Over the last half century, lesion and single-unit recording studies across multiple species converge on the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) as central to pain processing (1–7). Our response (i) identifies a flaw in Wager et al.’s analysis (8) that underestimates the dACC’s contribution to pain and (ii) presents dACC-wide posterior probability analyses that provide further evidence that pain is a better account of dACC function than executive, conflict, or salience processes. Issues regarding our use of Z-scores, the proper definition of selectivity, and whether one can categorize a neural region in terms of a particular function are addressed elsewhere (https://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/social-brain-social-mind/201601/more-evidence-pain-related-description-dacc).
In Wager et al.’s reply (8), they ask a different question than we did in our original article (9). Wager et al. have a reasonable approach if one were interested in predicting the research topic of a randomly selected study from the Neurosynth database that shows a dACC effect. If, however, the goal was to assess the psychological function of the dACC (i.e., structure-to-function mapping), which was our goal, then correcting for study number disparities across terms is essential (which Neurosynth wisely does by default).
Imagine a database consisting of 100,000 attention studies and 100 pain studies. If a voxel is activated in 1,000 attention studies and all 100 pain studies, we would draw two conclusions. First, a randomly drawn study from the 1,100 with an effect would likely be an attention study. Second, because 100% of the pain studies produced an effect and only 1% of attention studies did, we would also conclude that this voxel is more selective for pain than attention. Hit rates (e.g., the number of pain studies that activate a region divided by the total number of pain studies in Neurosynth) are more important for assessing structure-to-function mapping than the historical tendency to conduct more studies on some topics than others.
We attempted to recreate and then build on Wager et al.’s analysis (8). At Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates 0, 10, 34, we observed that 18% of 240 studies with effects were pain studies (Fig. 1A). However, Wager et al.'s analysis ignores the fact that cognitive terms have up to 5 times more studies in Neurosynth’s database than do pain terms (Fig. 1B) and thus can produce the numbers in Fig. 1A despite very low hit rates (Fig. 1C). After correcting for this disparity (Fig. 1D), 34% of the expected activations are from pain studies, which is nearly 5 times more than that from the largest cognitive term (7%) and more than from all cognitive terms combined.
We also conducted a series of Neurosynth analyses that (i) consider all dACC voxels and (ii) directly compare pain studies against equal numbers of executive, conflict, and salience studies. Across all dACC voxels, the average posterior probability was 0.68 for pain, double those observed for the other terms (0.32) (Fig. 2A). In addition, 86% of dACC voxels had higher posterior probabilities for pain than for each other term (Fig. 2B). This is strong evidence that pain has a stronger structure-to-function relationship with dACC than executive, conflict, or salience processes.
Footnotes
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
- 1.Hutchison WD, Davis KD, Lozano AM, Tasker RR, Dostrovsky JO. Pain-related neurons in the human cingulate cortex. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2(5):403–405. doi: 10.1038/8065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Sikes RW, Vogt BA. Nociceptive neurons in area 24 of rabbit cingulate cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1992;68(5):1720–1732. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.68.5.1720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Yamamura H, et al. Morphological and electrophysiological properties of ACCx nociceptive neurons in rats. Brain Res. 1996;735(1):83–92. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(96)00561-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Foltz EL, White LE., Jr Pain “relief” by frontal cingulumotomy. J Neurosurg. 1962;19(2):89–100. doi: 10.3171/jns.1962.19.2.0089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hurt RW, Ballantine HT., Jr Stereotactic anterior cingulate lesions for persistent pain: A report on 68 cases. Clin Neurosurg. 1974;21:334–351. doi: 10.1093/neurosurgery/21.cn_suppl_1.334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Brotis AG, Kapsalaki EZ, Paterakis K, Smith JR, Fountas KN. Historic evolution of open cingulectomy and stereotactic cingulotomy in the management of medically intractable psychiatric disorders, pain and drug addiction. Stereotact Funct Neurosurg. 2009;87(5):271–291. doi: 10.1159/000226669. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Fellows LK, Farah MJ. Is anterior cingulate cortex necessary for cognitive control? Brain. 2005;128(Pt 4):788–796. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Wager TD, et al. Pain in the ACC? Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2016;113:E2474–E2475. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1600282113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Lieberman MD, Eisenberger NI. The dorsal anterior cingulate cortex is selective for pain: Results from large-scale reverse inference. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2015;112(49):15250–15255. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1515083112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Yarkoni T, Poldrack RA, Nichols TE, Van Essen DC, Wager TD. Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Nat Methods. 2011;8(8):665–670. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]