Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Crim Justice. 2016 Feb 12;45:26–31. doi: 10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2016.02.005

Childhood and Adolescent Risk and Protective Factors for Violence in Adulthood

Eric F Dubow a,b, L Rowell Huesmann a, Paul Boxer a,c, Cathy Smith a
PMCID: PMC4979576  NIHMSID: NIHMS760194  PMID: 27524843

Abstract

Purpose

We use data from a community sample followed from ages 8 to 48. We focus on the main and risk-buffering effects of childhood and adolescent protective factors for predicting adulthood violence (official records and self reports).

Method

Males (N=436) from the Columbia County Longitudinal Study participated. The youth, their parents, and peers were first interviewed when the youth were age 8; the youth were later interviewed at ages 19, 30, and 48.

Results

Risk factors for adulthood violence included higher aggression and lower family socioeconomic status at ages 8 and 19. Protective factors included anxiety about behaving aggressively (ages 8 and 19), popularity (ages 8 and 19), family church attendance (age 8), lower negative family interactions (age 8), and higher educational aspirations (age 19). For youth with at least one risk factor, the sum of adolescent—but not childhood--protective factors reduced the likelihood of adulthood violence. The most critical adolescent risk-buffering protective factors were anxiety about behaving aggressively and educational aspirations.

Conclusions

Aggression and low family SES, even by age 8, place youth at risk for adulthood violence. Interventions to strengthen critical protective factors must continue into late adolescence to reduce the likelihood of adulthood violence among at-risk youth.

Keywords: adulthood violence, longitudinal, risk factors, protective factors


We use data from a prospective study of a community sample of males followed from ages 8 to 48. We examine childhood (age 8) and adolescent (age 19) risk and protective factors for adulthood violence. These predictors included individual and family variables posited by developmental-contextual models of the development of aggression, antisocial behavior, violence, and life-course offending (e.g., Catalano & Hawkins, 1996; Farrington & Ttofi, in press; Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990; Huesmann, 1998; Patterson, 1982; Thornberry, 2005).

The risk and resilience literature provides a framework for developmental-contextual models to specify the risk and protective resources that constitute an individual's developmental environment. Risk factors predict subsequent problems, whereas protective factors predict a decrease in problems. In recent literature on youth violence, there is debate about subtypes of protective effects (see Losel & Farrington, 2012; Farrington, Ttofi, & Loeber, 2014). For example, “direct protective effects” have main effects", predicting lower levels of problem behavior, and “buffering” protective effects predict lower levels of problem behavior more strongly for the youth with the risk factor than for those without it (for even further distinctions among types of protective effects, see Farrington et al., 2014 and Luthar, Cicchetti, & Becker, 2000). Empirical studies have identified risk and protective factors for youth violence across multiple contexts (e.g., family, peer group, neighborhood, school) and at the individual level (e.g., APA, 2013; Boxer, Huesmann, Dubow, et al., 2013; Dahlberg & Krug, 2002; Dubow & Luster, 1990; Farrington et al., 2014; Guerra et al., 2003; Herrenkohl, Tajima, Whitney, & Huang, 2005; Huesmann, Eron, & Dubow, 2002; Losel & Farrington, 2012; Rutter, 1990; Sampson, Morenoff, & Raudenbush, 2005; Smith & Farrington, 2003; Stouthamer-Loeber, Loeber, Wei, Farrington & Wikstrom, 2002; Tolan, Gorman-Smith, & Henry, 2003; Werner & Smith, 1992).

We focus on risk and protective factors at the individual and family levels. Based on studies cited above, at the individual level, the major risk factors for later aggressive and violent behavior are a previous history of aggression, impulsivity, and other externalizing problems, whereas individual protective factors include sociability, above average intelligence/academic achievement, positive academic motivation, positive future orientation, anti-aggressive cognitions, and religiosity. At the family level, risk factors include low socioeconomic status, poor parenting and having aggressive or antisocial parents, whereas the most significant protective factor is a positive family environment (e.g., low physical punishment, low inter-parental aggression, high parental monitoring). No one risk or protective factor by itself explains more than a small portion of individual differences in violent offending, and multiple protective factors from multiple levels appear to buffer the impact of risk factors.

We examine two risk factors (aggression and low family socioeconomic status) in childhood and adolescence for predicting violent offending in adulthood, and whether protective factors at the same developmental periods have direct or risk-buffering effects on adulthood violence. The strengths of studying these issues in the Columbia County Longitudinal Study are: the prospective research design includes interviews at four developmental periods (childhood, late adolescence, early adulthood, middle adulthood); data were collected using multiple methods (e.g., parent reports, peer nominations, self-reports, official criminal records); and the participants were the entire population of third grade males in a socioeconomically heterogeneous county in the U.S.

Method

Participants and Procedures

The Columbia County Longitudinal Study was initiated by Eron, Walder, and Lefkowitz (1971) as a longitudinal study of 436 boys and 420 girls who were in the third grade in 1960 (modal age = 8) in Columbia County, New York, a semi-rural area.1 All 38 public and private third-grade classrooms in the county participated. Over 90% of the sample was Caucasian. In this first wave, 85% of the participants‘ mothers and 71% of their fathers also were interviewed. The participants came from a broad range of socioeconomic backgrounds (based on Warner, Meeker, and Eells‘s (1960) scale), and displayed a wide range of intelligence. We report only on the males in this paper. In 1970–1972, 211 males were re-interviewed. They had a modal age of 19 years and had completed 12.6 years of education on average. In 1981–1982, 198 of the males were re-interviewed (modal age 30). During 1999–2002, 268 males (61% of the original sample) were re-interviewed; their mean age was 48.46 years old; their average education level was between some college and a college degree; their average occupational attainment was middle-class status (based on Stevens and Hoisington’s (1987) occupational prestige scores); and 69% of the original participants were living with their spouses.

Data collection procedures have been reported elsewhere (e.g., Dubow, Boxer, & Huesmann, 2009; Eron et al., 1971; Lefkowitz et al.,1977; Huesmann, Dubow, & Boxer, 2009; Huesmann, Eron, & Dubow, 2002; Huesmann et al., 1984). Age 8 data were derived from classroom-based peer-nominations and parent interviews. At age 19, participants were administered self-report measures and peer nominations were again obtained in individual interviews at a field office. At ages 30 and 48, interviews were conducted by computer in a field office and by mail/telephone for participants who could not come to the office.

Attrition Information

At age 48, we interviewed 61% (n = 268) of the original sample of males. A comparison of means on age 8 scores revealed no significant difference in father‘s occupational status; however, compared to participants who were re-interviewed at age 48, participants who were not re-interviewed had higher aggression, lower popularity, and lower IQ at age 8. These effect sizes ranged from r = .14 to r = .19. However, the plots of the distributions for these age 8 variables revealed that many of the high aggressive participants were re-sampled and there was no substantial restriction of range.

Measures

Adulthood Violence

Self-report of violence

Severe physical aggression was assessed at ages 30 and 48 through participants‘ self-reports of how often in the last year they engaged in each of four behaviors (e.g., choked someone, slapped or kicked someone, punched or beaten someone, knifed or shot at someone or threatened to do it; 1 = never to 4 = a lot (α = .66); because scores on some items may not be correlated with scores on other items, we view this measure as a reliable hierarchical index rather than a scale; see Streiner, 2003). At age 30, 193 participants completed the measure; 27% had non-zero scores. At age 48, 243 completed the measure; 25% had non-zero scores. We averaged the scores across the two time periods, and then dichotomized them into the lower 75% and upper 25% of severe physical aggression.

Official criminal arrest records for violent offenses

During the 2000 data collection, criminal history data were obtained from the New York State Criminal Justice Archive; driver‘s licence records were obtained from the NY State Department of Motor Vehicles. The criminal records include all arrests reported to the state agency since the participants were age 18. We coded for arrests corresponding to the two adulthood time points related to the interview dates: 1973–1982 (modal ages 21 to 30, early adulthood); and 1983–2000 (modal ages 31 to 48, middle adulthood). The New York State Criminal Justice Archive only contains records for persons who had been arrested in New York state; we do not have data on arrests that may have occurred in other states. We assessed whether each participant was a New York State resident during each of the crime reporting periods. We counted them as a resident: a) if they had a criminal record during the period; b) if they had a New York driver‘s license during the period or a subsequent period; or c) if we found that they had a home address in NY state during the period. We found that 319 were residents during early adulthood, 82 of them (25.7%) were official offenders, and 21 (6.6%) were arrested for a violent offense (one involving physical force against a person); 308 were residents during middle adulthood, 48 of them (15.6%) were official offenders, and 11 (3.6%) were arrested for a violent offense. Overall, 26 males had been arrested for a violent offense during adulthood.

Composite adulthood violence classification

We created a composite adulthood violence classification based on whether the participant had ever been arrested in adulthood for a violent offense and/or was in the upper 25% on the severe physical aggression score. Of the 372 males who were interviewed during at least one adult time point and/or who were NY state residents during at least one of these times (so we could compute an adulthood criminal history record for them), 276 (74%) were classified as non-violent and 92 (26%) were classified as violent.2

Risk Factors at Ages 8 and 19

Aggression

At age 8, aggression was measured using a peer nomination procedure (Eron et al., 1971) comprised of 10 items covering physical (e.g., “Who pushes and shoves other children?”), verbal (e.g., “Who says mean things?”), acquisitive (e.g., “Who takes other children‘s things without asking?”), and indirect (e.g., “Who makes up stories and lies to get other children into trouble?”) acts. The score represents the proportion of times the child was nominated by classmates on the ten items. At age 19, because participants had left high school, they first were presented with a list of those original participants who had attended school with them at age 8, and were asked to identify those whom they currently knew “well enough to answer some questions about” (9 of the original items were included). Each individual‘s score was computed as the number of times he/she was nominated on the nine questions divided by the number of times he or she could have been nominated (i.e., the number of participants who now knew the individual well). At each age, we also dichotomized scores into low risk (lower 75%) and high risk (upper 25%).

Family socioeconomic status (SES)

We created a composite SES variable of three measures at age 8: (a) Father‘s occupational level (coded on a 10-point scale of 0 = laborers to 9 = professionals); (b) Parents‘ educational level (ranging from 1 = under 7 years to 7 = graduate/professional training); and (c) Value of family housing (ranging from 1 = inexpensive rental to 4 = expensive owned. The composite score was derived through latent variable measurement modeling (Dubow, Boxer, & Huesmann, 2008). Individual scores were standardized, multiplied by factor weights observed in the measurement model, and summed to create the family background composite (high scores reflected higher SES). We also dichotomized scores into high risk (lower 25%) and low risk (upper 75%). At age 19, participants reported their father‘s occupation, which was coded using Warner et al.‘s 7-point scale (1 = laborers to 7 = professionals); in addition to the continuous score, we dichotomized scores into high risk (lower 25%) and low risk (upper 75%).

Protective Factors at Ages 8 and 19

For each of the following measures, we used continuous scores, and we also dichotomized scores so that the upper 25% represents high levels of the protective factor compared to the lower 75% of the sample.

Individual-level factors

A) Using peer nominations at both ages, popularity represents the proportion of times the participant was nominated by his or her classmates on two popularity items -- "Who would you like to have as a best friend?" and "Who would you like to sit next to in class?" B) Anxiety about behaving aggressively. At ages 8 and 19, Eron et al.‘s (1971) “anxiety over aggression” measure was used. Two peer-rated items measured this construct: “Who says ‘excuse me’ even when they have done nothing wrong?” and “Who will never fight even when picked on?” This measure has shown good validity (e.g., inverse correlations with aggression and positive correlations with popularity (Eron & Huesmann, 1984; Huesmann & Eron, 1986). C) Intellectual achievement/aspirations. At age 8, the child’s IQ was assessed with the California Short-Form Test of Mental Maturity (Sullivan, Clark, & Tiegs, 1957). At age 19, after most participants had already graduated from high school, we assessed educational aspirations using participants’ responses to the item, “What is the greatest amount of education you expect to have during your life?” along a 6-point scale (1 = less than high school to 6 = graduate education) (Lefkowitz et al., 1977). D) Depression was measured only at age 19, using the participant’s score on the MMPI Depression subscale (Hathaway & McKinley, 1940). The lower 25% of the distribution was designated as having a protective factor.

Contextual-level factors

A) Quality of family interaction at age 8 (Dubow et al., 2008) was assessed by a composite score of four measures: 1) Parental rejection is the sum of scores on 10 items about how "unsatisfied" the parent is with the child, e.g., "Are you satisfied with your child’s manners? Does your child read as well as he/she should?"; 2) Parents’ endorsement of hitting the child as a form of punishment was the sum of parents’ endorsement of physical punishment in response to two vignettes depicting child transgressions; 3) Parental disharmony measures the amount and seriousness of disputes between the parents. It is the sum of 10 items of the form, "Do you or your spouse ever leave the house during an argument? ; and 4) Parental aggression. Eron et al. (1971) adapted 4 items from the Walters and Zak (1959) aggression measure that assesses the parent’s tendency to become angry in variety of situations. The composite score was derived through latent variable measurement modeling. Individual scores were standardized, multiplied by factor weights observed in the measurement model, and then summed to create the negative family interaction composite (lowest 25% of the sample represents a protective factor). B) Religiosity at ages 8 and 19. Frequency of church attendance (0= never, 1= few times/year, 2= about 1/month, 3= few times/month, 4= once or more/week) was measured at age 8 via parent report and at age 19 via self-report.

Results

Table 1 includes data for men who were interviewed either at age 30 or 48 and/or were New York residents during either time period, and compares those classified as non-violent vs. violent on the continuous risk and protective factors from childhood and adolescence. T-tests showed that both risk factors at ages 8 and 19 discriminated the non-violent from the violent men: violent men had higher earlier levels of aggression and lower earlier levels of family socioeconomic status. In addition, four age 8 protective factors distinguished between the violent and non-violent men: non-violent men had higher aggression anxiety, were marginally more popular, had parents who attended church more frequently, and had marginally fewer negative family interactions. Three age 19 protective factors distinguished between the violent and non-violent men: non-violent men had higher aggression anxiety, were more popular, and had higher levels of educational aspirations.

Table 1.

Predicting the Adult Violence Classification of Men Separately from their Scores on the Assessed Risk and Protective Factors in Childhood and Adolescence

Means
Logistic Regression
Violent (n = 96) Non-violent (n = 276) t-value AOR p
Childhood risk factors
 Family socioeconomic status −0.18 0.66 5.78*** 0.42 <.001
 Peer-nominated aggression .180 .140 2.23* 1.24 .08
Logistic χ 2 (2) = 36.7, p <.001
Childhood protective factors
 Family’s religious attendance 2.00 2.36 2.03* 0.73 .02
 Quality of family interaction 0.09 0.01 1.94 1.27 .08
 IQ 96.31 99.05 1.57 ns
 Peer-nominated popularity .202 .234 1.68 ns
 Peer-nominated aggression anxiety .13 .150 2.36* ns
Logisticχ 2 (2) = 9.4, p < .01
Adolescent risk factors
 Family socioeconomic status 3.28 4.42 4.34*** 0.52 .001
 Peer-nominated aggression 0.14 0.06 3.99*** 2.04 <.001
Logisticχ2 (2) = 34.6, p < .001
Adolescent protective factors
 Religious attendance 2.02 2.09 0.26 ns
 Depression 57.17 56.87 0.16 ns
 Educational aspirations 4.09 4.90 3.20*** 0.66 .01
 Peer-nominated popularity 0.13 0.17 2.33* ns
 Peer-nominated aggression anxiety 0.03 0.07 3.77*** .47 .01

Logisticχ 2 (2) = 19.8, p < .001

Notes: T tests were computed for each predictor variable. Ns range from 187 (educational aspirations) to 372 (peer-nominated measures from childhood) across analyses. The logistic regressions were computed by forward stepwise entry with p<=.10 required for entry. The ns for each respective analysis were: childhood risk (nV=78, nNV=240), childhood protective (nV =74, nNV =232) adolescent risk (nV =47, nNV =142), and adolescent protective (nV =47, nNV =140). AOR = adjusted odds ratio. V = violent. NV = non-violent.

p < .10.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

***

p < .001.

Next, we computed forward stepwise logistic regressions (p<.10 required for entry) to explore the independent effects of the predictors. We ran four analyses with the standardized risk or protective factors from each time period separately. Overall, the models predicted criminal violence significantly (χ2(2) ranged from 9.39 to 36.74, p’s<.01; Hosmer-Lemeshow tests were non-significant). Lower SES and higher aggression at age 8 independently increased the risk of violence in adulthood, but the effect was marginal for aggression (AOR=2.37, p<.001 for SES; AOR=1.24, p<.10 for aggression). In adolescence, lower SES and higher aggression independently increased the risk of violence in adulthood (AOR=1.93, p=.001 for SES; AOR=2.04, p<.001 for aggression). Thus, having at least one risk factor at either earlier time point at least marginally increased the risk of violence during adulthood. Exploring age 8 protective factors, having parents who attended church more often and experiencing fewer negative family interactions reduced the risk of violence during adulthood, (AOR=.73, p<.05 for church attendance; AOR=.79, p<.10 for family interactions) though the effect was marginal for family interactions. At age 19, higher educational aspirations and aggression anxiety independently reduced the risk of violence in adulthood (AOR=.66, p=.01 for educational aspirations; AOR=.47, p=.01 for aggression anxiety).

Next, we explored interactive effects of cumulative childhood and adolescent risk and protective factors on adulthood violent offending. First, we calculated cumulative risk and protective factor sum scores based on the risk and protective factor variables that were significantly related to adulthood violence from the t-test results. For the risk factor sum variable, we summed the four dichotomized risk scores (family SES and peer-nominated aggression in both childhood and adolescence; 1 = upper quartile; 0 = lower 75%), so the sum risk score could range from zero to 4.3 We then dichotomized the risk factor sum score to differentiate between those participants who had no risk factors (n=87) and those who had at least one risk factor (n=124). Because the measured protective factors varied in childhood and adolescence, we summed the dichotomized protective factors separately for each time point (four variables in childhood, three in adolescence), and then trichotomized the sum of the protective factors at ages 8 and 19 into groups of participants with zero, one, and two or more protective factors (this trichotomization allowed for the maximum number of groups with at least five individuals in each cell when the protective factors were crossed with the two risk groups).

Using these risk and protective sum variables, we computed two forward stepwise logistic regressions (p<.10 required for entry) to predict adulthood violent offending-- one for age 8 protective factors and one for age 19 protective factors. As shown in Table 2, both logistic regressions predicted criminal violence significantly and fit the data well (Hosmer-Lemeshow tests were non-significant). Protective factors at age 19, but not at age 8, interacted with risk group to affect the prediction of adult violence (AOR=.306, p<.001).

Table 2.

Predicting the Adult Violence Classification of Men from their Combined Child and Adolescent Risk Group and their Childhood or Adolescent Protective Group Classification

Means
Logistic Regression
Violent (n = 96) Non-viol (n = 276) t-value AOR p
Prediction with childhood protective factors
 Combined risk factors 0.83 0.48 5.11*** 5.36*** .001
 Combined protective factors 0.63 0.94 3.25*** ns
 Interaction of risk and protective factors 0.78 0.35 3.29** ns
Logistic χ2 (1) = 19.6, p < .001
Prediction with adolescent protective factors
 Combined risk factors 0.83 0.48 5.11*** 10.4 .001
 Combined protective factors 0.44 0.93 3.96*** ns
 Interaction of risk and protective factors 0.27 0.41 1.48 0.31 .001
Logistic χ2 (2) = 33.9, p < .001
Percent of adult violent men who were in each Risk/Protective group
Combined child and adolescent risk group Childhood (age 8) protective group (Sum of childhood protective factors) Adolescent (age 19) protective group (Sum of adolescent protective factors)
0 1 2 or more χ2(2) 0 1 2 or more χ2(2)
No risk factors (n=87) 32 11 0 15.05** 8 13 8 0.56
At least one risk factor (n=124) 31 35 48 2.17 52 28 6 12.93**

Notes: Ns for t-tests range from 191 (combined risk factors, adolescent protective factors, interaction terms) to 372 (childhood protective factors). The logistic regressions were computed by forward stepwise entry with p<=.10 required for entry. AOR = adjusted odds ratio. V = violent. NV = non-violent. Chi squares in the bottom panel are tests of the null hypothesis that protective factors have no effect on whether a man ends up in the adult violent or non-violent group for men in the given risk group. N’s ranged from 82 (no risk factors) to 109 (at least one risk factor) across analyses.

p < .10.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

***

p < .001.

The bottom panels in Table 2 show what these interactions mean. As the number of age 8 protective factors increased for at-risk males, the chances of becoming a violent adult did not change significantly (χ2(2)=2.17, p>.10); however, a protective effect was present for males with no risk factors. As the number of age 8 protective factors increased for these males, the chances of becoming a violent adult decreased substantially.4 In contrast, age 19 protective factors significantly reduced the chances of "at risk" males becoming violent adults (χ2(2)=12.93, p<.01), but had no effect on whether "non-at-risk" males grew up to be violent men (χ2(2)=0.56, p>.10). We conclude that the sum of adolescent protective factors serves as buffer against the negative effects of risks, while childhood protective factors are not protective in the presence of risk.

We next examined which specific protective factors (four from childhood, three from adolescence) played a protective role. In the top panel of Table 3, we show that none of the specific age 8 protective factors decreased the chances of becoming a violent adult for those males with at least one risk factor. High age 8 aggression anxiety and age 8 popularity acted as a protective factors for boys with no risk factors but for not for boys with at least one risk factor. In fact, high aggression anxiety at age 8 unexpectedly predicted a greater chance of becoming a violent adult for "at-risk" males. However, the lower panel of Table 3 shows that high aggression anxiety and high educational aspirations at age 19 reduced the chances of becoming a violent adult for males with at least one risk factor (χ2(1)=7.02, p<.01 for aggression anxiety, and χ2(1)=8.65, p<.01 for educational aspirations). However, no protective factors decreased the chances of becoming a violent adult among youth with no risks.

Table 3.

Percent of Adult Violent Men who are in Each Risk/Specific-Protective-Factor Group

Childhood protective factors (age 8)

Popularity Aggression anxiety Family’s religious attendance Quality of family interactions
Combined child and adolescent risk group High Low χ2(1) High Low χ2(1) High Low χ2(1) High Low χ2(1)

No risk factors (n=87) 2 18 5.32* 2 18 5.67* 0 10 2.20 0 10 2.09

At least one risk factor (n=124) 44 35 0.75 58 32 4.45* 41 32 0.65 40 33 0.40
Adolescent protective factors (age 19)

Popularity Aggression anxiety Educational aspirations
Combined child and adolescent risk group High Low χ2(1) High Low χ2(1) High Low χ2(1)

No risk factors (n=87) 8 11 0.18 4 13 1.51 14 7 1.01

At least one risk factor (n=124) 29 39 0.75 13 43 7.02** 12 44 8.65**

Notes: Each Chi-square is a test of the null hypothesis that a specific protective factor has no effect on whether men in the given risk group end up in the group of violent adult men. For childhood protective factors, n’s ranged from 68 (no risk factors X quality of family interactions) to 109 (at least one risk factor X popularity and aggression anxiety). For adolescent protective factors, n’s ranged from 81 (no risk factors X educational aspirations) to 109 (at least one risk factor X popularity and aggression anxiety).

p < .10.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

***

p < .001.

In summary, specific protective factors during adolescence, but not childhood, reduced the likelihood of adulthood violence among participants with at least one risk factor, while specific protective factors during childhood, but not adolescence, reduced the likelihood of adulthood violence among participants with no risk factors

Discussion

Like the other studies in this special issue, we examined two childhood risk factors consistently shown to predict subsequent violence and criminality among youth and young adults: aggression and low family socioeconomic status (e.g., APA, 2013; Herrenkohl et al., 2012; Loeber & Farrington, 1998). Indeed, both at ages 8 and 19, these risk factors independently predicted violence in adulthood. Also, various age 8 and 19 individual and family variables reduced the risk of adulthood violence. Following Losel and Farrington (2012), we found direct independent protective effects (main effects) at age 8 for two family variables (attending church with one’s family and experiencing fewer negative family interactions); at age 19, two individual-level variables (higher levels of educational aspirations and aggression anxiety) independently reduced the risk for adulthood violence. These results for predicting violence through age 48 extend previous studies of risk and direct protective effects of similar variables on violence in late adolescence/early adulthood (see the special issue of the American Journal of Preventive Medicine, volume 43).

We note some minor differences between the results reported here and those in our previous publications. For example, Huesmann et al. (2002) found that age 8 peer-nominated aggression was the best predictor of violent criminal arrests at age 30, whereas an index of family SES was only a marginally significant predictor. Here, we found that a broader index of family SES was a significant predictor and aggression a marginally significant predictor of a combined measure of adulthood violence by age 48 (criminal arrests and self reports). So, the differences appear to be due to the specific measures of the predictors as well as the measures of adulthood violence.

Rutter (1990) and Losel and Farrington (2012) described risk-buffering protective factors as those factors that reduce the likelihood of negative outcomes in the presence of the risk factor but have less of an impact when the risk factor is not present. We did not find that the number of protective factors in childhood reduced adulthood violence for the at-risk youth; rather, the number of childhood protective factors reduced the likelihood of violence among youth with no risks. However, adolescent protective factors did play a risk-buffering role: for adolescents with at least one risk factor, 52% of those with zero protective factors were classified as violent in adulthood; 28 % were so classified if they had 1 protective factor; and only 6% were classified as violent if they had 2 or more protective factors. The number of protective factors had no effect on adulthood violence for adolescents who had no risk factors. Similar results have been reported by Stattin, Romelsjo, and Stenbacka (1997) who studied over 7,000 Swedish men during late adolescence (ages 18–20) and examined their criminal records between ages 18 and 36. Among men with high behavioral risks (e.g., antisocial behavior, academic problems) in late adolescence, individual-level variables (e.g., emotional maturity, intelligence) reduced the likelihood of criminality, but these resources did not play a protective role for those at low behavioral risk. Using data from the Cambridge Study in Delinquent Development, Farrington and Ttofi (2012) found that in childhood, good family supervision reduced the effect of early problem behaviors on criminal convictions through age 50, and good child-rearing and small family size reduced the effect of poor housing on adult convictions. Although we did not find risk-buffering effects of childhood protective factors, our findings for risk-buffering effects of age 19 educational aspirations and aggression anxiety reflect that bonding to society and self-regulation during late adolescence are critical for prosocial attachments that reduce the likelihood of violence in the presence of earlier risk. This is consistent with a variety of developmental and life-course models of offending (see Farrington & Ttofi, 2014).

Findings that late adolescent protective factors play a significant role in reducing risk for adulthood violence are important in the design of intervention programs. Although intervention programs to reduce delinquency, conduct problems, and violence have been targeted at children successfully (see APA, 2013; Farrington, in press), our findings suggest the need to continue to provide follow-up contact into adolescence to ensure that the success in reducing risks and enhancing protective factors continues into emerging adulthood. In cases where there is a high number of risk factors for violence and an absence of protective factors, these adolescents are candidates for empirically validated, intensive, home-based intervention approaches such as multisystemic therapy and functional family therapy, which have been shown to reduce criminal behavior and substance use (see Boxer & Goldstein, 2012).

Highlights.

  • Data come from a community sample followed from ages 8 to 48

  • Childhood aggression and low family SES placed youth at risk for adulthood violence

  • Adolescent protective factors reduced the likelihood of adulthood violence

Acknowledgments

This research has been supported by funding from the Columbia County Tuberculosis and Health Association and the Hudson (NY) Lions Club (1960 wave); the National Institute of Mental Health (1960, 1970 and 1981 waves); and the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (2000 wave; R01 HD36056).

The authors wish to acknowledge the pioneering contributions of Leonard Eron, Monroe Lefkowitz, and Leopold Walder to the Columbia County Longitudinal Study.

Footnotes

1

We chose to include males only in this report because only two females were ever arrested for a violent offense, and females were significantly less likely to report physical violence than males.

2

We chose to include any male for whom at least one of the following adulthood data points was available: self-report of severe physical aggression at age 30, self-report of aggression at age 48, official arrest records in early adulthood, or official arrest records in middle adulthood. We realize that the risk of opting against list-wise deletion is that we might misclassify potential violent adults as non-violent. This could occur, for example, if a non-interviewed participant was a NY state resident for whom we had an official record but did not have a violent arrest, yet had he been interviewed, would have self-reported an incident of violence.

3

The risk sum variable required the participant to have been interviewed at each time period, so we did not introduce bias for those only interviewed at age 8, and we were left with a sample of 211 males.

4

Note that the risk x age 8 protective factor interaction in the previous logistic regression was not significant. That result was obtained using a forward stepwise entry procedure (p<.10 for entry). A simultaneous entry procedure yielded a significant risk x age 8 protective factor interaction effect.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Contributor Information

Eric F. Dubow, Email: edubow@bgsu.edu.

L. Rowell Huesmann, Email: huesmann@umich.edu.

Paul Boxer, Email: pboxer@psychology.rutgers.edu.

Cathy Smith, Email: smithcat@umich.edu.

References

  1. American Psychological Association. Gun violence: Prediction, prevention and policy. 2013 Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/pubs/ifo/reports/gun-violence-prevention.aspx.
  2. Boxer P, Goldstein SE. Treating juvenile offenders: Best practices and emerging critical issues. In: Grigorenko E, editor. Handbook of Juvenile Forensic Psychology and Psychiatry. Springer; US: 2012. pp. 323–340. [Google Scholar]
  3. Boxer P, Huesmann LR, Dubow EF, Landau SF, Gvirsman SD, Shikaki K, Ginges J. Exposure to violence across the social ecosystem and the development of aggression: A test of ecological theory in the Israeli-Palestinian conflict. Child Development. 2013;84:163–177. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01848.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Catalano RF, Hawkins JD. The social development model: A theory of antisocial behavior. In: Hawkins JD, editor. Delinquency and Crime: Current Theories. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1996. pp. 149–197. [Google Scholar]
  5. Dahlberg LL, Krug EG. Violence: A global public health problem. In: Krug EG, Dahlberg LL, Mercy JA, Zwi AB, Lozano R, editors. World report on violence and health. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2002. pp. 1–21. [Google Scholar]
  6. Dubow EF, Boxer P, Huesmann LR. Childhood and adolescent predictors of early and middle adulthood alcohol use and problem drinking: The Columbia County Longitudinal Study. Addiction. 2008;103:36–47. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02175.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Dubow EF, Boxer P, Huesmann LR. Long-term effects of parents’ education on children’s educational and occupational success: Mediation by family interactions, child aggression, and teenage aspirations. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 2009;55:224–249. doi: 10.1353/mpq.0.0030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Dubow EF, Luster T. Adjustment of children born to teenage mothers: The contribution of risk and protective factors. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1990;52:393–404. [Google Scholar]
  9. Eron LD, Huesmann LR. The relation of prosocial behavior to the development of aggression and psychopathology. Aggressive Behavior. 1984;10:201–211. [Google Scholar]
  10. Eron LD, Walder LO, Lefkowitz MM. The Learning of Aggression in Children. Boston: Little Brown; 1971. [Google Scholar]
  11. Farrington DP. The developmental evidence base prevention. In: Towl G, Crighton D, editors. Forensic psychology. 2. Oxford: Blackwell; in press. [Google Scholar]
  12. Farrington DP, Ttofi MM. Protective and promotive factors in the development of offending. In: Bliessner T, Beelman A, Stemmler M, editors. Antisocial behavior and crime: Contributions of developmental and evaluation research to prevention and intervention. Cambridge, MA: Hogrefe; 2012. pp. 71–88. [Google Scholar]
  13. Farrington DP, Ttofi MM. Developmental and life-course theories of offending. In: Marizot J, Kazemian L, editors. The development of criminal and antisocial behavior: Theoretical foundations and practical implications. NY: Springer; in press. [Google Scholar]
  14. Farrington DP, Ttofi MM, Loeber R. Protective factors against delinquency. 2014 Manuscript submitted for publication. [Google Scholar]
  15. Gottfredson MR, Hirschi T. A General Theory of Crime. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  16. Guerra NG, Huesmann LR, Spindler A. Community violence exposure, social cognition, and aggression among urban elementary school children. Child Development. 2003;74:1561–1576. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00623. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hathaway SR, McKinley JC. The MMPI Manual. New York: Psychological Corporation; 1940. [Google Scholar]
  18. Herrenkohl TI, Tajima EA, Whitney SD, Huang B. Protection against antisocial behavior in children exposed to physically abusive discipline. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2005;36:457–465. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2003.09.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Huesmann LR. The role of social information processing and cognitive schema in the acquisition and maintenance of habitual aggressive behavior. In: Geen RG, Donnerstein E, editors. Human Aggression: Theories, Research, and Implications for Social Policy. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1998. pp. 73–109. [Google Scholar]
  20. Huesmann LR, Dubow EF, Boxer P. Continuity of childhood, adolescent, and early adulthood aggression as predictors of adult criminality and life outcomes: Implications for the adolescent-limited and life-course-persistent models. Aggressive Behavior. 2009;35:136–149. doi: 10.1002/ab.20300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Huesmann LR, Eron LD, editors. Television and the Aggressive Child: A Cross-National Comparison. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1986. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  22. Huesmann LR, Eron LD, Dubow EF. Childhood predictors of adult criminality: Are all risk factors reflected in childhood aggressiveness? Criminal Behavior and Mental Health. 2002;12:185–208. doi: 10.1002/cbm.496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Huesmann LR, Eron LD, Lefkowitz MM, Walder LO. The stability of aggression over time and generations. Developmental Psychology. 1984;20:1120–1134. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.20.6.1120. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  24. Lefkowitz MM, Eron LD, Walder LO, Huesmann LR. Growing Up to Be Violent: A Longitudinal Study of the Development of Aggression. New York: Pergamon; 1977. [Google Scholar]
  25. Loeber R, Farrington DP. Never too early, never too late: Risk factors and successful interventions for serious and violent juvenile offenders. Studies on Crime and Crime Prevention. 1998;7:7–30. [Google Scholar]
  26. Losel F, Farrington DP. Direct protective and buffering protective factors in the development of youth violence. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2012;43(2S1):S8–S23. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2012.04.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Luthar SS, Cicchetti D, Becker B. The construct of resilience: A critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Development. 2000;71:543–562. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Patterson GR. A Social Learning Approach. Coercive Family Processes. Vol. 3. Eugene, OR: Castalia; 1982. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Rutter M. Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanisms. In: Rolf J, Masten AS, Cicchetti D, Nuechterlein KH, Weintraub S, editors. Risk and protective factors in the development of psychopathology. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1990. pp. 181–214. [Google Scholar]
  30. Sampson RJ, Morenoff JD, Raudenbush S. Social anatomy of racial and ethnic disparities in violence. American Journal of Public Health. 2005;95:224–232. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2004.037705. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Smith CA, Farrington DP. Continuities in antisocial behavior and parenting across three generations. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2003;44:1–18. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00216.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Stattin H, Romelsjö A, Stenbacka M. Personal resources as modifiers of the risk of future criminality; an analysis of protective factors in relation to 18-year-old boys. British Journal of Criminology. 1997;37:198–223. [Google Scholar]
  33. Stevens G, Hoisington E. Occupational prestige and the 1980 U.S. labor force. Social Science Research. 1987;6:74–105. doi: 10.1016/0049-089X(87)90019-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  34. Stouthamer-Loeber M, Loeber R, Wei E, Farrington DP, Wikstrom PH. Risk and promotive effects in the explanation of persistent serious delinquency in boys. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2002;70:111–123. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.70.1.111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Streiner DL. Being inconsistent about consistency: When coefficient alpha does and doesn’t matter. Journal of Personality Assessment. 2003;80:217–222. doi: 10.1207/S15327752JPA8003_01. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Sullivan ET, Clark WW, Tiegs EW. California Short Form Test of Mental Maturity. Los Angeles: California Test Bureau; 1957. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  37. Thornberry TP. Explaining multiple patterns of offending across the life course and across generations. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science. 2005 Nov;602:156–195. doi: 10.1177/0002716205280641. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  38. Tolan PH, Gorman-Smith D, Henry DB. Developmental ecology of urban males’ youth violence. Developmental Psychology. 2003;39:274–291. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.39.2.274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Walters RH, Zak MS. Validation studies of an aggression scale. Journal of Psychology. 1959;47:209–218. [Google Scholar]
  40. Warner WL, Meeker M, Eells K. Social Class in America. New York: Harcourt; 1960. [Google Scholar]
  41. Werner EE, Smith RS. Overcoming the odds: High-risk children from birth to adulthood. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press; 1992. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES