Abstract
Objective
To examine fine motor differences between preschoolers with prenatal serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SRI) exposure and children of mothers with major depression disorder.
Study design
A subset of children (N=40), from a larger study on the effects of prenatal SRI and untreated major depression disorder participated in a kinematic task of visual motor and fine motor functions at ages 4–5 years: SRI exposure (n=15), untreated major depression disorder exposure (n=10), and the control group (n=15). The task was to reach and secure a peg then drop it in a small hole near the start position in the light condition with full visibility or in the glow condition in which a phosphorescent peg glows in the dark. Movement tracking software measured the positioning of the moving hand and fingers.
Results
In the Glow condition, the SRI group had a higher proportion of maximum aperture than the group with major depression disorder and the SRI group was slower than the major depression disorder group to drop the peg into the hole. In the Glow condition, the trajectory of the SRI group was less straight than the major depression disorder group, and the major depression disorder group had a straighter trajectory than the control group.
Conclusion
This study provides evidence that preschool aged children with prenatal SRI exposure have poorer fine motor and visual-motor control compared with prenatal untreated major depression disorder.
Keywords: Efference copy, visual-motor development, kinematics, grasp, reach
Newborns exposed to SRIs demonstrate significantly poorer motor quality compared with nonexposed infants.1–6 At 18 months of age, SRI exposed children showed worse performances in motor quality on the Bayley Behavioral Rating Scale7 and delayed motor milestones up to 40 months.8 Few studies, however, have examined motor development and motor control skills in SRI exposed children beyond 4 years of age. Moreover, the measurement tools used may not be precise enough to detect more nuanced effects of SRIs that could suggest delay in neural processes involved in motor control that may be of clinical importance.
A highly precise methodology to evaluate variation in fine motor movements is kinematics analysis, in which the trajectory of the reaching hand and the finger aperture were monitored by an optoelectronic motor analysis system. Acquisition and execution of fine motor skills requires the coordinated participation of multiple structures in the motor cortex, basal ganglia, cerebellum and spinal cord.9 Fine motor development involves modifications of the cortical representations of the body caused by sensory input including sensorimotor input affecting somatotopin maps, efference copy development, and visual-motor coordination.10 Somatotopin maps refer to adjacent neurons in neural tissue that respond selectively to stimuli presented to adjacent locations on the body. Efference copy is a series of copies of efferent signals from the motor cortex into the sensory cortex and the periphery. Together with internal models, efference copies can enable the brain to predict the effects of an action system.
Although kinematic analysis has been applied to normative preschool age children,11, 12 this study takes advantage of kinematic analysis to evaluate the impact of prenatal exposure to SRI on motor function at later preschool ages (4–5 years). Our hypotheses are that prenatal SRI exposure will result in less efficient reaching (eg, increased movement units [MUs], shorter proportion of time to the end of the first MU, slower peak velocity; longer phase duration; and less straightness). Further, we hypothesize that the Glow condition when the peg is seen but not the moving hand will be less efficient due to efference copy.
Methods
The subsample consisted of 40 children with a mean age of 4.75 years (range= 4.33 to 5.25 years) who participated in a larger study13 on the effects of prenatal SRI exposure on fetal, infant, and child outcomes: 15 exposed to SRI, 10 exposed to untreated mothers with major depression disorder, and 15 controls. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at Women & Infants Hospital of Rhode Island, and written informed consent was obtained from the parents.
Women aged 18 to 40 were recruited in the community and through mental health professionals, and were enrolled between 22 and 34 weeks of pregnancy. Women were included in the SRI exposure group if they were on a SRI for a minimum of four weeks during the pregnancy. Inclusion in the major depression disorder exposure group required a minimum of four consecutive weeks of symptomatic major depression disorder, as defined by the Structural Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Psychiatric Diagnoses.14 The non-exposure control group included women with no psychiatric diagnoses during pregnancy and at least one year prior, no use of psychotropic or other medication during the pregnancy, and mild or no depressive symptoms. The majority of the women taking SRIs were using sertraline (64%); the remainder used citalopram (24%) or escitalopram (4%). Exclusion criteria for the mothers were: use of anticonvulsants or antipsychotics,15, 16 alcohol consumption (>1 drink/week), cigarette and/or illegal drug use during pregnancy, diagnoses of psychotic or bipolar disorders, thyroid conditions, hypertension, and diabetes. Anxiety disorders were allowed in the SRI and major depression disorder groups as well as medications such as zolpidem, diphenhydramine, and other types of antidepressants/anxiolytics. Premature births (<36 weeks of gestation) or infants with known genetic, medical, or physical anomalies were excluded.
Children were instructed to reach and grasp a series of identical cylindrical pegs (5 cm in height × 2.5 cm in diameter) roughly an arm’s length away on a table and then return with the peg toward the start position, dropping the peg in a small opening (Figure; available at www.jpeds.com). The task was performed in two experimental conditions. In the Light condition, both hand and peg were visible. In the Glow condition, the lights were turned off and only the peg, coated with phosphorescent paint, was visible. The small opening for the drop was also coated with phosphorescent paint and visible in both conditions. The children grasped and dropped a total of 15 pegs in each lighting condition. The pegs were separated into sets with 5 pegs each. The Light and Glow conditions alternated every 5 pegs. If trials were unsuccessful or likely to be unscorable, an additional set of 5 pegs of the same condition were conducted.
Two cameras with infrared illuminator rings recorded the procedure from different angles. The children wore a glove on their dominant hand that had reflective markers on the distal aspect of the thumb and index finger, and on the proximal region between these two fingers that we called the “web”. Reflective markers were also placed on the pegs. The reflective markers appear as bright spots on the camera footage, allowing for later digitizing and tracking of movements of each marker using the Vicon Motus ® software (Vicon Motus, Boston, Massachusetts) at 100 frames per second. This provided coordinates for each marker in each frame.
The task had two phases of primary interest: reach and drop. The reach phase began with the web marker’s first movement towards the peg and ended when the child grasped the peg. The drop phase began when the mid-pinch (defined as the center between the index and thumb markers) was exactly at 4 cm from where the child drops the peg. The reach involved control of the hand and entire upper extremity, and was evaluated using the coordinates of the centrally located web marker. The drop involved fine motor control of the fingers and the variables in this phase used the coordinates of the distally located mid-pinch.
The dependent measures derived from the kinematic record of each trial are shown in Table I (available at www.jpeds.com). Definitions are included. The reach measures were chosen to allow comparison with other kinematic studies and to provide a detailed assessment of the development of reaching. The drop measures are unique to this study and provide additional challenge for a young child not only to release the peg into a small opening, but also to recalibrate speed during the approach to the target area. Final analyses tested kinematic parameters by study groups.
Table I.
Dependent measure | Definition |
---|---|
Reach Phase | |
Number of movement units (MU) |
A count of the number of speed peaks in a reach. A speed peak was defined as maximum speed between two minima where the difference between a minimum and the peak must be at least 10 mm/s (Clifton, RK, Rochat P, Robin DJ, Berthier NE. Multimodal perception in the control of infant reaching. 1994. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 29(4):876–886) The peak speed must also be greater than 20% of the maximum hand speed (Konczak J, Dichgans J. The development toward stereotypic arm kinematics during reaching in the first 3 years of life. 1997. Exp Brain Res 117(2):346–354) |
Proportion of time the first movement unit (MU) |
During the reach, the time from movement onset until the end of the first MU. A lower proportion indicates a jerky trajectory at the onset of the reach. |
Peak velocity (cm/s) | The highest velocity of the hand over the course of a reach (Babinsky E; Braddick O, Atkinson J. The effect of removing visual information on reach control in young children. 2012. Exp. Brain Res 222:291–302) |
Proportion of maximum aperture |
During the reach, the child opens his thumb and pointer fingers to prepare for the grasp. Higher proportion indicates a wider aperture than is necessary to grasp the object. |
Drop Phase | |
Phase duration (ms) | The time of the reach from entering the drop phase (4 cm from the hole) to releasing the object |
Number of minor MU | A count of minor speed peaks in the drop phase. A speed peak was defined as a maximum speed between two minima. The criterion of reaching greater than 20% of the maximum phase speed was not applied. |
Straightness | Determined by dividing the distance the hand traveled (hand Path) by the straight-line distance between entering the drop zone and the hand’s position at object release. A ratio of 1.0 would represent a perfectly straight trajectory. Increasing values reflect greater deviation from the straight line (von Hofsten. Development of visually guided reaching: the approach phase. 1979. J Hum Mov Stud 5:160–178) |
Statistical Analyses
Demographic characteristics were assessed using one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) for continuous measures or chi-square for categorical measures. Prior to statistical analysis, the frame-by-frame coordinates were incorporated into trials then into outcome variables within study groups. Generalized estimating equations (GEE) were used to compare the three groups (SRI, major depression disorder, and control) in each of the Light and Glow conditions independently. This extension of the general linear model allows more flexibility in the distributions of the dependent variable and accounts for the correlated nature of the observations within each subject. The general form of the GEE model used for these analyses incorporated a categorical variable for depression group (SRI, major depression disorder, and control) as well as the specification for the repeated nature of the data based upon the subject. Light and Glow conditions were examined in separate GEE models. Furthermore, three covariates that were related to one or more reaching condition were included in the models: mothers’ level of depression at the study visit, child’s mean age (months) at the study visit and gestational age (Table II). In addition, children varied in the number of sets (5 trials each) that were completed in the session. The greater the number of sets, the greater the likelihood of practice effects. Thus, number of sets was included as a covariate as well. Pairwise comparisons of the parameter estimates for any significant group main effects were examined with the Fisher least significant difference. Peak velocity models were adjusted for arm length.
Table II.
Characteristics | SRI N=15 |
major depression disorder N=10 |
control N=15 |
Group |
---|---|---|---|---|
Mother | N (%) | N (%) | N (%) | P value |
Marital status (% married) | 7 (46.7) | 7 (70.0) | 10 (66.7) | .405 |
High school graduate | 13 (100.0) | 9 (90.0) | 14 (93.3) | .540 |
Mean (SE) | Mean (SE) | Mean (SE) | ||
Maternal age at delivery (years) | 30.1 (1.2) | 26.60 (1.4) | 27.7 (1.3) | .148 |
Depression over pregnancy b | 21.7 (3.0) | 24.4 (3.0) | .550 | |
Depression severity at study visit | 16.3 (3.6) c | 11.2 (3.2) c | 6.2 (1.2) | .035 |
Index of social position at study visit | 39.9 (3.9) | 32.8 (3.3) | 44.2 (4.1) | .169 |
SRI use in pregnancy (day) | 133.9a | |||
Child | N (%) | N (%) | N (%) | P value |
Sex (% females) | 8 (53.3) | 6 (60.0) | 7 (46.7) | .805 |
Infant race (% nonwhite) | 6 (40.0) | 6 (60.0) | 3 (21.4) | .158 |
Infant ethnicity (% Hispanic) | 4 (26.7) | 5 (50.0) | 3 (20.0) | .259 |
Preterm delivery | 2 (13.3) | 1 (10.0) | 0 | .360 |
Mean (SE) | Mean (SE) | Mean (SE) | ||
Gestational age at birth | 39.3 (0.4) | 38.9 (0.5) | 40.0 (0.3) | .134 |
Mean age (months) at study visit | 57.1 (0.8) | 59.0 (1.0) | 55.9 (0.7) | .050 |
Number of days of SRI use in pregnancy: minimum=14; maximum=273.
SRI and major depression disorder were compared for depression over pregnancy. No differences found.
SRI and major depression disorder were compared for depression severity at the study visit. No differences found (P=.324)
SE = standard error
Child sex and Index of social position at study visit (socioeconomic status) were not associated with any reaching measures and were not included as covariates. All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 17.0.
Results
Demographic information of the children and their mothers is detailed in Table II. Depression over pregnancy did not differ between the SRI and the major depression disorder groups. (P=.550). Similarly, depression severity at the study visit did not differ between the SRI and major depression disorder groups (P=.324). With the control group included, the depression severity across groups was significant (P=.035). There were no other significant maternal or child differences among the three groups.
Adjusted means (SE) are shown in Table III. Significant pairwise effects were indicated by a (SRI or major depression disorder vs. control group) and b (SRI vs. major depression disorder).
Table III.
SRI n=15 |
major depression disorder N=10 |
control N=15 |
Group | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Reach Phase | Adjusted Means (SE) |
Adjusted P value |
|||
Number of movement units (MU) |
Light | 2.63 (0.17) | 2.54 (0.22) | 2.40 (0.16) | .664 |
Glow | 3.16 (0.21) | 2.90 (0.17) | 2.91 (0.17) | .585 | |
Proportion of time to the end of first MU |
Light | 0.22 (0.02) | 0.23 (0.01) | 0.25 (0.01) | .131 |
Glow | 0.19 (0.01) | 0.17 (0.01) | 0.18 (0.01) | .669 | |
Peak velocity (cm/s) | Light | 64.2 (1.9) | 61.0 (2.6) | 66.3 (3.0) | .384 |
Glow | 56.9 (2.0) | 58.2 (3.2) | 61.8 (2.8) | .392 | |
Proportion of maximum aperture |
Light | 0.65 (0.02) | 0.60 (0.02) | 0.63 (0.03) | .201 |
Glow | 0.66b (0.02) | 0.58b (0.02) | 0.64 (0.03) | .003** | |
Drop Phase | |||||
Phase duration (ms) | Light | 449 (28.5) | 382 (31.5) | 428 (43.0) | .287 |
Glow | 655b (31.8) | 441ba (34.1) | 581 (38.1) | <.001** | |
Number of minor MU | Light | 2.71 (0.26) | 2.72 (0.41) | 2.75 (0.49) | .996 |
Glow | 3.85 (0.35) | 2.98 (0.38) | 3.75 (0.32) | .192 | |
Straightness | Light | 1.57 (0.04) | 1.47 (0.06) | 1.48 (0.06) | .244 |
Glow | 1.68b (0.05) | 1.53ab (0.03) | 1.66 (0.04) | .004** |
Group P-value -
P<0.05,
P<.01
Adjusted Post hoc comparisons:
SRI or major depression disorder to control group, P<.05.
SRI to major depression disorder group, P<.05.
In the Glow condition, the efficiency of the reach and grasp is indicated by a lower proportion of maximum aperture. In this case, the SRI group had a higher maximum aperture than the major depression disorder group (P=.002). There were no significant differences between groups in the number of MU, proportion of time to the end of the first MU, or peak velocity in the Light or Glow conditions.
A shorter phase duration from the onset of the drop phase (4 cm from the hole) to the release of the peg indicates greater efficiency. In the Glow condition, the SRI group had a longer phase duration than the major depression disorder group (P<.001) indicating less efficiency. Further, the major depression disorder group had a shorter phase duration than the control group (P=.021), indicating less efficiency in the control group in this case.
Decreased straightness values in the drop phase indicate a straighter trajectory. In the Glow condition, the SRI group showed increased straightness values indicating a longer trajectory than the major depression disorder group (P=0.007). Further the major depression disorder group showed decreased straightness values than the control group (P=.011) indicating less efficiency in the control group. There were no significant differences between groups in the number of minor MU in the Light or Glow conditions.
Group differences were found in the Glow condition but not the Light condition. The SRI group showed less efficient reaching and grasping of a glowing peg without sight of the arm and hand than the major depression disorder group. Further the SRI group took longer to return the peg to the hole near to the start position in part due to greater curvature in the returning reach.
Discussion
Subtle deficits in fine motor control may not be observable from a standard motor exam. Kinematic testing is particularly well-suited for measurement of sensory and motor functions in reaching and discrimination tasks. Motor planning and execution of a directed reach includes processing information (e.g., attention, discrimination), localization of the target, and implementation of the reach by muscles and joints. Kinematic analysis measures how the reach was executed including straightness (distance the hand traveled compared with the shortest distance to the target), smoothness (number of movement units; a movement unit is defined as an acceleration followed by deceleration), velocity, and percentage of the total reach made up by the first movement unit.17, 18 Kinematic measures demonstrate developmental changes across infancy and early childhood toward greater efficiency with age.19 Recent evidence with 15-month-olds when infants could see the object (glowing) but not their hand showed poorer quality of movement than in the light condition.20 Successful reaching depends on how well the child processes sensory information such as sight to know where the object is. Kinematic tests are particularly sensitive to deficits in sensory-motor integration.
We established a kinematic task that included reaching, grasping, and dropping a peg into a small opening near the start position. Based on earlier findings of poorer motor function with younger children exposed to SRI, we anticipated that SRI exposure will compromise visual-motor coordination (reaching in the Light condition) and efference copy signals (reaching in the Glow condition) relative to children not exposed (control). We had anticipated deficits in spatial-visual motor coordination in the Light, which we assessed by comparing measures of smoothness (MU) in a specific motor task between exposure groups. However, we found no differences in reaching parameters in the Light.
We then compared the kinematic measures by group in a condition in which the child’s hand was not visible, requiring them to rely solely on efference copy for the task. We found that children with prenatal SRI exposure have poorer fine motor control at 4.33 to 5.50 years of age compared with children who were not exposed to SRIs. Our results are consistent with our hypothesis suggesting poorer input-mediated fine-tuning of motor control related to SRI exposure. Although previous studies have reported a connection between SRI exposure and motor deficits, negative motor findings tend to be resolved prior to the ages in the current kinematics sample.21, 22
The SRI group in our study did not show poorer efficiency and decreased straightness in the Light condition relative to the major depression disorder group. This finding suggests that children with prenatal SRI exposure do not have deficits in visual-motor coordination at this age. The limited motor coordination observed in the Glow condition could hinder normal development of efference copy, as efference copy is calibrated and perfected by visual and somatic input. Prenatal SRI exposure could also be responsible for altered connectivity within the efference copy signaling pathway. We suggest that fine motor deficits could affect the formation of neural pathways, as proprioception and efference copy were all significantly affected in the SRI group.
These fine motor deficits were particularly evident in the Drop condition when the SRI group performance was consistently poor on most variables in the Glow condition. Given the gradual development of fine motor control throughout the first decade of life, we might expect that SRI exposure effects may persist rather than resolve due to increased difficulty in fine motor tasks confronting the children.
Most of our findings were between the SRI group and the major depression disorder group. This could suggest that prenatal major depression disorder exposure does not affect visual-motor coordination of movement. A more plausible explanation is based on the known association of prenatal exposure to major depression disorder and attention deficits,23 whereby poor attention could cause the major depression disorder group to have poorer performances. We did not find evidence for attentional deficits on our kinematic task in these young children. In fact, the children of mothers with major depression disorder performed better than either the SRI group or the control group, though these finding was not statistically significant.
Based on our previous work with clinical and non-clinical populations of infants,24, 25 we developed a kinematics paradigm to measure fine motor control, including visual-motor coordination, and efference copy development of young children. Visual-motor coordination is best observed in the Light condition. In the Glow condition, the moving hand was not seen during the reach but efferent copies could provide guidance toward the target.
These findings support a connection between prenatal SRI use and fine motor deficits, possibly due to poor sensorimotor learning and efference copy development. Although our results show that there are deficits associated with prenatal SRI exposure, prenatal major depression disorder exposure might present risks to the developing fetus. Depression results in chronic elevation of norepinephrine and cortisol levels, which can result among other things in suppressed neurogenesis and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis dysregulation.26
More research is necessary before we can properly compare prenatal SRI verses major depression disorder exposure. In addition, some of the mothers of the SRI and major depression disorder groups also had other psychiatric diagnoses (mostly anxiety disorder) for which some were taking psychotropic medication. We did have strict exclusion criteria regarding other diagnoses and medications, but the ones we chose to allow were so common among depressed women that a population without any other psychiatric diagnosis would have been both difficult to find but also not representative of the depressed population.
The principal limitation of this study is its small sample size. Digitizing or processing multiple trials from a single individual is time intensive. There were many reasons for adding additional sets of trials due to unintended parent interventions, child obstructing the top of the peg needed for digitizing, or child playing with the task such as not using a pincer grasp or reaching with a curvilinear trajectory. These behaviors were not related to SRIs, major depression disorder or control, but could invalidate several trials, resulting in missing data and exclusion of the case if extensive. Studies of the effects of prenatal exposure to SRIs can be problematic if only comparing the exposed children to control children or only to major depression disorder-exposed. Our inclusion of both SRI and major depression disorder groups is a strength of our study but the small sample size may limit our ability to detect additional group differences (type 1 error). We were unable to determine if the effects of SRI exposure were related to the timing of the exposure. It is well known that the first trimester is critical for nervous system and organ development and when exposure to psychotropic drugs can have the most detrimental effects. It would also be important to study and compare subclasses of SRIs (selective SRIs and Serotonin/Norepinephrine Reuptake Inhibitors) as some SRIs are already known to have more deleterious effects than others.27
Limitations notwithstanding, we have studied a highly sophisticated, precise measure of motor control and related that to prenatal SRI exposure. The fact that these kinematic effects were observed at 4 to 5 years of age suggests that there may be long-term effects of prenatal SRI exposure as the demands on the motor system increase in complexity. This study also invites the use of kinematics for the study of other clinical populations in which motor involvement is suspected. Finally, study of the motor pathways offered through kinematics may enable us to understand the neural basis of these motor deficits.
Acknowledgments
We thank Matthew Hinckley, BA (Brown Center for the Study of Children at Risk, Women & Infants Hospital, RI), for development of the coding/analysis system, and Mary Roberts, MS (Memorial Hospital, RI), for developing the statistic models used for data analysis; Ava Runge ScB (Brown University, RI), and Katherine Wooten, BS (Brown Center for the Study of Children at Risk, Women & Infants Hospital, RI), for micro-coding the data for the study; and Barry Lester, PhD (Brown Center for the study of Children at Risk, Women & Infants Hospital, RI), for suggestions on the final manuscript.
Funded by National Institute of Mental Health (R01 MH078033 [to A.S.]).
Abbreviations
- SRI
Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitors
- MU
Movement Units
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
References
- 1.Lattimore KA, Donn SM, Kaciroti N, Kemper AR, Neal CR, Jr, Vazquez DM. Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) use during pregnancy and effects on the fetus and newborn: a meta-analysis. Journal of perinatology : official journal of the California Perinatal Association. 2005;25:595–604. doi: 10.1038/sj.jp.7211352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Borue X, Chen J, Condron BG. Developmental effects of SSRIs: lessons learned from animal studies. International journal of developmental neuroscience : the official journal of the International Society for Developmental Neuroscience. 2007;25:341–347. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2007.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Zeskind PS, Stephens LE. Maternal selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor use during pregnancy and newborn neurobehavior. Pediatrics. 2004;113:368–375. doi: 10.1542/peds.113.2.368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Santucci AK, Singer LT, Wisniewski SR, Luther JF, Eng HF, Dills JL, et al. Impact of prenatal exposure to serotonin reuptake inhibitors or maternal major depressive disorder on infant developmental outcomes. The Journal of clinical psychiatry. 2014;75:1088–1095. doi: 10.4088/JCP.13m08902. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Salisbury AL, Wisner KL, Pearlstein T, Battle CL, Stroud L, Lester BM. Newborn neurobehavioral patterns are differentially related to prenatal maternal major depressive disorder and serotonin reuptake inhibitor treatment. Depress Anxiety. 2011;28:1008–1019. doi: 10.1002/da.20883. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Moses-Kolko EL, Bogen D, Perel J, Bregar A, Uhl K, Levin B, et al. Neonatal signs after late in utero exposure to serotonin reuptake inhibitors: literature review and implications for clinical applications. JAMA. 2005;293:2372–2383. doi: 10.1001/jama.293.19.2372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Smith MV, Sung A, Shah B, Mayes L, Klein DS, Yonkers KA. Neurobehavioral assessment of infants born at term and in utero exposure to serotonin reuptake inhibitors. Early Hum Dev. 2013;89:81–86. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2012.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Wisner KL, Sit DK, Hanusa BH, Moses-Kolko EL, Bogen DL, Hunker DF, et al. Major depression and antidepressant treatment: impact on pregnancy and neonatal outcomes. The American journal of psychiatry. 2009;166:557–566. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08081170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Armand J, Edgley SA, Lemon RN, Olivier E. Protracted postnatal development of corticospinal projections from the primary motor cortex to hand motoneurones in the macaque monkey. Experimental brain research. 1994;101:178–182. doi: 10.1007/BF00243230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Nudo RJ, Milliken GW, Jenkins WM, Merzenich MM. Use-dependent alterations of movement representations in primary motor cortex of adult squirrel monkeys. J Neurosci. 1996;16:785–807. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-02-00785.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kuhtz-Buschbeck JP, Stolze H, Johnk K, Boczek-Funcke A, Illert M. Development of prehension movements in children: a kinematic study. Experimental brain research. 1998;122:424–432. doi: 10.1007/s002210050530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Babinsky E, Braddick O, Atkinson J. The effect of removing visual information on reach control in young children. Experimental brain research. 2012;222:291–302. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3216-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Salisbury AL, O'Grady KE, Battle CL, Wisner KL, Anderson GM, Stroud LR, et al. The Roles of Maternal Depression, Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitor Treatment, and Concomitant Benzodiazepine Use on Infant Neurobehavioral Functioning Over the First Postnatal Month. The American journal of psychiatry. 2016;173:147–157. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.14080989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSMIV-TR Axis 1 Disorders, Research Version, Non-patient Edition (SCID-I/NP 2007) New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Holmes LB, Harvey EA, Coull BA, Huntington KB, Khoshbin S, Hayes AM, et al. The teratogenicity of anticonvulsant drugs. The New England journal of medicine. 2001;344:1132–1138. doi: 10.1056/NEJM200104123441504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.McKenna K, Koren G, Tetelbaum M, Wilton L, Shakir S, Diav-Citrin O, et al. Pregnancy outcome of women using atypical antipsychotic drugs: a prospective comparative study. The Journal of clinical psychiatry. 2005;66:444–449. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v66n0406. quiz 546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Brooks VB. Some examples of programmed limb movements. Brain Res. 1974;71:299–308. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(74)90973-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Hofsten C. Development of visually directed reaching: the approach phase. Journal of Human Movement Studies. 1979;5:160–178. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Fetters L, Todd J. Quantitative assessment of infant reaching movements. Journal of motor behavior. 1987;19:147–166. doi: 10.1080/00222895.1987.10735405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Carrico RL, Berthier NE. Vision and precision reaching in 15-month-old infants. Infant Behav Dev. 2008;31:62–70. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Casper RC, Gilles AA, Fleisher BE, Baran J, Enns G, Lazzeroni LC. Length of prenatal exposure to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) antidepressants: effects on neonatal adaptation and psychomotor development. Psychopharmacology. 2011;217:211–219. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2270-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Pedersen LH, Henriksen TB, Olsen J. Fetal exposure to antidepressants and normal milestone development at 6 and 19 months of age. Pediatrics. 2010;125:e600–e608. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-3655. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Suri R, Lin AS, Cohen LS, Altshuler LL. Acute and long-term behavioral outcome of infants and children exposed in utero to either maternal depression or antidepressants: a review of the literature. The Journal of clinical psychiatry. 2014;75:e1142–e1152. doi: 10.4088/JCP.13r08926. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.LaGasse LL, Van Vorst RF, Brunner SM, Lester BM. Effects of in utero exposure to cocaine and/or opiates on infants' reaching behavior. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1998;846:405–407. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.LaGasse L, VanVorst R, Brunner S, Zucker M. Infants' understanding of auditory events. Annals of the New York Acadmy of Sciences. 1999;8:405–407. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Marcus S, Lopez JF, McDonough S, Mackenzie MJ, Flynn H, Neal CR, Jr, et al. Depressive symptoms during pregnancy: impact on neuroendocrine and neonatal outcomes. Infant Behav Dev. 2011;34:26–34. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.07.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Einarson A, Pistelli A, DeSantis M, Malm H, Paulus WD, Panchaud A, et al. Evaluation of the risk of congenital cardiovascular defects associated with use of paroxetine during pregnancy. The American journal of psychiatry. 2008;165:749–752. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07060879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]