Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: Psychol Med. 2016 Jan 8;46(6):1265–1275. doi: 10.1017/S0033291715002792

Taxometric evidence of a dimensional latent structure for depression in an epidemiological sample of children and adolescents

Richard T Liu 1
PMCID: PMC4958619  NIHMSID: NIHMS802398  PMID: 26744249

Abstract

Background

A basic phenomenological question of much theoretical and empirical interest is whether the latent structure of depression is dimensional or categorical in nature. Prior taxometric studies of youth depression have yielded mixed findings. In a step towards resolving these contradictory findings, the current taxometric investigation is the first to utilize a recently developed objective index, the comparison curve fit index (CCFI), to evaluate the latent structure of major depression in an epidemiological sample of children and adolescents.

Methods

Data were derived from Mental Health of Children and Young People in Great Britain. Participants were administered a structured diagnostic interview to assess for current depression. Parents (n = 683) were interviewed for children ages 5–16, and child interviews (n = 605) were conducted for those ages 11–16.

Results

MAMBAC (mean above minus below a cut), MAXEIG (maximum eigenvalue), and L-Mode (latent mode) analyses provided convergent support for a dimensional latent structure.

Conclusions

The current findings suggest that depression in youth is more accurately conceptualized as a continuous syndrome rather than a discrete diagnostic entity.

Keywords: classification, continuity, depression, latent structure, taxometrics


A fundamental nosological question of direct relevance to the theory, empirical study, and treatment of depression is whether this disorder is a qualitatively distinct syndrome (i.e., a taxon) or whether it possesses a dimensional latent structure, existing along a continuum of severity. Underlying current classification systems of mental disorders, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 5th ed. (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013) and the International Statistical Classification of Diseases, 10th revision (ICD-10; World Health Organization, 1992), is the view that psychopathology is best assessed in a categorical manner, essentially as phenomenological entities made distinct from normality by the presence and severity of their individual constellation of symptoms. In contrast to this position, however, several theorists have contended that mental illnesses are more accurately assessed as dimensional phenomena, with differences between individuals being a matter of degree rather than of type (Flett et al. 1997; Widiger & Samuel 2005; Helzer et al. 2006; Widiger & Edmundson 2014). Empirical research is required directly to delineate the latent structure of depression, and thereby to inform the ongoing theoretical debate regarding the nosology of this disorder (Sonuga-Barke 1998; Beauchaine 2003).

There is a growing corpus of empirical studies providing evidence consistent with the view that diagnostic models of depression, at least as currently defined, are phenomenologically too restrictive, potentially leading to the omission of less severe, yet still clinically significant, manifestations of this disorder (i.e., false negatives; for reviews in this area, see Flett et al. 1997; Solomon et al. 2001; Wesselhoeft et al. 2013). Subthreshold depression in children and adolescents, for example, appears to share common risk factors and similarly poor clinical outcomes with major depression (Gotlib et al. 1995; Wesselhoeft et al. 2013). Furthermore, subthreshold symptoms of depression in adolescence have been found prospectively to predict first lifetime onset of major depression in early adulthood (Klein et al. 2013) as well as recurrent major depression (Pettit et al. 2013).

Collectively, such findings have often been taken to be supportive evidence for a dimensional latent structure for depression (Nierenberg et al. 2010; Pietrzak et al. 2013; Wesselhoeft et al. 2013). This interpretation, however, may not be justified based on these findings alone. Although studies of subthreshold depression are important for evaluating the validity of the criterial threshold for this disorder under current diagnostic classification systems, they are incapable of directly informing our understanding of the latent structure of this construct. That is, these studies only indicate that the diagnostic criteria for depression, as defined in DSM-5 and ICD-10, likely exclude milder symptomatological presentations that are nonetheless associated with clinically meaningful distress and impairment. Several mutually exclusive possibilities exist that may account for these findings. First, it may be that depression is taxonic (i.e., categorical), but with a lower cutting point differentiating the taxon from its complement class than is featured in DSM-5 and ICD-10. Individuals with major and subthreshold depression may both potentially be members of the putative depression taxon, and non-depressed individuals its complement class. To the degree that individuals with subthreshold depression differ from both those with major depression and no depression, this may simply be a reflection of continuous variation within the putative taxon rather than evidence of a dimensional construct (for a detailed discussion of possible dimensionality within taxa, see Ruscio et al. 2006). An alternative possibility that is also consistent with the findings on subthreshold depression is that this disorder may indeed exist along a continuum of severity. A different empirical approach is therefore necessary to clarify the latent structure of this disorder.

One family of statistical procedures specifically developed to assess the taxonicity versus dimensionality of latent constructs is Meehl's (1995, 2004) taxometric methods. Among prior taxometric studies of depression in youth, three found evidence of taxonicity (Ambrosini et al. 2002; Solomon et al. 2006; Richey et al. 2009), whereas the remaining two reported findings more consistent with a dimensional solution (Whisman & Pinto 1997; Hankin et al. 2005). These studies are characterized by several limitations, however, which may bias their results toward taxonicity or dimensionality, and thereby complicate the interpretability of their findings. Most importantly, although the comparison curve fit index ([CCFI]; Ruscio & Kaczetow 2009; Ruscio et al. 2010) has been applied to taxometric studies in adults, no studies to date of childhood and adolescent depression have utilized the CCFI. This objective index of taxonicity is a recent development, as well as the most significant one thus far (Haslam et al. 2012), in the broader taxometric literature. The advantage of the CCFI lies in its ability objectively to differentiate between taxonic and dimensional data at a high level of accuracy and its robustness to a wide array of poor measurement conditions (Ruscio & Kaczetow 2009). Older studies relying on subjective interpretation based on visual inspection of taxometric graphical output are more vulnerable to spurious taxonic findings. Indeed, a recent quantitative review of the taxometric literature found evidence consistent with this possibility, studies using the CCFI being more likely to more likely to report dimensional findings than studies relying solely on visual inspection (Haslam et al. 2012).

Another threat to validity is inadequate sample size, with a minimum of 300 participants being generally recommended (Meehl 1995). Smaller samples, as was featured in two of the studies (Whisman & Pinto 1997, n = 160; Richey et al. 2009, nStudy 2 = 159), may result in unstable curves and a bias toward taxonicity (Ruscio et al. 2006). Alternatively, if too few cases of the putative taxon are included in a small sample, a bias toward dimensionality may result. Finally, a third concern relates to construct measurement. Specifically, several studies featured self-report measures of depression (e.g., Richey et al., 2009; Whisman & Pinto, 1997). Such measures may lead to spurious taxonic findings (Beauchaine & Waters 2003; Haslam et al. 2012). Still others have expressed concern that they may instead lend a dimensional bias (for a discussion of this issue, see Ruscio et al. 2009). Yet another study (Hankin et al. 2005) assessed for the presence of each depressive symptom at anytime over the past 12 months, regardless of whether it temporally overlapped with other symptoms included in the analyses. As mentioned previously by others (Solomon et al. 2006), such an approach may challenge the ability of the study instrument accurately to reflect the construct it was intended to measure, as temporally disconnected symptoms are unlikely to reflect the same underlying syndrome.

The present study aimed to bridge the gaps in the literature in several important ways. In addition to utilizing the CCFI objectively to evaluate data fit with taxonic and dimensional models, this study assessed the latent structure of depression in an epidemiological sample of children and adolescents, thereby addressing sample selection and sample size issues of past studies. The current investigation also used a structured clinical interview of DSM-IV major depression and related distress and impairment, the Development and Well-Being Assessment (DAWBA; Goodman et al. 2000).1 It thus addresses the stated need for research in this area using well-validated structured interviews in large representative samples (Solomon et al. 2001). Importantly, an advantage of the DAWBA as employed in the current study is that it exclusively assessed for current depression (i.e., four-week prevalence), in contrast to previous research involving clinical interviews assessing depressive symptoms over longer intervals. This point is notable, given sizeable differences observed between prospectively and retrospectively recalled rates of psychiatric disorders and symptoms (Wells & Horwood 2004; Moffitt et al. 2010; Copeland et al. 2011), related concerns about the validity of recall over long time intervals (Tanur 1992), a significant fall-off in recall of major depression over a 12-month period (Rogler et al. 1992), and evidence that reporting of psychiatric symptoms appears most reliable for the past four weeks (Goldberg 1972).2 Such concerns have led some to recommend focusing diagnostic assessments on the most recent one-month period (Shaffer et al. 1996).

Another feature unique to the present investigation is the inclusion of psychological distress and impairment items in the analyses. That impairment and distress have yet to be incorporated in taxometric studies of child and adolescent depression is important in as much as they are required fully to evaluate the latent structure of major depression (i.e., content validity). Functional impairment related to psychopathology in children has been found to predict severe emotional illness later in adolescence (Costello et al. 1999). In depressed children and adolescents, level of impairment is also positively associated with likelihood of seeking mental health treatment (Wu et al. 2001).

In summary, the current study conducted taxometric analyses of depression in an epidemiological sample of children and adolescents. An empirically validated structured diagnostic interview was used to assess parent- and child-reported DSM-IV depressive symptoms and related psychological distress and functional impairment over the past four-week period. Finally, to resolve the mixed findings in the literature on whether depression in children and adolescents better fits with a categorical or dimensional model, a relatively recently developed objective taxometric aid, the CCFI, was applied in the current study.

Method

Participants

The study sample was drawn from the 1999 and 2004 surveys of Mental Health of Children and Young People in Great Britain (Meltzer et al. 2000; Ford et al. 2003; Green et al. 2005). These national surveys were conducted with children and adolescents living in private households in England, Scotland, and Wales. Child diagnostic interviews were conducted with those of ages 11 to 16, and interviews with a parent were obtained for children of ages of ages 5 to 16. Children below age 11 were not administered diagnostic interviews on the grounds that prior research has found poor reliability in reporting of symptoms among children of this age (Fallon & Schwab-Stone 1994; Schwab-Stone et al. 1996). A total of 18,415 (unweighted) families participated in the surveys.

Consistent with standard procedures utilized in prior taxometric studies with interview-based measures of depression (e.g., the Kiddie-Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia, Ambrosini et al., 2002; the Composite International Diagnostic Interview, Prisciandaro & Roberts 2005; Slade & Andrews 2005; Slade 2007; Ahmed et al. 2011), only respondents who endorsed a two-week period of depressed mood, irritability, or anhedonia (i.e., the primary criterion symptom for DSM-IV major depression) within the most recent four weeks completed all remaining symptom and impairment questions in the diagnostic interview for depression, assessed over the same two-week period. As taxometric procedures require response data for all symptom questions, these respondents formed the subsample included in the current analyses (unweighted nparent = 683; nchild = 605). Thus, consistent with prior taxometric studies of depression featuring interview-based measures (e.g., Slade & Andrews, 2005), a small proportion of the entire sample was included in the current study.3 The low endorsement rate for depressed mood, irritability, and anhedonia was expected for a four-week assessment interval with a very young community sample.4 Additionally, although a smaller fraction met full diagnostic criteria for major depression (i.e., the putative taxon) over the last four weeks in the full sample (weighted prevalence rate = 0.65%),5 the prevalence of this disorder in the subsample was adequate for taxometric analysis (weighted prevalence rate = 10.01%).6 This subsample was 52.98% female, with a mean age of 12.00 years (SE = 0.09 years). The racial/ethnic composition of the subsample was 91.23% White, 3.62% Black, 3.15% South Asian, and 2.00% other. The median gross household income was £15,000 to £17,499.

Measure and indicator construction

The Development and Well-Being Assessment (DAWBA; Goodman et al. 2000), a structured diagnostic interview designed for epidemiological research, was used to assess for the presence of DSM-IV major depressive disorder during the most recent four-week period. This instrument has been found to detect higher rates of psychiatric diagnoses in clinic than in community samples (Goodman et al. 2000), and has demonstrated adequate validity (Goodman et al. 2000, 2002).

A total of 31 items reflecting the nine DSM-IV symptoms of depression and related psychological distress and impairment were used to construct the taxometric indicators included in the analyses. Each symptom item was dichotomous (i.e., presence versus absence of symptom), whereas distress and impairment items utilized a four-point response scale ranging from “not at all” to “a great deal”. Following standard taxometric procedures (Beauchaine 2003; Haslam 2003; Cole 2004), indicators reflecting different facets of the latent construct of depression were derived from these 31 items. Four indicators were created by summing conceptually related items into composite scores. These four indicators were: core depressive symptoms (14 items reflecting depressed mood, irritability, and anhedonia), somatic symptoms (6 items assessing fatigue, appetite and weight disturbance, sleep disturbance/hypersomnia, and psychomotor disturbance), cognitive symptoms (5 items reflecting worthlessness/guilt, concentration difficulties, and suicidality), and impairment (6 items on psychological distress, and impairment in family life, social life, academic performance, and leisure activities).

Data analysis

Three distinct taxometric procedures were employed in the present study: MAMBAC (mean above minus below a cut; Meehl & Yonce, 1994), MAXEIG (maximum eigenvalue; Waller & Meehl, 1998), and L-Mode (latent mode; Waller & Meehl, 1998).

MAMBAC requires at least two valid indicators, with one serving as the input indicator and another functioning as the output indicator. The difference in mean scores of the output indicator above and below a sliding cut-off score on the input indicator is plotted as a function of the input indicator cut-points. This procedure is repeated for every possible pair of indicators. In the current study, 50 cuts were made along each input indicator. Each indicator in a pair alternates as the input and output indicator, and thus two graphical MAMBAC plots are generated for each pair of indicators. The results of these analyses are averaged into a single MAMBAC curve.

MAXEIG requires at least three indicators. One indicator is designated the input indicator, and the interrelationship between the remaining indicators is evaluated in a series of overlapping “windows” (i.e., subsamples) ordered along the input indicator. Based on optimal analysis parameters (Walters & Ruscio 2010), the sample in the current study was split into 25 windows with 90% overlap between adjacent windows. The covariance matrix for the output indicators (variance values are replaced with 0’s such that only covariances remain) in each window is factor analyzed, and the eigenvalue of the first principal factor is then plotted on a graph with the windows of the input indicator on the x-axis. This procedure is repeated with each indicator serving as the input indicator.

L-Mode similarly requires at least three indicators. This is a factor analytic procedure for differentiating between taxonic and dimensional structures. It calculates the factor scores of cases on a one-factor latent variable, with the factor score density plot of the entire distribution then plotted.

For each taxometric procedure, simulated taxonic and dimensional comparison data were generated, approximating all distribution properties of the empirical data known to influence the shape of taxometric curves. Specifically, the simulated data were identical to the research data in terms of surface-level statistical properties of the observed indicators, such as sample size, means, standard deviations, indicator skew, and inter-indicator correlations, and differed only in terms of latent structure. The results for the empirical data were directly compared with those for simulated taxonic and dimensional data to determine which they most closely matched. For simulated data across all three taxometric techniques, a putative taxon base rate of .12 was used, as this was the unweighted prevalence rate of major depression in both parent-report and child-report data. This same taxon base rate was also used in estimating between-group validity and within-group correlations. Additionally, data for each model (i.e., taxonic and dimensional) were simulated 100 times to approximate sampling distributions for each model for each of the three taxometric procedures used in the current study. This approach of comparing the empirical data to simulated models of taxonicity and dimensionality with identical statistical properties, provides a much more accurate comparison than would be the case with a prototypical model.

The CCFI was calculated for each taxometric procedure to form an objective determination of whether the results matched the simulated taxonic or dimensional comparison data (Ruscio et al. 2007). It compares the root-mean-square residual (RMSR) of the fit between the curve for the actual data and for each simulated comparison curve. CCFI values range from 0 (dimensional structure) to 1 (taxonic structure), with 0.50 being equally supportive of dimensional and taxonic structures (Ruscio et al. 2010). CCFI values falling between the dual thresholds of 0.45 and 0.55 indicate ambiguous results (Walters & Ruscio 2013). These dual thresholds have been found to have an accuracy rate ranging from 95.8% for MAXEIG to 98.2% for MAMBAC (Ruscio et al. 2010). The CCFI is a relatively recent development in taxometric research, but an important one that appears to have resulted in appreciably reduced rates of spurious taxa in the taxometric literature (Haslam et al. 2012). All analyses were conducted using Ruscio's (2012) taxometric programs in RRO 8.0.1 beta.

Results

Indicator suitability analyses

Indicator properties, including skew, correlations, and validity, were assessed to determine suitability of the data for taxometric analysis. Highly skewed indicators were not observed in the present study (for all four indicators, skew ≤ 0.882). To avoid nuisance covariance in indicator construction, it is important for indicator correlations to be substantially smaller within the putative taxon and complement groups than within the full sample (Ruscio et al. 2006). It has been suggested that a sizeable difference between the full-sample and within-group indicator correlations should be present (Ruscio et al. 2006; Walters 2008). For both parent-report and child-report data, full sample rs ≥ 0.403 and taxon and complement rs ≤ 0.295. Finally, it has been recommended that the constructed indicators should separate the putative taxon from its complement at Cohen’s d ≥ 1.25 to achieve an acceptable minimum validity (Meehl 1995; Meehl & Yonce 1996). This condition was satisfied in the current study (for both parent-report and child-report data, mean d ≥ 1.765).7 Table 1 provides a summary of indicator characteristics and validity statistics.

Table 1.

Summary of indicator correlations and validity

Indicator correlations

1 2 3 Full
sample
Within
taxon
Within
complement
Cohen’s d
Parent-report 0.433 −0.125 0.295
  1. Core depressive symptoms 2.037
  2. Somatic symptoms 0.418 1.228
  3. Cognitive symptoms 0.429 0.451 1.927
  4. Distress and impairment 0.492 0.373 0.438 2.002
Child-report 0.403 −0.084 0.254
  1. Core depressive symptoms 2.001
  2. Somatic symptoms 0.289 1.290
  3. Cognitive symptoms 0.184 0.352 1.885
  4. Distress and impairment 0.231 0.144 0.322 1.882

Note: Cohen’s d = difference between the putative taxon and complement standardized using pooled within-group variances weighted by degrees of freedom.

Taxometric analyses

MAMBAC analyses produced 12 curves each for parent-report and child-report data. Figure 1 depicts the averaged graphical output relative to simulated taxonic and dimensional MAMBAC data for each response source. Both parent-report and child-report data more closely resembled dimensional distributions. Indeed, their corresponding CCFI values unambiguously favored dimensional solutions (CCFIparent = 0.315, CCFIchild = 0.185). MAXEIG procedures yielded relatively flat averaged curves for both parent-report and child-report data that more closely matched simulated dimensional than categorical data (see Figure 2). The CCFI values of 0.252 for parent-report data and 0.242 for child-report data provide unambiguous objective support for the presence of a continuous latent structure. The results of L-Mode analyses relative to simulated categorical and dimensional data are graphically presented in Figure 3. L-Mode curves for both parent-report and child-report data did not indicate evidence of bimodal distributions, and both CCFI values were below 0.45 (CCFIparent = 0.349, CCFIchild = 0.309), providing clear support for dimensionality. The mean CCFIs across all three taxometric procedures for parent-report data (0.305) and child-report data (0.245) were congruent with a dimensional latent structure for depression.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Taxometric results for MAMBAC curves relative to simulated taxonic and dimensional data. In each graph, the average curve for the sample data are represented by a dark line, with the gray area reflecting the middle 50% of the simulated values, and the light lines indicating the minimum and maximum simulated values at each data point. The top panels illustrate results for parent-reported data, and the bottom panels depict results for child-reported data.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Taxometric results for averaged MAXEIG curves relative to simulated taxonic and dimensional data. In each graph, the average curve for the sample data are represented by a dark line, with the gray area reflecting the middle 50% of the simulated values, and the light lines indicating the minimum and maximum simulated values at each data point. The top panels illustrate results for parent-reported data, and the bottom panels depict results for child-reported data.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Taxometric results for L-Mode curves relative to simulated taxonic and dimensional data. In each graph, the average curve for the sample data are represented by a dark line, with the gray area reflecting the middle 50% of the simulated values, and the light lines indicating the minimum and maximum simulated values at each data point. The top panels illustrate results for parent-reported data, and the bottom panels depict results for child-reported data.

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to investigate the latent structure of depression in children and adolescents using three mathematically non-redundant taxometric procedures (i.e., MAMBAC, MAXEIG, and L-Mode). It is the first taxometric study of depression in youth to date to utilize objective indices (i.e., CCFI values) in place of subjective visual inspection tests of taxonicity and dimensionality. As such, and in addressing several important methodological limitations of prior taxometric studies of depression in youth, it represents an important step toward resolving the mixed taxometric findings in the literature. The results across all analyses provided convergent support for a dimensional latent structure of depression in this age group. These results appear to be fairly robust, as they were consistent across data derived from multiple reporting sources (i.e., parent and child). The findings of dimensionality in the current investigation are also notably consistent with the broader taxometric literature for depression in adults (Haslam et al. 2012).

Evidence of dimensionality in the latent structure of depression in children and adolescents may lend clarity to the existing research on subthreshold depression. In particular, the finding that subthreshold depression is associated with several of the same risk factors and prognostic outcomes as major depression has led some researchers to conclude that depression exists along a continuum of severity (Nierenberg et al. 2010; Pietrzak et al. 2013; Wesselhoeft et al. 2013). The results of the current study provide direct support for this interpretation, and counter the alternative possibility that subthreshold depression and major depression, at least in children and adolescents, exist as part of a single categorical entity.

The limitations of the current investigation warrant mention. First, the current study did not include children below the age of 6. Thus, although homotypic continuity has been found between preschool depression and major depression later in childhood (Luby et al. 2009, 2014), the current findings cannot be generalized to depression in preschool children. A second limitation of this study is that it evaluated the latent structure of depression based on DSM-IV criteria. Thus, it cannot be ruled out that a depression taxon may exist which includes symptoms not part of DSM-IV criteria (e.g., hopelessness). Additionally, it is possible that only a subset of DSM-IV depressive symptoms form the putative depression taxon (i.e., a subtype of depression), the detection of which may have been obscured by the inclusion of non-taxon-relevant symptoms. Indeed, such a possibility is congruent with the view held by several researchers that major depression is a markedly heterogeneous condition (Spangler et al. 1997; Klein 2008; McGuffin 2008; Harkness et al. 2011). Nevertheless, it is worth observing that the heterogeneity of depression as currently conceptualized reflects a complex, multi-causal etiology, which is entirely consistent with a dimensional latent structure (Meehl 1977; Meehl & Golden 1982).

Finally, the present findings have implications for clinical prevention and treatment, as well as the study of this disorder in youth. The converging evidence across multiple taxometric analyses of a dimensional structure for depression complements prior research indicating that subthreshold depression is often associated with impairment of a degree comparable to that of major depression (Gotlib et al. 1995; Wesselhoeft et al. 2013). Collectively, they are congruent with the position that a movement away from a categorical conceptualization of depression in treatment planning may be warranted, and toward a greater emphasis on other characteristics of this disorder (e.g., symptom severity, degree of impairment). Within the context of quantitative research, the arbitrary dichotomization of a dimensional construct leads to a loss in measurement precision (Ruscio & Ruscio 2002) and statistical power (Cohen 1983; MacCallum et al. 2002), and to an increased risk of spurious statistical findings (Maxwell & Delaney 1993). Furthermore, the findings of dimensionality for depression may also inform statistical approaches to the study of this condition. That is, classical test theory and item response theory are more appropriate for analyses involving dimensional latent constructs, whereas other approaches, such as Bayes’s Theorem, are more ideally suited for taxonic constructs (Ruscio & Ruscio 2004).

Acknowledgments

The data in the current study were drawn from the Mental Health of Children and Young People in Great Britain 1999 and 2004 surveys. These surveys were undertaken by the Office of National Statistics, and the data are held by the UK Data Archive. These data have received Crown Copyright.

Financial support

Preparation of this paper was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number R01MH101138. The content is solely the responsibility of the author and does not necessarily represent the official views of the Office of National Statistics, the UK Data Archive, or the funding agency.

Footnotes

1

Although the DAWBA was also designed to assess for ICD-10 depressive episodes and symptoms, DSM-IV criterial symptoms were used in the current study so as to facilitate comparison with past taxometric research.

2

It is also worth noting within the present context that even recall for objectively occurring, rather than subjectively experienced, phenomena, such as negative life events, declines appreciably over a 12-month period (Brown & Harris 1982), with a pronounced fall-off occurring for recall of severe events after approximately seven months in adolescents (Monck & Dobbs 1985).

3

For a similar approach to the taxometric study of social anxiety disorder in an epidemiological sample, see Ruscio (2010).

4

Approximately 70% of the full unweighted sample was under age 13.

5

This low four-week prevalence rate is likely due to the high proportion of participants below age 12 and the rarity of depression in this age group (Kessler et al. 2005). If the current sample is limited to 13–16 year olds, the weighted four-week prevalence rate of depression is 2.17%, a figure comparable to the weighted four-week prevalence of major depression and dysthymia (2.6%) in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication Adolescent Supplement, with a sample of 13–17 year olds (Kessler et al. 2012).

6

Taxon base rates of P ≥ 0.1 are generally required for conducting taxometric analysis (Ruscio et al. 2006). Having an inadequate number of members of the putative taxon biases taxometric analyses toward dimensionality. Samples considerably larger than the minimum required for taxometric analysis (n ≥ 300), such as is featured in the present study, may offset potential concerns regarding low taxon base rates in as much as they yield an adequately higher raw number of cases of the putative taxon (Ruscio & Ruscio 2004). In the current study, 84 cases of major depression (unweighted) were included in the analyses utilizing parent data, and 74 cases (unweighted) in analyses based on child-report data.

7

Although the somatic symptoms indicator drawn from parent-reported data falls just short of this condition, it does not compromise the validity of the analyses when considered within the context of the highly valid remaining indicators.

References

  1. Ahmed AO, Green BA, Clark CB, Stahl KC, McFarland ME. Latent structure of unipolar and bipolar mood symptoms. Bipolar Disorders. 2011;13:522–536. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2011.00940.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ambrosini P, Bennett DS, Cleland CM, Haslam N. Taxonicity of adolescent melancholia: A categorical or dimensional construct? Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2002;36:247–256. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3956(02)00011-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  4. Beauchaine TP. Taxometrics and developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 2003;15:501–527. doi: 10.1017/s0954579403000270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Beauchaine TP, Waters E. Pseudotaxonicity in MAMBAC and MAXCOV analyses of rating-scale data: Turning continua into classes by manipulating observer’s expectations. Psychological Methods. 2003;8:3–15. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.8.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brown GW, Harris T. Fall-off in the reporting of life events. Social Psychiatry. 1982;17:23–28. [Google Scholar]
  7. Cohen J. The cost of dichotomization. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1983;7:249–253. [Google Scholar]
  8. Cole DA. Taxometrics in psychopathology research: An introduction to some of the procedures and related methodological issues. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2004;113:3–9. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Copeland W, Shanahan L, Costello EJ, Angold A. Cumulative prevalence of psychiatric disorders by young adulthood: A prospective cohort analysis from the Great Smoky Mountains Study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2011;50:252–261. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.12.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Costello EJ, Angold A, Keeler GP. Adolescent outcomes of childhood disorders: The consequences of severity and impairment. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1999;38:121–128. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199902000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Fallon T, Schwab-Stone M. Determinants of reliability in psychiatric surveys of children aged 6–12. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1994;35:1391–1408. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01282.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Flett GL, Vredenburg K, Krames L. The continuity of depression in clinical and nonclinical samples. Psychological Bulletin. 1997;121:395–416. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.3.395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Ford T, Goodman R, Meltzer H. The British Child and Adolescent Mental Health Survey 1999: The prevalence of DSM-IV disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2003;42:1203–1211. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200310000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Goldberg DP. The detection of psychiatric illness by questionnaire: A technique for the identification and assessment of non-psychotic psychiatric illness. Oxford University Press; 1972. [Google Scholar]
  15. Goodman R, Ford T, Meltzer H. Mental health problems of children in the community: 18 month follow up. BMJ. 2002;324:1496–1497. doi: 10.1136/bmj.324.7352.1496. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Goodman R, Ford T, Richards H, Gatward R, Meltzer H. The Development and Well-Being Assessment: Description and initial validation of an integrated assessment of child and adolescent psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2000;41:645–655. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Gotlib IH, Lewinsohn PM, Seeley JR. Symptoms versus a diagnosis of depression: Differences in psychosocial functioning. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1995;63:90–100. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.63.1.90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Green H, McGinnity A, Meltzer H, Ford T, Goodman R. Mental health of children and young people in Great Britain, 2004. Houndmills, UK: Palgrave Macmillan; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  19. Hankin BL, Fraley RC, Lahey BB, Waldman ID. Is depression best viewed as a continuum or discrete category? A taxometric analysis of childhood and adolescent depression in a population-based sample. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2005;114:96–110. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.1.96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Harkness KL, Stewart JG, Wynne-Edwards KE. Cortisol reactivity to social stress in adolescents: Role of depression severity and child maltreatment. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2011;36:173–181. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Haslam N. Categorical versus dimensional models of mental disorder: The taxometric evidence. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;37:696–704. doi: 10.1080/j.1440-1614.2003.01258.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Haslam N, Holland E, Kuppens P. Categories versus dimensions in personality and psychopathology: A quantitative review of taxometric research. Psychological Medicine. 2012;42:903–920. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711001966. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Helzer JE, Kraemer HC, Krueger RF. The feasibility and need for dimensional psychiatric diagnoses. Psychological Medicine. 2006;36:1671–1680. doi: 10.1017/S003329170600821X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kessler RC, Avenevoli S, Costello EJ, Georgiades K, Green JG, Gruber MJ, He J, Koretz D, McLaughlin KA, Petukhova M, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, Merikangas KR. Prevalence, persistence, and sociodemographic correlates of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication Adolescent Supplement. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2012;69:372–380. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Merikangas KR, Walters EE. Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005;62:593–602. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Klein DN. Classification of depressive disorders in the DSM-V: Proposal for a two-dimension system. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2008;117:552–560. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.117.3.552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Klein DN, Glenn CR, Kosty DB, Seeley JR, Rohde P, Lewinsohn PM. Predictors of first lifetime onset of major depressive disorder in young adulthood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2013;122:1–6. doi: 10.1037/a0029567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Luby JL, Gaffrey MS, Tillman R, April LM, Belden AC. Trajectories of preschool disorders to full DSM depression at school age and early adolescence: Continuity of preschool depression. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2014;171:768–776. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13091198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Luby JL, Si X, Belden AC, Tandon M, Spitznagel E. Preschool depression: Homotypic continuity and course over 24 months. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2009;66:897–905. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. MacCallum RC, Zhang S, Preacher KJ, Rucker DD. On the practice of dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychological Methods. 2002;7:19–40. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.7.1.19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Maxwell SE, Delaney HD. Bivariate median splits and spurious statistical significance. Psychological Bulletin. 1993;113:181–190. [Google Scholar]
  32. McGuffin P. Affective disorders. In: Murray RM, Kendler KS, McGuffin P, Wessely S, Castle D, editors. Essential Psychiatry. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 2008. pp. 250–283. [Google Scholar]
  33. Meehl PE. Specific etiology and other forms of strong influence: some quantitative meanings. Journal of Medicine and Philosophy. 1977;2:33–53. [Google Scholar]
  34. Meehl PE. Bootstraps taxometrics: Solving the classification problem in psychopathology. American Psychologist. 1995;50:266–275. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.50.4.266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Meehl PE. What’s in a taxon? Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2004;113:39–43. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.1.39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Meehl PE, Golden R. Taxometric methods. In: Kendall P, Butcher J, editors. Handbook of research methods in clinical psychology. New York: Wiley; 1982. pp. 127–181. [Google Scholar]
  37. Meehl PE, Yonce LJ. Taxometric analysis: I Detecting taxonicity with two quantitative indicators using means above and below a sliding cut (MAMBAC procedure) Psychological Reports. 1994;74:1059–1274. [Google Scholar]
  38. Meehl PE, Yonce LJ. Taxometric analysis: II Detecting taxonicity using covariance of two quantitative indicators in successive intervals of a third indicator (Maxcov procedure) Psychological Reports. 1996;78:1091–1227. [Google Scholar]
  39. Meltzer H, Gatward R, Goodman R, Ford T. Mental health of children and adolescents in Great Britain. London, UK: Stationery Office; 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Taylor A, Kokaua J, Milne BJ, Polanczyk G, Poulton R. How common are common mental disorders? Evidence that lifetime prevalence rates are doubled by prospective versus retrospective ascertainment. Psychological Medicine. 2010;40:899–909. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709991036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Monck E, Dobbs R. Measuring life events in an adolescent population: Methodological issues and related findings. Psychological Medicine. 1985;15:841–850. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700005079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Nierenberg AA, Rapaport MH, Schettler PJ, Howland RH, Smith JA, Edwards D, Schneider T, Mischoulon D. Deficits in psychological well-being and quality-of-life in minor depression: Implications for DSM-V. CNS Neuroscience & Therapeutics. 2010;16:208–216. doi: 10.1111/j.1755-5949.2009.00108.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Pettit JW, Hartley C, Lewinsohn PM, Seeley JR, Klein DN. Is liability to recurrent major depressive disorder present before first episode onset in adolescence or acquired after the initial episode? Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2013;122:353–358. doi: 10.1037/a0032655. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Pietrzak RH, Kinley J, Afifi TO, Enns MW, Fawcett J, Sareen J. Subsyndromal depression in the United States: prevalence, course, and risk for incident psychiatric outcomes. Psychological Medicine. 2013;43:1401–1414. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712002309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Prisciandaro JJ, Roberts JE. A taxometric investigation of unipolar depression in the National Comorbidity S-urvey. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2005;114:718–728. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.718. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Richey JA, Schmidt NB, Lonigan CJ, Phillips BM, Catanzaro SJ, Laurent J, Gerhardstein RR, Kotov R. The latent structure of child depression: A taxometric analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2009;50:1147–1155. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02085.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Rogler LH, Malgady RG, Tryon WW. Evaluation of mental health Issues of memory in the Diagnostic Interview Schedule. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 1992;180:215–222. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Ruscio AM. The latent structure of social anxiety disorder: Consequences of shifting to a dimensional diagnosis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2010;119:662–671. doi: 10.1037/a0019341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Ruscio AM, Ruscio J. The latent structure of analogue depression: Should the Beck Depression Inventory be used to classify groups? Psychological Assessment. 2002;14:135–145. doi: 10.1037//1040-3590.14.2.135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Ruscio J. Taxometric programs for the R computing environment: User’s manual. 2012 [Google Scholar]
  51. Ruscio J, Brown TA, Ruscio AM. A taxometric investigation of DSM-IV major depression in a large outpatient sample: Interpretable structural results depend on the mode of assessment. Assessment. 2009;16:127–144. doi: 10.1177/1073191108330065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Ruscio J, Haslam N, Ruscio AM. Introduction to the Taxometric Method: A Practical Guide. Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  53. Ruscio J, Kaczetow W. Differentiating categories and dimensions: Evaluating the robustness of taxometric analyses. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 2009;44:259–280. doi: 10.1080/00273170902794248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Ruscio J, Ruscio AM. Clarifying boundary issues in psychopathology: The role of taxometrics in a comprehensive program of structural research. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2004;113:24–38. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.1.24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Ruscio J, Ruscio AM, Meron M. Applying the bootstrap to taxometric analysis: Generating empirical sampling distributions to help interpret results. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 2007;42:349–386. doi: 10.1080/00273170701360795. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Ruscio J, Walters GD, Marcus DK, Kaczetow W. Comparing the relative fit of categorical and dimensional latent variable models using consistency tests. Psychological Assessment. 2010;22:5–21. doi: 10.1037/a0018259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Schwab-Stone ME, Shaffer D, Dulcan MK, Jensen PS, Fisher P, Bird HR, Goodman SH, Lahey BB, Lichtman JH, Canino G, Rubio-Stipec M, Rae DS. Criterion validity of the NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children Version 23 (DISC-23) Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1996;35:878–888. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199607000-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Shaffer D, Fisher P, Dulcan MK, Davies M, Piacentini J, Schwab-Stone ME, Lahey BB, Bourdon K, Jensen PS, Bird HR, Canino G, Regier DA. The NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children Version 23 (DISC-23): description, acceptability, prevalence rates, and performance in the MECA Study Methods for the Epidemiology of Child and Adolescent Mental Disorders Study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1996;35:865–877. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199607000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Slade T. Taxometric investigation of depression: Evidence of consistent latent structure across clinical and community samples. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;41:403–410. doi: 10.1080/00048670701261194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Slade T, Andrews G. Latent structure of depression in a community sample: A taxometric analysis. Psychological Medicine. 2005;35:489–497. doi: 10.1017/s0033291704003708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Solomon A, Haaga DAF, Arnow BA. Is clinical depression distinct from subthreshold depressive symptoms? A review of the continuity issue in depression research. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2001;189:498–506. doi: 10.1097/00005053-200108000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Solomon A, Ruscio J, Seeley JR, Lewinsohn PM. A taxometric investigation of unipolar depression in a large community sample. Psychological Medicine. 2006;36:973–985. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706007689. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Sonuga-Barke EJS. Categorical models of childhood disorder: A conceptual and empirical analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1998;39:115–133. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Spangler DL, Simons AD, Monroe SM, Thase ME. Respond to cognitive-behavioral therapy in depression: Effects of pretreatment cognitive dysfunction and life stress. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1997;65:568–575. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.65.4.568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Tanur JM. Questions about questions: Inquiries into the cognitive bases of surveys. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  66. Waller N, Meehl PE. Advanced Quantitative Techniques in the Social Sciences, 9. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 1998. Multivariate taxometric procedures: Distinguishing types from continua. [Google Scholar]
  67. Walters GD. The latent structure of alcohol use disorders: a taxometric analysis of structured interview data obtained from male federal prison inmates. Alcohol and Alcoholism. 2008;43:326–333. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agm165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Walters GD, Ruscio J. Where do we draw the line? Assigning cases to subsamples for MAMBAC, MAXCOV, and MAXEIG taxometric analyses. Assessment. 2010;17:321–333. doi: 10.1177/1073191109356539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Walters GD, Ruscio J. Trajectories of youthful antisocial behavior: categories or continua? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2013;41:653–666. doi: 10.1007/s10802-012-9700-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Wells JE, Horwood LJ. How accurate is recall of key symptoms of depression? A comparison of recall and longitudinal reports. Psychological Medicine. 2004;34:1001–1011. doi: 10.1017/s0033291703001843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Wesselhoeft R, Sørensen MJ, Heiervang ER, Bilenberg N. Subthreshold depression in children and adolescents – a systematic review. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2013;151:7–22. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.06.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Whisman MA, Pinto A. Hopelessness depression in depressed inpatient adolescents. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 1997;21:345–358. [Google Scholar]
  73. Widiger TA, Edmundson M. Diagnoses, dimensions, and DSM-5. In: Barlow DH, editor. The Oxford Handbook of Clinical Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press; 2014. pp. 256–280. [Google Scholar]
  74. Widiger TA, Samuel DB. Diagnostic categories or dimensions? A question for the Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders--fifth edition. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2005;114:494–504. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.494. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. World Health Organization. International Classification of Diseases, 10th Revision (ICD-10) Geneva, Switzerland: 1992. [Google Scholar]
  76. Wu P, Hoven CW, Cohen P, Liu X, Moore RE, Tiet Q, Okezie N, Wicks J, Bird HR. Factors associated with use of mental health services for depression by Children and adolescents. Psychiatric Services. 2001;52:189–195. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.52.2.189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES