Skip to main content
UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts logoLink to UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Jul 26.
Published in final edited form as: J Adolesc Health. 2015 Feb;56(2):153–159. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2014.10.268

The mediating role of deviant peers on the link between depressed mood and harmful drinking: Analyses in a population-based sample of adolescents

Francesca Pesola 1, Katherine H Shelton 2, Jon Heron 3, Marcus Munafò 4, Barbara Maughan 5, Matthew Hickman 3, Marianne BM van den Bree 1,*
PMCID: PMC4961232  EMSID: EMS69316  PMID: 25620300

Abstract

Purpose

One’s peer group can have a strong impact on depressed mood and harmful drinking in adolescence. It remains unclear whether affiliation with deviant peers explains the link between these traits. Our study aims to: a) explore the developmental relationship between harmful drinking and depressed mood in adolescence; and b) establish to which extent affiliation with deviant peers explains this relationship.

Methods

4,863 adolescents from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) were assessed between the ages of 14 and 16. Harmful drinking was established using age-appropriate measures: the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism (SSAGA) in mid-adolescence (age 14) and the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) in late adolescence (age 16). Depressed mood was measured by the Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (SMFQ) at both ages. Affiliation with deviant peers was assessed at age 15.

Results

Harmful drinking at age 14 predicted depressed mood two years later. This association was explained by affiliation with deviant peers and remained present even after adjustment for earlier depressed mood. Depressed mood at age 14 predicted harmful drinking at age 16 via affiliation with deviant peers; however, this indirect effect disappeared when adjusting for adolescents’ earlier harmful alcohol use (age 14). No gender differences were observed.

Conclusions

Adolescents who engage in early harmful drinking and subsequently become affiliated with a deviant peer group may be at particular risk of later depressed mood.

Keywords: harmful drinking, depressed mood, adolescence, deviant peers, ALSPAC, prospective birth cohort study, longitudinal

Introduction

Harmful drinking and depression frequently co-occur and the combination of both traits is associated with increased risk of a range of adverse outcomes, including lower quality of life, social impairment and suicide [1]. A number of studies have attempted to clarify the developmental nature of this relationship but results have been inconsistent [13].

Mixed results may be due to variation in research methodology and design across studies [4]. Discrepancies can also point to a complex aetiology which may be best understood by taking into account third variables to explain this developmental relationship [5]. The peer group represents a crucial social influence in adolescents’ lives and plays a role in alcohol initiation and progression [6, 7] as well as depression [8, 9] with evidence that specific risk factors may only lead to substance misuse in the presence of other risk factors [10].

Two hypotheses offer plausible explanations for the link between depression, drinking, and peer influence. The social learning hypothesis suggests that adolescents who are rejected by their normative peer group, due to either depression or harmful drinking, may gravitate towards a deviant peer group to reduce their sense of isolation [8, 11]. Subsequently, they adopt the group’s behaviours and norms through imitation and reinforcement [11]. The self-selection hypothesis suggests that adolescents actively seek peers who engage in similar behaviours [7, 12, 13]. Based on evidence that depression co-occurs with conduct problems, depressed adolescents may pursue antisocial activities and engage with deviant peers who share similar behaviours [1416]. Similarly, teenagers who engage in harmful drinking may pursue peers who share their drinking norms. Affiliation with deviant peers increases levels of depression as this peer group does not offer suitable support and activities conducted with deviant peers may lead to negative consequences (e.g., academic failure) [8].

Mid-adolescence is a critical age as it is characterised by an increase in depression rates and drinking behaviour and, additionally, the role of peers is at its peak [1719]. Thus, it is important to understand the relationship between these domains in order to develop effective interventions. However, to our knowledge, no study has explored this relationship. Using data from a large birth cohort study, we aimed to fill this gap and establish:

  • 1.

    the relationship between: a) depressed mood at age 14 and harmful drinking at age 16, and b) harmful drinking at age 14 and depressed mood two years later;

  • 2.

    to what extent these relationships are mediated by affiliation with deviant peers at age 15 (as depicted in figure 1).

Figure 1. Representation of the mediation models.

Figure 1

Panel a depicts model 1 which explores the predicting role of depressed mood on later problem drinking via affiliation with deviant peers; panel b depicts model 2 which explores the predicting role of problem drinking on depressed mood via deviant peer group.

Owing to evidence that depressed mood is a stronger predictor of harmful drinking for females than males [20, 21], we also aimed to:

  • 3.

    explore potential gender differences.

Materials & Method

Sample

The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) is an ongoing longitudinal cohort study, which started in 1991-1992. The core sample consisted of 14,541 pregnancies. These initial pregnancies resulted in 14,062 live births with 13,988 children alive at 1 year of age. The study website contains details of all the data available through a fully searchable data dictionary [22]. The ALSPAC cohort is similar to the overall UK population as indicated by comparisons with the 1991 census [23]. Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Law and Ethics Committee and Local Research Ethics Committees.

In the present study, adolescents were assessed at ages 13 (mean = 12.8, SD = 0.2), 14 (mean = 13.8, SD = 0.02), 15 (mean = 15.5, SD = 0.3) and 16 (mean = 16.7, SD = 0.2). At age 16, 9,996 questionnaires were sent to the study children and 5,126 were returned. Young people who returned their questionnaires were more likely to be female (43% vs. 28% male; χ2(1) = 338.8, p<.001), from a family with higher social class (48% vs. low 31%; χ2(1) = 326.1, p<.001) and education level (57% vs. low 32%; χ2(1) = 547.9, p<.001). Respondents who were less likely to return the questionnaires were more likely to be smokers (44% vs. non-smoker 36%; χ2(1) = 14.8, p<.001) and to report an onset of alcohol use prior to age 13 (40% vs. later 35%; χ2(1) = 12.4, p<.001). The analyses presented below are based on 4,863 (60% female) respondents with complete information on the outcome measures (i.e., depressed mood and harmful drinking).

Measures

Alcohol measure at age 14

Alcohol information at age 14 (predictor in model 2) was collected using the adolescent version of the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism (SSAGA) [24]. Following the methodology used by Saraceno and colleagues (2002), four items were used to estimate a harmful drinking measure: 1) frequency of drinking without parents’ permission; 2) frequency of having a whole drink; 3) largest number of whole drinks within a 24-hour period; and 4) whether the adolescent had ever been drunk (α = .78).

Alcohol measures at age 16

At age 16, when alcohol use is more established than at age 14, we used the Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test (AUDIT; outcome in model 1) [25]. This is a brief screening tool to identify individuals with alcohol-related problems and comprises 10 multiple-choice items (α=.77). Two AUDIT items were dropped as they were rare in the sample (<10%; revised 8-item α= .78); i.e., ‘how frequently have you had a drink first thing in the morning’ and ‘a family member/friend/doctor has shown concern about your drinking’. Previous studies, similarly, found these items did not seem to capture harmful drinking in adolescence [26].

Depressed mood

Depressed mood was measured using the Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (SMFQ) at ages 14 (i.e., predictor in model 1) and 16 (i.e., outcome in model 2) [27]. The questionnaire comprises 13 items (age 14: α = .86; age 16: α = .91). To improve model estimation, we dropped two items at age 14 as they were rare (<10%); i.e., I have ‘been a bad person’ and ‘done everything wrong’ (revised 11-item α = .84).

Deviant peers at age 15

Affiliation with deviant peers (i.e., mediator) was assessed using items from the Edinburgh Study of Youth Transitions and Crime (ESYTC) to capture deviant activity in the peer network. The questionnaire asked respondents to report whether their friends had engaged in delinquent behaviour (e.g., shoplifting) and substance use in the past year. Rare (<10%) or highly frequent (>80%) items were excluded from the analyses as they were not very informative and produced small cell-counts which can lead to estimate bias. Hence, a 12-item construct was derived (α= .85).

Confounders

Background confounders were selected a priori because they are related to depressed mood, deviant peers and harmful drinking [1, 28]. They were from two domains: family environment (i.e., parental drinking and depression at 12 years), and socio-economic status (i.e., financial difficulties at 11 years and parental education at the 32nd week of gestation) as reported by the child’s mother as well as partner.

The model was also adjusted for the Strengths & Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) conduct problem scale which consists of five items based on carers’ report at age 13 [29]. Carers were asked whether their children had engaged in specific deviant behaviours (e.g., lying) in the past six months. Stealing ‘from home, school or elsewhere’ was rare (i.e., <10%) and we dropped this item. The reliability Cronbach’s α for the four-item scale (α= .58) was comparable to that reported for the five-item scale [29]. The scores at ages 13 and 16 were correlated (rho = .52, 95% CI: .50 to .54), indicating consistency over time.

The model was also adjusted for peers’ deviant behaviour at age 13. This consisted of five items (α=.70) equivalent to those assessed at age 15 (step 2). Respondents were asked whether their friends had engaged in delinquent behaviours in the past six months and offered a ‘yes/no’ response.

Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics on the main measures were calculated by summing the questionnaire items for complete cases. Our main aims were tested using structural equation modelling (SEM) on the imputed data. Latent constructs in the SEM analysis were defined using questionnaire items (Table S1).

Preliminary confirmatory factor analyses were conducted to assess the measurement model fit to the data. The weighted least squares mean variance (WLSMV) estimator in Mplus 7 [30] was used and model fit evaluated with the following goodness-of-fit indices: chi-square (p >.05); Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI > .95); Comparative Fit Index (CFI >.95); and Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA <.05) [31]. SEM analyses were conducted to assess whether affiliation with deviant peers explained the relationship between depressed mood at age 14 and harmful drinking at age 16 (model 1; figure 1a) and, similarly, between harmful drinking at age 14 and depressed mood at age 16 (model 2; figure 1b).

The indirect impact of the deviant peer group on the relationship between depression and harmful drinking was estimated using the product of coefficient method (i.e., path a*path b), which has good power and low type I error [32]. To estimate the strength of the indirect effect, we calculated the effect proportion mediated (i.e., Bindirect/BindirectBdirect). This proportion was calculated using the absolute values for the direct and indirect effects [32, 33]. Standard errors were calculated using the Delta method, which allowed us to calculate 95% confidence intervals equivalent to those obtained using 5000 bootstrap cycles (Table S2).

Following our main analyses, we conducted a series of sensitivity analyses where we adjusted our models for the confounders described above [33]. In step 1, we adjusted the model for background variables while in step 2 we adjusted it for both background variables and the SDQ conduct problems scale. This step allows us to assess whether affiliation with deviant peers is mostly driven by the adolescents’ own conduct problem, following evidence that harmful drinking and depressed mood are co-morbid with conduct problems [14]. Finally, following recent guidelines [34], we further adjusted the models for an earlier measure of the mediator (i.e., affiliation with deviant peers at age 13; step 3) and earlier outcome measures (i.e., age. model 1: harmful drinking; model 2: depressed mood; step 4) at age 14.

Moderation by gender

We assessed whether gender moderated the direct, indirect and total effects. This was achieved using a multi-group approach in which additional parameters capturing the difference in the strength of the direct, indirect and total paths between males and females (i.e., female – male) were derived using the model-constraint option. An advantage of this approach was that the resulting z-test (i.e., estimate/SE) of the difference effect can be pooled across imputed datasets, while this is not the case for likelihood based chi-square tests.

Prior to exploring gender-specific patterns, measurement invariance across gender was tested using multi-group confirmatory factor analysis (MGCFA) to ensure our main measures were well-specified and captured equivalent constructs across genders. This method compares two models; i.e., a baseline model where loadings and thresholds are unconstrained vs. a model where they are constrained across genders [30]. Measurement invariance is established if: 1) the constrained model offers a good fit to the data (i.e., configural invariance), and 2) the difference of model fit between the constrained and the unconstrained model is small (ΔCFI<.01 & RMESEA <.015; i.e., metric invariance) [35].

Missing data and imputation

All longitudinal studies are affected by attrition [36] and the ALSPAC study, which has been running for 20 years, is no exception. However, sophisticated imputation strategies have been developed which address bias introduced by drop-out. We used multiple imputation by chained equations (MICE) in Stata 11 with 50 imputations. Multiple imputation relies on the assumption that data are missing at random (MAR) [37]. We included earlier measures of the main variables and auxiliary variables, associated with the outcomes and their missingness, in our imputation model to increase the likelihood of the MAR assumption and improve model estimation [37].

Our analyses are based on imputed cases with complete information on the outcome measures, as outcome imputation has been found to lead to biased estimates [37]. Sensitivity analyses showed that the distributions of the imputed items and complete cases were comparable. Moreover, analyses based on imputed and complete cases produced equivalent result patterns (Table S3).

Results

Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations are summarised in Table 1 for complete cases.

Table 1. Spearman correlations, means and standard deviation (SD) for main variables. These figures are based on the scales’ scores for cases with complete information on both models (N = 1,883) and were calculated in Stata 11.

1. AUDIT 16 2.SMFQ 16 3.SSAGA1 14 4. SMFQ 14 5.PEERS 15 6.PEERS 13 7. SDQ 13
1 1.00
2 0.30** 1.00
3 0.32** 0.33** 1.00
4 0.33* 0.43** 0.13* 1.00
5 0.44** 0.24** 0.29** 0.18** 1.00
6 0.12* 0.09* 0.25** 0.11* 0.22** 1.00
7 0.03ns 0.10* 0.001ns 0.08ns 0.01ns 0.04ns 1.00

Mean 6.5 5.6 0.1 4.8 2.9 0.8 2.2
SD 5.0 5.3 1.2 4.3 2.8 1.0 0.8
**

p-values <0.001

*

p-values < 0.0

1

Following the approach followed by Saraceno and colleagues (2010), harmful drinking at age 14 was constructed using a Principal Component Analysis (PCA) approach. PCA extrapolates components with mean of 0 and standard deviation of 1.

The confirmatory factor analyses showed that the measurement model offered a good fit to the data for both models (Table S4). The structural model fit and results for the model adjusted with all confounders (step 4) are reported in Figure S1.

Model 1 (depressed mood and later harmful drinking)

Depressed mood at age 14 was associated with more deviant peer behaviour at age 15 (B =.21, 95% CI: .16 to .26, z = 8.2, p<.001; path a) which was, subsequently, associated with more harmful drinking at age 16 (B =.34, 95% CI: .30 to .38, z = 18.9, p<.001; path b). Depressed mood at age 14 predicted greater harmful drinking two years later (Btotal =.08, 95%CI: .04 to .11, z = 3.8, p<.001) and 96% of this association was explained by affiliation with deviant peers (Bindirect = 0.07, 95% CI: .05 to .09, z = 7.8, p <.001). Adjusting for the earlier measure of deviant peers reduced the indirect effect, which disappeared after subsequently adjusting for adolescents’ earlier harmful alcohol use at age 14 (Table 2).

Table 2. Imputed data analyses: Model 1: Total, indirect and direct effects of depressed mood (i.e., predictor) on harmful drinking (i.e., outcome) via deviant peers (i.e., mediator) with [95% CI].

Step 0 Step 1 Step 2 Step 3 Step 4
Total Effect .075
[.04, .11]
.074
[.03, .11]
.056
[.02, .10]
.022
[-.02, .06]
-.014
[-.05, .03]
Indirect effect .072
[.05, .09]
.069
[.05, .09]
.055
[.04, .07]
.021
[.003, .04]
.008
[-.004, .02]
Direct effect .003
[-.04, .04]
.005
[-.03, .04]
.001
[-.04, .04]
.001
[-.04, .04]
-.022
[-.06, .02]
Effect proportion mediated
(indirect/total ratio)
96% 93% 100% 100% 27%

Step 0: Unadjusted for covariates

Step 1: Adjusted for background covariates: financial difficulties, family education level, parents’ alcohol consumption, and parents’ depression

Step 2: Adjusted for background covariates + conduct problems scale (SDQ)

Step 3: Adjusted for background covariates + conduct problems scale (SDQ) + earlier mediator at age 13 (i.e., deviant peers)

Step 4: Adjusted for background covariates + conduct problems scale (SDQ) + deviant peers at age 13 + earlier outcome at age 14 (i.e., harmful drinking)

Model 2 (harmful drinking and later depressed mood)

Harmful drinking at age 14 was associated with more deviant peer behaviour at age 15 (B =.50, 95%CI: .46 to .54, z = 23.7, p<.001; path a) which was, subsequently, associated with greater depressed mood at age 16 (B =.18, 95%CI: .13 to .23, z = 7.3, p<.001; path b). Harmful drinking at age 14 predicted greater depressed mood two years later (Btotal =.09, 95%CI: .05 to .13, z = 4.2, p<.001) and 95% of this association was explained by deviant peers (Bindirect = 0.09, 95% CI: .07 to .12, z = 7.2, p<.001). After adjusting the model for depressed mood and deviant peers in early adolescence, the indirect effect was weakened but still present (Bindirect = .04, 95% CI: .02 to .06; Table 3), explaining 38% of the total effect (Figure S1b).

Table 3. Imputed data analyses: Model 2: Total, indirect and direct effects of harmful drinking (i.e., predictor) on depressed mood (i.e., outcome) via deviant peers (i.e., mediator) with [95% CI].

Step 0 Step 1 Step 2 Step 3 Step 4
Total Effect .092
[.05, .13]
.079
[.04, .12]
.042
[.01, .08]
.015
[-.03, .06]
-.026
[-.07, .01]
Indirect effect .087
[.07, .12]
.087
[.06, .11]
.071
[.05, .09]
.046
[.03, .07]
.039
[.02, .06]
Direct effect -.005
[-.05, .04]
-.008
[-.06, .04]
-.029
[-.08, .02]
-.032
[-.08, .02]
-.065
[-.11, -.02]
Effect proportion mediated
(indirect/total ratio)
95% 91% 71% 59% 38%

Step 0: Unadjusted for covariates

Step 1: Adjusted for background covariates: financial difficulties, family education level, parents’ alcohol consumption, and parents’ depression

Step 2: Adjusted for background covariates + conduct problems scale (SDQ)

Step 3: Adjusted for background covariates + conduct problems scale (SDQ) + earlier mediator at age 13 (i.e., deviant peers)

Step 4: Adjusted for background covariates + conduct problems scale (SDQ) + deviant peers at age 13 + earlier outcome at age 14 (i.e., depressed mood)

Moderation by gender

The MGCFA showed the unconstrained model offered a good fit to the data and constraining factor loadings across genders did not decrease the model fit (Table S6). We, therefore, conducted multi-group analyses with factor loadings and thresholds fixed to be equal across gender. These analyses showed that the total, indirect and direct effects did not differ for males and females (Table 4).

Table 4. Imputed data analysis: Total, indirect and direct effects [95% CI] for male and female respondents, estimated difference score (i.e., female-male) with [95%CIs) and p value. Model 1: Effect of depressed mood (i.e., predictor) on harmful drinking (i.e., outcome) via deviant peers (i.e., mediator) & Model 2: Effect of harmful drinking (i.e., predictor) on depressed mood (i.e., outcome) via deviant peers (i.e., mediator).

Model 1 Model 2

Estimate & 95%CIs Difference [95%CIs] Estimate & 95%Cis Difference [95%CIs]
Total effect Male .04 [-.02, .10] .47 [-.03, .13],
p= 0.2
.09 [.03, .16] -.01 [-.09, .07],
Female .09 [.04, .14] .08 [.03, .13] p = 0.8

Indirect effect Male .08 [.05, .12] .001 [-.04, .04],
p = 1.0
.12 [.07, .17] -.01 [-.07, .05],
Female .08 [.06, .11] .11 [.08, .14] p = 0.8

Direct effect Male -.04 [-.11, .02] .05 [-.03, .12],
p= 0.3
-.03 [-.10, .05] -.002 [-.10, .10],
Female .01 [-.05, .05] -.03 [-.09, .03] p= 1.0

Discussion

Our results showed that depressed mood experienced at age 14 was associated with greater harmful drinking at age 16 and, conversely, harmful drinking at age 14 was linked to higher levels of depressed mood two years later. This is, to our knowledge, the first study to indicate that affiliation with deviant peers mediates this association. These mechanisms were equivalent across gender. A previous study, based on ALSPAC data, found that depressed mood at age 10 predicted harmful drinking at 14 among females only [20]. No observed gender differences may be due to the fact that, by the age of 15, British male and female adolescents engage in similar drinking patterns [19]. Moreover, our findings show that the indirect mechanisms via peers were not explained by family environment or socio-economic status.

The indirect effect via peers was weakened, but remained present in both models, after adjusting for deviant peers at age 13. These results suggest that affiliation with deviant peers is not driven by the adolescents’ own deviant behaviour. Thus, results support the social learning hypothesis that adolescents, isolated by their normative peer group due to their depressed mood or alcohol misuse, engage with deviant peers. Subsequently, they adopt their behaviours and may experience greater depressed mood [8, 11].

Further sensitivity analyses showed that affiliation with deviant peers at age 15 accounted for approximately 40% of the relationship between harmful drinking at age 14 and depressed mood two years later (model 2) after adjusting for depressed mood at baseline. This suggests that affiliation with deviant peers creates a vulnerability to developing depressed mood in youngsters who engage in harmful drinking. After adjusting the model for earlier depressed mood, a negative direct link was found between harmful drinking at age 14 and depressed mood two years later. Thus, our results suggest that higher levels of depressed mood at age 16 are mostly elicited by affiliation with deviant peers and early depressed mood rather than the physiological effects of alcohol [38]. Our results seem to disentangle the association between harmful drinking at age 14 and depressed mood two years later by capturing two separate processes, which may be related to drinking motives [39]. Indeed, we observed a (positive) indirect effect via deviant peers which captures the mechanism for adolescents who, rejected by their normative peer, gravitate towards a deviant peer group and may engage in drinking to conform (i.e., social learning hypothesis). As discussed, affiliation with deviant peers leads to negative mood as they do not offer suitable support [8]. In contrast, the (negative) direct effect may capture drinking amongst those adolescents who engage in alcohol use to socialise. Our findings are in line with evidence that alcohol use is widespread among UK adolescents [19] and that drinking to socialise is a common practice in adolescence [40]. Drinking to socialise may not lead to depressed mood as it is associated with increased friendship quality and quantity [40]. Future studies aiming to replicate our findings and to further explore these hypotheses will shed further light on the differences in risk of depression amongst alcohol-using adolescents.

In contrast, adjusting for harmful drinking at baseline weakened both the total effect of depressed mood on harmful drinking at age 16 and its indirect effect via peers (model 1). Our study corroborates evidence that alcohol use in early adolescence is a strong predictor of drinking patterns in late adolescence [41]. Thus, a more extended timeframe starting before age 14 would allow more optimal opportunities to capture the relationship between depressed mood and later harmful alcohol use.

The strengths of our study include a large prospective cohort study design and measures validated in adolescence. The study suffers from attrition; however, we used multiple imputation methods to reduce bias and increase power [37]. There are further limitations to acknowledge. Two items from the AUDIT and SMFQ were rare in the present sample and, therefore, dropped from the analyses. These exclusions may limit comparisons with existing research findings. However, inclusion of these items would likely have hindered the predictive ability of our model and lead to bias. The item indexing the number of peers who had ever drunk alcohol was excluded from the peer’s construct as approximately 98% of the adolescents reported knowing someone who drinks. Items assessing peers’ alcohol consumption (i.e., quantity and frequency) would have allowed us to capture harmful drinking but were not available. A further limitation is that the term ‘friend’ was not further defined in the questionnaire. Hence, we are not able to describe the nature of the affiliation in more detail. Nonetheless, the term captures the wider peer network which is generally influential during adolescence [42, 43]. Finally, information on peers was reported by the study children themselves and this can lead to over-estimation and confirmation bias [44]. Nonetheless, research shows that perception of peers’ drinking is an important risk factor for heavy drinking in adolescence [45].

Owing to the fact that harmful drinking was assessed using age-appropriate but different measures, we were not able to conduct cross-lagged model analyses. Nonetheless, we adjusted the model for baseline measures to take into account the developmental nature of both harmful drinking and depressed mood. Finally, drinking measures were based on self-report, which may be subject to over- or under-reporting, however, the ALSPAC study team employed methods to ensure confidentiality procedures by ensuring anonymity and confidentiality [41].

In summary, our results indicate that effective intervention programs which focus on adolescent drinking and depression should take into account their co-occurrence. Mental health providers who are in contact with young people with depressed mood should be aware of the likelihood of harmful drinking. In addition, they may be able to identify adolescents at risk of depression among those engaging in harmful drinking and attending alcohol programs.

Our results also indicate that interventions should take into account adolescent’s peer affiliations and socialisation. These programs could aim to modify norms within the group and reduce peers’ influence on vulnerable individuals, thereby addressing group level processes and individual susceptibility to such processes. Indeed, universal, school-based programs, which challenge beliefs that alcohol misuse is normative and provide adolescents with intrapersonal skills (e.g., assertiveness), which boost their resistance self-efficacy, have been successful at reducing harmful drinking [46].

Supplementary Material

Figure S1
Tables S1-S4

Acknowledgments

We are extremely thankful to the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists and nurses. The UK Medical Research Council and the Wellcome Trust (Grant ref: 092731) and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. This publication is the work of the authors and Francesca Pesola and Marianne van den Bree will serve as guarantors for the contents of this paper. This research was funded by ERAB - The European Foundation for Alcohol Research (ref: EA 10 08) and Mental Health Research Network Cymru [ref: VANM/010410(36)].

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None.

Implications and Contribution:

Harmful alcohol use at age 14 was found to be linked to higher levels of depressed mood two years later. This association acted indirectly via affiliation with deviant peers at age 15 and suggests that isolation from the normative peer group due to alcohol misuse increases risk of depression.

References

  • 1.Saraceno L, Munafo M, Heron J, et al. Genetic and non-genetic influences on the development of co-occurring alcohol problem use and internalizing symptomatology in adolescence: a review. Addiction. 2009 Jul;104(7):1100–1121. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02571.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Crum RM, Green KM, Storr CL, et al. Depressed mood in childhood and subsequent alcohol use through adolescence and young adulthood. Archives of general psychiatry. 2008 Jun;65(6):702–712. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.6.702. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Marmorstein NR. Longitudinal associations between alcohol problems and depressive symptoms: Early adolescence through early adulthood. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2009;33(1):49–59. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00810.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Marmorstein NR. Longitudinal associations between depressive symptoms and alcohol problems: The influence of comorbid delinquent behavior. Addictive behaviors. 2010 Jun;35(6):564–571. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.01.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.MacKinnon DP, Krull JL, Lockwood CM. Equivalence of the mediation, confounding and suppression effect. Prev Sci. 2000 Dec;1(4):173–181. doi: 10.1023/a:1026595011371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Huang GC, Unger JB, Soto D, et al. Peer Influences: The Impact of Online and Offline Friendship Networks on Adolescent Smoking and Alcohol Use. Journal of Adolescent Health. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2013.07.001. (0) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Sieving RE, Perry CL, Williams CL. Do friendships change behaviors, or do behaviors change friendships? Examining paths of influence in young adolescents’ alcohol use. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2000;26(1):27–35. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(99)00056-7. 1// [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Brendgen M, Vitaro F, Bukowski WM. Deviant friends and early adolescents’ emotional and behavioral adjustment. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2000;10(2):173–189. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Mason WA, Hitchings JE, Spoth RL. The interaction of conduct problems and depressed mood in relation to adolescent substance involvement and peer substance use. Drug and alcohol dependence. 2008;96(3):233–248. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.03.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Glaser B, Shelton KH, van den Bree MB. The moderating role of close friends in the relationship between conduct problems and adolescent substance use. The Journal of adolescent health : official publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine. 2010 Jul;47(1):35–42. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.12.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Petraitis J, Flay BR, Miller TQ. Reviewing Theories of Adolescent Substance Use - Organizing Pieces in the Puzzle. Psychological Bulletin. 1995;117(1):67–86. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Fisher LA, Bauman KE. Influence and Selection In the Friend-adolescent Relationship: Findings from Studies of Adolescent Smoking and Drinking1. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 1988;18(4):289–314. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Osgood DW, Ragan DT, Wallace L, et al. Peers and the Emergence of Alcohol Use: Influence and Selection Processes in Adolescent Friendship Networks. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2013;23(3):500–512. doi: 10.1111/jora.12059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Mason WA, Hitchings JE, Spoth RL. The interaction of conduct problems and depressed mood in relation to adolescent substance involvement and peer substance use. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2008;96(3):233–248. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.03.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Arnett JJ. The Developmental Context of Substance use in Emerging Adulthood. Journal of Drug Issues. 2005 Apr 1;35(2):235–254. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Trudeau L, Mason WA, Randall GK, et al. Effects of Parenting and Deviant Peers on Early to Mid-Adolescent Conduct Problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2012 Nov 01;40(8):1249–1264. doi: 10.1007/s10802-012-9648-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Abela JRZ, Parkinson C, Stolow D, et al. A Test of the Integration of the Hopelessness and Response Styles Theories of Depression in Middle Adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2009 May 19;38(3):354–364. doi: 10.1080/15374410902851630. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Wells JE, Horwood LJ, Fergusson DM. Drinking patterns in mid-adolescence and psychosocial outcomes in late adolescence and early adulthood. Addiction. 2004 Dec;99(12):1529–1541. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00918.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hibell B, Guttormsson U, Ahlstrom S, et al. The 2011 ESPAD Report: Substance Use among Students in 36 European Countries. 2012 [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Saraceno L, Heron J, Munafo M, et al. The relationship between childhood depressive symptoms and problem alcohol use in early adolescence: findings from a large longitudinal population-based study. Addiction. 2012 Mar;107(3):567–577. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03662.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Strandheim A, Holmen TL, Coombes L, et al. Alcohol intoxication and mental health among adolescents--a population review of 8983 young people, 13-19 years in North-Trondelag, Norway: the Young-HUNT Study. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2009;3(1):18. doi: 10.1186/1753-2000-3-18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.ALSPAC. Bristol,UK: Department of Social Medicine, Bristol University; 2012. http://www.bris.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/data-access/data-dictionary/ (Archived by WebCite® at http://www.webcitation.org/6HTBgPiAd) [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Emond A, Doemer R, Mytton J, et al. Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: Longitudinal Analysis of Risk of Injury in the Road Environment in Childhood and Adolescence. Department for Transport; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Bucholz KK, Cadoret R, Cloninger CR, et al. A new, semi-structured psychiatric interview for use in genetic linkage studies: a report on the reliability of the SSAGA. Journal of studies on alcohol. 1994;55(2):149–158. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1994.55.149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Babor T, Higgins-Biddle J, Saunders J, et al. The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test: Guidelines for Ase in Primary Care. 2nd. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Chung T, Colby SM, Barnett NP, et al. Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test: Factor Structure in an Adolescent Emergency Department Sample. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Research. 2002;26(2):223–231. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Angold A, Costello EJ, Messer SC, et al. Development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. Int J Method Psych. 1995 Dec;5(4):237–249. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ. Prospective childhood predictors of deviant peer affiliations in adolescence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1999 May;40(4):581–592. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Goodman R. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997 Jul;38(5):581–586. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User’s Guide. Sixth Edition. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 1998-2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Hooper D, Coughlan J, Mullen MR. Structural Equation Modelling: Guidelines for Determining Model Fit. Electronic Journal of Business Research Methods. 2008;6(1):53–60. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.MacKinnon DP, Lockwood CM, Hoffman JM, et al. A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychol Methods. 2002 Mar;7(1):83–104. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.7.1.83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.MacKinnon D. Introduction to Statistical Mediation Analysis. New York: Taylor & Francis Group; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Cole DA, Maxwell SE. Testing mediational models with longitudinal data: questions and tips in the use of structural equation modeling. J Abnorm Psychol. 2003 Nov;112(4):558–577. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.112.4.558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Milfont T, Fischer R. Testing measurement invariance across groups: Applications in cross-cultural research. International Journal of Psychological Research. 2010;3:112–131. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Young AF, Powers JR, Bell SL. Attrition in longitudinal studies: who do you lose? Aust N Z J Public Health. 2006 Aug;30(4):353–361. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-842x.2006.tb00849.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.White IR, Royston P, Wood AM. Multiple imputation using chained equations: Issues and guidance for practice. Statistics in Medicine. 2011;30(4):377–399. doi: 10.1002/sim.4067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Jacobus J, Tapert SF. Neurotoxic Effects of Alcohol in Adolescence. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2012 Dec 10; doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185610. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Kuntsche E, Knibbe R, Gmel G, et al. Why do young people drink? A review of drinking motives. Clin Psychol Rev. 2005 Nov;25(7):841–861. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Hoel S, Eriksen BM, Breidablik HJ, et al. Adolescent alcohol use, psychological health, and social integration. Scandinavian journal of public health. 2004;32(5):361–367. doi: 10.1080/14034940410027894. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Heron J, Macleod J, Munafo MR, et al. Patterns of alcohol use in early adolescence predict problem use at age 16. Alcohol Alcohol. 2012 Mar-Apr;47(2):169–177. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agr156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Mundt MP. The impact of peer social networks on adolescent alcohol use initiation. Academic pediatrics. 2011 Sep-Oct;11(5):414–421. doi: 10.1016/j.acap.2011.05.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Shakya HB, Christakis NA, Fowler JH. Parental influence on substance use in adolescent social networks. Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine. 2012 Dec;166(12):1132–1139. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2012.1372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Bekman NM, Cummins K, Brown SA. Affective and personality risk and cognitive mediators of initial adolescent alcohol use. Journal of studies on alcohol and drugs. 2010 Jul;71(4):570–580. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2010.71.570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Donovan JE. Adolescent alcohol initiation: A review of psychosocial risk factors. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2004;35(6):529.e527–529.e518. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2004.02.003. 12// [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Faggiano F, Vigna-Taglianti F, Burkhart G, et al. The effectiveness of a school-based substance abuse prevention program: 18-Month follow-up of the EU-Dap cluster randomized controlled trial. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2010;108(1–2):56–64. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.11.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Figure S1
Tables S1-S4

RESOURCES