Abstract
The family environment can either play a detrimental or a protective role in symptom severity for people with schizophrenia. The current study examined both patient and caregiver perspectives of the family environment in an ethnically diverse sample of 221 patients with schizophrenia. We hypothesized that environments characterized by high levels of perceived caregiver criticism, low perceived caregiver warmth, and low family cohesion (from both the patient and caregiver perspective) would predict greater symptom severity. As expected, results demonstrated that lower patient ratings of family cohesion and caregiver warmth were associated with greater symptom severity. However, once put into a hierarchical regression analysis, only patient ratings of family cohesion remained significant. Ethnic patterns were also examined and revealed that family cohesion may be particularly protective for ethnic minorities. Study implications are discussed.
Keywords: patient perspective, symptom severity, family cohesion, caregiver warmth
1. Introduction
Schizophrenia is a severe and chronic mental illness that affects the entire family, with family members often becoming life-long caregivers for patients. Due to strong familial involvement in the care of the diagnosed individual, the family environment has been studied extensively as an influential psychosocial factor that relates to prognosis. The majority of studies have examined family environmental constructs from an independent observer’s perspective through use of clinical assessment tools such as Expressed Emotion (EE) generally being measured with the Camberwell Family Interview (CFI) or the Five-Minute Speech Sample (FMSS). However, few studies have focused on how patients perceive family attitudes and how these perceptions influence patient outcomes. Some previous findings suggest that patient perceptions may actually be better predictors of patient outcomes such as symptom severity (Cutting and Docherty, 2000; Kopelowicz et al., 2002; Onwumere et al., 2009; Tompson et al., 1995; Weisman et al., 2006). Even fewer studies have examined both patient perspectives of their family environment and family member perspectives within the same study. The current study will do so with the aim of providing a more comprehensive assessment of protective and risk factors (from patient and family member perspectives) and their links to patient symptom severity.
1.1 Perceptions of Protective and Risk Factors
For patients with schizophrenia, regular contact with high-EE family members (family members who are highly critical, hostile, and/or emotionally over-involved) has been well established as a psychosocial risk factor known to contribute to worsening of patient symptomatology and higher relapse rates (Cutting and Docherty, 2000; Linszen et al., 1997; Scazufca and Kuipers, 2001; Tompson et al., 1995; Van Humbeeck et al., 2004; Weisman et al., 2005). The extant literature has largely focused on the criticism component of EE as it is widely recognized as a risk factor that exacerbates the illness (Hooley and Parker, 2006; Weisman et al., 2006). Since criticism is viewed as the most important component of EE, previous studies that have examined patient perceptions of EE have primarily focused on perceived criticism from the patient’s perspective. The relationship between higher Perceived Criticism (PC) and greater symptom severity has been demonstrated in schizophrenia as well as other mental disorders. For example, Barrowclough et al. (2003) found a significant relationship between patient ratings of more criticism from relatives and increased positive symptoms (mediated by a negative self-evaluation). Similarly, Onwumere et al. (2009) found that PC was associated with higher ratings on the general psychopathology subscale of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS), independent of CFI ratings. Once again, these results indicate that patient perspectives may be better predictors of symptom severity when compared with scores on clinical assessment measures. Collectively, previous findings suggest that patient perceptions of greater levels of PC are associated with poorer outcomes for patients such as increases in symptom severity, and ultimately, rehospitalization.
Understandably, the majority of family environment studies have focused on risk factors which exacerbate the illness (e.g., criticism). However, positive family characteristics such as warmth and family cohesion may have a protective effect that aids in symptom recovery (González-Pinto et al., 2011; López et al., 2004). Family Cohesion (FC) is a family environmental construct which directly assesses patient perceptions of family unity through a self-report measure. Perceptions of low FC have been found to be significantly associated with increased symptom severity across various disorders, including schizophrenia. For example, Weisman et al. (2005) found that Hispanic/Latino (H/L) and African-American patients who reported greater levels of family cohesion had fewer psychiatric symptoms and reported feeling less distress. Similar findings exist with regard to the construct of warmth. For example, Bertrando et al. (1992) found that in a sample of Italian patients with schizophrenia, relapse rates were lower when the patients came from families that were rated as higher on warmth. Interestingly, even if the family was rated as high-EE, high warmth still served as a protective factor against relapse.
Scant literature has examined protective factors despite the value and insight they may offer. While previous studies have obtained patient and family member perceptions of family cohesion, to the best of our knowledge, patient (and caregiver) perceptions of warmth have yet to be examined. Instead, previously reviewed studies have used independent observer ratings of warmth from the CFI. Based on previous literature demonstrating the discrepancies between clinical assessment ratings and patient perceptions of their family environment (e.g., Medina-Pradas and colleagues (2013) found no correlation between patient perceptions of caregiver criticism and caregiver EE ratings from the CFI), we may be missing valuable information by not obtaining the patient’s perspective on the protective factor of warmth. Furthermore, in order to determine which risk and protective factors have the most impact on patient symptom severity, it seems important to obtain multiple perspectives on the family environment and examine them simultaneously within studies as these variables do not occur in isolation. In fact, discrepancies in perceptions of the family environment between patients and their caregivers are quite common (King and Dixon, 1995). Several studies have demonstrated discrepancies in patient and family member perspectives of the family environment. For example, Cañivé et al. (1995) found that patient, father, and mother ratings on the Family Environment Scale were poorly correlated with each other. Weisman et al. (2005) similarly found that patient and family member ratings of the family environment (specifically family cohesion) did not correspond. Therefore, directly examining patient and family member perspectives of their family environment within one study is crucial in determining which provides better prediction of psychopathology.
1.2 Ethnic Differences
Little research has examined the role that ethnicity plays in family environmental constructs (López et al., 2004). Yet previous findings suggest that there may be a cultural disconnect in “outsider” or independent observer ratings compared to patient perceptions of the same interactions (Guada et al., 2011; Tompson et al., 1995; Weisman et al., 2006). Prior literature also suggests that some constructs may function differently or may be of more or less importance for individuals from different cultural backgrounds (Kopelowicz et al., 2002; Herman et al., 2007; Rosenfarb et al., 2006; Tompson et al., 1995; Weisman et al., 2005). In fact, some studies have found differential results by ethnicity demonstrating the importance of these constructs, particularly for ethnic minority patients. For example, in a previous study conducted by Weisman and colleagues (2005), results demonstrated that higher levels of family cohesion were significantly associated with less emotional distress for H/L and African-American patients with schizophrenia. Interestingly, these results were not found in Caucasian patients. In a sample of adolescents with depression, Herman et al. (2007) similarly found that for African-American adolescents, low family cohesion was significantly associated with increased depression symptomatology. However, in line with Weisman et al. (2005), this relationship was not significant for Caucasian adolescents. The aforementioned studies are just two examples which seem to suggest that it not only makes sense to obtain patient and family member perspectives on their family environments but to also examine these constructs within a cultural context.
1.3 The Current Study
The current study examines several family environmental risk and protective factors from family member and patient perspectives. More specifically, patient perceptions of primary caregiver’s criticalness and primary caregiver’s warmth, and both patient and primary caregiver’s perceptions of overall family cohesion are examined. The current study also addresses previous study limitations by examining these constructs in a large ethnically diverse sample with the ability to assess for differential patterns based on one’s ethnicity. It is hoped that findings from this study will offer a more complete picture of influential family environmental factors that play a role in patient symptom severity. The overall goal is to better predict factors that lead to increased symptom severity so that we may provide more effective treatment to patients by focusing our efforts on reducing risk factors within their homes and cultivating protective factors. The results will be examined first with the full sample and then stratified by ethnicity. This will allow us to carefully examine the strength and directionality of relationships in each ethnic group.
1.4 Hypotheses
It was expected that higher patient ratings of perceived criticism, lower patient perceptions of warmth, and lower ratings of family cohesion (from both the patient and the family member perspective) would be associated with more severe psychiatric symptoms (Arshad et al., 2011; Barrowclough et al., 2003; González-Pinto et al., 2011; Hooley and Teasdale, 1989; Lebell et al., 1993; López et al., 1999; López et al., 2004; Medina-Pradas et al., 2011; Medina-Pradas et al., 2013; Tompson et al., 1995; Weisman et al., 2005). We also assessed a comprehensive model for predicting symptom severity that included both protective and risk factors in the same analysis. This allowed us to assess each variable’s unique role in predicting symptom severity, when other important, related variables were controlled. Because this analysis was largely exploratory, no specific hypotheses were offered.
We also stratified results by patient ethnicity in order to determine if different patterns of associations were present. Stratification allowed us to examine the strength and directionality of relationships as well as compare results across ethnicities. Based on prior research, we hypothesized that when results were stratified by ethnicity, protective factors would carry more weight for ethnic minority patients (Herman et al., 2007; López et al., 2004; Weisman et al., 2005) and the risk factor of perceived caregiver criticism would carry more weight for Caucasian patients (Barrowclough et al., 2003; Onwumere et al., 2009).
2. Methods
2.1 Subjects
The current study is part of a larger treatment study examining how a family-focused, Culturally Informed Treatment for Schizophrenia (CIT-S) and other psychosocial factors relate to patient and caregiver functioning in individuals diagnosed with schizophrenia and their family members (see Weisman, 2005; Weisman de Mamani et al., 2014 for a more detailed description of the larger project). Results from this intervention study demonstrated that when compared to a psychoeducation only control condition, patients assigned to the CIT-S condition had significantly lower psychiatric symptom severity at treatment termination (while controlling for baseline symptoms; Weisman de Mamani et al., 2014). Furthermore, caregivers in the CIT-S condition also demonstrated reduced levels of caregiver burden at treatment termination in comparison to those in the control condition (Weisman de Mamani & Suro, 2015). Patients without family members were invited to participate in a multifamily group version of the study which we are currently evaluating. However, only baseline data prior to any intervention was examined for the current study. Participants were recruited for a schizophrenia treatment intervention study from advertisements displayed in local hospitals, newspapers, and in the cars of Miami’s above-ground rail system, the Metrorail. Individuals interested in participating in the study responded to our advertisements by calling our telephone number and completing a short eligibility phone screen with one of our research associates. Patients who appeared to meet criteria for schizophrenia were then invited to schedule an in-person assessment at our clinic. Patients who did not have any family members who were willing or available to participate in the family treatment intervention trial were channeled into the multi-family group portion of the intervention trial. The group intervention covered the same material covered in the CIT-S treatment condition, simply in a multi-family group format.
The sample consisted of 221 patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder (69.2% male, 30.8% female) with a mean age of 40.97 (SD = 11.45). Patients self-identified their ethnicity as Caucasian (15.8%), African-American (41.2%), H/L (36.7%), or Other (2.3%). Nine patients had missing data for ethnicity (4.1%). Patients also identified their education level with 3.2% of the sample completing less than grade 8, 2.7% completing grade 8, 19.9% completing some high school, 26.7% graduating from high school, 34.8% completing some college, 8.1% completing a college degree, and 1.4% completing an advanced degree. 3.2% of the sample had missing data for education level. Assessments were conducted in the participant’s preferred language (either English or Spanish) with 91.9% of patients choosing to complete their assessment in English and 8.1% completing the assessment in Spanish. At the time of the assessment, patient diagnosis of either schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder was confirmed through use of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV, Patient Edition (SCID-I/P, Version 2.0), Psychotic Symptoms module (First et al., 2002). Exclusion criteria for patients included being acutely psychotic at the time of the assessment (as rated by BPRS items scored greater than 5) and unable to provide informed consent, not meeting criteria for schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, being suicidal at the time of the assessment, having a recent suicide attempt (within the last year), or having been involuntarily hospitalized within the past 3 months. Patients who had been recently incarcerated for violent crimes were also ineligible to participate in the current study.
Patients could have multiple family members participate with them in the study. However, the current study only used data from the family member who was reported to be the patient’s primary caregiver. Eligibility to participate in the current study as a caregiver was dependent on patient eligibility (please see above). Primary caregivers were defined as the relative who reported spending the most time with the patient. Primary caregivers who participated in the study were required to be in contact with the patient for a minimum of 1 or more hours per week over the past three months. However, 34.2% of our sample indicated that patients either lived with their primary caregivers or spent over 100 hours per week together. Of the 221 patients in this sample, 114 of them also had primary caregivers participate with them. The remaining patients (n=107) did not. The sample of caregivers (n=114) had a mean age of 48.70 (SD = 13.47) and was 43.9% male and 56.1% female. Primary caregivers self-identified their ethnicity as Caucasian (21.1%), African-American (31.6%), H/L (46.5%), or Other (0.9%). Primary caregivers had their assessments conducted in their preferred language (either English or Spanish) with 77.2% choosing to complete their assessment in English and 22.8% completing the assessment in Spanish. With regard to their relationship to the patient, primary caregivers in this study identified as mother (28.9%), father (8.8%), significant other (29.8%), sister (4.4%), brother (5.3%), daughter (3.5%), son (3.5%), friend (10.5%), uncle (0.9%), niece (0.9%), grandmother (0.9%), or cousin (2.6%). While a large portion of primary caregivers were significant others (29.8%), the majority were mothers and fathers (37.7%).
2.2 Translation of Measures
All measures were translated from English to Spanish using an editorial review board approach consisting of members from diverse backgrounds including Cuba, Colombia, Nicaragua, Costa Rica, Mexico, and Puerto Rico. This approach is considered to be a more effective translation method when compared with translation-back translation as the editorial review board takes into consideration within-group language variations (Geisinger, 1994). The measures were first translated by a native Spanish speaker into Spanish. This translator then met with the editorial review board in which each member independently reviewed the Spanish version and compared it to the English version. Next, members of the board discussed any discrepancies and attempted to come to an agreement about the most generic and most easily understood wording. The board then met a second time to once again independently compare the English and Spanish versions of the measures and discuss any remaining discrepancies until a consensus was reached on all items.
2.3 Measures
Demographics
Participants completed a demographics questionnaire in which they provided information such as age, gender, race/ethnicity, years of education, and hours of social contact (i.e., in-person interactions, communication via telephone, email, etc.) between patient and relative/caregiver.
Eligibility for Current Study
The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV, Patient Edition (SCID-I/P, Version 2.0), Psychotic Symptoms module (First et al., 2002) was used to determine if a patient met lifetime criteria for a diagnosis of either schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. The SCID-I/P has demonstrated high inter-rater reliability for both symptoms and diagnoses (Ventura et al., 1998). Inter-rater reliability for the current study was determined by having the Principal Investigator (Amy Weisman de Mamani) and all other interviewers watch six videotapes of SCID-I/P interviews and provide their independent determinations of patient diagnoses. Inter-rater agreement for the current study using Cohen’s Kappa is 1.0.
Perceptions of EE (Primary Caregiver Warmth and Criticism)
The Perceptions of EE Scale was designed by Weisman and colleagues and used in a previous study (see Weisman et al., 2006). This 2-item instrument includes the following two questions: 1) In describing your relative, would you say (relative’s name) is not at all warm, somewhat warm, or very warm? 2) In describing your relative, would you say (relative’s name) is not at all critical, somewhat critical, or very critical? Patients must choose one of the three options provided and are encouraged to choose the response that best describes their primary caregiver. These items were coded such that higher scores are indicative of more warmth and more criticism (answers ranging from 1 to 3).
Family Cohesion
Patient and caregiver perceptions of family unity were measured through the Cohesion subscale of the Family Environment Scale (FES) (Moos and Moos, 1981). Participants were read the following prompt: The following are statements about families. Circle T if the statement is true or mostly true for most members of your family. Circle F if the statement is false or mostly false for most members of your family. Answer questions based on the LAST 3 MONTHS or SINCE YOUR LAST ASSESSMENT. This subscale consists of 9 true/false statements designed to assess the degree to which patients feel their family provides help, support, and commitment to each other (Weisman et al., 2005). For example, “Family members really help and support one another.” The FES is an easy to administer and score self-report measure that obtains family members’ perceptions of their family environment. Furthermore, the psychometric properties of the FES have been demonstrated in English, Spanish, and Chinese and across ethnicities including Chinese, H/L, and African-Americans (McEachern and Kenny, 2002; Phillips et al., 1998; Weisman and López, 1996). A total score on the FES Cohesion subscale was calculated by summing the participants’ scores on each T/F item. Since some of the items are reverse-coded, these items were reversed so that higher total scores were indicative of perceptions of more cohesion. This scale is reported to have demonstrated adequate to good internal reliability (Cronbach’s alpha .63–.78) (Moos and Moos, 1981; Weisman et al., 2005). Internal reliability for the overall study sample (patients and caregivers combined), using Cronbach’s alpha was .794. Patients only =.832 (English= .832; Spanish= .724). Caregivers only = .641 (English= .652; Spanish= .577).
Symptom Severity
Current severity of patient symptomatology was measured through use of the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS) (Lukoff et al., 1986; Overall and Gorham, 1962). A total BPRS score was obtained by summing patient scores on all 24 items. Higher scores were indicative of greater symptom severity. Each of the 24 items is assessed using a 7-point anchor rating with 1 indicating “not present” to 7 indicating “extremely severe.” It should be noted that in the current study, patients who were acutely psychotic (i.e., scoring a “6” or “7” on any of the critical items) were ineligible to participate. The BPRS is reported to have good reliability and has been reported as having intraclass coefficients ranging from .74–1.00 on scale items (Weisman et al., 2005). The Principal Investigator (Amy Weisman de Mamani) completed a UCLA BPRS training and quality assurance program and has demonstrated reliability with the course creator, Dr. Joseph Ventura. Dr. Weisman de Mamani trained all graduate student interviewers. Interviewers then coded six training videotapes selected by Dr. Joseph Ventura. Intraclass correlations between interviewers and consensus ratings of Dr. Ventura ranged from .79 to .98 for total BPRS scores.
2.4 Data Analyses
All analyses were conducted using SPSS Statistics software, Version 21. Variables of interest were calculated such that higher scores were reflective of greater levels of the construct being measured (e.g., more warmth, more family cohesion, greater symptom severity). Normality was assessed and all variables were examined for any outliers. The relationships between primary study variables (Patient Perceptions of Caregiver Warmth, Patient Perceptions of Caregiver Criticism, Patient Perceptions of Family Cohesion, and Caregiver Perceptions of Family Cohesion) and select demographic variables (age, gender, education, primary language, hours of social contact between patient and relative) were examined as potential covariates. If determined to be significant, covariates were controlled for in the primary analyses.
The primary analyses were aimed at assessing family environmental risk and protective factors that may predict patient symptom severity. A series of zero-order correlations were conducted between all primary variables that were continuous and a correlation matrix was prepared. Results from this matrix allowed us to test whether higher patient ratings of perceived criticism, lower patient perceptions of warmth, and lower ratings of family cohesion (from both the patient and the family member perspective) were associated with more severe psychiatric symptoms (when no other variables were being controlled). Results from the correlation matrix were used to identify significant predictors of symptom severity that could be further examined in a simultaneous regression analysis to evaluate a more comprehensive model of both risk and protective factors of symptom severity. All continuous independent variables were centered prior to regression analyses (Aiken and West, 1991; McClelland and Judd, 1993). We also used this matrix to assess for the possibility of excessively high levels of multicollinearity among predictor variables. If predictor variables had an absolute Pearson’s r value of .7 or above, the variable with the higher correlation with patient symptom severity would be retained in the analyses and the other variable would be dropped. While thresholds used in previous studies range from a more restrictive .4 to a less restrictive .85, a cut off of .7 is the most commonly selected threshold (Dormann et al., 2013). If significant covariates were identified, hierarchical regression analyses were used and covariates were added in step 1 of the model and predictor variables in step 2. R2 was first examined to determine the percent of symptom severity that could be accounted for by the linear combination of variables. Standardized beta weights of each predictor were also examined to evaluate the relative contribution of each predictor while partialling out the effects of the others.
3. Results
3.1 Preliminary Analyses
Missing data
Missing data were present for all study variables but appeared to be missing at random with no indication of systematic response biases. Little’s Missing Completely At Random (MCAR) test was non-significant and supports the presence of MCAR data, χ2 (151) = 169.650, p = .142. A listwise deletion approach was used for all analyses.
Study Variables
All variables were assessed for normality and outliers. According to Curran et al. (1996), non-normality issues arise when univariate values are 2.0 or larger for skewness and 7.0 or larger for kurtosis. All study variables’ skewness and kurtosis values were within normal limits and no transformations were required. In fact, most values were within a conservative −1 to +1 range (see Table 1 for specific values).
Demographic Variables
Information for all demographic variables such as gender, ethnicity, education, caregiver relationship to patient, primary language, and amount of weekly social contact between caregiver and patient, can be found in the Subjects section. A correlation matrix was created to assess the relationships between continuous variables and identify any potential covariates (see Table 2). Education was found to be significantly correlated with BPRS scores such that patients with lower education were found to have higher BPRS scores indicating greater symptom severity (r= −.163, p= .019).
Independent sample t-tests were conducted to examine the relationships between dichotomous categorical variables (gender of patient, gender of caregiver, primary language) and continuous study variables. It was found that patients whose primary language was Spanish, had significantly higher family cohesion FES scores (M = 16.14, SD= 2.03) when compared to patients whose primary language was English (M= 14.26, SD= 2.87), equal variances not assumed, t(17.162)= −3.226, p= .005. Patients with a primary language of Spanish, also had significantly lower levels of education (M = 3.22, SD= 1.31), as compared to English-speaking patients, (M = 4.30, SD= 1.13), t(212)= 3.823, p< .001. Patient language and level of education were both considered significant covariates and were controlled for in subsequent primary analyses. Ethnicity was not examined as a covariate because results are later stratified by ethnicity.
3.2 Primary Analyses
Results from Correlation Matrix
Our first study hypothesis predicted that higher patient ratings of perceived criticism, lower patient perceptions of warmth, and lower ratings of family cohesion (from both the patient and the family member perspective) would be associated with more severe psychiatric symptoms. Results from zero-order correlations partially supported this hypothesis as lower patient ratings of perceived caregiver warmth were associated with greater patient symptom severity scores on the BPRS (r= −.149, p= .042) and lower patient ratings of family cohesion were also associated with higher BPRS scores (r= −.297, p< .001). Patient perceived caregiver criticism and lower ratings of family cohesion (from the family member perspective) were not significantly associated with symptom severity. Please see table 2 for correlation matrix.
Regression Analysis with significant predictors from correlation matrix
Multicollinearity among predictor variables was assessed and determined to not be problematic as the largest correlation was .395. Therefore, no variables were dropped from the analysis. Since significant covariates were identified, hierarchical regression was used for this analysis. In order to determine how well symptom severity was predicted by primary study variables over and above the covariates, patient education and patient primary language were added in block 1 of the model and the centered predictor variables were added in block 2 (patient ratings of caregiver warmth and patient family cohesion scores). Results indicated that the covariates of patient education and language accounted for approximately 6.4% of the variability in patient symptom severity, R2= .064, F(2,173)= 5.936, p= .003. Only the partial correlation between symptom severity and education was significant, t(173)= 3.409, β = .253, partial r= .251, p= .001. The linear combination of the predictor variables and the relationship with symptom severity was significant over and above the covariates, and explained an additional 9.0% of the variability in patient symptom severity, R2= .154, R2 change= .090, F(2,171)= 9.100, p< .001. Education was once again significant, t(171)= 3.264, β = .232, partial r= .242, p= .001. Patient family cohesion scores were also a significant predictor, t(171)= −3.669, β = −.285, partial r= −.270, p= .001. Since patient ratings of family cohesion was found to be a significant predictor of symptom severity, our first study hypothesis was partially supported by hierarchical regression results.
Results stratified by ethnicity
Because previous literature suggests the link between family environment and symptom severity may vary by ethnicity, we re-ran results stratified by ethnicity. Only Caucasian (n= 35), African-American (n= 91), and H/L (n= 81) patients were included in the analyses. The means and standard deviations for all continuous study variables are presented in tables 3 through 7. Overall sample mean and standard deviation is listed followed by results stratified by ethnicity. Following Herman et al. (2007), López et al. (2004), and Weisman et al. (2005), we hypothesized that protective factors would carry more weight for ethnic minority patients. We also hypothesized that the risk factor of patient perceived caregiver criticism would carry more weight for Caucasian patients (following Barrowclough et al., 2003 and Onwumere et al., 2009). This hypothesis was partially supported as family cohesion remained significant for H/L (F(3,57)= 3.678, p=.017, β = −.374, p=.006) and African-American patients (F(3,74)= −2.529, p=.064, β = −.306, p=.016). Interestingly, this pattern did not hold for Caucasian patients as neither the overall model nor any predictors were significant (F(3,25)= .119, p=.948, FC β= −.059, p=.796, WARM β = .133, p=.593, CRIT β= .093, p=.707).
4. Discussion
The overarching objective of the current study was to examine risk and protective factors of the family environment utilizing a combination of patient and caregiver perspectives. Because the variables examined do not act in isolation, the current study aimed to determine which variables have the most impact on patient symptom severity when examined collectively. An additional aim of the present study was to assess whether patterns varied by ethnicity. Our study hypotheses were partially supported. Patient perceptions of greater caregiver warmth and patient perceptions of higher levels of family cohesion were associated with lower levels of patient symptom severity. However, contrary to expectations, the constructs of patient perceived caregiver criticism and caregiver perceptions of family cohesion were not significantly associated with patient symptom severity.
Results from our analyses seem to suggest that the presence of protective factors in the home may have a greater impact on patient symptom severity than the presence (or perception) of risk factors. Alternatively, even if risk factors are present in the family environment, protective factors may buffer against their detrimental influence. Our findings are in line with previous studies which demonstrate that protective factors such as family warmth and positive statements are associated with lower symptom severity, better social functioning, and lower relapse rates (Bertrando et al., 1992; Ivanović et al., 1994; López et al., 2004; Medina-Pradas et al., 2013; O’Brien et al., 2006). Findings from the current study also illustrate the weight that patient perception measures may carry when compared to other family member perspectives. Interestingly, our results are in line with Weisman et al. (2005) in which the authors found (in an unrelated sample), that patient and family member ratings of family cohesion did not correspond. Thus, our findings indicate that while assessing all family members’ perspectives may be important, seeking the patient perspective (particularly for protective factors) may be most useful in terms of predicting patient symptom severity.
When examining results stratified by ethnicity, study hypotheses were partially supported in that the protective factor of family cohesion was significantly associated with symptom severity in ethnic minority patients. Interestingly, none of the primary study variables were associated with symptom severity in our Caucasian subsample. Given that the majority of our sample was comprised of H/L and African-American patients, this could indicate that ethnic minority patients may have driven the majority of findings and that family cohesion may carry more weight for these patients. However, it is interesting to note that these differential patterns replicate results from a previous study with an entirely different sample (see Weisman et al., 2005). It is also noteworthy that patients with a primary language of Spanish had lower levels of education yet had significantly higher family cohesion scores when compared to patients with a primary language of English. Findings seem to suggest the importance of family cohesion for ethnic minority patients, especially H/L patients.
4.1 Limitations, Future Directions, Conclusion
There were several limitations in the present study. First, this sample was comprised of cross-sectional data which prevents us from being able to speak to any longitudinal influences or directions of the associations such as how the relationships observed here could potentially impact patient relapse rates across time. Additionally, it is possible that patients with more severe symptoms on the BPRS (e.g., suspiciousness) may perceive their family members as less supportive because of the nature of the symptoms. It is also possible that patients’ symptoms of paranoia could negatively impact their perception of family cohesion. Similarly, we excluded patients who were acutely psychotic; thus, study findings may not apply to this population. Secondly, our sample was comprised primarily of ethnic minority patients (~83%) with a comparatively smaller sub-sample of Caucasian patients. Therefore, the results for Caucasians may be unreliable. However, they appear to replicate previous results giving us more confidence in the finding. Finally, our reliance on self-report measures, including single-item measures of patient perceived warmth and criticism, is another limitation of the study. Corroborating these findings with objective measures will be important in future research. In other words, we believe that an evaluation that includes both objective and subjective measures will provide the most comprehensive assessment.
In the future, information from the present study could be used to tailor assessments and treatments for patients with schizophrenia. For instance, though a few researchers have expressed concerns that patients may not be able to provide accurate assessments (e.g., Kohler et al., 2010; Poole et al., 1997), utilizing self-report measures to obtain the patient’s perspective on their family environment could offer valuable information regarding the likelihood of current and future relapses. Knowing that patients hold negative perceptions of certain aspects of the family environment (i.e., lower perceptions of warmth and/or cohesion) could raise an early red flag for clinicians, given its association with symptomatology. Thus, when patient perceptions indicate that something is amiss, clinicians may choose to directly target familial functioning and perceptions of family cohesion prior to addressing other issues (e.g., education about the illness, social skills). Fortunately, the measures used in this study to assess patient perceptions are quick and easy to use, require minimal training, and can offer crucial insight into the family environment.
Table 1.
Variable | N* | Mean | Standard Deviation |
Skewness | Kurtosis | Possible Range |
Observed Range |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Patient age | 216 | 40.97 | 11.45 | −.167 | −.872 | 18–100+ | 18–65 |
Caregiver age | 112 | 48.70 | 13.47 | −.141 | −.320 | 14–100+ | 16–81 |
IP FES | 200 | 14.40 | 2.855 | −.454 | −1.166 | 9–18 | 9–18 |
CG FES | 107 | 15.26 | 2.065 | −.548 | −.483 | 9–18 | 10–18 |
FES discrepancy | 96 | 2.15 | 2.08 | 1.209 | .879 | 0–9 | 0–8 |
IP WARM | 192 | 2.55 | .6202 | −1.064 | .085 | 1–3 | 1–3 |
IP CRIT | 192 | 2.03 | .7755 | −.045 | −1.331 | 1–3 | 1–3 |
BPRS | 211 | 55.57 | 13.15 | −.233 | .267 | 24–168 | 24–87 |
IPfes= patient FES total score; CGfes= caregiver FES total score; FES discrepancy = discrepancy between patient and caregiver FES scores; IPwarm= patient perception of caregiver warmth; IPcrit= patient perception of caregiver criticism; BPRS = patient total BPRS scores
differences in sample sizes vary by variable due to missing data.
Table 2.
Variable | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. IPwarm | 1 | ||||||||
2. IPcrit | −.342** | 1 | |||||||
3. IPfes | .395** | −.191** | 1 | ||||||
4.CGfes | .059 | .005 | .256* | 1 | |||||
5. IPage | −.010 | −.029 | −.070 | −.078 | 1 | ||||
6. CGage | −.170 | .199 | .047 | .048 | −.048 | 1 | |||
7. IPedu | −.085 | −.012 | .074 | −.023 | −.072 | .058 | 1 | ||
8. Hours | .173 | .078 | −.103 | .022 | −.133 | −.041 | −.193 | 1 | |
9. BPRS | −.149* | .096 | −.297** | −.101 | .096 | −.017 | −.163* | .051 | 1 |
IPwarm= patient perception of caregiver warmth; IPcrit= patient perception of caregiver criticism; IPfes= patient FES total score; CGfes= caregiver FES total score; IPage= patient age; CGage= caregiver age; IPedu= patient education; Hours= weekly number of hours of social contact between patient and caregiver; BPRS = patient total BPRS scores
p<.05
p<.01
Table 3.
M | SD | |
---|---|---|
Overall sample | 14.40 | 2.86 |
By ethnicity | ||
Caucasian (n=34) | 14.68 | 2.67 |
African-American (n=84) | 14.24 | 2.94 |
H/L (n=73) | 14.48 | 2.85 |
Table 4.
M | SD | |
---|---|---|
Overall sample | 15.26 | 2.07 |
By ethnicity | ||
Caucasian (n=23) | 15.35 | 1.85 |
African-American (n=36) | 14.97 | 2.14 |
H/L (n=52) | 15.37 | 2.11 |
Table 5.
M | SD | |
---|---|---|
Overall sample | 2.55 | .620 |
By ethnicity | ||
Caucasian (n=32) | 2.44 | .564 |
African-American (n=83) | 2.65 | .614 |
H/L (n=69) | 2.51 | .633 |
Table 6.
M | SD | |
---|---|---|
Overall sample | 2.03 | .7755 |
By ethnicity | ||
Caucasian (n=32) | 1.81 | .780 |
African-American (n=84) | 2.00 | .760 |
H/L (n=69) | 2.13 | .803 |
Table 7.
M | SD | |
---|---|---|
Overall sample | 55.57 | 13.15 |
By ethnicity | ||
Caucasian (n=33) | 55.18 | 12.48 |
African-American (n=89) | 56.16 | 11.98 |
H/L (n=78) | 54.40 | 14.42 |
Footnotes
Conflict of Interest
None declared.
References
- Aiken LS, West SG. Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions. Thousand Oaks, C.A: Sage Publications, Inc.; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Arshad S, Qaider F, Adil A. Impact of Caregivers’ Expressed Emotion on Their Mental Health and Relapse Symptom Severity Among Schizophrenics. Journal of Behavioral Sciences. 2011;21:1–17. [Google Scholar]
- Barrowclough C, Tarrier N, Humphreys L, Ward J, Gregg L, Andrews B. Self-Esteem in Schizophrenia: Relationships Between Self-Evaluation, Family Attitudes, and Symptomatology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2003;112:92–99. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bertrando P, Beltz J, Bressi C, Clerici M, Farma T, Invernizzi G, Cazzullo CL. Expressed Emotion and Schizophrenia in Italy: A study of an Urban Population. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1992;161:223–229. doi: 10.1192/bjp.161.2.223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cañivé JM, Sanz-Fuentenebro J, Vázquez C, Qualls C, Fuentenebro F, Tuason VB. Family Environment Predictors of Outcome in Schizophrenic Patients in Spain: A Nine-Month Follow-Up Study. Acta Psychiatry Scand. 1995;92:371–377. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1995.tb09599.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curran P, West S, Finch J. The Robustness of Test Statistics to Nonnormality and Specification Error in Confirmatory Analysis. Psychological Methods. 1996;1:16–29. [Google Scholar]
- Cutting LP, Docherty NM. Schizophrenia Outpatients’ Perceptions of Their Parents: Is Expressed Emotion a Factor? Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2000;109:266–272. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dormann CF, Elith J, Bacher S, Buchmann C, Gudrun C, Carré G, García Marquéz JR, et al. Collinearity: A Review of Methods to Deal with It and a Simulation Study Evaluating Their Performance. Ecography. 2013;36:27–46. [Google Scholar]
- First M, Spitzer R, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition with Psychotic Screen (SCID-I/P W/ PSY SCREEN) New York: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Geisinger KF. Cross-Cultural Normative Assessment: Translation and Adaptation Issues Influencing the Normative Interpretation of Assessment Instruments. Psychological Assessment. 1994;6:304–312. [Google Scholar]
- González-Pinto A, Ruiz de Azúa S, Ibáñez B, Otero-Cuesta S, Castro-Fornieles J, Montserrat GB, Ugarte A, et al. Can Positive Family Factors Be Protective Against the Development of Psychosis? Psychiatry Research. 2011;186:28–33. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.05.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guada J, Hoe M, Floyd R, Barbour J, Brekke JS. The Importance of Consumer Perceived Criticism on Clinical Outcomes for Outpatient African Americans with Schizophrenia. Community Mental Health Journal. 2011;47:637–645. doi: 10.1007/s10597-010-9366-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herman KC, Ostrander R, Tucker CM. Do Family Environments and Negative Cognitions of Adolescents With Depressive Symptoms Vary by Ethnic Group? Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2007;21:325–330. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.21.2.325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hooley JM, Parker HA. Measuring Expressed Emotion: An Evaluation of the Shortcuts. Journal of Family Psychology. 2006;20:386–396. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.20.3.386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hooley JM, Teasdale JD. Predictors of Relapse in Unipolar Depressives: Expressed Emotion, Marital Distress, and Perceived Criticism. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1989;98:229–235. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.98.3.229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ivanović M, Vuletić Z, Bebbington P. Expressed Emotion in the Families of Patients with Schizophrenia and Its Influence on the Course of Illness. Social Psychiatry Epidemiology. 1994;29:61–65. doi: 10.1007/BF00805622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King S, Dixon MJ. Expressed Emotion, Family Dynamics and Symptom Severity in a Predictive Model of Social Adjustment for Schizophrenic Young Adults. Schizophrenia Research. 1995;14:121–132. doi: 10.1016/0920-9964(94)00016-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kohler CG, Walker JB, Martin EA, Healey KM, Moberg PJ. Facial Emotion Perception in Schizophrenia: A Meta-Analytic Review. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2010;36:1009–1019. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kopelowicz A, Zarate R, González V, López SR, Ortega P, Obregon N, Mintz J. Evaluation of Expressed Emotion in Schizophrenia: A Comparison of Caucasians and Mexican-Americans. Schizophrenia Research. 2002;55:179–186. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(01)00193-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lebell MB, Marder SR, Mintz J, Mintz LI, Tompson M, Wirshing W, Johnston-Cronk K, et al. Patients’ Perceptions of Family Emotional Climate and Outcome in Schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1993;162:751–754. doi: 10.1192/bjp.162.6.751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linszen DH, Dingemans PM, Nugter MA, Van der Does JW, Scholte WF, Lenior MA. Patient Attributes and Expressed Emotion As Risk Factors for Psychotic Relapse. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 1997;23:119–130. doi: 10.1093/schbul/23.1.119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- López SR, Nelson KA, Snyder KS, Mintz J. Attributions and Affective Reactions of Family Members and Course of Schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1999;108:307–314. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.108.2.307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- López SR, Polo AJ, Karno M, Hipke KN, Jenkins JH, Vaughn C, Snyder KS. Ethnicity, Expressed Emotion, Attributions, and Course of Schizophrenia: Family Warmth Matters. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2004;113:428–439. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.3.428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukoff D, Nuechterlein KH, Ventura J. Appendix A: Manual for Expanded Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS) Schizophrenia Bulletin. 1986;12:594–602. [Google Scholar]
- McClelland GH, Judd CM. Statistical Difficulties of Detecting Interactions and Moderator Effects. Psychological Bulletin. 1993;114:376–390. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.114.2.376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McEachern AG, Kenny MC. A Comparison of Family Environment Characteristic Among White (Non-Hispanic), Hispanic and African Caribbean Groups. Journal of Multicultural Counseling and Development. 2002;30:40–59. [Google Scholar]
- Medina-Pradas C, Navarro JB, López SR, Grau A, Obiols JE. Dyadic View of Expressed Emotion, Stress, and Eating Disorder Psychopathology. Appetite. 2011;57:743–748. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.08.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Medina-Pradas C, Navarro JB, Pousa E, Montero MI, Obiols JE. Expressed and Perceived Criticism, Family Warmth, and Symptoms in Schizophrenia. Spanish Journal of Psychology. 2013;16:1–8. doi: 10.1017/sjp.2013.25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moos RH, Moos B. Manual for the Family Environment Scale. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1981. [Google Scholar]
- O’Brien MP, Gordon JL, Bearden CE, López SR, Kopelowicz A, Cannon TD. Positive Family Environment Predicts Improvement in Symptoms and Social Functioning Among Adolescents at Imminent Risk for Onset of Psychosis. Schizophrenia Research. 2006;81:269–275. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Onwumere J, Kuipers E, Bebbington P, Dunn G, Freeman D, Fowler D, Garety P. Patient Perceptions of Caregiver Criticism in Psychosis: Links with Patient and Caregiver Functioning. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2009;197:85–91. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181960e57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Overall JE, Gorham DR. The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale. Psychological Reports. 1962;10:799–812. [Google Scholar]
- Phillips MR, West CL, Shen Q, Zheng Y. Comparison of Schizophrenic Patients’ Families and Normal Families in China, Using Chinese Versions of FACES-II and the Family Environment Scales. Family Process. 1998;37:95–106. doi: 10.1111/j.1545-5300.1998.00095.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poole J, Corwin F, Vinogradov S. Cognitive and Clinical Correlates of Inaccurate Affect Recognition in Schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. 1997;24:121. [Google Scholar]
- Rosenfarb IS, Bellack AS, Aziz N. Family Interactions and the Course of Schizophrenia in African American and White Patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2006;115:112–120. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.115.1.112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scazufca M, Kuipers E. Perception of Negative Emotions in Close Relatives By Patients with Schizophrenia. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2001;40:167–175. doi: 10.1348/014466501163616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tompson MC, Goldstein MJ, Lebell MB, Mintz LI, Marder SR, Mintz J. Schizophrenic Patients’ Perceptions of Their Relatives’ Attitudes. Psychiatry Research. 1995;57:155–167. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(95)02598-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Humbeeck G, Van Audenhove C, Storms G, De Hert M, Pieters G, Vertommen H, Peuskens J, Heyrman J. Expressed Emotion in the Client-Professional Dyad: A Comparison of Three Expressed Emotion Instruments. European Journal of Psychological Assessment. 2004;20:237–246. [Google Scholar]
- Ventura J, Liberman RP, Green MF, Shaner A, Mintz J. Training and Quality Assurance with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-I/P) Psychiatry Research. 1998;79:163–173. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(98)00038-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weisman de Mamani A, Suro G. The effect of a culturally informed therapy on self-conscious emotions and burden in caregivers of patients with schizophrenia: A randomized clinical trial. Psychotherapy. 2015;53:57–67. doi: 10.1037/pst0000038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weisman de Mamani A, Weintraub MJ, Gurak K, Maura J. A Randomized Clinical Trial to Test the Efficacy of a Family-Focused, Culturally-Informed Therapy for Schizophrenia. Journal of Family Psychology. 2014;6:800–810. doi: 10.1037/fam0000021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weisman AG, Rosales G, Kymalainen J, Armesto J. Ethnicity, Expressed Emotion, and Schizophrenia Patients’ Perceptions of Their Family Members’ Criticism. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2006;194:644–649. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000235504.39284.f1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weisman A. Integrating Culturally Based Approaches with Existing Interventions for Hispanic/Latino Families Coping with Schizophrenia. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training. 2005;42:178–197. [Google Scholar]
- Weisman A, Rosales G, Kymalainen J, Armesto J. Ethnicity, Family Cohesion, Religiosity and General Emotional Distress in Patients with Schizophrenia and Their Relatives. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2005;193:359–368. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000165087.20440.d1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weisman AG, López SR. Family Values, Religiosity and Emotional Reactions to Schizophrenia in Mexican and Anglo-Americans Cultures. Family Process. 1996;35:227–237. doi: 10.1111/j.1545-5300.1996.00227.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]