Abstract
Purpose
Desistance scholars primarily focus on changing social roles, cognitive transformations, and shifting identities to understand the cessation of serious crime and illicit drug use in adulthood. In the current study, we move the spotlight away from adulthood and toward adolescence, the developmental stage when the prevalence of offending and substance use peak and desistance from most of these behaviors begins. Our primary hypothesis is that changes in perceived psychic rewards surrounding initial forays into marijuana use strongly predict adolescents’ decisions to cease or persist that behavior. In addition, based on social learning expectations, we hypothesize that peer perceptions and behaviors provide mechanisms for perceptual change.
Methods
We test these hypotheses using longitudinal data of marijuana use, perceptions, and peer networks from the PROmoting School-community-university Partnerships to Enhance Resilience (PROSPER) study. We estimate hazard models of marijuana initiation and within-person models of perceptual updating for youth from grades 6 to 12 (n=6,154).
Results
We find that changes in marijuana’s perceived psychic rewards surrounding initiation differentiated experimenters from persisters. Experimenters had significantly lower updated perceptions of marijuana as a fun behavior compared to persisters and these perceptions dropped after the initiation wave. In contrast, persisters updated their perceptions in upward directions and maintained more positive perceptions over time. Inconsistent with social learning expectations, initiators’ updated perceptions of marijuana as a fun activity were not explained by peer-reported behaviors or attitudes.
Keywords: Desistance, Adolescence, Cognition, Marijuana, Delinquency
Since Blumstein and colleagues’ (1986) seminal report to the U.S. National Academy of Sciences, identifying and understanding the dimensions of individual criminal careers has assumed a central place in criminological research. More recently, life course criminologists have focused particular attention on the desistance process, with a multitude of studies attempting to define, model, and explain the deceleration and cessation of criminal offending over age. Building on the work of Sampson and Laub (1990; 1993; 1995; Laub and Sampson 1993 Laub and Sampson 2003; Laub, Nagin, and Sampson 1998; Sampson, Laub and Wimer 2006), the majority of desistance researchers have examined the association between adult social role transitions – such as getting married, becoming a parent, joining the military, or getting a full-time job – and decreased offending, arrest, and substance use over time (Giordano, Cernkovich, and Rudolph 2002; King, Massoglia, and MacMillan 2007; Kreager, Matsueda, and Erosheva 2010; Maruna 2001; Massoglia and Uggen 2010; Staff, Greene, Maggs, and Schoon 2014; Uggen 2000; Warr 1998). With increasingly sophisticated methodological designs (e.g., fixed effects analyses, propensity score matching, and interrupted time series), studies in this vein have documented how social role transitions can serve as “turning points” and lead to reductions in criminal behavior and substance use in adulthood.
Although informative, the scholarly attention to desistance among those who get married, have children, or settle into full-time jobs has meant that researchers generally focus on crime in adulthood (when such transitions are common) and overlook the cessation of less-serious risk behaviors (e.g., substance use, minor delinquency, unprotected sex, and driving under the influence) that occurs earlier in the life course.1 This omission is unfortunate because, according to the well-established aggregate age-crime distribution, adolescence is the developmental stage when desistance from most risky behaviors begins. Indeed, in their review of the age-crime literature, Loeber and Farrington (2012:13) note the “highest concentration of desistance takes place during late adolescence and early adulthood, irrespective of age of onset.” Stated differently, a vast amount of research has shown that the frequency and prevalence of self-reported offending, particularly for minor offenses, peak sharply in mid-adolescence (at approximately ages 15–16), suggesting that many offenders begin desisting almost immediately after they initiate crime and long before they cohabit, get married, enlist, or obtain career-relevant work (Sweeten, Piquero, and Steinberg 2013). As such, social bonds related to union formation and full-time employment appear insufficient to explain adolescents who experiment or flirt with delinquent behaviors and who do not undergo these more adult-like transitions (Osgood 2012; Giordano, Schroeder, and Cernkovich 2007).
Two theories in developmental psychology prioritize adolescent-limited delinquency and provide mechanisms for this behavioral trajectory. In Moffitt’s (1993) typological theory of crime, individuals with adolescent-limited antisocial behavior (asserted to be the majority of adolescents) commit delinquent acts to overcome a “maturity gap” whereby their biological age is incommensurate with their social age. Thus, they offend to assert autonomy and desist once they reach social maturity. In Steinberg’s (2008) social neuroscience explanation of adolescent risk-taking, the rise and fall of adolescent delinquency primarily results from age-graded changes in brain development. Accordingly, the decline in risk-taking at adolescent’s end is due to changes in the brain’s cognitive control system that heighten self-regulation.
Although their recognition of the adolescent peak in risk-taking is essential for understanding the age-crime curve, both Moffitt’s (1993) and Steinberg’s (2008) theories generally over-predict the frequency of offending by average adolescents. The mechanisms they offer are assumed to operate for most adolescents over an extended age range (e.g., ages 13–18), yet research of adolescent behavioral trajectories across a wide array of samples and behaviors invariably finds that the majority of surveyed respondents are likely to be sporadic, stably-low, or rare offenders (Jennings and Reingle 2012). If the proposed mechanisms for increased adolescent offending are ever-present during the adolescent life-stage, why don’t more adolescents increase these behaviors once they initiate them? In other words, as opportunities and the prevalence of risk-taking behaviors among same-age peers increase over adolescence, why do so many adolescents maintain such infrequent and low-level behavioral trajectories?
To address this research question and extend desistance theories, we build on Giordano et al.’s (2002 (2007) interactionist theory of cognitive transformation, Matza’s (1964) concept of “drift”, and social learning conceptions of peers and perceptual updating to understand cognitive changes at the point of adolescent delinquency initiation. We particularly emphasize changes in perceived rewards (i.e., whether the behavior is perceived as “fun”) as critical for understanding when delinquency onset is followed closely by cessation. We then test this model using within-person longitudinal behavioral, perceptual, and peer network data from the PROSPER study. Due to data limitations, we focus our analyses on marijuana use initiation and persistence, but propose that similar processes should hold for other minor delinquent behaviors common to adolescent-limited offending. Results from this project will not only help to explain experimentation of risky behaviors that occurs in an important and understudied age range, but also differentiate onset from persistence and potentially help to explain criminal desistance occurring in adulthood.
BACKGROUND
In their influential life-course theory of informal social control, Sampson and Laub (1990; 1995; 2006; Laub and Sampson 1993 Laub and Sampson 2003; Laub, Nagin, and Sampson 1998) argue that adult social bonds – including good marriages, stable jobs, and military service—are important for understanding desistance from crime and illicit drug use because these transitions allow individuals to “knife off” criminal opportunities through greater involvement, commitment, and belief in conventional institutions. Supporting this reasoning, research continues to find that adult social role transitions are associated with criminal desistance above and beyond social selection processes (Giordano et al. 2002; Maruna 2001; Warr 1998). Research in this vein, and the longitudinal methods they employ, have solidified the life-course approach at the forefront of criminology (Loeber and Welsh 2012).
In a critique of Sampson and Laub (1993), Giordano and colleagues (2002) argue that explaining desistance through changes in external controls ignores the “up front” cognitive work done by actors themselves. Building on the symbolic interactionist tradition of Mead (1964) and qualitative desistance research of Maruna (2001), Giordano et al. (2002) suggest that desistance primarily results from cognitive transformations whereby adults with criminal identities 1) become open to change, 2) are exposed to environmental stimuli (i.e., “hooks for change”) that are perceived as incompatible with continued criminal behavior, 3) contemplate alternative, non-criminal, identities, and 4) redefine criminal behavior as negative and personally irrelevant. Emphases on the structure-agency link and situationally-induced meanings of crime and adult roles most distinguish this cognitive perspective from Sampson and Laub’s (1993) original life-course theory. Indeed, in subsequent writings, Sampson and Laub (2006; Laub and Sampson 2003) acknowledge that they initially overlooked the importance of agency for the behavioral changes observed in their Glueck sample.2 The emphasis on active choice to explain the association between adult role transitions and desistance is important because it attends to why some individuals are more likely than others to select themselves into adult transitions, why the transitions themselves are more or less likely to result in behavioral change, and why some individuals desist in the absence of role transitions. The last point, that many offenders desist absent significant adult role transitions, is of particular relevance to the argument we will lay out.
In subsequent research, Giordano, Schroeder, and Cernkovich (2007) extend their interactionist theory of desistance to the domain of emotions. Here, they argue that adding changing and socially-malleable emotions (e.g., excitement, anger, love, and shame) to their earlier neo-Meadian model of cognitive transformation provides a full accounting for why 1) many offenders continue offending in the presence of a role transition (i.e., they do not undergo a substantial change in the emotional self) and 2) many offenders desist from offending in the absence of a role transition (i.e., they undergo diminutions of negative emotional identities and positive emotions connected to crime). The idea that the excitement and thrill of crime diminishes over time is of particular salience to our own ideas and provides common ground between our study and Giordano et al.’s (2007) influential work.
A contention of Giordano et al.’s (2002(2007) theories, and one which we take issue, is that human agency and rational decision-making is more important for behavior in adulthood than adolescence. Giordano and colleagues state (2002: 998–99) “the somewhat larger social and spatial arena of adulthood presents options that were not available earlier. In addition, adults, compared with children, have greater behavioral leeway, that is, ability to influence the specific course of action they will take.” This assertion that adults are more reflective and agentic than adolescents provides the authors room to add cognitive concepts to Sampson and Laub’s theory of adult informal social control, but also implies that one should not look for similar mechanisms to explain juvenile desistance: “Our theoretical emphasis seems well suited to a focus on adult development” (p. 999). We take an opposing view and argue in the next section that cognitive transformation processes are equally, if not more, present during adolescence than adulthood and thus help to explain the rise and fall of adolescent risk-taking.
COGNITION AND ADOLESCENT EXPERIMENTATION
Compared to childhood, adolescence is the developmental stage when the constraints of parents and teachers increasingly relax and youth are able to exert greater freedom, explore new identities, and participate in unstructured peer socializing (Meeus 2011; Osgood, Wilson, Bachman, O’Malley, and Johnston 1996; Warr 2005). Indeed, the weakening of family and school bonds during this life course stage is at the heart of Sampson and Laub’s (1993) age-graded explanation for adolescent delinquency and substance use. As mentioned above, these authors argue that it is only with the accumulation of social bonds in adulthood, such as a strong marriage and job stability, that crime and drug use are expected to decline over age.
But are changes in informal social controls sufficient to explain both the rapid rise and fall of adolescent offending? Here, we look to Matza’s (1964) concept of “drift” for assistance. Matza (1964:182) concurs with Cohen and Short’s (1958:30) statement, “Delinquency…cannot be assumed to be a potentiality of human nature which automatically erupts when the lid is off,” and argues that theories in the social control tradition (e.g., Sampson and Laub 1993) are incomplete without providing “…an impetus by which the potential for delinquency may be realized” (see also Matsueda 1997). For Matza, and consistent with the tenets of classical criminology, it is the offender’s will, or agency, that is missing from social control and other positivist theories of crime. Matza asserts that the loosening of constraints in adolescence allows individuals greater capacity (although never full freedom) to actively flirt with both criminal and conventional behaviors. Youth therefore drift between available lines of delinquent and non-delinquent actions and are “guided gently” by experiences, norms, and opportunities. In sum, Matza (1964:29) pushes us toward environmentally-conditioned decision-making processes to understand the “substantial majority of juvenile delinquents who do not become adult criminals.”
Matza’s (1964) concept of will and his invocation of classical criminology today most closely align with rational choice and subjective expected utility theories of offender decision-making (Becker 1968; Cornish and Clarke 1986). These perspectives assert that individuals will commit crime if the anticipated benefits from crime exceed its costs, with subjective preferences typically assessed using survey data (McCarthy 2002).3 An important consideration for this approach is then identifying the benefits and costs most relevant for criminal decision-making (Matsueda, Kreager, and Huizinga 2006). For adolescent experimentation with minor delinquency, we argue that perceived rewards are particularly salient. Matza and Sykes (1961:713) argued that perceived “excitement, ‘thrills,’ or ‘kicks,’” are central values underlying delinquent behavior. Similarly, Katz (1988) suggested that the primary returns of delinquency are its visceral benefits (i.e., excitement and “sneaky thrills”). Empirical analyses also demonstrate that subjective perceptions of delinquency as fun behavior are strong correlates of various types of crimes (see Baker and Piquero 2010 for a review), including auto theft (Copes 2003), general theft (McCarthy and Hagan 2001; Matsueda, Kreager, and Huizinga 2006), fencing (Steffensmeier 1986), drug use (Pfefferbaum and Wood 1994), excessive alcohol use (Ven Vander 2011; Weiss 2013), and drug selling (McCarthy and Hagan 2001).
Although empirical research demonstrates a clear association between perceived excitement and delinquency, how and why within-individual perceptions change through adolescence remains relatively underexplored. We argue that changes surrounding the point of initiation are particularly useful in understanding delinquency experimentation. The novice has limited experiential bases for establishing baseline perceptions of a behavior’s rewards, increasing the likelihood that such perceptions will undergo substantial and rapid change upon the initial experience. The uninitiated teenager must rely on social sources (e.g., peers, family, and the media) to form a preliminary opinion which may be distorted widely from the experience itself. As Matza (1964) argues, and consistent with social learning expectations (Akers 2011, Sutherland 1947), adolescent peer groups (i.e., situations of company) are likely primary sources of initial perceptions of delinquency’s rewards. Additionally, peers may exaggerate these rewards, particularly if they are inexperienced themselves. Zinberg (1984: 83) observed “The beginning user looked for a guide to show him what to do and how to do it correctly and safely…[he] tended to overestimate greatly his more experienced peers’ understanding of the drug.” Many novices may then downwardly adjust their perceptions of delinquency (but not necessarily their signals to peers) once they experience the behavior and realize that their naïve expectations are unmet or inflated. Alternatively, others may find the behavior meets or exceeds expectations, increasing the likelihood of behavioral persistence. A between-person divergence in perceptions of delinquency as fun following initiation would then be a strong predictor of persistence vs. short-lived experimentation.
The updating of perceptions upon experiencing delinquency should be shaped by the social contexts of initiation. Howard Becker (1973[1963]) provides a now classic example of the interactional process surrounding initiation of marijuana smoking. Becker highlights that through interactions with more experienced users, the marijuana novice learns how to effectively smoke the drug as well as recognize the symptoms of being high. In addition, the initiate learns from others how to enjoy the effects. Becker (1973[1963]:53) notes “the taste for such experience is a socially acquired one, not different from [other] acquired tastes.” Absent experienced peers, adolescents who initiate delinquent behavior should be less likely to perceive the event as fun and therefore more likely to avoid or resist future behavioral opportunities. Indeed, Warr (1998) and Sweeten et al. (2013) found that changes in self-reported delinquent peers were the strongest mediators of the age-crime association.4 We interpret these findings as consistent with diverging perceptions of delinquency over age. Adolescents with delinquent peers are more likely to enjoy delinquency, which in turn embeds them further in delinquent networks and increased delinquency over time. However, adolescents who try, but do not enjoy, delinquency are more likely to break their delinquent ties and desist from the behavior. Delinquent peers should therefore both predict initiation and mediate the association between perceptions and desistance.
A second reason why the novice may downwardly adjust his or her excitement perceptions after initiating a delinquent behavior relates to the perceived benefits of novelty itself. For the uninitiated, risk-taking should be associated with an anticipatory thrill regardless of the behavior’s outcome (Baldwin 1985). By definition, such anticipation should diminish once the behavior is experienced and its mystery is at least partially dispelled. Indeed, one would expect that, over time and with continued criminal involvement, the psychic rewards of crime would necessarily decline as the behavior and its outcomes become better understood and habituated (Giordano, Schroeder, and Cernkovich 2007). Research shows that there is a strong correlation between novel stimuli and risk taking behavior (Zuckerman 1990). Thus, even with increasing opportunities and reduced social bonds through the adolescent period, the loss of novelty associated with experiencing delinquency may itself be a turning point toward desistance.
The downward adjustment of perceived psychic rewards following behavioral initiation is not necessarily inconsistent with Steinberg’s (2008) theory of brain development, but would amend his argument in influential ways. He argues that the biologically-driven remodeling of the brain’s dopaminergic pathways at puberty, along with the increased salience of adolescent peer relations, increases adolescents’ sensitivity to sensation and novelty-seeking behaviors. Thus, experiencing many new and stimulating situations, rather than repeating a risky behavior multiple times, remains consistent with both our theory and Steinberg’s (2008) hypothesis. Indeed, initiating a novel behavior could be more rewarding than repeating a previously experienced behavior because (1) uncertainty is itself exciting and the reduction in uncertainty over time makes behavioral repetition less rewarding than the initial experience, or (2) pre-initiation perceptions of excitement may exceed post-initiation perceptions, also making the novel behavior more appealing than the repeated behavior. With regard to Steinberg’s (2008) theory, we would then expect that many adolescents increase the variety of risk-taking behaviors more than the frequency of any given behavior. Although we are unaware of research that looks explicitly at this question, the expectation is consistent with results from latent class trajectories of adolescent risk-taking that typically finds large numbers of youth having behavioral trajectories marked by low and sporadic delinquency or risk behaviors. For example, in a comprehensive review of over 100 studies using latent trajectory models for delinquency, aggression, and violence, approximately 90% placed a majority (i.e., >50%) of their samples in trajectories labeled low, sporadic, near zero, rare, non-offender, stable-low, low-risk, or low-level desistor/declining (Jennings and Reingle 2012).
Although emphasizing perceived psychic rewards (i.e., whether the behavior is “fun”), we also acknowledge that other benefits and costs contribute to adolescents’ offending calculi. A long line of research has focused on the possible deterrent effects of formal sanction perceptions. Several studies document the updating of risk perceptions as predictive of offending over time (Anwar and Loughran 2011; Lochner 2007; Matsueda et al. 2006; Pogarsky, Piquero, and Paternoster 2004). Interestingly, studies that compare risk perceptions and anticipated rewards find that perceived rewards are more predictive than perceptions of arrest for theft (Matsueda et al 2006; McCarthy and Hagan 2001) and drug selling (McCarthy and Hagan 2001), but less predictive of violence (Matsueda et al. 2006). We would argue that these results are consistent with our conception of delinquency’s psychic rewards being particularly important for less serious offending (i.e., property offenses and substance use). Additionally, Matsueda et al. (2006) found that delinquency’s perceived social rewards (i.e., peer status or “coolness”) are strongly predictive of both property and violent offending. Perceived coolness is especially salient among adolescent substance users (Jacques and Wright 2015; Parker, Aldridge and Measham 1998). On the cost side of the equation, McCarthy and Hagan (2001) and Grasmick and Bursik (1990) document that perceptions of delinquency’s moral costs (i.e., perceived “wrongfulness”, shame, or guilt) are actually stronger negative correlates of offending than are perceived formal sanctions. In sensitivity analyses for the present study, we include measures of perceived “coolness” and “wrongfulness” to isolate the independent effects of perceived psychic rewards on behavioral initiation and persistence.
DATA AND MEASURES
Our analyses are based upon data from adolescent participants in the ongoing PROmoting School-community-university Partnerships to Enhance Resilience (PROSPER) longitudinal study (Spoth, Greenberg, Bierman, and Redmond 2004; Spoth, Redmond, Shin, Greenberg, Clair, and Feinberg 2007). In the Fall of 2002 and 2003, two successive cohorts of 6th grade students residing in 28 rural communities in Iowa and Pennsylvania were selected to complete confidential surveys focused on their drug use attitudes and behaviors.5 Students first completed pencil and paper questionnaires administered during school hours in the Fall of 6th grade. Enrolled students then completed up to seven follow-up surveys during the spring semester of each school year from the 6th through 12th grade. Across the eight waves, participation rates ranged from 86% to 90% of enrolled students, and students who participated in the first wave went on to participate in an average of 5 additional in-school surveys ( ). There were over 8,000 respondents at the first wave.
Our analyses are based upon 6,154 youth residing in 27 PROSPER communities who provided data on marijuana use and all covariates in the Fall of the 6th grade, completed at least one consecutive follow-up survey,6 and attended a school that administered the social network section of the survey necessary for operationalizing peer marijuana use and attitudes.7 A small number of respondents (n=126) who had used marijuana prior to the Fall of 6th grade were excluded because we lacked marijuana perceptions measured before initiation.
Column 1 of Table 1 lists descriptive statistics for our analytical sample measured at PROSPER wave 1. The majority of the sample is white and from an intact family. Approximately one-third of the sample is eligible to receive reduced-cost school lunch. In unlisted analyses, we also compared our analytical sample to other PROSPER respondents. The analytical sample is generally more conventional than the PROSPER sample (i.e., significantly less delinquent, more religious, and stronger bonds with school and family). Our sample also has more girls and whites than the larger PROSPER sample. These differences are not surprising given that we require our sample to not have used marijuana prior to 6th grade and to have completed at least one consecutive follow-up survey (i.e., be present in school for multiple survey waves). Nevertheless, our sample does include over 1,000 marijuana initiators, allowing for a rigorous test of our hypotheses related to adolescent behavioral experimentation and changing risk perceptions.
Table 1.
Descriptive Statistics, Wave 1
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Analytical Sample | Non Initiators | Initiators | Experimenters | Persisters | |||
(n= 6,154) | (n=5,145) | (n=1,009) | (n=236) | (n=773) | |||
Variables | Mean/% | Mean/% | testa | Mean/% | Mean/% | testa | Mean/% |
Perceptions of MJ as Fun | 1.16 | 1.15 | *** | 1.23 | 1.18 | 1.24 | |
Number of MJ Friends | 0.02 | 0.02 | *** | 0.03 | 0.05 | 0.03 | |
Friends’ Mean Fun Perceptions | 1.19 | 1.18 | ** | 1.22 | 1.21 | 1.22 | |
Friendship Nominations Received | 3.52 | 3.33 | *** | 4.24 | 4.39 | 4.20 | |
Delinquency | 0.66 | 0.56 | *** | 1.03 | 0.74 | * | 1.12 |
Alcohol Use | 6% | 5% | *** | 12% | 10% | 13% | |
Risk and Sensation Seeking | 1.89 | 1.83 | *** | 2.07 | 1.99 | 2.10 | |
School Adjustment and Bonding | 4.16 | 4.20 | *** | 4.05 | 4.11 | 4.03 | |
Grades | 4.31 | 4.31 | 4.30 | 4.40 | * | 4.27 | |
Religiosity | 2.96 | 2.99 | ** | 2.91 | 2.97 | 2.89 | |
Both Biological Parents | 66% | 67% | * | 64% | 67% | 63% | |
Family Relations | 0.20 | 0.22 | *** | 0.16 | 0.20 | 0.14 | |
Reduced Price School Lunch | 29% | 29% | * | 26% | 21% | * | 28% |
Sex (1=Male) | 46% | 46% | 44% | 43% | 44% | ||
Race (1=White) | 88% | 88% | 90% | 90% | 90% |
p < .05;
p <. 01;
p <. 001 (two-tailed tests)
Comparisons were t-tests for continuous variables and chi-square tests for proportions
Dependent Variables: Marijuana Initiation and Persistence
In each wave respondents were asked if they had used marijuana in the past year. Approximately 16% (n=1,009) of the analytical sample had initiated by spring of the 12th grade, whereas the remaining 84% (n=5,145) did not report marijuana use during the observation period.8 Columns 2 and 3 of Table 1 compare variable means (or percentages) measured at wave 1 for marijuana initiators and non-initiators in our sample. Initiators have significantly higher prior delinquency, alcohol use, sensation seeking, marijuana friends, and perceptions of marijuana as fun than non-initiators. Initiators are also less religious, have more friends, are less likely to have intact families, and have poorer family relations than non-initiators.
Among sampled youth who initiated marijuana, 77% (n=773) reported using marijuana more than twice in the initiation wave or at least once in the next wave and were classified as persisters.9 The remaining 23% (n=236) reported using marijuana only once or twice in the wave they initiated and no use in the subsequent wave and were classified as experimenters. Columns 4 and 5 of Table 1 compare variable means (or percentages) at wave 1 by marijuana use category. There are relatively few observed differences between the two groups, with persisters reporting significantly more prior delinquency, lower grades, and more receipt of reduced price school lunch than experimenters.
Focal Predictor Variable: Perceptions of Marijuana as Fun
A unique feature of the PROSPER study is that youth were asked each year to self-report the risk and rewards of marijuana use. Perceptions of MJ as fun is a time-varying measure capturing responses to the survey item, “Smoking marijuana (pot) lets you have more fun.” Possible responses ranged from “1” (strongly disagree) to “5” (strongly agree) and in the Fall of the 6th grade, the average student strongly disagreed that smoking marijuana is a fun activity (sample meanwave 1=1.16).
Time-Varying and Time-Invariant Controls
Our models include controls for several time-varying factors that might render spurious any observed association between perceived rewards and marijuana onset and/or persistence. At each wave, students nominated up to seven best or close friends in the same school grade. Friendship nominations were collected using an open name generator where students wrote the first and last names of each friend on the survey form, which were then matched to student rosters. Names were matched 80% of the time. We used this peer nomination data to create a peer-reported measure of the number of friends who use marijuana (ranging from 0 to 7), friends’ mean fun perceptions from marijuana use (see above) and the number of friendship nominations (ranging from 0 to 20) that respondents received at each survey wave (i.e., indegree or peer acceptance). Delinquency at each wave is operationalized as a variety scale indicating whether respondents committed 12 self-reported delinquent behaviors in the past year (ranging from 0 to 12). Alcohol use at each wave is derived from the item, “During the past month, how many times have you had beer, wine, wine coolers, or other liquor?” (coded 1= “1 or more times”; 0=“not at all”). Risk and sensation seeking is the mean of three items (α = 0.77), respondent (1) does what feels good, regardless of consequences, (2) does something dangerous on a dare, and (3) does crazy things just to see the effect on others, rated on a scale of 1 (“Definitely would not”) to 5 (“Definitely would”). School adjustment and bonding is the mean of eight items (α = 0.80), which students rated on a scale of 1 (“Never true”) to 5 (“Always true”). These items asked whether students liked school, got along with their teachers, and felt like they belonged at school. We also include measures of self-reported grades at each wave based upon the question “What grades do you generally get in school?” Responses ranged from (1) “Mostly lower than D’s” to (5) “Mostly A’s”. Religiosity is derived from responses to the question “How often do you go to church or religious services?” which ranged from (1) “Never” to (4) “More than once a week.” Respondents also reported at each wave whether they currently resided with both biological parents (1=yes) and their family relations, assessing child-parent affective quality (12 items), joint activities (6 items), parental monitoring (5 items), inductive reasoning (3 items), and family cohesion (7 items). Finally, we controlled for sociodemographic background by including measures of gender (1=male), race/ethnicity (1=white), and whether the child reported receiving a reduced price lunch (1=yes in the past year), as well as age and cohort trends by including seven dummy variables for survey wave and 53 dummy variables for community and cohort (i.e., 2 cohorts * 27 communities – 1 community-cohort that did not participate in network portion of study).
In sensitivity analyses, we also include in our models additional measures of respondents’ and friends’ perceived rewards and costs of marijuana use. Each year, students reported their perceived wrongfulness of marijuana use (“How wrong do you think it is for someone your age to use marijuana or pot?”), with responses ranging from 1 (“Not at all wrong) to 4 (“Very wrong”). Respondents were also asked whether they gained social rewards from using marijuana (“Smoking marijuana (pot) makes you look cool.”), with responses ranging from 1 (“Strongly Disagree”) to 5 (“Strongly Agree”). We include these perceptual variables in sensitivity analyses because (1) changes in the perceived psychic rewards of marijuana may result in changes in marijuana’s perceived wrongfulness, thus complicating the temporal ordering of concepts and (2) the correlation between perceived psychic rewards and social rewards is quite high (r = .78), leaving little independent variation to distinguish the two measures.10
ANALYSES
Using the longitudinal data, the analysis strategy proceeded in three steps: First, we examine unadjusted mean value fluctuations in the perceptions of marijuana as fun use over the waves among youth who never used marijuana as well as those who initiated and then either persisted or desisted. Second, we used discrete-time logit (hazard) models for event history analysis to assess whether perceptions that marijuana use is fun, along with other covariates theoretically related to marijuana use, increase the risk of time-to-initiation (Blossfeld and Rohwer 2002; Singer and Willett 2003). Discrete-time logit models are advantageous over models predicting marijuana initiation at particular end dates (i.e., by the end of high school) because the models allow for time-varying covariates (i.e., perceptions, friends who use marijuana, family relations, etc.) to be entered into the model and appropriately adjust for the right-hand censoring of children who were lost to attrition, who were no longer enrolled in the school district, or who did not initiate by the end of the 6-year observation period. The estimated hazard of initiation will be generated from 6,154 children who have been followed 23,297 person-years. We will test if the predicted probability of initiation increases when youth perceive the drug as fun, controlling for other risk and protective factors. Finally, limiting the analysis sample to 1,009 youth who had initiated by end of high school and were not missing data on the predictor variables, in a series of logistic regression models we estimated the factors influencing who persisted or desisted.11 These analyses will allow us to assess the predictive power of changes in perceived psychic rewards from the other risk factors as well as whether peer marijuana use mediates the perceived reward-persistence association.
RESULTS
In Figure 1, we first longitudinally compare mean values for marijuana’s perceived psychic rewards between non-initiators, persisters, and experimenters. For the initiating categories (i.e., persisters and experimenters), three year means are presented for those who initiated in waves 3–8 (i.e., grades 7–12). For example, the dashed line with a square at each wave corresponds to the mean psychic rewards for marijuana persisters in the wave before they initiated (i.e., wave 2) and two waves after they initiated marijuana (i.e., waves 3–4). As expected, those adolescents who never used marijuana in the observation period had low perceptions of the drug as fun across all waves, although these perceptions do increase somewhat over time. Marijuana initiators who continued using the drug more than 1 or 2 times (i.e., persisters) had a large increase in perceived fun between their pre- and post-initiation waves, and this difference was fairly similar regardless of the age at which they initiated. In contrast, initiators who ceased use after 1 or 2 times saw smaller increases in perceived psychic rewards than persisters in the wave after initiation, followed by a decrease in the next wave. For experimenters who initiated marijuana in wave 6 (i.e., 10th grade), their post-initiation perceptions were not significantly different from the non-user category. This is in line with previous findings that show experimenters and abstainers closely resemble one another in their attitudes towards drugs (Parker et al. 1998). It is also worth noting that pre-initiation perceptions for the persisting group were always significantly higher than the non-using group and significantly higher than the experimenting group in wave 3. The pre-initiation values for the experimenting group were also significantly different from non-users in waves 2 through 5. In total, the descriptive comparison of perceived psychic rewards suggests that marijuana initiators update their perceptions of marijuana in a positive direction upon first use and that the updated perceptions are larger and more stable for persisters than experimenters. It is also interesting that, prior to initiation, none of the categories have impressions of marijuana as a fun activity (i.e., the mean is always under 2 on a 5-point scale) and even after initiation, the highest group’s perceptions only approximate the mid-point on the scale.
Figure 1.
Mean Perceptions of Marijuana as Fun by Initiation Wave and Persistence vs. Experimentation Status
We next estimate a discrete time model of marijuana initiation between waves 2 and 8. Table 2 lists coefficients and standard errors for lagged (i.e., t−1) independent variables included in the model. The large amount of power obtained from 23,297 person-waves results in precise coefficients that, for the most part, are in theoretically-expected directions. Of most importance for the current paper, the positive and statistically significant (p < .001) estimate for perceived pyschic rewards (OR=1.34; 95% CI=1.24–1.44) suggests that adolescents with higher pre-initiation perceptions of marijuana as a fun activity were at greater risk of subsequently initiating marijuana during our observation period. This finding is consistent with results from Figure 1 and suggests that marijuana initiators were exposed to more favorable messages of marijuana use prior to using the drug themselves.
Table 2.
Discrete Time Logistic Regression of Marijuana Initiation
Variable | b | SE | |
---|---|---|---|
Time Varying Covariates (t−1): | |||
Perceptions of MJ as Fun | 0.29 | *** | 0.04 |
Number of MJ Friends | 0.16 | ** | 0.06 |
Friends’ Mean Fun Perceptions | 0.30 | *** | 0.06 |
Friendship Nominations Received | 0.06 | *** | 0.01 |
Delinquency | 0.14 | *** | 0.02 |
Alcohol Use | 0.99 | *** | 0.08 |
Risk and Sensation Seeking | 0.27 | *** | 0.04 |
School Adjustment and Bonding | −0.16 | ** | 0.06 |
Grades | −0.19 | *** | 0.05 |
Religiosity | −0.05 | 0.03 | |
Family Relations | −0.25 | ** | 0.09 |
Both Biological Parents | −0.32 | *** | 0.07 |
Reduced Price School Lunch | 0.02 | 0.09 | |
Time Stable Covariates: | |||
Sex (1=Male) | −0.19 | ** | 0.07 |
Race (1=White) | −0.16 | 0.11 | |
Wave: | |||
2 (6th grade) | −5.01 | *** | 0.45 |
3 (7th grade) | −4.16 | *** | 0.44 |
4 (8th grade) | −3.65 | *** | 0.44 |
5 (9th grade) | −3.47 | *** | 0.44 |
6 (10th grade) | −3.49 | *** | 0.44 |
7 (11th grade) | −3.21 | *** | 0.44 |
8 (12th grade) | −2.96 | *** | 0.44 |
n=23,297 person-waves, 6,154 persons
NOTE: Models include fixed effects for 53 community-cohorts (not shown)
p < .05;
p <. 01;
p <. 001 (two-tailed tests)
The other estimates provide few surprises. Having friends who use marijuana increases the risk of marijuana initiation, as does those friends’ perceptions of marijuana as a fun experience, prior delinquency, alcohol use, and risk and sensation seeking. Respondents who initiated marijuana also had higher peer acceptance (i.e., more received friendship nominations) than non-initiating youth. Consistent with social control theory (Hirschi 2002[1969]), school bonding, higher grades, stronger family relations, and residing with both biological parents reduces the risk of marijuana initiation. Males are also less likely to initiate marijuana than are females. Finally, the negative and close to monotonically decreasing estimates for wave mean that the risk of marijuana initiation is low at earlier waves but increases over time.
Table 3 lists estimates from four stepwise models of marijuana persistence among our PROSPER sample who initiated marijuana between waves 2 and 8 (i.e., 7th and 12th grade). Model 1 lists coefficients for time stable covariates and, for time-varying covariates, estimates for the lagged (i.e., t−1) period capturing baseline differences between marijuana persisters and experimenters. Only two variables in the model have significant associations with marijuana persistence. First, persisters receive greater numbers of friendship nominations prior to initiation than do experimenters. Each additional friend corresponds to a 5% increase in the odds (OR=1.05; 95% CI=1.00–1.10) of persisting vs. desisting marijuana use. Second, the significant estimate for wave of initiation means that persisters are more likely to initiate marijuana at younger ages than are experimenters. Of interest to our study, pre-initiation perceptions of marijuana as fun do not differentiate persisters and experimenters at standard (i.e., p<.05) levels of significance.
Table 3.
Logistic Regressions of Marijuana Persistence
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | Model 4 | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Variables | b | SE | b | SE | b | SE | b | SE | ||||
Time Varying Covariates (t−1): | ||||||||||||
Perceptions of MJ as Fun | 0.17 | 0.11 | 0.49 | *** | 0.12 | 0.48 | *** | 0.12 | 0.49 | *** | 0.11 | |
Number of MJ Friends | −0.02 | 0.16 | 0.01 | 0.16 | 0.28 | 0.19 | 0.31 | 0.20 | ||||
Friends’ Mean Fun Perceptions | 0.17 | 0.16 | 0.17 | 0.17 | 0.03 | 0.19 | 0.01 | 0.19 | ||||
Friendship Nominations Received | 0.05 | * | 0.02 | 0.05 | * | 0.03 | 0.04 | 0.03 | 0.04 | 0.03 | ||
Delinquency | 0.02 | 0.04 | 0.02 | 0.04 | 0.02 | 0.04 | 0.04 | 0.04 | ||||
Alcohol Use | 0.08 | 0.18 | 0.06 | 0.18 | 0.02 | 0.18 | 0.35 | 0.23 | ||||
Risk and Sensation Seeking | 0.12 | 0.07 | 0.07 | 0.08 | 0.06 | 0.07 | 0.09 | 0.10 | ||||
School Adjustment and Bonding | −0.17 | 0.13 | −0.09 | 0.15 | −0.12 | 0.15 | 0.06 | 0.16 | ||||
Grades | −0.11 | 0.10 | −0.14 | 0.10 | −0.14 | 0.10 | −0.27 | 0.14 | ||||
Religiosity | −0.02 | 0.08 | −0.05 | 0.08 | −0.05 | 0.08 | −0.09 | 0.09 | ||||
Both Biological Parents | −0.20 | 0.20 | −0.15 | 0.20 | −0.15 | 0.20 | −0.09 | 0.21 | ||||
Family Relations | −0.14 | 0.22 | −0.09 | 0.23 | −0.08 | 0.23 | −0.21 | 0.26 | ||||
Reduced Price School Lunch | −0.03 | 0.23 | 0.03 | 0.24 | 0.05 | 0.23 | 0.14 | 0.28 | ||||
Wave | −0.22 | *** | 0.06 | −0.21 | *** | 0.06 | −0.22 | *** | 0.06 | −0.21 | *** | 0.06 |
Time Stable Covariates: | ||||||||||||
Sex (1=Male) | −0.07 | 0.19 | −0.10 | 0.19 | −0.09 | 0.19 | −0.08 | 0.20 | ||||
Race (1=White) | 0.04 | 0.25 | 0.15 | 0.24 | 0.12 | 0.26 | 0.19 | 0.25 | ||||
Time Varying Covariates (t−[t−1]): | ||||||||||||
Δ Perceptions of MJ as Fun | 0.41 | *** | 0.07 | 0.41 | *** | 0.07 | 0.37 | *** | 0.08 | |||
Δ Number of MJ Friends | 0.34 | ** | 0.11 | 0.36 | ** | 0.10 | ||||||
Δ Friends’ Mean Fun Perceptions | −0.13 | 0.11 | −0.16 | 0.11 | ||||||||
Δ Friendship Nominations Received | 0.01 | 0.04 | 0.01 | 0.04 | ||||||||
Δ Delinquency | 0.06 | 0.04 | ||||||||||
Δ Alcohol Use | 0.41 | * | 0.18 | |||||||||
Δ Risk and Sensation Seeking | 0.06 | 0.12 | ||||||||||
Δ School Adjustment and Bonding | 0.25 | 0.17 | ||||||||||
Δ Grades | −0.31 | 0.16 | ||||||||||
Δ Religiosity | −0.10 | 0.08 | ||||||||||
Δ Both Biological Parents | −0.04 | 0.42 | ||||||||||
Δ Family Relations | −0.45 | 0.27 | ||||||||||
Δ Reduced Price School Lunch | 0.20 | 0.33 | ||||||||||
Constant | 2.40 | ** | 0.70 | 1.39 | 0.82 | 1.74 | * | 0.85 | 1.11 | 0.93 | ||
Wald Chi2, df | 85.41, | *** | 33.59, | *** | 11.57, | ** | 25.33, | ** |
n=1,009
NOTE: Robust Standard errors adjust for 53 community-cohorts
p < .05;
p <. 01;
p <. 001 (two-tailed tests)
Model 2 adds a change measure (i.e., the difference between the initiation wave [t] and pre-initiation wave [t−1]) of perceived psychic rewards to Model 1. The positive and significant association between this measure and persistence suggests that marijuana initiators who increase their perceptions of marijuana as a fun activity are more likely to persist than are those who do not update their perceptions in a positive direction. An increase of one point between the pre-initiation and post-initiation waves is expected to increase the odds of persistence by just over 50% (OR = 1.51; 95% CI=1.31–1.74). Additionally, including the change measure of perceived psychic rewards into the model increases the estimate and significance of the baseline measure of perceived fun. Net of changes in perceptions, persisters have higher baseline perceptions of marijuana as a fun behavior.
Model 3 tests if changes in peer contexts explain the association between changes in marijuana perceptions and continued use by including covariates for (1) change in number of marijuana-using friends, (2) change in friends’ perceptions, and (3) change in incoming friendship nominations to Model 2. Although increases in marijuana-using friends is a significant positive correlate of persistence, the perceived psychic rewards estimate in Model 3 remains very similar to Model 2. Changes in friends’ perceptions neither predicts persistence nor attenuates the association between changes in respondents’ perceived psychic rewards and marijuana persistence. Inconsistent with our social learning hypothesis, our network measure of peer marijuana use appears to explain little of the association between changing perceptions of marijuana as fun and marijuana persistence.
Finally, Model 4 adds change measures for our other time-varying covariates to Model 3. Of these, only increases in alcohol between the pre-initiation and post-initiation waves significantly predicts marijuana persistence. Initiating alcohol consumption between the pre-initiation and post-initiation waves increases the odds of persistence by almost 50% (OR = 1.50; 95% CI=1.06–2.12). Model 4 does not, however, attenuate much of the estimate for changes in perceived psychic rewards. This variable remains a strong and significant predictor of marijuana persistence (OR=1.44; 95% CI=1.24–1.68).
Predicting Changes in Perceptions of Fun
To better understand the lack of mediation observed in our models of marijuana persistence, we estimated ordinary least squares models with changes in marijuana perceptions as the outcome and present coefficients and robust standard errors for these models in Appendix A. Model specification closely parallels that of Table 3. Model 1 shows that, of the background measures, respondents with higher prior perceived fun showed less change in perceptions over time. Those who were risk and sensation seekers had increased perceived fun over time, as did those with high religiosity. Those who were bonded to school saw decreases in perceived fun over time.
Our primary interest relates to Model 2, which adds measures for changing peer contexts, including changes in marijuana-using friends, friends’ mean perceptions, and number of friendship nominations received. None of these estimates approached statistical significance. It thus appears that strong, network-based measures of peer behavior and perceptions do not predict changes in perceived fun of marijuana from the pre-initation to post-initiation waves. In contrast, Model 3 shows that increases in delinquency, alcohol use, and risk and sensation seeking and decreases in grades all predict increased perceptions of marijuana as a fun activity.
Adding Other Rewards and Costs
Two additional appendices display results from models including measures of respondents’ and their friends’ perceived social rewards (“coolness”) and moral costs (“wrongfulness”) of marijuana. Appendix B adds these variables to the discrete time model of marijuana initiation listed in Table 2. There is a significant negative odds ratio for perceived wrongfulness, indicating that the risk of initiation was reduced when respondents felt marijuana use was wrong. Net of other covariates, perceived social rewards was not associated with marijuana initiation, nor were the perceptions of social and moral costs from friends. Including all additional perceptual measures attenuated approximately 10% of the perceived fun coefficient from Table 2, but this estimate remained significant. Thus, even after accounting for those who perceived marijuana as morally wrong, adolescents who viewed marijuana as a fun behavior were more likely to initiate the drug than those who did not.
Finally, appendix C adds baseline (i.e., t−1) and change score (i.e., t−[t−1]) measures of respondents’ and friends’ perceived social rewards and perceived wrongfulness of marijuana to Model 4 of Table 3. Adolescent respondents who perceived marijuana as “wrong” prior to initiation or who increased their perceptions of “wrongfulness” or “coolness” after initiation were significantly less likely to persist using the drug. The change in social rewards finding must be interpreted in light of the high correlation between perceived fun and coolness. With the fun variables (i.e., baseline and change score) removed from the model (not shown), change in coolness remains a (non)significant positive predictor of marijuana persistence. Regardless, adding the other perceptual measures of marijuana’s costs and rewards attenuates none of the baseline or change score estimates of perceived fun from Model 4, Table 3.
DISCUSSION
Over the past twenty years, the study of desistance has taken a central place in criminological discourse. Scholars have forwarded a number of theories purporting to explain desistance from serious crime. Notably, theories have emphasized that important life events, such as a “good” marriage and stable employment, are embedded in the desistance process and that these events serve as turning points in adult criminal trajectories (Sampson and Laub 1993). Alternatively, theories of cognitive transformations (Giordano, Cernkovich, and Rudolph 2002; 2007) and identity change (Paternoster and Bushway 2009) place emphasis on the cognitive processes associated with leaving behind a life of crime. Although extremely informative, extant accounts of the desistance process focus on adult processes. Specifically, Sampson and Laub (1993:302) note: “our major thesis is that… turning points are important concepts in understanding processes of change in the adult life course.” Yet, the sharp peak in the prevalence of delinquency commonly observed in mid- to late-adolescence suggests that, for most individuals, the deceleration or cessation of offending and risk-taking occurs earlier in the life course and that the majority of offenders stop the behavior shortly after they initiate (Loeber and Farrington 2012). This leads to a gap between the criminological emphasis on desistance in adulthood and the fact that most individuals desist prior to entering their adult years. The current study sought to fill some of this gap by forwarding a theory of adolescent experimentation.
A salient theme in the desistance literature surrounds the timing of turning points, or “hooks for change.” For example, some scholars suggest that structural factors can initiate the desistance process (Sampson and Laub 1993), whereas other scholars argue that structural factors operate in tandem with the cognitive processes of change (Giordano et al. 2002). Still, others forward that agentic changes in an offender identity predate both desistance and life course transitions (Paternoster and Bushway 2009). The commonality among extant desistance theories is their emphasis on explaining the behaviors of established, or serious, offenders. Such emphasis is perhaps warranted, as it has long been understood that a small minority of serious offenders are responsible for a large proportion of crime (Wolfgang and Ferracuti 1967). However, shifting the spotlight to this small population misses potentially different mechanisms for the desistance process among the majority of less-serious offenders. Moreover, understanding desistance that occurs among adolescent experimenters may also contribute to understanding desistance occurring among more serious and chronic offenders, as well as point to potential interventions to reduce minor delinquency, prevent early commitment to criminal lines of action, and thus have a larger societal impact (i.e., see the prevention paradox; Rose 1981). The current study thus shifted focus away from established offenders and toward adolescent desistance that occurred shortly after behavioral onset.
Using longitudinal data from 1,009 teenagers who had initiated marijuana use, we found that, prior to initiation, perceptions of marijuana as a fun behavior were similar among those youth who subsequently persisted and those who stopped their use. Indeed, the average marijuana user did not expect the drug to be exciting, regardless of whether they continued use or not. Upon initiation, however, there was a clear distinction in perceptions between those who stopped the behavior and those who persisted. Marijuana experimenters had significantly lower perceptions of the drug as fun compared to persisters, eventually settling to levels that were little different from non-users. In contrast, persisters updated their perceptions in an upward direction and maintained more positive perceptions over time, thus departing dramatically from both experimenters and non-users. These patterns suggest that the experience of marijuana itself strongly predicts subsequent perceptions, which in turn are strongly correlated with continued use. The observed patterns thus document important variation in the perceptual updating process applied to a health-risk behavior.
Our results have several implications for dominant life course and criminological theories. First, our results challenge the hypothesis, drawn from Becker’s (2015[1953]) seminal work, that marijuana persistence is conditional on the peer contexts of initiation. Using network-derived measures of peer marijuana use and perceptions, we found little mediation of the association between perceptions of marijuana as a fun activity and continued marijuana use. Indeed, in supplementary analyses, we found these peer variables uncorrelated with changes in marijuana perceptions. It thus appeared that peers were unnecessary for marijuana initiators to update their perceptions of their first marijuana experience. If peers are not the mechanisms for such perceptual change, who or what are? One possibility is that individuals are able to experience the effects of marijuana without strong peer guidance, because the act is not as complicated as Becker originally hypothesized. In fact, in the preface to the recent re-release of his earlier work, Becker (2015[1953]) commented that today’s drug culture incorporates widely shared knowledge of the techniques and subjective effects of marijuana that can inform and enhance neophytes’ initial marijuana experiences. This knowledge is also able to spread widely through social media and internet sources, further reducing the need for situational definitions provided directly by peers. Simultaneously, Becker (2015[1953]) recognized that the level of marijuana’s active ingredient, THC, is higher today than when he originally proposed his theory and therefore does not require perfect technique to experience the drug’s physiological effects (see also Hallstone 2002). Similarly, marijuana initiates may be able to come to their own conclusions of the marijuana experience without peer advice. For example, novices may be unconvinced by peers that marijuana’s “high” is rewarding or may enjoy the feeling even if experienced users are not present. Our results thus implicate variable definitions of situations that might accompany novel experiences as the primary drivers of perceptual change surrounding marijuana initiation.
Second, the temporal proximity of onset and desistance found among marijuana experimenters also qualifies developmental explanations of adolescent risk-taking that typically rely on mechanisms that are less malleable to changing environmental circumstances. For example, Moffitt (1993) argues that the cessation of adolescent-limited risk-taking should coincide with reductions in the maturity gap that accompany the transition to adulthood. Given the young age of this sample, it is unlikely that the adolescent experimenters that we observed in this study stopped using marijuana because they felt more adult. Similarly, onset followed by cessation appears inconsistent with Steinberg’s (2008) theory of brain development, unless as mentioned earlier, novelty seeking is itself the dominant reward and many adolescents move from one risk-taking behavior to another. Future research should examine these possibilities using other longitudinal adolescent samples.
Third, given that cognitive changes surrounding behavioral initiation strongly predict persistence, identity transformation may not be a necessary condition for desistance, at least during this life stage. Indeed, we would argue that it is the absence of a stable identity that facilitates behavioral cessation so close to the point of initiation. We lack identity measures and therefore cannot directly test this hypothesis, but future research should examine whether an unsettled identity during adolescence increases delinquent experimentation. A related question is whether cognitive transformations similar to what we observe can occur during adulthood without a contemporaneous shift in identity. It is plausible that the accumulation of crime’s physical, monetary, emotional, and social costs and simultaneous decay in its rewards are enough to alter an individual’s behavioral trajectory. It could then be this agentic movement away from crime that results in identity transformation and paves the way for other conventional role transitions, such as employment, marriage, and parenthood. We would therefore expect that changes in criminal behavioral could occur even in the absence of life course transitions, but remain consistent with Giordano et al. (2002; 2007) and link cognitive transformations with symbolic interaction processes. Our thesis also departs from Giordano et al. and Paternoster and Bushway (2009) in asserting that cognitive transformation precedes identity transformation and directly influences future behaviors, at least among adolescent-limited delinquents.
Our study also has implications for a now dormant criminal career debate. Criminal career researchers have long argued that various dimensions of the criminal career, such as onset and persistence, might require separate explanatory models and therefore justify the criminal career perspective. However, empirical evidence has generally shown that behavioral prevalence, onset, or initiation, and persistence can be explained by similar factors (Nagin and Farrington 1992; Smith and Brame 1994; Paternoster and Triplett 1988), such as delinquent peers and gender. Since studies in this vein have not found support for the need to use separate explanatory models for initiation and persistence, scholars have largely abandoned the endeavor. Results from this study potentially re-open this debate. In models of marijuana initiation, we find significant correlations for the “usual suspects” from criminological theory, including prior delinquency, school and parental bonds, impulsivity, and gender. However, we also found these correlates were often much less consequential in distinguishing between experimentation and persistence. Instead, consistent with the concept of cognitive transformation we found strong evidence that changes in perceptions of marijuana as fun differentiated the two behavioral trajectories. In light of our results, we postulate that criminal career research has been hindered by inattention to factors that change at the point of behavioral onset, when situations of substantial uncertainty are re-evaluated given new information. Given our findings, we urge scholars to revisit the criminal career proposition with a focus on situationally-specific cognitive processes surrounding initial offending experiences.
Although we focused our analysis on marijuana use, we posit that similar cognitive processes operate for other minor delinquent behavior, all of which incorporate some sensual element of thrill, danger, or adventure (Katz 1988; Maggs et al. 1998; Matsueda et al. 2006). Although substance use differs in important ways from other forms of delinquency (e.g., substances directly impact a person’s physiology and drug use typically peaks in early adulthood versus adolescence), we argue that initial experiences with both behaviors are situationally-defined and result in substantial cognitive transformations and strong potential for desistance. In other words, regardless of form, most adolescents experiment with problem behaviors and many do not continue after their initial exploration. Moreover, perceived psychic costs and rewards, whether they be thrill, pain, fear, or boredom, apply to all risky behaviors and likely vary substantially between individuals and over time. We expect our findings are applicable across adolescent problem behaviors.
A limitation of our analyses is that it is unclear how representative our results are to all adolescents. PROSPER collects data only from predominantly white rural communities, and our sample is generally more conventional than the broader PROSPER sample, complicating generalizability. However, it should be kept in mind that our theoretical interest is not on serious delinquents (who would likely be underrepresented in our sample), but rather minor and adolescent-limited delinquents, of which there are adequate numbers in our sample. Moreover, there is little reason to suspect that the processes we examine, such as proximal marijuana perceptions and individual decisions to continue marijuana use over time, should vary dramatically by social context. We encourage further studies to replicate our findings across different samples, but suspect that the patterns we observe will be robust to such contextual variations.
Though the PROSPER study follows respondents over an extended age range, the variables we are most interested in were only collected through the end of high school. This right censoring raises the issue that many of the adolescents we label as “experimenters” may actually return to the behavior after the observation window. Although a valid concern, it is likely that such behavioral reuptake will be sporadic or intermittent rather than continuous use, resulting in respondent profiles that are closer to non-users than chronic or frequent users (Bushway and Patersnoster 2013). Moreover, by focusing on the peak age range of initiation (and desistance), we are able to measure the pre- and post-initiation perceptions central to our cognitive thesis and generally unavailable to other researchers.
Data limitations require us to operationalize perceived psychic rewards with a single item. Ideally, we would want more detailed measures of marijuana’s psychopharmacological effects – such as euphoria, paranoia, laughter, hunger, and dizziness (Becker 2015[1953])— and the emotions individuals attached to those experiences. We feel confident that our measure captures global perceptions of the marijuana experience, but future research would benefit from finer-grained measurement of marijuana perceptions before and after initiation.
It is also important to acknowledge that, over time and repeated use, addictive substances such as marijuana may result in neurological adaptations and the usurpation of the brain’s reward centers (i.e., tolerance and dependence) that predict continued use in the absence of perceptions of the substance as “fun”, “thrilling”, or “exciting” (Hyman, Malenka, and Nestler 2006; Lupica, Riegel, and Hoffman 2004). Indeed, one of the criteria for a substance use disorder according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5: American Psychiatric Association 2013) is “continued substance use despite having persistent or recurrent social or interpersonal problems caused or exacerbated by the effects of the substance.” Our argument is not that substance use is forever perceived as fun for persistent users, but rather that persistent users are more likely than experimenters to perceive early use as a fun activity.
Finally, regarding policy, our results suggest that interventions aimed at helping adolescents to downwardly adjust definitions of deviant behavior as they begin experimentation may be particularly promising. Although differences in the perceived rewards between persisters, experimenters, and non-users become substantial over time, the average adolescent in all of these categories is at most ambivalent about marijuana’s psychic rewards (i.e., even the average persister is not sure that marijuana use is fun). This means that there is an opening to redefine the behavior in a way that de-glamorizes the drug and further erodes its perceived rewards. The experimenter, who has experienced the drug and found it wanting, would be a strong ally in helping lower expectations among non-users and those at risk of persisting. Experimenters can acknowledge their own preconceived notions and the attractiveness of experiencing a new behavior and taking risks, but also present the experience as anti-climactic and less interesting over time. A strategy focused on realistic perceptions of the drug’s physiological and social consequences would then treat adolescents as agents capable of weighing the risks and rewards to make their own behavioral decisions. Lowering expectations for other first-time users, or those at risk of using, could alter perceptual trajectories in ways that keep use to short-lived experimentation.
Appendix A. OLS Regressions of Change in Perceptions of Marijuana as Fun
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Variables | b | SE | b | SE | b | SE | |||
Time Varying Covariates (t−1): | |||||||||
Perceptions of MJ as Fun | −0.77 | *** | 0.04 | −0.77 | *** | 0.04 | −0.74 | *** | 0.04 |
Number of MJ Friends | −0.05 | 0.06 | 0.02 | 0.07 | 0.02 | 0.07 | |||
Friends’ Mean Fun Perceptions | 0.04 | 0.07 | 0.08 | 0.09 | 0.10 | 0.09 | |||
Friendship Nominations Received | −0.01 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.06 | 0.00 | 0.02 | |||
Delinquency | 0.02 | 0.02 | 0.02 | 0.02 | 0.03 | 0.02 | |||
Alcohol Use | 0.05 | 0.09 | 0.04 | 0.09 | 0.25 | * | 0.10 | ||
Risk and Sensation Seeking | 0.15 | ** | 0.05 | 0.14 | ** | 0.05 | 0.23 | *** | 0.06 |
School Adjustment and Bonding | −0.21 | * | 0.09 | −0.21 | * | 0.09 | −0.21 | 0.11 | |
Grades | 0.06 | 0.05 | 0.07 | 0.05 | 0.04 | 0.06 | |||
Religiosity | 0.07 | * | 0.03 | 0.07 | * | 0.03 | 0.06 | 0.04 | |
Both Biological Parents | −0.14 | 0.09 | −0.14 | 0.09 | −0.13 | 0.09 | |||
Family Relations | −0.15 | 0.12 | −0.14 | 0.12 | −0.10 | 0.12 | |||
Reduced Price School Lunch | −0.20 | 0.12 | −0.19 | 0.12 | −0.10 | 0.13 | |||
Wave | −0.04 | 0.03 | −0.04 | 0.03 | 0.01 | 0.03 | |||
Time Stable Covariates: | |||||||||
Sex (1=Male) | 0.06 | 0.09 | 0.06 | 0.09 | 0.04 | 0.09 | |||
Race (1=White) | −0.23 | 0.14 | −0.26 | 0.14 | −0.24 | * | 0.11 | ||
Time Varying Covariates (t−[t−1]): | |||||||||
Δ Number of MJ Friends | 0.08 | 0.05 | 0.08 | 0.05 | |||||
Δ Friends’ Mean Fun Perceptions | 0.07 | 0.06 | 0.06 | 0.06 | |||||
Δ Friendship Nominations Received | 0.02 | 0.02 | 0.02 | 0.02 | |||||
Δ Delinquency | 0.04 | * | 0.02 | ||||||
Δ Alcohol Use | 0.23 | ** | 0.08 | ||||||
Δ Risk and Sensation Seeking | 0.18 | ** | 0.05 | ||||||
Δ School Adjustment and Bonding | −0.16 | 0.09 | |||||||
Δ Grades | −0.14 | * | 0.06 | ||||||
Δ Religiosity | −0.04 | 0.05 | |||||||
Δ Both Biological Parents | −0.23 | 0.17 | |||||||
Δ Family Relations | −0.04 | 0.13 | |||||||
Δ Reduced Price School Lunch | 0.20 | 0.15 | |||||||
Constant | 2.70 | ** | 0.40 | 2.56 | *** | 0.42 | 1.78 | *** | 0.44 |
F-value, df | 43.97, | *** | 2.69, 3 | 11.42, | *** |
n=1,009
NOTE: Robust Standard errors adjust for 53 community-cohorts
p < .05;
p <. 01;
p <. 001 (two-tailed tests)
Appendix B. Discrete Time Logistic Regression of Marijuana Initiation with Social Rewards and Moral Costs
Variable | b | SE | |
---|---|---|---|
Time Varying Covariates (t−1): | |||
Perceptions of MJ as Fun | 0.20 | ** | 0.07 |
Social Rewards | 0.03 | 0.08 | |
Moral Costs | −0.28 | *** | 0.05 |
Number of MJ Friends | 0.14 | * | 0.06 |
Friends’ Mean Fun Perceptions | 0.32 | *** | 0.09 |
Friends’ Mean Social Rewards | −0.11 | 0.10 | |
Friends’ Mean Moral Costs | −0.07 | 0.08 | |
Friendship Nominations Received | 0.06 | *** | 0.01 |
Delinquency | 0.13 | *** | 0.02 |
Alcohol Use | 1.00 | *** | 0.08 |
Risk and Sensation Seeking | 0.26 | *** | 0.04 |
School Adjustment and Bonding | −0.13 | * | 0.06 |
Grades | −0.19 | *** | 0.05 |
Religiosity | −0.05 | 0.03 | |
Family Relations | −0.23 | ** | 0.09 |
Both Biological Parents | −0.32 | *** | 0.07 |
Reduced Price School Lunch | 0.03 | 0.09 | |
Time Stable Covariates: | |||
Sex (1=Male) | −0.18 | * | 0.07 |
Race (1=White) | −0.14 | 0.11 | |
Wave: | |||
2 (6th grade) | −3.57 | *** | 0.61 |
3 (7th grade) | −2.70 | *** | 0.60 |
4 (8th grade) | −2.20 | *** | 0.60 |
5 (9th grade) | −2.01 | ** | 0.60 |
6 (10th grade) | −2.09 | *** | 0.59 |
7 (11th grade) | −1.81 | ** | 0.59 |
8 (12th grade) | −1.61 | ** | 0.58 |
n=23,169 person-waves, 6,146 persons
NOTE: Models include fixed effects for 53 community-cohorts (not shown)
p < .05;
p <. 01;
p <. 001 (two-tailed tests)
Appendix C. Logistic Regression of Marijuana Persistence with Social Rewards and Moral Costs
Variables | b | SE | |
---|---|---|---|
Time Varying Covariates (t−1): | |||
Perceptions of MJ as Fun | 0.36 | * | 0.16 |
Social Rewards | 0.02 | 0.19 | |
Moral Costs | −0.35 | ** | 0.12 |
Number of MJ Friends | 0.30 | 0.20 | |
Friends’ Mean Fun Perceptions | 0.02 | 0.29 | |
Friends’ Mean Social Rewards | 0.02 | 0.38 | |
Friends’ Mean Moral Costs | 0.05 | 0.25 | |
Friendship Nominations Received | 0.04 | 0.03 | |
Delinquency | 0.04 | 0.04 | |
Alcohol Use | 0.30 | 0.23 | |
Risk and Sensation Seeking | 0.07 | 0.11 | |
School Adjustment and Bonding | 0.07 | 0.15 | |
Grades | −0.27 | 0.14 | |
Religiosity | −0.07 | 0.09 | |
Both Biological Parents | −0.08 | 0.22 | |
Family Relations | −0.16 | 0.28 | |
Reduced Price School Lunch | 0.19 | 0.28 | |
Wave | −0.26 | *** | 0.07 |
Time Stable Covariates: | |||
Sex (1=Male) | −0.06 | 0.20 | |
Race (1=White) | 0.29 | 0.26 | |
Time Varying Covariates (t−[t−1]): | |||
Δ Perceptions of MJ as Fun | 0.39 | *** | 0.10 |
Δ Social Rewards | −0.22 | * | 0.11 |
Δ Moral Costs | −0.39 | *** | 0.10 |
Δ Number of MJ Friends | 0.35 | ** | 0.11 |
Δ Friends’ Mean Intrinsic Rewards | −0.15 | 0.16 | |
Δ Friends’ Mean Social Rewards | −0.09 | 0.22 | |
Δ Friends’ Mean Moral Costs | −0.06 | 0.21 | |
Δ Friendship Nominations Received | 0.01 | 0.04 | |
Δ Delinquency | 0.05 | 0.04 | |
Δ Alcohol Use | 0.31 | 0.17 | |
Δ Risk and Sensation Seeking | 0.06 | 0.13 | |
Δ School Adjustment and Bonding | 0.29 | 0.19 | |
Δ Grades | −0.31 | 0.16 | |
Δ Religiosity | −0.11 | 0.07 | |
Δ Both Biological Parents | 0.25 | 0.38 | |
Δ Family Relations | −0.39 | 0.28 | |
Δ Reduced Price School Lunch | 0.27 | 0.34 | |
Constant | 2.26 | 1.39 |
n=997 (12 cases lost due to missing data on social rewards and moral costs)
NOTE: Robust Standard errors adjust for 53 community-cohorts
p < .05;
p <. 01;
p <. 001 (two-tailed tests)
Footnotes
Grants from the W.T. Grant Foundation (8316), National Institute on Drug Abuse (R01-DA018225), and National Institute of Child Health and Development (R24-HD041025) supported this research. The analyses used data from PROSPER, a project directed by R. L. Spoth, funded by the National Institute on Drug Abuse (RO1-DA013709) and the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (AA14702). The content of this article is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.
Indeed, transitions to full-time work, marriage, and childbirth tend to not only be uncommon early in the life course, but some adult roles are correlated with increased crime and delinquency rather than desistance in adolescence, such as when teenagers spend long hours on the job (see Staff, Mont’Alvao, and Mortimer 2015).
Although recognizing agency as important and an understudied component of desistance, Laub and Sampson (2003) continue to emphasize external informal controls, and the often arbitrary nature of actors unpredictably stumbling into them, as primary desistance mechanisms (i.e., “desistance by default”; Paternoster and Bushway 2009).
A typical assumption of these approaches is that rational decision-making is bounded or “constrained by limits of time, ability and the availability of relevant information.” (Cornish and Clarke 1986:1).
Although recent research using network-based measures of peer delinquency and sophisticated analyses of longitudinal network data suggest that the magnitude of peer influence effects is less than previously thought (for reviews, see Carrington 2011; Haynie and Kreager 2013; Papachristos 2011; Young and Rees 2013).
Each community was selected because it had a public school district with 1,300–5,200 enrolled students, and had at least 15% of students eligible for reduced-cost lunches.
For the discrete time models, a small percentage (~2%) of person-waves following a missing wave were not included in our analyses as we lack information about marijuana initiation during the time of the missing survey.
One of the Pennsylvania schools did not agree to participate in the network portion of the study, resulting in 27 school districts available for our analyses.
Approximately 36% of youth in the larger PROSPER sample had ever used marijuana by the Spring of the 12th grade. This percentage is only somewhat less than what is reported in the nationally representative Monitoring the Future (MTF) study. For instance, about 42% of 12th graders in the 2008 and 2009 MTF cohorts have ever used marijuana. The difference between the two samples is likely due to PROSPER consisting of a sample of rural communities in Iowa and Pennsylvania.
As we lack the post-initiation wave for those respondents who initiate in wave 8, we relied on their initiation year marijuana use to classify persistence. In unlisted analyses, we restricted analyses to waves 1–7 and found results virtually identical to those reported.
Due to prior theory and research, we chose to focus analyses on perceived psychic rewards as a primary independent variable rather than perceived social rewards or a latent measure combining the two measures.
In supplemental logistic regression models (not shown but available upon request), we used multiple imputation to retain youth who had initiated but had missing data on at least one of the predictor variables. The results from models based upon the imputed data did not substantially differ from those shown in Table 3.
Contributor Information
Derek A. Kreager, Pennsylvania State University
Daniel T. Ragan, University of New Mexico
Holly Nguyen, Pennsylvania State University.
Jeremy Staff, Pennsylvania State University.
References
- Akers Ronald L. Social Learning and Social Structure: A General Theory of Crime and Deviance. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Anwar Shamena, Loughran Thomas A. Testing a Bayesian learning theory of deterrence among serious juvenile offenders. Criminology. 2011;49(3):667–698. [Google Scholar]
- Baker Thomas, Piquero Alex R. Assessing the perceived benefits—criminal offending relationship. Journal of Criminal Justice. 2010;38(5):981–987. [Google Scholar]
- Baldwin John D. Thrill and adventure seeking and the age distribution of crime: Comment on Hirschi and Gottfredson. American Journal of Sociology. 1985;90:1326–1330. [Google Scholar]
- Becker Gary S. Crime and punishment: An economic approach. Journal of Political Economy. 1968;76(2):169–217. [Google Scholar]
- Becker Howard S. Becoming a Marihuana User. Chicago, Il: University of Chicago Press; 2015[1953]. [Google Scholar]
- Becker Howard S. Outsiders: Studies in the Sociology of Deviance. New York: Macmillan; 1973[1963]. [Google Scholar]
- Blossfeld Hans-Peter, Rohwer Gotz. Techniques of Event History Modeling. 2nd. Nahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Blumstein Alfred, Roth Jacqueline, Roth Jeffery A, Visher Christy. Criminal Careers and Career Criminals. Vol. 1. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Carrington Peter J. Crime and Social Network Analysis. In: Scott John, Carrington Peter J., editors. The SAGE Handbook of Social Network Analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2011. pp. 236–255. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen Albert K, Short James F. Research in delinquent subcultures. Journal of Social Issues. 1958;14(3):20–37. [Google Scholar]
- Copes Heith. Streetlife and the rewards of auto theft. Deviant Behavior. 2003;24(4):309–332. [Google Scholar]
- Cornish Derek B, Clarke Ronald V. The Reasoning Criminal: Rational Choice Perspectives on Offending. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers; 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Giordano Peggy C, Schroeder Ryan D, Cernkovich Stephen A. Emotions and crime over the life course: A neo-Meadian perspective on criminal continuity and change. American Journal of Sociology. 2007;112(6):1603–1661. [Google Scholar]
- Giordano Peggy C, Cernkovich Stephen A, Rudolph Jennifer L. Gender, crime, and desistance: Toward a theory of cognitive transformation. American Journal of Sociology. 2002;107(4):990–1064. [Google Scholar]
- Grasmick Harold G, Bursik Robert J., Jr Conscience, significant others, and rational choice: Extending the deterrence model. Law and Society Review. 1990;24(3):837–861. [Google Scholar]
- Hallstone Michael. Updating Howard Becker’s theory of using marijuana for pleasure. Contemporary Drug Problems. 2002;29(4):821–845. [Google Scholar]
- Haynie Dana L, Kreager Derek A. Peer Networks and Crime. In: Cullen Francis T, Wilcox Pamela., editors. The Oxford Handbook of Criminological Theory. New York: Oxford University Press; 2013. pp. 257–273. [Google Scholar]
- Hirschi Travis. Causes of Delinquency. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction; 2002[1969]. [Google Scholar]
- Hyman Steven E, Malenka Robert C, Nestler Eric J. Neural mechanisms of addiction: the role of reward-related learning and memory. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2006;29:565–98. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.113009. 21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacques Scott, Wright Richard. Code of the Suburb. Chicago, Il: University of Chicago Press; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Jennings Wesley G, Reingle Jennifer M. On the number and shape of developmental/life-course violence, aggression, and delinquency trajectories: A state-of-the-art review. Journal of Criminal Justice. 2012;40(6):472–489. [Google Scholar]
- Katz Jack. Seductions of Crime: Moral and Sensual Attractions in Doing Evil. Washington, DC: Basic Books; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- King Ryan D, Massoglia Michael, MacMillan Ross. The context of marriage and crime: gender, the propensity to marry, and offending in early adulthood. Criminology. 2007;45(1):33–65. [Google Scholar]
- Kreager Derek A, Matsueda Ross L, Erosheva Elena A. Motherhood and criminal desistance in Disadvantaged Neighborhoods. Criminology. 2010;48(1):221–258. [Google Scholar]
- Laub John, Sampson Robert J. Shared Beginnings, Divergent Lives: Delinquent Boys to Age 70. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Laub John H, Sampson Robert J. Turning points in the life course: Why change matters to the study of crime. Criminology. 1993;31:301–325. [Google Scholar]
- Laub John H, Nagin Daniel S, Sampson Robert J. Trajectories of change in criminal offending: Good marriages and the desistance process. American Sociological Review. 1998;63(2):225–238. [Google Scholar]
- Lochner Lance. Individual perceptions of the criminal justice system. American Economic Review. 2007;97:444–60. [Google Scholar]
- Loeber Rolf, Welsh Brandon C. The Future of Criminology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Loeber Rolf, Farrington David P. From Juvenile Delinquency to Adult Crime: Criminal Careers, Justice Policy and Prevention. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Lupica Carl R, Riegel Arthur C, Hoffman Alexander F. Marijuana and cannabinoid regulation of brain reward circuits. British Journal of Pharmacology. 2004;143(2):227–234. doi: 10.1038/sj.bjp.0705931. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maggs Jennifer L, Hurrelmann Klaus. Do substance use and delinquency have differential associations with adolescents’ peer relations? International Journal of Behavioral Development. 1998;22(2):367–388. [Google Scholar]
- Maruna Shadd. Making Good: How Ex-Convicts Reform and Rebuild Their Lives. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Massoglia Michael, Uggen Christopher. Settling down and aging out: Toward an interactionist theory of desistance and the transition to adulthood. American Journal of Sociology. 2010;116(2):543–582. doi: 10.1086/653835. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsueda Ross L. Cultural deviance theory: The remarkable persistence of a flawed term. Theoretical Criminology. 1997;1(4):429–452. [Google Scholar]
- Matsueda Ross L, Kreager Derek A, Huizinga David. Deterring delinquents: A rational choice model of theft and violence. American Sociological Review. 2006;71(1):95–122. [Google Scholar]
- Matza David, Sykes Gresham M. Juvenile delinquency and subterranean values. American Sociological Review. 1961;26(5):712–719. [Google Scholar]
- Matza David. Delinquency and Drift. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers; 1964. [Google Scholar]
- McCarthy Bill, Hagan John. When crime pays: Capital, competence, and criminal success. Social Forces. 2001;79(3):1035–1060. [Google Scholar]
- Bill McCarthy. New economics of sociological criminology. Annual Review of Sociology. 2002;28:417–442. [Google Scholar]
- Meeus Wim. The study of adolescent identity formation 2000–2010: A review of longitudinal research. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2011;21:75–94. [Google Scholar]
- Moffitt Terrie E. Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: a developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review. 1993;100(4):674–701. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nagin Daniel S, Farrington David P. Onset and persistence of offending. Criminology. 1992;30:501–524. [Google Scholar]
- Osgood D Wayne, Wilson Janet K, O’Malley Patrick M, Bachman Jerald G, Johnston Lloyd D. Routine activities and individual deviant behavior. American Sociological Review. 1996;61:635–655. [Google Scholar]
- Osgood D Wayne. Some future trajectories for life course criminology. In: Leober Rolf, Welsh Brandon C., editors. The Future of Criminology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Papachristos Andrew V. The Coming of a Networked Criminology? In: MacDonald John., editor. Measuring Crime and Criminality. New Brunswick: Transaction; 2011. pp. 101–140. [Google Scholar]
- Parker Howard, Aldridge Judith, Measham Fiona. Illegal Leisure The Normalization of Adolescent Recreational Drug Use. New York, NY: Routledge; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Paternoster Raymond, Bushway Shawn. Desistance and the feared self: Toward an identity theory of criminal desistance. The Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology. 2009;99:1103–1156. [Google Scholar]
- Paternoster Raymond, Triplett Ruth. Disaggregating self-reported delinquency and its implications for theory. Criminology. 1988;26(4):591–626. [Google Scholar]
- Pfefferbaum Betty, Wood Peter B. Self-report study of impulsive and delinquent behavior in college students. Journal of Adolescent Health. 1994;15(4):295–302. doi: 10.1016/1054-139x(94)90602-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pogarsky Greg, Piquero Alex R, Paternoster Raymond. Modeling change in perceptions about sanction threats: The neglected linkage in deterrence theory. Journal of Quantitative Criminology. 2004;20(4):343–369. [Google Scholar]
- Rose Geoffrey. Strategy of prevention: lessons from cardiovascular disease. British Medical Journal. 1981;282(6279):1847–1851. doi: 10.1136/bmj.282.6279.1847. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sampson Robert J, Laub John H. Crime and deviance over the life course: The salience of adult social bonds. American Sociological Review. 1990;55:609–627. [Google Scholar]
- Sampson Robert J, Laub John H. Crime in the Making: Pathways and Turning Points through Life. Boston, MA: Harvard University Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Sampson Robert J, Laub John H, Wimer Christopher. Does Marriage Reduce Crime? A Counterfactual Approach to Within-Individual Causal Effects. Criminology. 2006;44(3):465–508. [Google Scholar]
- Sampson Robert J, Laub John H. Socioeconomic achievement in the life course of disadvantaged men: Military service as a turning point, circa 1940–1965. American Sociological Review. 1995;61:347–367. [Google Scholar]
- Singer Judith D, Willett John B. Applied Longitudinal Data Analysis: Modeling Change and Event Occurrence. New York: Oxford University Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Smith Douglas A, Brame Robert. On the initiation and continuation of delinquency. Criminology. 1994;32(4):607–629. [Google Scholar]
- Spoth Richard, Redmond Cleve, Shin Chungyeol, Greenberg Mark, Clair Scott, Feinberg Mark. Substance-use outcomes at 18 months past baseline: The PROSPER community–university partnership trial. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2007;32(5):395–402. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2007.01.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spoth Richard, Greenberg Mark, Bierman Karen, Redmond Cleve. PROSPER community–university partnership model for public education systems: Capacity-building for evidence-based, competence-building prevention. Prevention Science. 2004;5:31–39. doi: 10.1023/b:prev.0000013979.52796.8b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Staff Jeremy, Greene Kaylin, Maggs Jennifer L, Schoon Ingrid. Family transitions and changes in drinking from adolescence through midlife. Addiction. 2014;109:227–236. doi: 10.1111/add.12394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Staff Jeremy, Mont’Alvao Arnaldo, Mortimer Jeylan T. Children at Work. In: Bornstein Marc, Leventhal Tama., editors. Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science: Volume 4: Ecological Settings and Processes. New Jersey: John Wiley and Sons; 2015. pp. 345–374. [Google Scholar]
- Steffensmeier Darrell J. The Fence: In the Shadow of Two Worlds. Lantham, MD: Rowman and Littlefield; 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Steinberg Laurence. A Social Neuroscience Perspective on Adolescent Risk-Taking. Developmental Review. 2008;28:78–106. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sutherland Edwin Hardin. Principles of Criminology. Philadelphia: Lippincott; 1947. [Google Scholar]
- Sweeten Gary, Piquero Alex R, Steinberg Laurence. Age and the explanation of crime, revisited. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2013;42(6):921–938. doi: 10.1007/s10964-013-9926-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uggen Christopher. Work as a turning point in the life course of criminals: A duration model of age, employment, and recidivism. American Sociological Review. 2000;65(4):529–546. [Google Scholar]
- Ven Vander Thomas. Getting Wasted Why College Students Drink Too Much and Party so Hard. New York, NY: New York University Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Warr Mark. Life-course transitions and desistance from crime. Criminology. 1998;36(1998):183–216. [Google Scholar]
- Warr Mark. Making delinquent friends: Adult supervision and children’s affiliations. Criminology. 2005;43(1):77–106. [Google Scholar]
- Weiss Karen G. Party School: Crime, Campus, and Community. Boston, MA: Northeastern University Press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Wolfgang Marvin Eugene, Ferracuti Franco. The Subculture of Violence: Toward an Integrated Theory in Criminology. Tavistock Publications; 1967. [Google Scholar]
- Young Jacob TN, Rees Carter. Social Networks and Delinquency in Adolescence: Implications for Life-Course Criminology. In: Gibson Chris L, Krohn Marvin D., editors. Handbook of Life-Course Criminology. New York: Springer; 2013. pp. 159–180. [Google Scholar]
- Zinberg Norman E. Drug, Set and Setting: The Basis for Controlled Intoxicant Use. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Zuckerman Marvin. The psychophysiology of sensation seeking. Journal of Personality. 1990;58(1):313–345. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1990.tb00918.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]