological issues for the molecuraly genetic analysis of ADHD. Behavioral Brain Functions. 2006;2:27. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-2-27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. LaGasse L, Seifer R, Wright L, Lester B, Tronick E, Bauer C, Shankaran S, Bada H, Smeriglio V. The Maternal Lifestyle Study (MLS): The caretaking environment of infants exposed to cocaine/opiates. Pediatric Research. 1999;45:247A. [Google Scholar]
  54. Leark R, Dupuy T, Greenberg L, Corman C, Kindschi C. T.O.V.A., Professional Manual, Version 7.0. LosAlamitos, CA: Universal Attention Disorders, Inc; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  55. Lee K, Bull R, Ho RM. Developmental changes in executive functioning. Child Development. 2013;84(6):1933–53. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Lehto J, Juujärvi P, Kooistra L, Pulkkinen L. Dimensions of executive functioning: Evidence from children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2003;21(1):59–80. [Google Scholar]
  57. Lester B, Bada H, Bauer C, Shankaran S, Whitaker T, LaGasse L, Hammond J. Maternal Lifestyle Study in four sites in the United States, 1993–2011. Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research; 2014. ICPSR34312-v2. [distributor], http://doi.org/10.3886/ICPSR34312.v2. [Google Scholar]
  58. Lester B, Bagner D, Liu J, LaGasse L, Seifer R, Bauer C, Shankaran S, Bada H, Higgins R, Das A. Infant neurobehavioral dysregulation: Behavior problems in children with prenatal substance exposure. Pediatrics. 2009;124:1354–61. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-2898. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Little RJA. A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 1998;83(404):1198–1202. [Google Scholar]
  60. Loe IM, Feldman HM, Huffman LC. Executive function mediates effects of gestational age on functional outcomes and behavior in preschoolers. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 2014;35(5):323–33. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Luciana M, Conklin HM, Hooper CJ, Yarger RS. The development of nonverbal working memory and executive control processes in adolescents. Child Development. 2005;76(3):697–712. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00872.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Martel MM, Nigg JT, Wong MM, Fitzgerald HE, Jester JM, Puttler LI, Glass JM, Adams KM, Zucker RA. Childhood and adolescent resiliency, regulation, and executive functioning in relation to adolescent problems and competence in a high-risk sample. Development and Psychopathology. 2007;19(2):541–63. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407070265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Matthys W, Vanderschuren LJ, Schutter D. The neurobiology of oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder: altered functioning in three mental domains. Development and Psychopathology. 2013;25(01):193–207. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412000272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. McDonald RP, Ho MH. Principles and practice in reporting structural equation analyses. Psycholological Methods. 2002;7(1):64–82. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.7.1.64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Messinger D, Lester BM. Prenatal substance exposure and human development. In: Fogel A, Shanker S, editors. Human Development in the 21st Century: Visionary Policy Ideas from Systems Scientists. Council on Human Development; Bethesday, MD: 2005. Feb 12, [Google Scholar]
  66. Miller M, Hinshaw SP. Does childhood executive function predict adolescent functional outcomes in girls with ADHD? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2010;38(3):315–26. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9369-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Miller M, Loya F, Hinshaw WP. Executive functions in girls with and without childhood ADHD: developmental trajectories and associations with symptom change. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2013;54(9):1005–15. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology. 2000;41(1):49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Murray KT, Kochanska G. Effortful control: Factor structure and relation to externalizing and internalizing behaviors. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2002;30(5):503–14. doi: 10.1023/a:1019821031523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Muthen B, Muthen L. Mplus Users Guide. 7. Los Angeles, CA: Muthen & Muthen; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  71. Nigg JT. Temperament and developmental psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47(3–4):395–422. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01612.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Nigg JT, Quamma JP, Greenberg MT, Kusche CA. A two-year longitudinal study of neuropsychological and cognitive performance in relation to behavioral problems and competencies in elementary school children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1999;27(1):51–63. doi: 10.1023/a:1022614407893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Ochsner KN, Gross JJ. The cognitive control of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2005;9(5):242–49. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Ochsner KN, Silvers JA, Buhle JT. Functional imaging studies of emotion regulation: a synthetic review and evolving model of the cognitive control of emotion. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2012;1251(1):E1–E24. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06751.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Ornstein TJ, Iddon JL, Baldacchino AM, Sahakian BJ, London M, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Profiles of cognitive dysfunction in chronic amphetamine and heroin abusers. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000;23(2):113–26. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00097-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Petersen SE, Posner MI. The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2012;35:73–89. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Reitz E, Dekovic M, Meijer AM. Relations between parenting and externalizing and internalizing problem behaviour in early adolescence: child behaviour as moderator and predictor. Journal of Adolescence. 2006;29(3):419–36. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Rhoades BL, Greenberg MT, Domitrovich CE. The contribution of inhibitory control to preschoolers’ social–emotional competence. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2009;30(3):310–20. [Google Scholar]
  79. Romer D, Betancourt L, Giannetta J, Broadsky N, Farah M, Hurt H. Executive cognitive functions and impulsivity as correlates of risk taking and problem behavior in preadolsecents. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(13):2916–26. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Rothbart MK, Sheese BE, Rueda MR, Posner MI. Developing mechanisms of self-regulation in early life. Emotion Review. 2011;3(2):207–13. doi: 10.1177/1754073910387943. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Rueda MR, Posner MI, Rothbart MK. The development of executive attention: contributions to the emergence of self-regulation. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2005;28(2):573–94. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Sahker E, McCabe JE, Arndt S. Differences in successful treatment completion among pregnant and non-pregnant American women. Archives of Women’s Mental Health. 2015 Apr 1; doi: 10.1007/s00737-015-0520-5. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Schoemaker K, Mulder H, Dekovic M, Matthys W. Executive functions in preschool children with externalizing behavior problems: a meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2013;41(3):457–71. doi: 10.1007/s10802-012-9684-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Shankaran S, Bauer C, Bada H, Lester B, Wright L, Katsikiotis V. Maternal Lifestyle Study: Patterns of cocaine use in term pregnancy and effect on birth weight. Pediatric Research. 1996;39:279A. [Google Scholar]
  85. Shing YL, Lindenberger U, Diamond A, Li SC, Davidson MC. Memory maintenance and inhibitory control differentiate from early childhood to adolescence. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2010;35(6):679–97. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2010.508546. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Stormshak EA, Bierman KL, McMahon RJ, Lengua LJ. Parenting practices and child disruptive behavior problems in early elementary school. Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 2000;29(1):17–29. doi: 10.1207/S15374424jccp2901_3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Sulik MJ, Blair C, Mills-Koonce R, Berry D, Greenberg M Family Life Project I. Early parenting and the development of externalizing behavior problems: longitudinal mediation through children’s executive function. Child Development. 2015;86(5):1588–603. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Thorell LB. Do delay aversion and executive function deficits make distinct contributions to the functional impact of ADHD symptoms? A study of early academic skill deficits. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;48(11):1061–1070. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01777.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Wagner NJ, Propper C, Gueron-Sela N, Mills-Koonce WR. Dimensions of maternal parenting and infants’ autonomic functioning interactively predict early internalizing behavior problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2015 Jun 11; doi: 10.1007/s10802-015-0039-2. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Whitmer AJ, Banich MT. Inhibition versus switching deficits in different forms of rumination. Psychological Science. 2007;18(6):546–53. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01936.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Willams BR, Ponesse JS, Schachar RJ, Logan GD, Tannock R. Development of inhibitory control across the life span. Developmental Psychology. 1999;35(1):205–13. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.35.1.205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Williams PG, Suchy Y, Kraybill ML. Five-factor model personality traits and executive functioning among older adults. Journal of Research in Personality. 2010;44(4):485–91. [Google Scholar]
  93. Zelazo PD. The development of conscious control in childhood. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2004;8(1):12–7. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Zelazo PD, Muller U, Frye D, Marcovitch S. The development of executive function. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2003;68(3):11–27. doi: 10.1111/j.0037-976x.2003.00260.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Zhou Q, Chen S, Main A. Commonalities and differences in the research on children’s effortful control and executive function: A call for an integrated model of self-regulation. Child Development Perspectives. 2012;6:112–21. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES