Decades of research indicate a temporal dynamic of visual-object categorization, depending on the level of category abstraction. To understand the neural mechanism of the temporal course of object categorization, we studied responses of neurons in inferotemporal cortex of macaque monkeys to presentation of natural object images. We observed that inferior temporal neurons represent mid-level categories (e.g., human faces) earlier than superordinate (e.g., animal)- and subordinate (e.g., face identity)-level categories.
Keywords: category representation, hierarchical abstraction, inferior temporal cortex, object recognition, temporal dynamics
Abstract
Object categories are recognized at multiple levels of hierarchical abstractions. Psychophysical studies have shown a more rapid perceptual access to the mid-level category information (e.g., human faces) than the higher (superordinate; e.g., animal) or the lower (subordinate; e.g., face identity) level. Mid-level category members share many features, whereas few features are shared among members of different mid-level categories. To understand better the neural basis of expedited access to mid-level category information, we examined neural responses of the inferior temporal (IT) cortex of macaque monkeys viewing a large number of object images. We found an earlier representation of mid-level categories in the IT population and single-unit responses compared with superordinate- and subordinate-level categories. The short-latency representation of mid-level category information shows that visual cortex first divides the category shape space at its sharpest boundaries, defined by high/low within/between-group similarity. This short-latency, mid-level category boundary map may be a prerequisite for representation of other categories at more global and finer scales.
NEW & NOTEWORTHY
Decades of research indicate a temporal dynamic of visual-object categorization, depending on the level of category abstraction. To understand the neural mechanism of the temporal course of object categorization, we studied responses of neurons in inferotemporal cortex of macaque monkeys to presentation of natural object images. We observed that inferior temporal neurons represent mid-level categories (e.g., human faces) earlier than superordinate (e.g., animal)- and subordinate (e.g., face identity)-level categories.
we recognize objects at different levels of hierarchical abstraction. Psychologists have commonly divided these levels of abstraction into three groups: superordinate (e.g., animal), mid-level or basic level (e.g., bird), and subordinate (e.g., eagle). Theoretical (Mack et al. 2009; Rosch et al. 1976) and psychophysical (Mack and Palmeri 2015; Murphy and Brownell 1985; Tanaka and Taylor 1991) studies have indicated a more rapid, perceptual access to the mid-level category information than the higher or the lower levels, suggesting speeded neural processing of the mid-level information. Other studies have challenged the mid-level advantage by demonstrating rapid access to the superordinate-level category information (Fabre-Thorpe et al. 2001; Macé et al. 2009; Poncet and Fabre-Thorpe 2014; Wu et al. 2014).
The time course of representation of category information in the neural responses involved in object recognition is poorly understood. Several studies have attempted to address this question using human magnetoencephalography (MEG) (Carlson et al. 2013; Pantazis et al. 2014) and event-related potential (ERP) (Thorpe et al. 1996), yielding conflicting results. MEG data show systematically longer latencies for more abstract category information (animate vs. inanimate) (Carlson et al. 2013; Pantazis et al. 2014), whereas ERP data show very short latencies for superordinate (animal) categories (Thorpe et al. 1996). However, MEG and ERP recordings have limited spatial resolutions and convey collective neural activities that may have originated from different parts of the brain. Furthermore, MEG and ERP signals reflect both the inputs and outputs to the cortex and may fail to reveal potential delays caused by local processing of information in the cortex. Thus the exact timing of the emergence of category information in specific neural structures cannot be revealed by these methods. The studying of the precise time course of the representation of categories requires recording the spiking activity of individual neurons.
In nonhuman primates, inferior temporal (IT) cortex lies at the end of the ventral visual pathway and is thought to be responsible for visual object recognition. IT is a purely visual area that contains various category-selective neurons (Bruce et al. 1981; Fujita et al. 1992; Kiani et al. 2007; Tanaka et al. 1991). It has been suggested that global (e.g., face category) and fine (e.g., face identity) information is conveyed by earlier and later parts of the IT neural responses, respectively (Matsumoto 2004; Sugase et al. 1999), indicating faster representation of more abstract information. This hypothesis, however, does not explain faster behavioral responses to basic-, mid-level categories.
To investigate the time course of categorical representation, we recorded the responses of IT neurons to a large number of diverse visual stimuli with multiple levels of hierarchical category abstraction. We show that IT population and single cells represented mid-level categories (e.g., faces and bodies) earlier than superordinate (e.g., animate/inanimate)- and subordinate (e.g., face identity)-level categories. These findings put constraints on models of visual object recognition by showing a stepwise processing of category information over time.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
We analyzed responses of neurons in the IT cortex of two male macaque monkeys (Kiani et al. 2005, 2007). Details of the experimental procedure have been explained in our previous publications. Briefly, responses of 674 neurons were recorded as the monkeys viewed a rapid presentation of a large number of natural and artificial visual stimuli. The stimulus set consisted of over 1,000 colored photographs of various objects on a gray background. The stimuli were presented at the center of a cathode ray tube monitor and were scaled to fit in a 7° window. To present our large stimulus set in the limited time that we could reliably record from each neuron, we used a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) (Edwards 2003; Földiák et al. 2004; Keysers et al. 2001). Each stimulus was shown for 105 ms without any interstimulus blank interval. During the presentation, the monkeys were required to maintain fixation within a window of ±2° at the center of the screen. The monkeys were rewarded with a drop of juice every 1.5–2 s for maintaining fixation
Calculation of the Temporal Dynamics of Category Information across the IT Population
At each moment in time, the representation of each stimulus by the IT neural population can be quantified by a vector whose elements are the firing rates of individual neurons. This population representation is a point in RN space, where N is the number of recorded neurons. To ensure that the population representation is not biased by the high firing-rate neurons, we normalized the responses of each neuron by subtracting the mean and dividing by the SD of responses to all stimuli (z-score normalization).
We used nonclassical multidimensional scaling (MDS) on correlation distance (Shepard 1980; Torgerson 1958) and principal component analysis (PCA) (Pearson 1901) to illustrate the separation of IT responses for different categories in two dimensions. The MDS analysis was performed for a pair of categories, e.g., animate and inanimate, and two separate, 20 ms time intervals.
In the PCA method, the eigenvectors of covariance matrix were used to make a transform matrix from the high-dimensional neural space to the lower-dimensional data. We calculated the principle components for neural responses from 65 to 170 ms after stimulus onset. Then, with the use of the first two components with the greatest eigenvalues, we constructed a constant two-dimensional (2D) axis for representation of the high-dimensional neural-response space. The first two components (with the greatest eigenvalues) explain the most variance. We projected the neural responses in a 20-ms window on the 2D axis that was constructed by PCA. The window was moved in steps of 1 ms to make animations that show the arrangements of categories in time.
We used two different indices to quantify the discriminability of object categories based on the responses of the IT neural population: “separability index” (SI) and “classification accuracy.” Both indices were calculated using the neural response in a 20-ms window. The indices were, therefore, calculated for the high-dimensional neural responses. The window was moved in steps of 1 ms to measure the time course of the two indices.
Separability index.
The separation of two categories or groups of images based on IT population responses can be defined using the scatter matrix of the category members in RN. The scatter matrix could be considered as an estimation of covariance in high-dimensional space. Two factors bear on separability: the within-category scatter and the between-category scatter (Duda et al. 2001). SI was defined as the ratio of between-category scatter and within-category scatter. The computation was performed in three steps. First, we calculated the center of mass of each category in RN and also the mean across all categories, total mean.
Mean of each category
Total mean
where is the population representation of stimulus j in category i, Ci is the set of stimuli that belong to category i, and ni is the number of stimuli in Ci. n is the number of total stimuli.
Second, we calculated the between- and within-category scatters.
Within-category scatter for each category
Total within-category scatter
Between-category scatter
where M is the set of categories for which SI is calculated. Note that Si is the covariance matrix of neural responses calculated for the members of category i. SW and SB are estimates of neural covariance matrices based on category members and the categories, respectively. Because we use the representation of images in neural space, the dimension of SW and SB matrices is N·N (N is number of neurons).
Finally, SI was computed as
where ||S|| indicates the norm of S. In this paper, we use a spectral norm (or ||S||) (Horn and Johnson 1990), because it takes into account both the variance and covariance of neural responses. Spectral norm is the largest singular value of S, and the singular value of matrix S describes the length of its geometrical expansion across major axes in the neural space. However, we obtained similar results with other matrix norms, such as trace and Frobenius norm (data not presented in this report).
Our method of calculating separability has several advantages. It can be applied to high-dimensional datasets (here, 674 neurons) with a limited number of data points. In our data, in some cases (such as human identity), it was as low as 6 and as high as 500. Finally, it properly takes into account both the variance and covariance of neural responses. Our method is closely related to Fisher information (Duda et al. 2001) and to the use of ANOVA in low-dimensional datasets (Lehmann and Romano 2006). Basically, SI is an index for evaluation of clustering quality. To calculate the SE of SI, we used a bootstrapping process (Efron and Tibshirani 1994). All of the calculations were repeated 500 times on a random selection of stimuli. We used the SD of bootstrap samples to compute confidence intervals and significances.
The SI was also used to measure the separation of two categories of stimuli that are described by physical shape features extracted from images. Each image is a point in RN space, where N is the number of physical properties that define an image shape feature space. We calculated shape feature space using different physical models, including V1, V2, and V4 [the outputs of different layers in the Hierarchical Model (HMAX)] (Riesenhuber and Poggio 1999), foot print (Sripati and Olson 2010), and a combination of object area with three other basic global properties of the images—luminance, contrast, and aspect ratio (basic properties) (Baldassi et al. 2013). With the use of the foot-print model, we calculated the physical difference between each pair of images and made a distance matrix obtained from the foot-print model. Then, we used nonclassical MDS on the foot-print distance matrix to represent images on high-dimension feature space.
Classification accuracy.
For each category pair reported in this paper, we trained a support vector machine (SVM) classifier with a linear kernel (Cortes and Vapnick 1995) using the neural responses to 70% of the stimuli. Then, we measured the classification accuracy of the model for the remaining 30% of the stimuli. Training was done by using a least-square method for finding the separating hyperplane (Suykens and Vandewalle 1999). When classification among more than two categories was desired (face identity), a majority voting procedure between every pair of classes (one-vs.-one, max-wins voting strategy) was used (Hsu and Lin 2002). This means that all potential pairwise classifiers were run, and the label of test stimuli was obtained by majority vote of all classifiers. Because chance level depends on the number of categories involved in each classification, we report the normalized classification accuracy defined as (accuracy − chance)/(1 − chance). To calculate the SE of classification accuracy, we repeated the calculations 500 times. In each repetition, the stimuli were randomly partitioned into training and test sets.
To compare the time course of the representation of different categories, we measured the onset and peak times of the separability and classification accuracy indices. Peak time was defined as when the index exceeded 90% of its maximum for 2 ms or more. Onset time was defined as when the index exceeded 10% of its maximum for 10 ms or more. The main results are not sensitive to the change in the mentioned thresholds (10% and 90%). To measure the SE of the onset and peak times for SI and classification accuracy, we used a bootstrapping method. To compare latencies of different categories, we used the estimation confidence interval using bootstrap samples.
We also applied agglomerative cluster analysis (Johnson 1967) to the neural distances in the early (85–105 ms; poststimulus) and late (155–175 ms) phases of response and computed the tree structure. This is an unsupervised analysis, and no prior assumption is implied. Then, for all of the tested categories, we computed the nodes in the tree that best matched each category. The average of the two following ratios was used as a score for the match between category and the tree: ratio 1 = (number of category members under the node)/(total members of the category), and ratio 2 = (number of category members under the node)/(total stimuli under the node) (Kiani et al. 2007). To compare change in hierarchical clustering score for different categories in early and late phases of the response, we applied the estimation confidence interval using bootstrap samples.
Calculation of the Temporal Dynamics of Category Information in Individual Neurons
We used receiver operating characteristic (ROC) analysis (Green and Swets 1989) as a robust measure for the separation of response distributions elicited by each pair of the tested categories. The area under ROC (AUROC) quantifies the performance of an ideal observer for discriminating two categories based on the responses of a single neuron (0.5 indicates chance, and 1.0 indices perfect categorization performance). For each neuron and for each pair of categories, we measured AUROC for a sliding, 20-ms window in steps of 1 ms. For conditions in which separation of more than two categories was required (e.g., face identity) AUROC was calculated for all category pairs and then was averaged across the pairs. The peak and onset times of the ROC analysis were measured as they were done for the population indices.
A neuron was defined as category selective if its responses to images of a specific category (e.g., faces) were greater than its responses to other images (one-tailed t-test, P < 0.05). The degree of selectivity of each neuron for one category (C1) vs. another category (C2) was computed by d′ index, based on following formula (Green and Swets 1989)
where M(C1) and M(C2) are the mean response of the cell to categories C1 and C2, respectively, and σ2(C1) and σ2(C2) are the variances of the cell responses in categories C1 and C2, respectively.
RESULTS
To examine the time course of category representation in the neural responses of IT cortex, we used our previously reported data of spiking activity of IT neurons (Kiani et al. 2005, 2007). In this experiment, two macaque monkeys passively viewed visual stimuli while responses of individual IT neurons (n = 674) were recorded. The recording sites included all subdivisions of IT except posterior IT (area TEO). The stimuli were images of >1,000 colored photographs of isolated natural and artificial objects. A large number of visual object categories at different levels of abstractions were included in the stimulus set, allowing us to explore the time course of category information at different levels of hierarchy (Fig. 1). Category structure of the stimulus set was first formed on an intuitive basis by dividing the stimuli into animate and inanimate categories. Animate stimuli were further divided into faces and bodies, inanimate stimuli into natural and artificial, bodies into human and animal body, and faces into primate and nonprimate faces. Finally, to provide a base for comparison of the relative time of emergence of category (mid-level or basic level) compared with identity (subordinate level) information, human faces were divided into four different individuals for whom we had at least six different images. Monkey faces were also divided into rhesus and nonrhesus faces. These categorical divisions are consistent with previous reports about natural categorical representations in the monkey and human IT (Chao et al. 1999; Kiani et al. 2007; Kriegeskorte et al. 2008; Martin et al. 1996).
Figure 2 shows the projection of the stimuli on a 2D map, created by applying MDS to the responses of the IT neural population. Each point represents one of the stimuli in the categories shown in each panel. MDS creates a low-dimensional map of the stimuli so that the distances of stimuli on the map match with the distances of the population response patterns elicited by the stimuli. We performed MDS for two separate time intervals: 1) the initial IT responses during 85–105 ms and 2) the later responses during 155–175 ms after stimulus onset. MDS analysis revealed that the early responses conveyed category information, separating faces and bodies and primate faces and nonprimate faces but failed to discriminate the higher (animates and inanimates) and lower level (face identities). The later responses, however, differentiated mid- as well as superordinate- and subordinate-level categories. On average, 67% of variance was captured by the first two dimensions of the MDS in the early phase of responses and 74% in the late phase.
To visualize the time course of category representation, we constructed a constant 2D axis using the first two PCA components derived from the neural response, from 65–170 ms after stimulus onset. We projected the neural responses in a 20-ms window on the 2D axis. The window moved at 1 ms steps from 0 to 210 ms after stimulus onset (see materials and methods). We concatenated the 2D maps that were generated at consecutive times into a movie. In Supplemental Movie 1, the actual stimuli are shown. Sixty-two percent of variance was captured by the first two dimensions of the PCA (Fig. 3). Supplemental Movie 1 shows the temporal evolution of the differential representation of the categories by the IT population. Consistent with the MDS result shown in the movie, human and monkey (primate) faces start to move away from other stimuli soon after the stimulus onset. Later, bodies separate from faces and the other stimuli. Finally, animate and inanimate images form two distinct clusters. At this time, different human face stimuli form separate clusters.
To quantify the strength and reliability of category representation in the IT neural population, we used an index that measures the ratio of between-category distances and within-category scatter of the stimuli based on IT neural responses (SI; see materials and methods). High values of SI indicate that the tested categories have large, between-category distances and small, within-category scatter. Figure 4A shows the time course of SI for different levels of object categorization. SI reached significant values for all categorization levels at some point after the stimulus onset. We observed earlier onset of significant SI values for body vs. face and for primate (monkey and human) vs. nonprimate faces (mid-level) compared with SI values for animates vs. inanimates (superordinate level) and for face identities (subordinate level). The mid-level categorizations had both an earlier onset (P < 0.001) and earlier peak (P < 0.001; Fig. 4B; onset latencies: animate vs. inanimate 105.9 ± 0.64 ms, face vs. body 82.3 ± 0.74 ms, primate faces vs. nonprimate faces 83.3 ± 1.26 ms, and face identity 103 ± 14.7 ms; peak latencies of the above-mentioned categories were 148.7 ± 3.3 ms, 121.9 ± 3.2 ms, 105.1 ± 3.6 ms, and 152.2 ± 16.5 ms, respectively). Six time windows of the movie were selected to illustrate the representation of stimuli at baseline and around onset and peak latencies of categorization. The earlier separation of mid-level categories in the movie is consistent with the earlier onset and peak latencies of SI.
To examine the time course of representation of other mid-level categories, the onset latencies for a wide range of categories were calculated and are provided in Table 1. A trend toward earlier representation of mid-level categories was observed in all of the tested conditions.
Table 1.
Category Name | Onset, ms | Peak, ms |
---|---|---|
Superordinate Level | ||
Animate vs. inanimate | 105.9 ± 0.64 | 148.7 ± 3.3 |
Mid-level | ||
Human vs. animal | 79.5 ± 0.73* | 97.8 ± 4.00* |
Monkey vs. animal | 87.4 ± 4.74* | 108.7 ± 4.75* |
Face | ||
Human vs. animal | 82.9 ± 1.13* | 102.4 ± 3.09* |
Monkey vs. animal | 87.5 ± 2.93* | 123.4 ± 7.21* |
Human vs. bird | 88.8 ± 2.46* | 139.2 ± 12.58 |
Monkey vs. bird | 93.4 ± 4.84† | 127.8 ± 8.88† |
Human vs. cat | 88.0 ± 2.04* | 136.9 ± 6.83‡ |
Monkey vs. human | 89.4 ± 2.18* | 139.2 ± 3.74† |
Rhesus vs. nonrhesus | 100.3 ± 11.63 | 125.3 ± 7.34* |
Body | ||
Human vs. animal | 98.7 ± 5.24 | 133.3 ± 10.7 |
Human vs. 4-limb | 93.8 ± 3.26† | 126.7 ± 9.74‡ |
Bird vs. 4-limb | 97.8 ± 6.74 | 125.8 ± 13.78‡ |
Human vs. bird | 101.4 ± 4.45 | 138.9 ± 11.6 |
Inanimate | ||
Artificial vs. natural | 91.6 ± 5.1† | 126.4 ± 4.57* |
Car vs. furniture | 98.6 ± 9.07 | 131.2 ± 5.53† |
Car vs. common tools | 102.8 ± 7.91 | 134.8 ± 6.20† |
Subordinate Level | ||
Human identity | 103.3 ± 14.70 | 152.2 ± 16.50 |
Woman vs. man | 103.5 ± 15.61 | 173.6 ± 49.27 |
Monkey identity | 128.5 ± 28.13 | 156.2 ± 24.75 |
SI, separability index.
Significant against superordinate:
P < 0.001;
P < 0.01;
P < 0.05.
In our analysis of category time course, there was no impact of the selected level of the contrasted category (e.g., all other stimuli, higher-level category, and categories at the same level with the one being tested) on the main finding. Onset and peak times of almost all of the tested face and body categories were earlier than the related times for superordinate categorization (Tables 2 and 3).
Table 2.
Peak | Onset | |
---|---|---|
Animate vs. Inanimate | 105.9 ± 0.64 | 148.7 ± 3.3 |
Face | ||
vs. all other | 81.4 ± 0.56* | 111.2 ± 2.71* |
vs. inanimate | 83.7 ± 0.54* | 124 ± 3.15* |
vs. other animate | 81.4 ± 0.7* | 117.2 ± 3.2* |
Primate | ||
vs. all other | 81.3 ± 0.58* | 100.8 ± 3* |
vs. inanimate | 84.3 ± 0.49* | 111.7 ± 3.17* |
vs. other animate | 82.1 ± 0.65* | 100.9 ± 2.65* |
Nonprimate | ||
vs. all other | 91.8 ± 4.49* | 131.7 ± 5.25† |
vs. inanimate | 97.8 ± 1.18* | 126.2 ± 2.26* |
vs. other animate | 101.2 ± 7.21 | 139.2 ± 7.28 |
Monkey | ||
vs. all other | 86.9 ± 2.4* | 108.6 ± 6.61* |
vs. inanimate | 92.1 ± 1.85* | 124.3 ± 2.37* |
vs. other animate | 90 ± 3.29* | 123.9 ± 4.61* |
Human | ||
vs. all other | 82.5 ± 0.53* | 98.3 ± 0.96* |
vs. inanimate | 84.4 ± 0.52* | 108.2 ± 2.45* |
vs. other animate | 82.4 ± 0.53* | 99.9 ± 1.51* |
Cat | ||
vs. all other | 81.3 ± 9.81† | 120.6 ± 70.41 |
vs. inanimate | 103 ± 1.93 | 129 ± 3.49* |
vs. other animate | NS | NS |
Bird | ||
vs. all other | 82.3 ± 7.3* | 88.6 ± 6.11* |
vs. inanimate | 101.9 ± 3.71 | 129.8 ± 5.87* |
vs. other animate | NS | NS |
Body | ||
vs. all other | 93.9 ± 0.63* | 119.8 ± 3.21* |
vs. inanimate | 96.1 ± 0.58* | 127.5 ± 2.81* |
vs. other animate | 93.6 ± 1.64* | 124.6 ± 5.05* |
Four-limb | ||
vs. all other | 95 ± 0.84* | 120.5 ± 2.75* |
vs. inanimate | 97.9 ± 0.79* | 126.4 ± 2.67* |
vs. other animate | 95 ± 1.43* | 126.7 ± 5.45* |
Human | ||
vs. all other | 89.2 ± 1.69* | 131.7 ± 20.8 |
vs. inanimate | 92.8 ± 2.65* | 124.4 ± 4.06* |
vs. other animate | 88.3 ± 2.96* | 154.7 ± 20.21 |
Monkey | ||
vs. all other | 95.9 ± 3.24* | 118.4 ± 5.36* |
vs. inanimate | 99.1 ± 2.5† | 128.1 ± 4.42* |
vs. other animate | 95.9 ± 4.7‡ | 127.5 ± 10.21‡ |
Bird | ||
vs. all other | 95.7 ± 1.64* | 120.5 ± 4.63* |
vs. inanimate | 98.1 ± 1.41* | 126.9 ± 3.19* |
vs. other animate | 95.8 ± 2.12* | 125.4 ± 7.21* |
Cat | ||
vs. all other | NS | NS |
vs. inanimate | 99.3 ± 2.41† | 124.8 ± 5.1* |
vs. other animate | 101.6 ± 4.99 | 121.5 ± 6.29* |
Dog | ||
vs. all other | NS | NS |
vs. inanimate | 99.8 ± 2.53† | 127.4 ± 4.23* |
vs. other animate | 97 ± 5.54 | 124.8 ± 9.59† |
NS, not significant.
Significant against superordinate:
P < 0.001;
P < 0.01;
P < 0.05.
Table 3.
Category Name | Onset, ms | Peak, ms |
---|---|---|
Animate | 105.9 ± 0.64 | 148.7 ± 3.3 |
Human | 83.5 ± 0.52* | 113.1 ± 4.18* |
Animal | 94.6 ± 0.61* | 140.8 ± 5.17 |
Monkey | 95.3 ± 1.19* | 126.3 ± 3.02* |
Bird | 98.3 ± 1.41* | 127 ± 2.65* |
Cat | 100.6 ± 1.75† | 128.4 ± 3.77* |
Reptile | 93.7 ± 8.37 | 126.5 ± 6.38* |
Insect | 96.7 ± 4.59‡ | 122.5 ± 4.8* |
Butterfly | 97.1 ± 6.41 | 120.3 ± 4.15* |
Hand | 94.6 ± 2.49* | 132.1 ± 7.11‡ |
Fish | 99.5 ± 10.9 | 136 ± 6.78‡ |
Significant against superordinate:
P < 0.001;
P < 0.01;
P < 0.05.
To examine the contribution and significance of face stimuli in the earlier emergence of mid- compared with superordinate-level categories, we discarded the neural responses to face stimuli and repeated the SI time course analysis. Mid-level advantage did not depend on responses to faces (Table 4). We also excluded face neurons from our cell population and calculated onset and peak times of various body category pairs (i.e., body images without visible faces). The mid-level advantage was observed in the nonface IT cell population using only body stimuli. To explore the effect of face information on representation of body categories, we used 29 pairs of exactly similar body images but with and without face (12 pairs of 4-limb; 9 pairs of bird body; and 8 pairs of other body images) and repeated the SI time course analysis for these categories against inanimate stimuli (Table 5; body category consists of all 29 pairs). In each selected pair, all shape features are the same except faces. The latencies extracted from the SI time course of bodies with and without face tested against inanimate, and the average of SI values [window (70–170) ms] was considered as a measure of represented category information. The results confirm the importance of face information in representation of category information (greater average SI value for bodies with face than bodies without face; SIbodies-with-face = 0.58 ± 0.01, SIbodies-without-face = 0.26 ± 0.01; P < 0.001) but also show that the main temporal structure reported here does not purely depend on face stimuli. Category information of both image groups (bodies with and without face) emerged earlier than superordinate. There was no significant difference between latencies of bodies with and without face.
Table 4.
Category Name | Onset, ms | Peak, ms |
---|---|---|
Superordinate Level | ||
Animate vs. inanimate | 105.9 ± 0.64 | 148.7 ± 3.3 |
vs. inanimate | ||
Nonface animate | 96.4 ± 1.25* | 129.3 ± 3.63* |
Body | 98.6 ± 1.83* | 128.8 ± 3.83* |
Human body | 91.3 ± 3.25* | 122.4 ± 2.49* |
Four-limb body | 96.4 ± 2.09* | 126.6 ± 4.17* |
Bird body | 96.3 ± 1.27* | 129.4 ± 4.08* |
Other animate | 93.4 ± 3.63* | 123.6 ± 4.2* |
vs. other animate | ||
Body | 97.2 ± 2.55* | 129.4 ± 5.03* |
Human body | 97.8 ± 3.11† | 128.8 ± 4.88* |
Four-limb body | 97.4 ± 3.01† | 131 ± 6.04† |
Bird body | 100.7 ± 3.52 | 131.8 ± 6.68‡ |
Significant against superordinate:
P < 0.001;
P < 0.01;
P < 0.05.
Table 5.
Category Name | Onset, ms |
Peak, ms |
||
---|---|---|---|---|
Animate | 105.9 ± 0.64 | 148.7 ± 3.3 | ||
with Face | without Face | with Face | without Face | |
Body | 98.1 ± 1.44* | 95.8 ± 2.46* | 125.2 ± 3.17* | 125.6 ± 4.07* |
Bird body | 100.7 ± 2.91† | 104.0 ± 7.81 | 125.2 ± 4.8* | 126.7 ± 6.33* |
Four-limb body | 98.1 ± 2.47* | 96.5 ± 3.31‡ | 123.5 ± 3.52* | 123.5 ± 4.11* |
Significant against superordinate:
P < 0.001;
P < 0.05;
P < 0.01.
To test further the temporal dynamics of category information in the IT population code, we trained biologically plausible linear classifiers (Hung et al. 2005; Meyers et al. 2008) to discriminate categories at different levels of abstraction based on the IT neural responses. Fig. 5A shows the classification accuracy. The classification accuracy was cross validated and normalized so that zero indicates that the classifier is at chance, and one indicates that the classifier performs perfectly. As expected from our SI and MDS analysis, classification accuracy increased significantly sooner for the mid-level categorizations than for the low and high levels (Fig. 5A). The earliest time that a downstream structure could discriminate the mid-level categories above chance level was 72.5 ± 2.9 and 76.1 ± 1.7 ms after stimulus onset for faces/bodies and for primate/nonprimate faces, respectively (Fig. 5B). The earliest significant discriminations for the high- and low-level discrimination were 10–30 ms later (Fig. 5B; P < 0.01; animate and inanimate discrimination, 85.4 ± 4.7 ms; face-identity discrimination, 95.7 ± 8.2 ms). The differences were even larger (35–50 ms, P < 0.01) for the time of peak performance (Fig. 5B; faces vs. bodies, 116.6 ± 4.4 ms; primate vs. nonprimate faces, 106.8 ± 4.6 ms; animate vs. inanimate, 135 ± 7.5 ms; and face identity, 150.4 ± 5.1 ms).
We used an agglomerative cluster analysis at early and late phases of response to examine the time course of category organization at a different level of hierarchy in an unsupervised way (see materials and methods for details). Figure 6 illustrates the hierarchical cluster trees computed at early (Fig. 6, A and B) and late (Fig. 6C) phases of neural response. Face, body, other animate, and inanimate category members at the lowest level of hierarchy are indicated. Face stimuli clustered at the earliest time interval (Fig. 6A; 85–105 ms), and following them, 10 ms later (Fig. 6B; 95–115 ms), body images clustered. Both face and body images (mid-level) clustered earlier than superordinate animate stimuli (Fig. 6C; 155–175 ms). Consistent with our previous analysis, the hierarchical analysis at different time bins provides unsupervised evidence for later clustering of superordinate rather than mid-level categories. For all tested categories depicted in Table 1, we calculated the hierarchical score based on ratios 1 and 2 [ratio 1 = (number of category members under the node)/(total members of the category), and ratio 2 = (number of category members under the node)/(total stimuli under the node)]. Table 6 shows the degrees of match between categories and nodes in the reconstructed tree for the early (85–105 ms) and late (155–175 ms) phases of responses. The categories with significant value across at least one of the phases were included in Table 6 (significance was computed against chance value calculated in 0–20 ms by 95% confidence interval). Table 6 also indicates the increase in hierarchical score from early to late phase of response [(Scorelate − Scoreearly)/Scorelate]. Consistent with our main finding, the superordinate categories (animate and inanimate) have a bigger change in hierarchical scores than all tested mid-level categories. So the unsupervised organization of mid-levels and superordinate in cluster tree at the early phase of response also supports earlier representation of mid-level categories in both animate and inanimate categories.
Table 6.
Category Name | Scoreearly, 85–105 ms | Scorelate, 155–175 ms | (Slate − Searly)/Slate |
---|---|---|---|
Animate | 0.68 ± 0.01 | 0.77 ± 0.05 | 0.13 ± 0.08 |
Human | 0.78 ± 0.03 | 0.73 ± 0.04 | −0.07 ± 0.05* |
Animal | 0.67 ± 0.00 | 0.62 ± 0.04 | −0.06 ± 0.06* |
Face | 0.69 ± 0.03 | 0.67 ± 0.03 | −0.03 ± 0.05† |
Primate face | 0.79 ± 0.03 | 0.71 ± 0.04 | −0.09 ± 0.05* |
Human face | 0.94 ± 0.03 | 0.87 ± 0.05 | −0.08 ± 0.06* |
Rhesus face | 0.51 ± 0.00 | 0.5 ± 0.04 | −0.02 ± 0.08† |
Body | 0.60 ± 0.00 | 0.56 ± 0.03 | −0.06 ± 0.05* |
Four-limb body | 0.56 ± 0.00 | 0.56 ± 0.02 | 0.00 ± 0.04† |
Inanimate | 0.76 ± 0.00 | 0.83 ± 0.03 | 0.09 ± 0.04 |
Natural | 0.70 ± 0.00 | 0.73 ± 0.03 | 0.05 ± 0.05 |
Artificial | 0.56 ± 0.01 | 0.55 ± 0.02 | 0.00 ± 0.03* |
IT, inferior temporal; Slate, Scorelate; Searly, Scoreearly.
Significant against corresponding superordinate:
P < 0.01;
P < 0.05.
To assess the timing of category representation in the single-cell responses, the AUROC was used (see materials and methods). AUROC quantifies the performance of an ideal observer for discriminating two categories based on the responses of a single neuron. The time courses of AUROC of individual neurons show later emergence of superordinate and subordinate categories.
To choose neurons for single-cell analysis, we defined selectivity of single cells, measured by d′ (see materials and methods), to three pairs of categories: animate vs. inanimate, face vs. body, and primate faces vs. nonprimate faces. For each of these category pairs, we performed a randomization test on the d′ (randomizing class labels) and defined selectivity based on the significance of a randomization test (P < 0.05). There were 126 neurons selective to all of the tested category pairs. Peak and onset time were defined as when the AUROC exceeded 90% and 10% of its maximum for 2 and 10 ms, respectively.
Figure 7 illustrates single-cell latencies for different levels of object categories. Each point in the scatter plots depicts one category-selective cell. Onset latencies are earlier for mid-level (faces vs. bodies, 81.6 ± 1.20 ms; primate faces vs. nonprimate faces, 78.3 ± 1.16 ms) compared with superordinate (animate vs. inanimate, 87.4 ± 1.43 ms, one-tailed t-test, P < 0.05)- and subordinate (human face identity, 85.7 ± 4.85 ms, one-tailed t-test against the latency of face vs. body categorization, P < 0.05)-level categorizations. A larger difference existed in the peak latencies (peak latency of faces vs. bodies, 113.1 ± 2.23 ms; primate vs. nonprimate faces, 107.8 ± 2.19 ms; animate vs. inanimate, 141.4 ± 2.62 ms, P < 0.001; face identity, 143.0 ± 3.77 ms, P < 0.001). Similar results were obtained when we used all responsive neurons with significant AUROC at 65–170 ms (one-tailed t-test).
To examine the relation between category selectivity of individual neurons and onset/peak time of category discrimination, we measured the correlation between d′ values of each neuron and onset/peak time of category discrimination for various category pairs. There was a significant correlation between selectivity and onset/peak latency for almost all of the tested category pairs (onset latencies: animate vs. inanimate r = −0.29, P < 0.001; face vs. body r = −0.15, P < 0.01; primate faces vs. nonprimate faces r = −0.13, P < 0.01; and face identity r = 0.15, P = 0.21; peak latencies of the above-mentioned categories: r = −0.36, P < 0.05; r = −0.41, P < 0.05; r = −0.32, P < 0.05; and r = −0.19, P < 0.05, respectively).
To examine further the temporal dynamic of category information in the IT neural ensemble, consisting of neurons without any category selectivity, we calculated SI for 157 neurons that show no significant response to face, body, animate, and inanimate categories (one-tailed t-test, P < 0.05). Interestingly, we found a similar trend for the time course of category representation in the population activity of these noncategory neurons (Fig. 8). The time course of SI in these neurons shows earlier manifestation of mid-level categories in responses of all of the IT subpopulation (Fig. 8B; onset latencies: animate vs. inanimate 135.6 ± 16.21 ms, face vs. body 96.2 ± 4.34 ms, primate faces vs. nonprimate faces 105.1 ± 10.87 ms, and face identity 132.9 ± 32.32 ms; peak latencies of the above-mentioned categories: 193.8 ± 7.91 ms, 190.8 ± 8.19 ms, 143.3 ± 7.05 ms, and 169.6 ± 44.83 ms, respectively).
It is possible that within/between physical feature similarities of visual stimuli determine the time course of the tested pairs of categories. To address this issue, we measured the physical similarity of images using V1, V2, and V4 models. These models are constructed using the outputs of different layers in the HMAX model (Riesenhuber and Poggio 1999). In addition, we used foot-print (Sripati and Olson 2010) and basic global properties of the images (object area, luminance, contrast, and aspect ratio) (Baldassi et al. 2013). We then used the ratio of within/between class physical similarity values of different pairs of categories (25 mid-levels and 1 superordinate) to calculate SI. So SI was used to measure the separation of two categories of stimuli that are described by both physical shape features and neural response. The distributions of onset and peak latencies for 25 pairs of mid-level categories, which are extracted from the time course of SI computed by neural data, are shown in Fig. 9A. Animate/inanimate onset and peak category representation times were always later than the latencies of tested, 25 pairs of mid-levels categories. There is a significant difference between the mean of onset and peak latencies of 25 pairs of tested mid-level categories and the peak and onset latencies of animate vs. inanimate (Fig. 9A; t-test, P < 0.001). With the use of each of the above-mentioned physical models, we calculated SI values for physical distinction of different pairs of categories (25 mid-levels and 1 superordinate). All of the SI values for physical distinction were bias corrected by random shuffling of category labels, 1,000 times in each tested category pair. Here, the negative values show nonsignificant physical distinctions. We plotted the SI values extracted from physical features against the onset and peak latencies extracted from the time course of SI computed by neural data for each pair of 26 tested categories (Fig. 9B). These analyses revealed no correlation between latency of category representation and within/between physical similarity for any of the tested models.
To study further the effect of within-category member similarity on the neural representation of mid-level categories, we selected a subset of faces and bodies with equal within-similarity distances (diverse faces and similar bodies). The basic global properties of the images (object area, luminance, contrast, and aspect ratio) (Baldassi et al. 2013) were used to compute within-category distance. New animate, face, and body categories were made using face and body shape distance-matched stimuli. There is no significant difference in mean of within-similarity distances for selected bodies and faces (within-similarity distance before selection: face = 0.43 ± 0.24, body = 0.62 ± 0.41; within-similarity distance after selection: face = 0.51 ± 0.37, body = 0.50 ± 0.33; t-test, P = 0.14). Consistent with our main findings, the early representation of mid-level categories compared with superordinate was observed when using neural responses of these shape-distance-matched categories (Table 7). These results also suggest that highly similar face stimuli are not the major factor in the late representation of superordinate category.
Table 7.
Category Name | Onset | Peak |
---|---|---|
Modified animate vs. inanimate | 99.6 ± 2.00 | 146.9 ± 4.51 |
Diverse face vs. similar body | 88.4 ± 5.33* | 131.6 ± 3.96† |
Divers face vs. inanimate | 89.0 ± 1.37† | 128.6 ± 2.94† |
Similar body vs. inanimate | 97.3 ± 0.75 | 127.1 ± 2.47† |
Diverse face vs. other animate | 83.6 ± 1.48† | 120.7 ± 4.27† |
Similar body vs. other animate | 93.2 ± 1.93† | 123.8 ± 6.8† |
Significant against corresponding superordinate:
P < 0.05;
P < 0.001.
To examine a potential, functional difference between IT subdivisions, we compared the temporal dynamics of category representation using data of neurons located in areas lower bank of superior temporal sulcus (STS), ventral inferotemporal cortex (TEv), dorsal inferotemporal cortex (TEd), as well as the posterior part of TE (TEp)/anterior part of TE (TEa; with and without STS neurons). The locations of neurons were defined using MRI and electrophysiological mapping (Kiani et al. 2007). Whereas the results of the SI analysis were noisier, due to smaller sample sizes, earlier representation of mid-level categories compared with superordinate and subordinate was observed in all of the tested areas (Table 8).
Table 8.
Animate vs. Inanimate |
Face vs. Body |
Primate Face vs. Nonprimate Face |
Human Identity |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Onset | Peak | Onset | Peak | Onset | Peak | Onset | Peak | |
STS | 96.0 ± 0.88 | 131.3 ± 2.90 | 85.4 ± 0.84* | 125.9 ± 4.47 | 84.9 ± 1.63* | 119.3 ± 7.85 | 105 ± 20.86 | 145.7 ± 13.04 |
TEd | 95.5 ± 0.83 | 138.4 ± 4.53 | 89.1 ± 0.75* | 120.9 ± 2.18* | 85.4 ± 1.40* | 115.6 ± 9.77† | 93.1 ± 6.63 | 160 ± 12.79 |
TEv | 99.9 ± 0.59 | 145.8 ± 3.30 | 81 ± 0.83* | 121.3 ± 3.69* | 83.5 ± 1.38* | 104.5 ± 7.52* | 16.9 ± 16.23 | 158.9 ± 31.35 |
TEp (with STS) | 93.1 ± 0.76 | 130.0 ± 2.94 | 89.2 ± 0.97* | 119.4 ± 3.98† | 88.3 ± 1.7† | 124.0 ± 11.14 | 120.6 ± 15.73 | 155.3 ± 9.09 |
TEa (with STS) | 99.8 ± 0.75 | 146.8 ± 2.85 | 81.0 ± 0.72* | 124.8 ± 4.33* | 82.5 ± 1.16* | 103.1 ± 3.25* | 104.6 ± 14.84 | 156.3 ± 19.46 |
TEp (without STS) | 93.9 ± 0.77 | 130.2 ± 2.95 | 89.8 ± 0.93* | 119.6 ± 3.75† | 88.4 ± 1.69* | 121.0 ± 6.67 | 134.7 ± 8.14 | 158.3 ± 10.11 |
TEa (without STS) | 100 ± 0.80 | 148.5 ± 3.51 | 80.8 ± 0.74* | 124.3 ± 3.89* | 82.9 ± 1.47* | 103 ± 2.23* | 102.8 ± 13.81 | 155.5 ± 20.7 |
Significant against corresponding superordinate:
P < 0.001;
P < 0.01.
To control the potential impact of spike contamination evoked by preceding stimuli that may occur in RSVP, we divided trials into two groups: trials in which an animate image preceded the stimulus and those in which an inanimate image preceded the stimulus (Fig. 10). We then calculated the SI values of each group separately. The advantage of mid-level categories was preserved in both conditions (condition a, onset latencies: animate vs. inanimate 118 ± 0.14 ms, face vs. body 97.1 ± 0.31 ms, primate faces vs. nonprimate faces 97.4 ± 0.52 ms, and face identity 117.1 ± 1.62 ms; peak latencies of the above-mentioned categories: 160.4 ± 1.5 ms, 128.6 ± 0.89 ms, 112.6 ± 1.11 ms, and 164.4 ± 3.62 ms, respectively; condition b, onset latencies: animate vs. inanimate 116 ± 0.22 ms, face vs. body 91 ± 0 ms, primate faces vs. nonprimate faces 91.5 ± 0.52 ms, and face identity 102.4 ± 30.51 ms; peak latencies of the above-mentioned categories: 147.1 ± 0.69 ms, 120.7 ± 0.92 ms, 107.4 ± 0.61 ms, and 198.9 ± 32.5 ms, respectively).
DISCUSSION
In this paper, we studied the time course of visual object category representation in neural responses of the IT cortex of macaque monkeys. We found that IT neurons represent mid-level categories (e.g., human faces) earlier than superordinate (e.g., animal)- and subordinate (e.g., face identity)-level categories. Responses of the IT neural population, both category-selective and nonselective neurons, show a temporal order of category information with earlier emergence of mid-level category information. A similar time course of category information was observed in the activity of individual neurons with strong category selectivity.
Psychophysical studies have shown that perceptual access to the mid-level category information occurs earlier than access to the higher- or lower-level categories. These findings suggest an expedited neural processing of the mid-level information (Mack and Palmeri 2015; Rosch et al. 1976; Tanaka and Taylor 1991). Our results suggest the neural signature of this mid-level advantage phenomenon. The short-latency emergence of mid-level category information shows that the category shape space is divided first at its sharpest boundaries, defined by high/low within/between-group similarity. This short-latency, mid-level category boundary map may be used for representation of category boundaries at higher or lower levels of abstraction in later stages of the neural processing. In addition, these findings put constraints on the models of object recognition by showing temporally structured processing of category information in the visual cortex. Our study surpasses past literature and reconciles seemingly contradictory results in previous studies, as we explain below. However, it should be noted that the neurophysiological data reported here were collected from passively viewing monkeys; therefore, relevance of the temporal course of neural responses to behavioral visual categorization cannot be directly established in our study and needs further investigations.
Neural correlates of object categorization have been widely investigated in the ventral visual pathway (Bruce et al. 1981; Kiani et al. 2007; Kriegeskorte et al. 2008; Tanaka 2003). Cells in IT cortex respond selectively to specific categories at different levels of abstraction (Desimone et al. 1984; Fujita et al. 1992; Kiani et al. 2007; Tsao 2006). However, previous studies have generated contradictory results about the time course of categorical representations. The recording of IT responses to faces of humans and monkeys has revealed that IT neurons distinguish monkey faces from human faces earlier than they distinguish face identities (Matsumoto 2004; Sugase et al. 1999). These authors have hypothesized a coarse-to-fine temporal gradient for the representation of information in IT and have speculated that global information could act as a header to prepare downstream areas for processing of finer-grained information.
On the other hand, recent MEG studies in humans have suggested an opposite temporal gradient in which finer-grained information about visual stimuli is represented before more global information. The decoding of MEG signals results in earlier recognition of exemplar members (i.e., image identities) than more abstract categories (e.g., animate vs. inanimate) (Carlson et al. 2013). Multivariate pattern classification of the time course of human MEG signals shows a delayed representation of superordinate categories compared with individual images (Pantazis et al. 2014). With the use of data from our lab [Kiani et al. (2007) and current results], Pantazis et al. (2014) report a similar pattern in their human MEG data and the category structure of the spiking activity of IT neurons. There is, however, an unresolved discrepancy between the latency of category representations in IT spiking activity and MEG and EEG studies. Unlike the MEG and EEG studies that show earlier representation of individual stimuli, IT-spiking activity supports an earlier representation of mid-level categories. The discrepancy is likely to originate from differences in signals recorded by the two techniques. Whereas spiking activity represents neuronal outputs, MEG and EEG signals reflect a complex combination of synaptic inputs, neuronal outputs, synchrony, and spatial alignment of charges along axons and dendrites in large populations of neurons (Ikeda et al. 2002; Murakami and Okada 2006; Okada et al. 1997). The resolution of the discrepancy between the two techniques requires a deeper understanding of the mapping of MEG and EEG signals to spiking activity. However, it is likely that the earlier representation of individual stimuli in MEG and EEG signals reflects a larger difference in IT input, whereas the earlier representation of mid-level categories in spiking activity emerges from the processing of the input information by IT neurons.
Our results are immune to several confounding factors that have been shown to bias measurements of response latencies. First, note that within-category heterogeneity of stimuli increases monotonically from subordinate- to mid- to superordinate-level categories. Therefore, delayed representation of superordinate and subordinate categories cannot be attributed to their higher diversity or stimulus dissimilarity compared with mid-level categories. Secondly, the differential time course of category representations cannot be attributed to the number of cells selective for each category. The temporal advantage of mid-level categories persisted even after exclusion of category-selective cells. Thirdly, our results were not biased by a preferential representation of mid-level categories in an individual monkey. The main results were independently replicated in each monkey. Fourthly, the observed results were not shaped by the number of stimuli that belonged to each category. To examine the potential impact of sample-size bias, we equalized the number of samples in pairs of categories and repeated the SI and SVM analyses. Mid-level category information emerged earlier in all of the tested category pairs. Fifthly, spike contamination, evoked by preceding stimuli that may occur in RSVP, did not impact the mid-level advantage. Finally, the differential time course of the category representations could not be attributed to a systematic difference in luminance or contrast across the categories. The nested structure of the hierarchical categories removes concerns about biases caused by such stimulus inhomogeneity. Ruling out these confounds enhances the reliability of our conclusions about the slower emergence of superordinate and subordinate category information in IT cortex.
To be able to present a large number of stimulus images, we used an RSVP paradigm with short stimulus-presentation duration and no interstimulus interval. It is plausible that neural responses in the RSVP condition are different than when longer-presentation duration with long interstimulus intervals is used. It should be noted that in natural life, as we explore the visual world, retinal images of visual stimuli change relatively rapidly without any no-stimulation intervals. So the potential interaction of stimuli in the RSVP condition on neural responses may mimic the real-world condition better than stimulation paradigms with long interstimulus intervals. In addition, cells in the monkey IT cortex preserve their stimulus selectivity in RSVP as fast as 14–28 ms/stimulus (Edwards 2003; Földiák et al. 2004; Keysers et al. 2001). Furthermore, backward masking has a minimal effect on the initial part of neuronal responses when the stimulus onset asynchrony is <80 ms (Kovács et al. 1995; Rolls and Tovee 1994). We have previously used the same image set and tested IT neural response latency of human and animal faces for two presentation methods: 245 ms presentation time with 245 ms blank interval and 105 ms presentation time without any blank interval (Kiani et al. 2005). We found no significant difference between face response latency of these two presentation methods.
With the consideration of the limitations of the stimulus set, we could calculate SI values for only 3 subordinate categories compared with 16 mid-level conditions. On the other hand, we only examine one superordinate category pair (animate vs. inanimate) with high within-member shape variability. A more balanced stimulus image set is needed to understand fully the subordinate vs. mid-level category information time courses.
The psychophysical studies that have challenged the mid-level advantage by demonstrating rapid access to the superordinate-level category information (Fabre-Thorpe et al. 2001; Macé et al. 2009; Poncet and Fabre-Thorpe 2014; Wu et al. 2014) are not necessarily contradictory to our findings. As in the animal-detection experiments, subjects could rely on detection of face or body (or even face or body components) and perform the task efficiently. The short-latency, mid-level category information (animal face or animal body) could underlie the short-latency behavioral responses in these animal-detection tasks. Consistent with our results, a new, functional MRI and psychophysics study with a large real-world object image set shows that participants were significantly faster for basic-level categorization than superordinate and subordinate. With the use of multivoxel analysis, it suggests that neural patterns of mid-level (basic-level) categories represent an optimal level of within-category similarity (category cohesion) and between-category dissimilarity (category distinctiveness) (Iordan et al. 2015).
One explanation for the mid-level advantage is that mid-level and superordinate categories might be represented in IT and prefrontal cortex, respectively. Mid-level categories have low within-class to between-class shape variability and high behavioral saliency. Recognition of these categories can take place earlier, since the perceptual processing of category information occurs within the visual cortex and does not need involvement of the higher brain areas, such as prefrontal cortex. Categorization of objects at higher levels of the category hierarchy requires more complex computations, since at these levels, within- to between-class shape variability is relatively high, and parsing the objects into correct groups is more time consuming, resulting in delayed perceptual access. Consistent with this hypothesis, neural representation of novel category boundaries has been shown in the prefrontal cortex (Freedman et al. 2003), whereas short-latency category information of highly familiar objects has been indicated in the IT neural responses (Anderson et al. 2008; Hung et al. 2005; Kiani et al. 2005).
An alternative explanation for earlier emergence of the mid-level categories in our data might be the impact of face images on the temporal course of category information reported here. Faces and bodies with face comprised a large proportion of our animate images. In our image set, faces—and especially human faces—were highly similar. The low within-category shape distance of faces and short-latency responses to faces (Kiani et al. 2005) may result in early emergence of the mid-level animate categories in our analysis. Our control analysis of neural responses to 29 pairs of exactly similar bodies, with and without face, suggests that earlier mid-level category information observed here does not exclusively depend on neural responses to faces.
Learning is shown to diminish the time difference between categorization at the mid- and subordinate levels (Johnson and Mervis 1997; Tanaka and Taylor 1991). Long-term learning can redefine the perceptual category boundaries mapped in IT (Baker et al. 2002; Seger and Miller 2010). Consistent with these findings, in our data, earlier onset of category information was observed for some of the more familiar mid-level categories (e.g., human and monkey face/body) but not the other less familiar ones (e.g., bird bodies). The peak response time of mid-level categories, however, was earlier than the superordinate and subordinate ones in all of the tested categories, including the less familiar ones (Table 1). This finding implies that the learning-dependent, expedited processing of some mid-level categories affects onset but not peak time of category representation in IT.
With the use of an entirely data-driven method, we have previously shown that neural responses in IT cortex discriminate animate from inanimate objects (Kiani et al. 2007; Kriegeskorte et al. 2008). However, this finding does not necessarily imply semantic representation of object categories. Animate/inanimate category boundary representation in IT could be defined by similarity of physical features within the animate images. The nature of these critical/diagnostic features is not known. Our current results, using the same data, show that the temporal order of emergence of category information is not affected by the degree of within/between-category shape similarity, defined by several predominant shape-representation models. We believe both phenomena emerge because of the selectivity of IT neurons to critical features that distinguish and discriminate biologically relevant categories. Some recent studies have questioned the presence of a true animate/inanimate boundary representation in IT (Baldassi et al. 2013; Yamins et al. 2014). The discrepancies observed in these studies and in our previous and current reports could be due to differences in the recorded IT area, stimulus set size, and stimulus shape variability. Neurons in the anterior IT tend to be more category selective (Kiani et al. 2007; Tanaka et al. 1991). Thus a population analysis performed using a larger proportion of posterior neurons could fail to show animate/inanimate differentiation. Furthermore, monkeys in our study were raised as pets and had far-richer visual experience than the regular experimental monkeys. Formation of category-boundary representation has been shown to be experience dependent (Anderson et al. 2008).
In summary, we examined the time course of category information in the neural responses of IT cortex of macaque monkeys viewing a large number of object images and found an earlier representation of mid-level categories both in the IT population and in single-unit responses. The faster emergence of mid-level category information suggests that visual cortex first divides the category shape space at its sharpest boundaries based on high within-group and low between-group similarity. This mid-level advantage in IT neural responses provides a mechanism for the mid-level advantage in perception and behavior.
DISCLOSURES
No conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise, are declared by the authors.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
R.K. and H.E. conception and design of research; R.K. and H.E. performed experiments; M-R.A.D., A-H.V., and R.K. analyzed data; M-R.A.D., A-H.V., R.K., M.N.A., B.N.A., and H.E. interpreted results of experiments; M-R.A.D. prepared figures; M-R.A.D., A-H.V., R.K., M.N.A., B.N.A., and H.E. drafted manuscript; M-R.A.D., A-H.V., R.K., M.N.A., B.N.A., and H.E. edited and revised manuscript; M-R.A.D. and H.E. approved final version of manuscript.
Supplementary Material
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors thank S. Lehky for a critical reading of the manuscript.
REFERENCES
- Anderson B, Mruczek RE, Kawasaki K, Sheinberg D. Effects of familiarity on neural activity in monkey inferior temporal lobe. Cereb Cortex 18: 2540–2552, 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baker CI, Behrmann M, Olson CR. Impact of learning on representation of parts and wholes in monkey inferotemporal cortex. Nat Neurosci 5: 1210–1216, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baldassi C, Alemi-Neissi A, Pagan M, DiCarlo JJ, Zecchina R, Zoccolan D. Shape similarity, better than semantic membership, accounts for the structure of visual object representations in a population of monkey inferotemporal neurons. PLoS Comput Biol 9: e1003167, 2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruce C, Desimone R, Gross CG. Visual properties of neurons in a polysensory area in superior temporal sulcus of the macaque. J Neurophysiol 46: 369–384, 1981. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlson T, Tovar D, Alink A, Kriegeskorte N. Representational dynamics of object vision: the first 1000 ms. J Vis 13: 1–19, 2013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chao LL, Haxby JV, Martin A. Attribute-based neural substrates in temporal cortex for perceiving and knowing about objects. Nat Neurosci 2: 913–919, 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cortes C, Vapnick V. Support-vector networks. Mach Learn 20: 273–297, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Desimone R, Albright TD, Gross CG, Bruce C. Stimulus-selective neurons in the macaque. J Neurosci 4: 2051–2062, 1984. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duda RO, Hart PE, Stork DG. Pattern Classification (3rd ed). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Edwards R. Color sensitivity of cells responsive to complex stimuli in the temporal cortex. J Neurophysiol 90: 1245–1256, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Efron B, Tibshirani RJ. An Introduction to the Bootstrap. Boca Raton, FL: CRC, 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Fabre-Thorpe M, Delorme A, Marlot C, Thorpe S. A limit to the speed of processing in ultra-rapid visual categorization of novel natural scenes. J Cogn Neurosci 13: 171–180, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Földiák P, Xiao D, Keysers C, Edwards R, Perrett DI. Rapid serial visual presentation for the determination of neural selectivity in area STSa. Prog Brain Res 144: 107–116, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freedman DJ, Riesenhuber M, Poggio T, Miller EK. A comparison of primate prefrontal and inferior temporal cortices during visual categorization. J Neurosci 23: 5235–5246, 2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fujita I, Tanaka K, Ito M, Cheng K. Columns for visual features of objects in monkey inferotemporal cortex. Nature 360: 343–346, 1992. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green DM, Swets JA. Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics. Newport Beach, CA: Peninsula, 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Horn RA, Johnson CR. Matrix Analysis. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Hsu C-W, Lin C-J. A comparison of methods for multiclass support vector machines. IEEE Trans Neural Netw 13: 415–425, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hung CP, Kreiman G, Poggio T, DiCarlo JJ. Fast readout of object identity from macaque inferior temporal cortex. Science 310: 863–866, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ikeda H, Leyba L, Bartolo A, Wang Y, Okada YC. Synchronized spikes of thalamocortical axonal terminals and cortical neurons are detectable outside the pig brain with MEG. J Neurophysiol 87: 626–630, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iordan MC, Greene MR, Beck DM, Fei-Fei L. Basic level category structure emerges gradually across human ventral visual cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 27: 1427–1446, 2015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson KE, Mervis CB. Effects of varying levels of expertise on the basic level of categorization. J Exp Psychol Gen 126: 248–277, 1997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson SC. Hierarchical clustering schemes. Psychometrika 32: 241–254, 1967. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keysers C, Xiao DK, Földiák P, Perrett DI. The speed of sight. J Cogn Neurosci 13: 90–101, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiani R, Esteky H, Mirpour K, Tanaka K. Object category structure in response patterns of neuronal population in monkey inferior temporal cortex. J Neurophysiol 97: 4296–4309, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiani R, Esteky H, Tanaka K. Differences in onset latency of macaque inferotemporal neural responses to primate and non-primate faces. J Neurophysiol 94: 1587–1596, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kovács G, Vogels R, Orban GA. Cortical correlate of pattern backward masking. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 92: 5587–5591, 1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kriegeskorte N, Mur M, Ruff DA, Kiani R, Bodurka J, Esteky H, Tanaka K, Bandettini PA. Matching categorical object representations in inferior temporal cortex of man and monkey. Neuron 60: 1126–1141, 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lehmann EL, Romano JP. Testing Statistical Hypotheses. New York: Springer, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Macé MJ, Joubert OR, Nespoulous JL, Fabre-Thorpe M. The time-course of visual categorizations: you spot the animal faster than the bird. PLoS One 4: e5927, 2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mack ML, Palmeri TJ. The dynamics of categorization: unraveling rapid categorization. J Exp Psychol Gen 144: 551–569, 2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mack ML, Wong AC, Gauthier I, Tanaka JW, Palmeri TJ. Time course of visual object categorization: fastest does not necessarily mean first. Vision Res 49: 1961–1968, 2009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin A, Wiggs CL, Ungerleider LG, Haxby JV. Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge. Nature 379: 649–652, 1996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsumoto N. Population dynamics of face-responsive neurons in the inferior temporal cortex. Cereb Cortex 15: 1103–1112, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyers EM, Freedman DJ, Kreiman G, Miller EK, Poggio T. Dynamic population coding of category information in inferior temporal and prefrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol 100: 1407–1419, 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murakami S, Okada Y. Contributions of principal neocortical neurons to magnetoencephalography and electroencephalography signals. J Physiol 575: 925–936, 2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy GL, Brownell HH. Category differentiation in object recognition: typicality constraints on the basic category advantage. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 11: 70–84, 1985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Okada YC, Wu J, Kyuhou S. Genesis of MEG signals in a mammalian CNS structure. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 103: 474–485, 1997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pantazis D, Oliva A, Cichy RM. Resolving human object recognition in space and time. Nat Neurosci 17: 1–10, 2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pearson K. On lines and planes of closest fit to systems of points in space. Philos Mag 2: 559–572, 1901. [Google Scholar]
- Poncet M, Fabre-Thorpe M. Stimulus duration and diversity do not reverse the advantage for superordinate-level representations: the animal is seen before the bird. Eur J Neurosci 39: 1508–1516, 2014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riesenhuber M, Poggio T. Hierarchical models of object recognition in cortex. Nat Neurosci 2: 1019–1025, 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Tovee MJ. Processing speed in the cerebral cortex and the neurophysiology of visual masking. Proc Biol Sci 257: 9–15, 1994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosch E, Mervis CB, Gray WD, Johnson DM, Boyes-Braem P. Basic objects in natural categories. Cogn Psychol 8: 382–439, 1976. [Google Scholar]
- Seger AC, Miller EK. Category learning in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 33: 203–219, 2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shepard RN. Multidimensional scaling, tree-fitting, and clustering. Science 210: 390–398, 1980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sripati AP, Olson CR. Global image dissimilarity in macaque inferotemporal cortex predicts human visual search efficiency. J Neurosci 30: 1258–1269, 2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sugase Y, Yamane S, Ueno S, Kawano K. Global and fine information coded by single neurons in the temporal visual cortex. Nature 400: 869–873, 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suykens JA, Vandewalle J. Least squares support vector machine classifiers. Neural Process Lett 9: 293–300, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Tanaka JW, Taylor M. Object categories and expertise: is the basic level in the eye of the beholder? Cogn Psychol 23: 457–482, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Tanaka K. Columns for complex visual object features in the inferotemporal cortex: clustering of cells with similar but slightly different stimulus selectivities. Cereb Cortex 13: 90–99, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanaka K, Saito HA, Fukada Y, Moriya M. Coding visual images of objects in the inferotemporal cortex of the macaque monkey. J Neurophysiol 66: 170–189, 1991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thorpe S, Fize D, Marlot C. Speed of processing in the human visual system. Nature 381: 520–522, 1996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Torgerson WS. Theory and Methods of Scaling. New York: Wiley, 1958. [Google Scholar]
- Tsao DY. A cortical region consisting entirely of face-selective cells. Science 311: 670–674, 2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu C-T, Crouzet SM, Thorpe SJ, Fabre-Thorpe M. At 120 ms you can spot the animal but you don't yet know it's a dog. J Cognitive Neurosci 27: 1–10, 2014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamins DL, Hong H, Cadieu CF, Solomon EA, Seibert D, DiCarlo JJ. Performance-optimized hierarchical models predict neural responses in higher visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 111: 8619–8624, 2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.