Abstract
Performing two competing tasks can result in dividing cognitive resources between the tasks and impaired motor adaptation. In previous work we have reported impaired learning when participants had to switch from one visual displacement adaptation task to another. Here we examined whether or not a secondary task had a similar effect on adaptation to a visual displacement . The resource dividing task involved simultaneously adapting to a step visual displacement whilst vocally shadowing an auditory stimulus . The switching task required participants to adapt to opposing visual displacements in an alternating manner with the left and right hands. We found that both manipulations had a detrimental effect on adaptation rate. We then integrated these tasks and found the combination caused a greater decrease in adaptation rate than either manipulation in isolation. Experiment 2 showed that adaptation to a gradually imposed visual displacement was influenced in a similar manner to step adaptation. Therefore although gradual adaptation involves minimal awareness it still can be disrupted by a cognitively demanding secondary task. We propose that awareness and cognitive resource can be regarded as qualitatively different but that awareness may be a marker of the amount of resource required. For example, large errors are both noticed and require substantial cognitive resource to connect. However a lack of awareness does not mean an adaptation task will be resistant to interference from a resource consuming secondary task.
Keywords: Attention, Cognitive, Interference, Learning, Motor Adaptation
Introduction
During motor adaptation a distinction is often made between explicit and implicit levels of information processing (Hwang et al. 2006; Malfait and Ostry, 2004; Vangheluwe et al. 2006). Specifically, during adaptation to gradual changes, limited visual and kinesthetic input arise from the perturbation – these sesnory inputs occur beyond awareness (Ingram et al. 2000; Klassen et al. 2005; Malfait and Ostry, 2004) and thus the resulting adaptation is regarded as implicit. In contrast, adaptation to abrupt changes in the environment such as a sudden load or prismatic displacement relies substantially on the use of high-level explicit (cognitive) information which is independent of the implicit, predictive changes in muscle activity (Criscimagna-Hemminger et al. 2003; Malfait and Ostry, 2004). An assumption underlying this separation is that explicit tasks have greater engagement of attention and cognitive resources than implicit tasks. However Mazzoni and Krakauer (2006) showed that an explicit adaptation strategy does not interfere with the underlying adaptation to a step visual displacement.. Though this suggests that step and gradual adaptation may both involve implicit processes, there is still the distinction that step adaptation involves awareness: it has an additional explicit component that requires cognitive resources.
Secondary tasks have often been used to assess the cognitive requirements of an adaptive task. Eversheim and Bock (2001) showed that four separate secondary tasks interfered with adaptation to a step visual displacement. Importantly the time-course of adaptation was different for each task, suggesting interference was sensitive to the resource requirements of the secondary task. In addition Taylor and Thoroughman (2007) showed a resource dividing secondary task which required participants to concurrently perform an auditory frequency discrimination task resulted in impaired adaptation to an abrupt dynamic force-field. We have previously reported a similar result with a switching task in which the two hands adapted to opposing step visual displacements during alternating unimanual movements (Galea and Miall, 2006). Despite the apparent similarity of the two results, it is possible that the interference observed from task switching in our alternating task is fundamentally different from the resource dividing task effects reported by Taylor and Thoroughman (2007) and Eversheim and Bock (2001).
As previously mentioned gradual adaptation has been shown to occur without awareness; supporting the notion that it is implicit (Ingram et al. 2000; Klassen et al. 2005; Malfait and Ostry, 2004). However, it is not known whether substantial cognitive resources are still required for the task. Hence, the aim of this study was to assess how dividing and switching secondary tasks influenced the rate of adaptation during a visuomotor displacement that either involved a large (step) or small (gradual) amount of visual error. It is possible that the secondary tasks may only influence adaptation when the errors are large, which would engage high cognitive demands. In Experiment 1, the effects of a dividing and switching task on the learning of a single large visuomotor displacement are investigated. In Experiment 2, the effects of a dividing and switching task on the learning of a gradually increasing visuomotor displacement are investigated.
Experiment 1
Experiment 1 was conducted to assess whether a secondary cognitive task (resource dividing) had a different effect on unimanual adaptation than alternating adaptation to a visual displacement in the opposite limb (resource or task switching). Task switching could require switching between either a learning environment (i.e., learning a single-step clockwise rotation with the right hand and a gradual counter-clockwise rotation with the left hand) or switching between two more similar (but independent) learning environments (i.e., learning different singlestep rotations with the right and left hands). In addition, the combination of a switching (opposite limb adaptation) and dividing (secondary cognitive task) task on adaptation rate was assessed. This provided a baseline as to the amount of interference observed during adaptation which could then be used during experiment two to assess whether similar interference occurred during gradual adaptation.
Method
Participants
36 right-handed participants (25 female; age=20 ±2 years, mean±SD) participated in the experiment. Handedness (mean = 75, decile R.4) was assessed using the ten-item version of the Edinburgh inventory (Oldfield, 1971). All participants signed a consent form and were either given course credit or financial compensation for their participation. This study was approved by the school ethics committee and was in accordance with ethical standards of the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki.
Materials
Each participant sat at a table facing a computer screen approximately 1m in front of them. A marker from an Optotrak (NDI, Ontario, Canada) position tracking system was attached to the index finger of each hand. Goggles were worn with horizontal blinders that ensured the participant could see the screen, but their arms were not visible.
The experimental set up is shown in figure 1. The presentation on the computer screen consisted of two white solid discs (20mm in diameter) indicating in real-time the 2-D position of the participant’s index fingers on the tabletop, and four yellow circles (35mm in diameter). The two yellow circles at the bottom of the screen were the starting positions (140mm horizontally separate) while the two at the top represented the targets. The targets were 120mm vertically parallel to the starting positions. An angular displacement could be applied to either disc representing their finger positions. This rotation of visual feedback was calculated relative to the starting circle of each hand, in a clockwise (CW) or counter clockwise (CCW) direction.
Procedure
The participants were asked to place their index fingers on two markers which indicated the starting positions on the table. These were slightly raised and textured, providing tactile cues of the starting positions. The participants were informed that their index fingertips were the only part of their body which should be in contact with the table. They were told that the experiment involved making forward pointing movements on the table in front of them, which corresponded to upward vertical movements of the cursors on the screen.
The participants were informed that when a target circle turned green they should make a pointing movement towards that target with the corresponding finger while ensuring their other finger did not leave the starting position. The participants had online feedback (white discs) but were instructed to make a single straight movement towards the target and once their finger had touched the table to not make any additional corrections. Movement time was constrained to 300-600ms; if this was not achieved the disc representing their finger changed colour to red if they were too slow and blue if they were too fast.
The starting circles then turned green indicating to the participant that they should move back. When both fingers were again placed within the starting positions a ‘dwell’ period began and the participants had to wait for the next target to turn green. The ‘dwell’ time was varied from 2 to 8 seconds (mean = 4 seconds) so that participants could not anticipate when to move and were unable to engage in a rhythmic movement. The instructions to the participants placed an emphasis on producing fast and straight movements to the target.
There were 6 groups (n=6) with 3 test phases (table 1). For all groups the pre-adaptation and post-adaptation phases consisted of 20 trials for each active limb. For all groups only one hand was active at any one time. For unimanual groups only the right hand was active. For the alternating groups each hand moved towards a target in an alternating fashion.
Table. 1. Experiment one groups (n =6).
Group (n = 6) |
Pre-adaptation 20 trials for each active limb |
Adaptation 60 trials for each active limb |
Post-adaptation 20 trials for each active limb |
---|---|---|---|
Rs- | null | R = CW | null |
Rs+ | null | R = CW (+ secondary task) |
null |
RsLs- | null | R = CW L = CCW |
null |
RsLs+ | null | R = CW L = CCW (+ secondary task) |
null |
RsLg- | null | R = CW L = gradual CCW |
null |
RsLg+ | null | R = CW L = gradual CCW (+ secondary task) |
null |
For two groups the pre-adaptation and adaptation phases consisted of unimanual right hand pointing movements performed independently (Rs-) or with an additional secondary cognitive task (Rs+). For the four other groups, the pre-adaptation and adaptation phases involved alternating left and right hand pointing movements performed either independently (RsLs-, RsLg-) or with an additional secondary cognitive task (RsLs+, RsLg+). The pre-adaptation phase was always under veridical conditions. The adaptation phase involved a 30-degree clockwise (CW) step (s) visual displacement for the right hand. For the RsLs- and RsLs+ groups the left hand was exposed to a 30-degree counter clockwise (CCW) step displacement. In contrast within the RsLg- and RsLg+ groups the left hand was exposed to a CCW gradual (g) displacement, which began at 0-degrees and incremented by 0.25-degrees per trial. During the post-adaptation phase all groups moved towards targets under veridical conditions, with no secondary cognitive task.
These six groups experienced conditions with different combinations of attentional components. The Rs- group experienced a single adaptation task and is used as the adaptation baseline. The Rs+, RsLs- and RsLg- groups each experienced two tasks: the Rs+ group experienced unimanual adaptation and a secondary shadowing task while the RsLs- and RsLg- groups experienced opposing adaptation in the left and right hands. Comparing these groups will identify whether there are different effects on the adaptation rate when the task is constantly divided or repeatedly switched. The RsLs+ and RsLg+ groups experienced three tasks as the adaptation phase involved opposing adaptation in the left and right hands along with a secondary shadowing task. These groups will show the effects of simultaneously switching and dividing the task.
The secondary vocal shadowing task involved the participants listening to an audio story through headphones and repeating it back to the experimenter word for word. Each participant was told to concentrate on this task and to try and not miss any words. The secondary task ensured the participant was constantly engaged throughout the experiment, ensuring that attention was divided during the entire performance of the adaptation tasks. Taylor and Thoroughman (2007) have previously shown that an audio discrimination task can disrupt motor adaptation.
Analysis
The 2-D position of each index finger in the horizontal plane was continuously recorded at a rate of 60Hz via the Optotrak system. Using Matlab (Mathworks, Matick, MA) all kinematic data were filtered at 10Hz with a low-pass Butterworth filter and numerically differentiated to calculate 2-D velocity. The onset and end of each movement were determined as the point at which velocity crossed 10% of the peak velocity for each movement. In addition the endpoint had to represent 90% of the distance between the starting position and the target for the trial to be regarded as valid.
Angle at maximum velocity (angle) was calculated as the angular difference between a straight line from the start position to the target and the positional marker at maximum velocity (Sainburg and Wang, 2002). For each participant, an average pre-adaptation (baseline) value was subtracted from all adaptation and post-adaptation trials.
For each participant a separate state-space model was applied to the angle data for the adaptation and post-adaptation phases to estimate an adaptation coefficient (Diedrichsen et al. 2005; Donchin et al. 2003; Thoroughman and Shadmehr, 2000).
(Eq.1) |
(Eq.2) |
The error on trial n is noted by en and is expressed as a function of the perturbation pn, the internal state of the system sn and noise (En). The second equation states that the internal state (s) changes by a certain proportion of the experienced error, where the amount of change is determined by the adaptation rate A. The matlab function nfitlin was used to estimate the free parameter A, in order to maximize the fit between observed and unstructed errors.
The adaptation rate (A) was used in a two-way between-subjects ANOVA comparing the factors TASK (step, gradual, none) and LOAD (shadowing, none). Note that the task manipulation refers to the task performed by the left hand, and the data analyzed are from the right hand. This allows us to assess the impact of TASK on step adaptation performance within the right hand. Independent t-tests were performed on all significant interactions. For the post-adaptation phase a Pearson correlation was performed which compared each participant’s adaptation rate with their de-adaptaion rate within the post-adaptation phase. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM, and effects were considered significant if p≤0.05.
Results
Summary
Figure 2 shows a typical participant’s performance within the Rs- group. The pre-adaptation phase was characterised by fairly accurate performance (A). When initially exposed to the visual displacement the participant produced large errors in their movement (B). However, over subsequent trials participants adapted to the displacement and produced similar performance to baseline (D). When the displacements were suddenly removed, large aftereffects occurred in the opposite direction (C). The same pattern of performance was observed within all groups.
In our previous paper (Galea and Miall, 2006), only end position error showed differences between alternating and unimanual performance. However a pilot study of the current task revealed observable differences for end position error, average displacement error and angle at maximum velocity (angle). It is possible that within our previous study, where the movement was 30cm, instead of 12cm, angle at maximum velocity was not an appropriate or subtle enough measurement due to the large amount of online correction which took place during the longer duration movement. Since Taylor and Thoroughman (2007) showed that dividing attention impaired motor adaptation but not online correction, we decided to concentrate on the feed-forward aspect of the movement which reflects the state of the internal model (Tseng et al. 2007) and therefore we focus our analyses on angle.
Pre-adaptation
The groups were compared across the last 10 trials of the pre-adaptaiton phase for angle. A 3 (TASK: step, gradual, none) × 2 (LOAD: shadowing, none) between-subjects two-way ANOVA was used to compare the average of these 10 trials for each paticipant across the groups. Note that the task manipulation refers to the task performed by the left hand, and the data analyzed are from the right hand. The main effect of TASK (F(2,30)=0.4, p=0.5), LOAD (F(1,30)=0.005, p=0.9) and the interaction between TASK and LOAD (F(2,30)=0.005, p=0.9) were all non-significant (Rs-= 1.1°±1.3°, Rs+= 1.1°±1.2°, RsLs-= 1.8°±1.1°, RsLs+= 1.9°±1.1°, RsLg-= 1.6°±1.8°, RsLg+= 2.0°±1.3°, mean±SEM). Thus baseline performance across the groups was equivalent.
Adaptation
Each group’s performance was characterised by a large initial error which was rapidly reduced over subsequent trials. For each participant, an average was calculated across the initial 60 trials of right-hand adaptation for total movement time, time to peak velocity and peak angular velocity. Two-way between subject ANOVAs [TASK(3) × LOAD(2)] compared the right hand performance across the 6 groups. There were no significant main effect of TASK (F(2,30)<1.6, p>0.2), LOAD (F(1,30) <0.05, p>0.8) or interaction between TASK and LOAD (F(2,30) <0.7, p>0.4) for total movement time (mean= 496 ± 40ms), time to peak velocity (mean=172 ± 22ms) or peak velocity (mean=0.52 ± 0.42m/sec).
The left and right hand’s performance within the RsLs- and the RsLs+ group were compared using two seperate repeated-measures ANOVAs [HAND(2) × TRIAL(10) ]. For both group comparisons the main effect of HAND was not significant (each F(1, 100)<2.8, each p>0.09), indicating similar adaptation across the left and right hands.
To assess adaptation, a state-space model (Eq. 1/2) was fitted to the individual participants’ angle data (nonlinear least squares fit, mean R2 = 0.58). Figure 3(A) shows the predicted curves for a sample participant within each group while figure 3(B) shows the average adaptation rate (A: Eq.2) for each group. A two-way between subject ANOVA [TASK: step, gradual, none) × LOAD (shadowing, none)] compared the adaptation rate across the six groups. There was a significant effect for TASK (F(2,30)=41, p=0.0005), LOAD (F(1,30)=95, p=0.0005) and these factors interact (F(2,30)=5.3, p=0.01; figure 3B). Independent t-tests revealed that the rate of adaptation for the baseline group (Rs-: 0.71±0.03, mean±SEM) was significantly faster than the five other groups (t(10)>3.6, p<0.005, one-tailed). Performing the baseline task with the right hand and gradually adapting with the left hand (RsLg-: 0.57±0.02) lead to faster adaptation than when shadowing (Rs+: 0.28±0.01; RsLg+: 0.22±0.06; RsLs+: 0.06±0.03) or switching between two opposing step-perturbations (RsLs-: 0.23±0.05), (t(10)>5.0, p<0.001, one-tailed).There were no significant differences between the remaining groups with just one additional cognitive demand (Rs+, RsLs-),or the group which switched between step and gradual and were exposed to the shadowing task (RsLg+; t(10)<1.1, p>0.3, two-tailed). The tasks with one additional cognitive demand (Rs+, RsLs- and RsLg-) and the task which involved two additional cognitive demands however one was switching between step and gradual adaptation (RsLg+) also lead to faster adaptation than when two additional cognitive demands were present (RsLs+; t(10)>2.2, p<0.05, one-tailed).
The error rates for the secondary cognitive task were only recorded for the Rs+ and RsLs+ groups. This was taken as the number of words either missed or not correctly repeated by the participant. An independent t-test revealed that the RsLs+ group exhibited significantly greater error than the Rs+ group (30±3% vs. 17±2%, respectively; t(10)=3.4, p=0.003).
Post-adaptation
For all groups post-adaptation was characterized by a large initial error which was corrected over subsequent trials (deadaptation; figure 2D). To assess the rate of deadaptation following the reintroduction of the veridical environment a state-space model (Eq. 1/2) was applied to the individual participant angle data (nonlinear least squares fit, mean R2 = 0.62). The deadaptation rate (A) value was then correlated with the participant’s adaptation rate, with the data being collapsed across groups, and a positive correlation was present (r=0.51, n=36, p=0.01, two-tailed; figure 5A).
Discussion
The aim of experiment 1 was to directly assess the effects of either a secondary resource switching or dividing task on adaptation to a single-step visual displacement. All groups adapted to the visual displacement and showed aftereffects in the post-adaptation phase suggesting that participants formed an internal model of the new sensorimotor transformation and were not simply performing a strategic adjustment (Bock et al. 2005).
As the Rs- group had a significantly faster adaptation rate than the other five groups, it is clear that the switching and dividing tasks had a detrimental effect on the speed at which participants adapt to a suddenly imposed visual displacement. As there were no significant differences between the adaptation rates of the Rs+, RsLs- and RsLg+ groups, it may be the case that the alternating motor task (switching) and secondary cognitive task (dividing) had a similar effect on performance. A combination of these tasks (RsLs+) had an additive detrimental effect on the rate of adaptation.
Gradual adaptation by one arm, used as a switching task within the RsLg- group, had only a slight detrimental effect on adaptation of the other arm. As a result the RsLg+ group behaved similarly to the groups challenged with dual-tasks that required either switching between (RsLs-) or divided (Rs+) resources available to the adaptation task. This suggests that gradual adaptation involved minimal cognitive resources (this is further explored in experiment 2).
The RsLs+ group was exposed to a combination of switching (alternating task) and dividing (secondary cognitive task) tasks. The reduced adaptation rate in comparison to the Rs+ and RsLs- groups suggest that these processes had an additive effect on performance. Importantly, the RsLs+ group had significantly more errors within the secondary cognitive task than the Rs+ group. This supports the view that the alternating adaptation of both limbs demanded a greater amount of cognitive resources than unimanual adaptation. As only one arm was moving at any one time this is unlikely to be the result of the other limb acting like an additional dividing task and that the increase in error was a result of switching between the two adaptive states.
In direct contrast to the present results, Bock et al. (2005) found no significant difference between unimanual and alternating adaptation. However, because they only compared the groups at one time point it is possible that their measure of performance might not have been sensitive enough to assess differences in the rate of adaptation.
As noted above, there was a particularly interesting difference between the RsLg- group and the RsLs-, in that gradual adaptation within the opposite limb did not have the same detrimental effect as step adaptation. This may suggest that gradual adaptation does not require the same cognitive resources as step adaptation. We decided to explore this possibility further.
Experiment 2
Experiment 1 showed that step adaptation is a cognitively demanding process which can be negatively influenced by switching and dividing tasks. It has been proposed that step and gradual motor adaptation may occur at an explicit and implicit level of awareness, respectively (Hwang et al. 2006; Malfait and Ostry, 2004). Additionally, gradual adaptation has been shown to occur when awareness is absent; supporting the notion that it is implicit (Ingram et al. 2000; Klassen et al. 2005; Malfait and Ostry, 2004). However, Mazzoni and Krakauer (2006) showed that explicit information does not benefit the adaptation rate to a step visual displacement. While this suggests that step adaptation may involve implicit processes , the present results clearly show that it can be negatively influenced by cognitively demanding secondary tasks.
Thus, it is unclear whether gradual adaptation also requires cognitive resources which may be affected by cognitively demanding switching and dividing tasks.
Experiment 2 was similar to experiment 1 but the right hand was always exposed to a gradual visual displacement. We hypothesize that if gradual adaptation occurs without a significant reliance on cognitive resources, then the addition of dividing and switching tasks should not affect the adaptation rate. However, if gradual adaptation requires substantial cognitive resources, despite the absence of awareness, then learning rates should be negatively influenced by the dividing and switching tasks.
Method
Participants
36 right-handed participants (19 female, mean age: 22 ± 2 years) participated in the experiment. All participants signed a consent form and were either given course credit or money for their participation.
Materials, Procedure and Data analysis
The materials and general procedure were identical to the previous experiment. There were six new groups (n=6) that adapted to a gradual displacement imposed on their right arm, which began at 0degrees and incremented by 0.25degrees per trial up to 30 degrees. The displacement angle was then held at 30degrees for an additional 12 trials. Each active limb performed 132 trials during the adaptation phase. The six groups (Rg-, Rg+, RgLg-, RgLg+, RgLs-, RgLs+; table 2) matched those of experiment 1 except that they experienced gradual adaptation with their right arm instead of step adaptation.
Table. 2. Experiment two groups (n =6).
Group (n = 6) |
Pre-adaptation 20 trials for each active limb |
Adaptation 132 trials for each active limb |
Post-adaptation 20 trials for each active limb |
---|---|---|---|
Rg- | null | R = gradual CW | null |
Rg+ | null | R = gradual CW (+ secondary task) |
null |
RgLg- | null | R = gradual CW L = gradual CCW |
null |
RgLg+ | null | R = gradual CW L = gradual CCW (+ secondary task) |
null |
RgLs- | null | R = gradual CW L = CCW |
null |
RgLs+ | null | R = gradual CW L = CCW (+ secondary task) |
null |
The state space model described in Eq.1/2 was again applied to each participant’s angle data. The estimated adaptation rate was used in a two-way between-subjects ANOVA comparing the factors TASK (gradual, step, none) and LOAD (shadowing, none). Note that the task manipulation refers to the task performed by the left hand, and the data analyzed are from the right hand. Independent t-tests were performed on all significant interactions. For the post-adaptation phase a Pearson correlation was performed which compared each participant’s adaptation rate with their deadaptaion rate within the post-adaptation phase.
Results
Pre-adaptation
A 3 (TASK: gradual, step, none) × 2 (LOAD: shadowing, none) between-subjects two-way ANOVA compared an average of the last 10 trials of the pre-adaptation phase for each paticipant across the groups. The main effect of TASK (F(2, 30)<2.7, p>0.08), LOAD (F(1, 30) <0.03, p>0.8) and the interaction between TASK and LOAD (F(2, 30) <0.2, p>0.5) where all non-significant (Rg-= 0.9°±1.5°, Rg+= 1.2°±1.3°, RgLg-= 0.8°±1.6°, RgLg+= 2.1°±1.0°, RgLs-= 1.5°±1.3°, RgLs+= 1.3°±1.4°, mean±SEM).
Adaptation
Each group’s performance was initially characterised by a small amount of error which gradually increased across subsequent trials. Importantly when asked at the end of the study if they noticed a visual displacement during the adaptation phase the Rg- and RgLg- groups reported no awareness of the perturbation.
For each participant, an average was calculated across the initial 122 trials of right-hand adaptation for total movement time, time to peak velocity and peak angular velocity. Two-way between subject ANOVAs [TASK(3) x LOAD(2)] compared the right hand performance across the 6 groups. There were no significant main effect of TASK (F(2, 30) <1, p>0.3, LOAD (F(1, 30) <0.06, p>0.7) or interaction between TASK and LOAD (F(2, 30) <1.1, p>0.2) for total movement time (mean= 480 ± 56ms), time to peak velocity (mean=163 ± 34ms) or peak velocity (mean=0.58 ± 0.22m/sec).
To assess adaptation, a state-space model (Eq. 1/2) was fitted to the individual participant angle data (nonlinear least squares fit, mean R2 =0.52). Figure 4(A) shows the predicted curves for a sample participant within each group while figure 4(B) shows the average adaptation rate (A: Eq.2) for each group. A two-way between subject ANOVA [TASK: step, gradual, none) x LOAD (shadowing, none)] compared the adaptation rate across the six groups. There was a significant effect for TASK (F(2, 30) =20, p=0.001), LOAD (F(1, 30) =63, p=0.001) and interaction (F(2, 30) =6.5, p=0.004; figure 4B). Independent t-tests revealed that the rate of adaptation for the baseline group (Rg-: 0.1±0.01, mean±SEM) was significantly larger than all the other groups (t(10)>4.4, p<0.002, one-tailed), except for switching between two gradual-perturbations (RgLg-: 0.09±0.004). This group was also faster than the tasks involving one additional cognitive demand (Rg+ 0.04±0.003; RgLs-: 0.04±0.005) or two additional cognitive demands (RgLg+: 0.04±0.007; RgLs+: 0.02±0.005; t(10)>6.5, p<0.002, one-tailed). There were no significant differences between the groups which involved either a switching (RgLs-) or shadowing (Rg+) task or the group which involved a switching task between two gradual-perturbations and a shadowing task (RgLg+; t(10)<0.3, p>0.7). These three groups had a faster adaptation rate than the group which involved switching between a step and gradual perturbation and a shadowing task (RgLs+; t(10)>2, p<0.05).
In summary the adaptation phase showed that the Rg- and RgLg- groups adapted at a faster rate than the four other groups. The gradual adaptation used as a switching task only had a marginal effect on adaptation and so the RgLg+ group had a similar adaptation rate to the two other groups exposed to either the switching or dividing task (Rg+, RgLs-). These were all faster than the RgLs+ group who were exposed to a step adaptation switching task and dividing task.
Finally we wanted to assess the adaptation rate for the left hand for the switching task which either involved gradual adaptation in the right hand (RgLg-) or step adaptation (RsLg-). The adaptation rate for the left hand was significantly slower within the RsLg- group (0.05±0.004) than the RgLg- group (0.08±0.002; t(10)=3.1, p=0.01). This suggests that during the switching task, step adaptation within the left or right hand significantly slowed the learning rate for gradual adaptation in the opposite hand but not vice-versa.
Post-adaptation
To assess the deadaptation that occurred following the reintroduction of the veridical environment a state-space model (Eq. 1/2) was applied to the individual participant angle data (nonlinear least squares fit, mean R2 = 0.66). The deadaptation rate (A) value was then correlated with the participant’s adaptation rate with the data being collapsed across groups. A Pearson’s correlation revealed a significant positive correlation (r=0.55, n=36, p=0.01, two-tailed; figure 5B).
Discussion
The aim of this study was to assess how cognitively demanding secondary tasks influence the rate of adaptation to either a step or gradual visual displacement. We found that both were negatively influenced by a switching task, a vocal echoing task or a combination of both.
Previous research has proposed an explicit and implicit level of information processing within motor adaptation (Hwang et al. 2006; Malfait and Ostry,2004; Vangheluwe et al. 2006). Gradual adaptation has been shown to occur without awareness (Ingram et al. 2000; Klassen et al. 2005; Malfait and Ostry, 2004) and thus is regarded as implicit. In contrast, step adaptation was thought to rely heavily on the use of high-level explicit (cognitive) information which is independent of the implicit, predictive changes in muscle activity (Criscimagna-Hemminger et al. 2003; Malfait and Ostry, 2004). However Mazzoni and Krakauer (2006) showed that conflicting explicit information does not interfere with the adaptation rate to a step visual displacement; suggesting that both step and gradual adaptation may actually be implicit.
The current results suggest a distinction between awareness and cognitive load during adaptation. There was a clear difference between step and gradual adaptation. Step adaptation invoked a large initial error which was quickly corrected for whereas gradual adaptation involved small initial errors which steadily increased (because of incomplete adaptation). Step adaptation induced awareness of the perturbation whereas gradual adaptation did not. If cognitive load and awareness during adaptation depend on the same underlying process then step adaptation should be negatively influenced by the switching and dividing task whereas gradual adaptation should not. However as both forms of adaptation were negatively influenced by the cognitively demanding secondary tasks, it suggests that each involves a resource consuming cognitive component. Conversely gradual adaptation within the switching task only had a minimal affect on adaptation rate indicating that it does not consume substantial cognitive resources. This leads to the paradox of step adaptation slowing the learning rate of gradual adaptation whereas gradual adaptation does not slow the learning rate of step adaptation. It is possible that step adaptation may consume more of a common resource than gradual adaptation, with this resource also being used by the secondary cognitive task. When gradual adaptation is being performed with step adaptation as the secondary task, then step adaptation steals significant resources. However when gradual adaptation is the secondary task it steals very little resource. To support this view we have shown that when gradual adaptation in the left hand is the secondary task, the right hand’s learning rate is slower for step adaptation in comparison to gradual adaptation. However the learning rate of step adaptation in the right hand is not affected. This provides additional evidence that the resource consuming step adaptation slows gradual adaptation but not vice-versa.
It must be noted that awareness was not quantitatively assessed across the two studies. However when the Rg- and RgLg- groups were asked at the end of the study if they noticed a visual displacement during the adaptation phase they reported being unaware. The average error across the adaptation phase was 4 degrees for the Rg- group; however errors in the other groups were larger (RgLg+, RgLs-, RgLs+) so we cannot be certain participants remained unaware. Slachevsky et al. (2001) used a similar visual adaptation task where the displacement was increased by one degree per trial and asked participants after each trial whether they were aware of the visual displacement. The authors found that participants remained unaware until approximately 30degrees. In our experiment, the angle was incremented by 0.25 degrees per trial, so we propose that gradual adaptation would have involved minimal awareness.
Finally, a slower deadaptation rate in the post-adaptation phase would suggest better retention of the information learnt during the adaptation phase (Hadipour-Niktarash et al. 2007). The rate of adaptation and deadaptation was correlated across both step and gradual adaptation. This implies that by increasing the cognitive demands of the task and as a result causing adaptation to take longer, the information was retained for longer during deadaptation. This is consistent with evidence that gradual adaptation is more robust (Ingram et al, 2000, Klassen et al, 2005). This supports the view that the rate at which the initial adaptation is learnt directly influences retention (Smith et al. 2006). Smith et al. (2006) propose that motor adaptation depends on at least two distinct neural systems that retain information at different rates, namely a fast and slow system. They propose that information which is learnt more slowly will be retained for longer than information which is learnt faster. The present results support this idea and suggest that the adaptation rate weighting system which affects retention can be manipulated by introducing task switching or dividing tasks during adaptation to a step or gradual visual displacement.
In conclusion, the present study provides evidence that adaptation to a step or gradual visual displacement can be disrupted with cognitively demanding secondary tasks. Importantly, step adaptation slows the learning rate of gradual adaptation however gradual adaptation does not slow the learning rate of step adaptation. This suggests that during the switching task these adaptation processes share a common cognitive resource. When gradual adaptation is being performed with step adaptation as the secondary task, step adaptation consumes a lot of cognitive resources. However when gradual adaptation is the secondary task it has more limited cognitive resource demands, so it does not influence the step adaptation learning rate. Therefore although gradual adaptation involves minimal awareness it can still be disrupted with a cognitively demanding secondary task but not vice-versa. We propose that awareness and cognitive resources can be regarded as qualitatively different but that awareness is maybe a marker of the amount of resources allocated to a task.
Acknowledgments
We thank Jonathan Winter and Steve Caulder for technical assistance. This work was supported by a grant from the Wellcome trust and an EPSRC studentship.
References
- Bock O, Worringham C, Thomas M. Concurrent adaptations of left and right arms to opposite visual distortions. Experimental Brain Research. 2005;162(4):513–519. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-2222-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Criscimagna-Hemminger SE, Donchin O, Gazzaniga MS, Shadmehr R. Learned dynamics of reaching movements generalize from dominant to nondominant arm. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2003;89:168–176. doi: 10.1152/jn.00622.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diedrichsen J, Hashambhoy Y, Rane T, Shadmehr R. Neural correlates of reach errors. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:9919–31. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1874-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donchin O, Francis JT, Shadmehr R. Quantifying generalization from trial-by-trial behavior of adaptive systems that learn with basis functions: theory and experiments in human motor control. Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23:9032–45. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-27-09032.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eversheim U, Bock O. Evidence for processing stages in skill acquisition: a dual-task study. Learning and Memory. 2001;8(4):183–9. doi: 10.1101/lm.39301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galea JM, Miall RC. Concurrent adaptation to opposing visual displacements during an alternating movement. Experimental Brain Research. 2006;175:676–688. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0585-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hadipour-Niktarash A, Lee CK, Desmond JE, Shadmehr R. Impairment of retention but not acquisition of a visuomotor skill through time-dependent disruption of primary motor cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27(49):13413–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2570-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hwang EJ, Smith MA, Shadmehr R. Dissociable effects of the implicit and explicit memory systems on learning control of reaching. Experimental Brain Research. 2006;173:425–37. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0391-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ingram HA, van Donkelaar P, Cole J, Vercher JL, Gauthier GM, Miall RC. The role of proprioception and attention in a visuomotor adaptation task. Experimental Brain Research. 2000;132(1):114–26. doi: 10.1007/s002219900322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klassen J, Tong C, Flanagan J. Learning and recall of incremental kinematic and dynamic sensorimotor transformations. Experimental Brain Research. 2005;164:250–259. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-2247-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malfait N, Ostry DJ. Is interlimb transfer of force-field adaptation a cognitive response to the sudden introduction of load? Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:8084–8089. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1742-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mazzoni P, Krakauer JW. An implicit plan overrides an explicit strategy during visuomotor adaptation. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26(14):3642–5. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5317-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handiness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9:97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sainburg RL, Wang JS. Interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations: independence of direction and final position information. Experimental Brain Research. 2002;145:437–447. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1140-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slachevsky A, Pillon B, Fourneret P, Pradat-Diehl P, Jeannerod M, Dubois B. Preserved adjustment but impaired awareness in a sensory-motor conflict following prefrontal lesions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2001;13(3):332–40. doi: 10.1162/08989290151137386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith MA, Ghazizadeh A, Shadmehr R. Interacting adaptive processes with different timescales underlie short-term motor learning. PLoS Biology. 2006;4:1035–1043. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor JA, Thoroughman KA. Divided attention impairs human motor adaptation but not feedback control. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2007;98(1):317–26. doi: 10.1152/jn.01070.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thoroughman KA, Shadmehr R. Learning of action through adaptive combination of motor primitives. Nature. 2000;407(6805):742–7. doi: 10.1038/35037588. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tseng YW, Diedrichsen J, Krakauer JW, Shadmehr R, Bastian AJ. Sensory prediction errors drive cerebellum-dependent adaptation of reaching. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2007;98(1):54–62. doi: 10.1152/jn.00266.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vangheluwe S, Wenderoth N, Swinnen SP. Learning and transfer of an ipsilateral coordination task: evidence for a dual-layer movement representation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2005;17:1460–70. doi: 10.1162/0898929054985392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]