Abstract
Many group-living animals coordinate movements with acoustic signals, but so far most studies have focused on how group movements are initiated. In this study, we investigated movement patterns of wild sooty mangabeys (Cercocebus atys), a mostly terrestrial, forest-dwelling primate. We provide quantitative results showing that vocalization rates of mangabey subgroups, but not of focal individuals, correlated with focal individuals’ current movement patterns. More interestingly, vocal behaviour predicted whether individuals changed future speed, and possibly future travel direction. The role of vocalizations as a potential mechanism for the regulation of group movement was further highlighted by interaction effects that include subgroup size and the quality of poly-specific associations. Collectively, our results suggest that primate vocal behaviour can function beyond travel initiation in coordination and regulation of group movements.
Keywords: Group movement, Vocal communication, Poly-specific association, Group cohesion, Sooty mangabey, Cercocebus atys
Introduction
Living in groups can convey considerable benefits for individuals, such as increased predator detection and foraging success. Nevertheless, group living incurs costs; for example, through increased competition among individuals for resources or exposure to diseases (Elgar, 1989; Sterck, Watts & van Schaik, 1997; Krause & Ruxton, 2002; Altizer et al., 2003; Majolo, de Bortoli Vizioli & Schino, 2008). To benefit from the presence of others, individuals need to regulate group cohesion, especially during travel, and signals such as vocalizations are likely to play a key role in this context (Boinski & Garber, 2000; Conradt & Roper, 2005; da Cunha & Byrne, 2009; Petit & Bon, 2010; Fischer & Zinner, 2011a; Fischer & Zinner, 2011b).
Most previous research in group coordination has focused on how movements are initiated (e.g., Stewart & Harcourt, 1994; Radford, 2004; Bousquet, Sumpter & Manser, 2011), which has revealed a variety of mechanisms (Conradt & Roper, 2005; Petit & Bon, 2010). In contrast, we are not aware of empirical work that has looked at the role of vocalizations during travel, such as how speed and changes in direction are determined, and how movements are terminated. As such, initiation and termination of group movements may only represent the extreme ends of a more complex phenomenon, which may require additional communicative mechanisms that are used to regulate movement patterns along the way.
In addition, researchers working on movement initiations normally study discrete bouts of movements, such as when travelling from a resting place to a food resource. In such situations typically one or several leader individuals are followed or a group consensus is reached, through the production of specific vocal and other behavioural signals (e.g., Black, 1988; Fletcher, 2007; Bousquet, Sumpter & Manser, 2011). It is possible that such discrete and conspicuous movement bouts are typical only for species that exploit clumped food or water resources (e.g., Asensio et al., 2011; Noser & Byrne, 2014). Mechanisms for species foraging on relatively evenly distributed food resources may be different, especially if foraging involves more or less continuous travel.
Vocalizations are particularly well studied with respect to their role in initiating movements (e.g., Poole et al., 1988; Bousquet, Sumpter & Manser, 2011; Fischer & Zinner, 2011a), perhaps because acoustic signals can bridge the continuum between local and global information of individuals within a group much better than visual signals. The latter can function only in the local domain (Conradt & Roper, 2005; Petit & Bon, 2010, see also Couzin et al. (2005) and Strandburg-Peshkin et al. (2015)) and the use of which is therefore constrained by habitat characteristics. Thus, vocalizations might be particularly well suited for the propagation of local information through large groups, especially if individuals are spread beyond an individual’s visual range in dense habitats.
A range of studies have suggested that some vocalizations function in the context of maintaining group cohesion, though without addressing the specific mechanisms by which cohesion is regulated on an individual level (e.g., Janik & Slater, 1998; da Cunha & Byrne, 2009; Mumm, Urrutia & Knörnschild, 2014). However, simply demonstrating a relationship between vocal rates and behavioural markers of cohesion falls short of elucidating the mechanisms by which such signals function in group cohesion (Palombit, 1992; da Cunha & Byrne, 2009). In our opinion, it is crucial to address changes in addition to states of individual behaviour, i.e., investigating the temporal dynamics of movement behaviour. This approach highlights the spatio-temporal quality of how group cohesion can be achieved by individuals moving in space and time.
In this study, we investigated to what extent vocal behaviour plays a role in regulating individual movements during travel, beyond travel initiations. Crucially, we addressed not only the relationship between current vocal behaviour and current movement patterns, but also whether current vocal behaviour predicted future movement, i.e., changes in movement. We conducted our study on sooty mangabeys (Cercocebus atys), a forest dwelling, terrestrial primate species that lives in large groups of up to 100 individuals (McGraw, Zuberbühler & Noë, 2007). Sooty mangabey foraging behaviour mainly consists of individuals searching for edible items on the forest floor (McGraw, Zuberbühler & Noë, 2007). As a result, their movements are rarely in the form of distinct travel bouts from one resource to the next but are characterised by continuous movements with variable speed in a general direction. Within this general direction, individuals follow their own paths, which often differ from the general direction of the group.
Our focus in this study is on describing these spatio-temporal movement patterns during foraging and relate them to vocal production. Particularly, we studied the relationships between individual movement speed and direction and (1) vocal rates of focal animals and (2) vocal rates of individuals in the focal animal’s immediate vicinity (subgroup). We focused on the two most common mangabey vocalizations, grunts and twitters, whose exact communicative functions are currently unknown, although they are predominantly given during foraging (Range & Fischer, 2004). Preliminary observations have suggested that both call types play a role in group coordination. All other vocalizations were also recorded but then pooled into an ‘other vocalizations’ category.
Sooty mangabeys frequently form associations with other, arboreal primate species, mostly Diana monkeys, Cercopithecus diana and red colobus, Procolobus badius, (McGraw, Zuberbühler & Noë, 2007), presumably to increase predator detection (Bergmüller, 1998; McGraw & Bshary, 2002; Heymann, 2011), which enabled us to test whether movement patterns were further influenced by vocal rates of other species in addition to their mere presence.
Methods
We observed adult individuals in a free-ranging, habituated group of sooty mangabeys of approximately 90 individuals at Taï National Park in Côte d’Ivoire (Range & Fischer, 2004; Janmaat, Byrne & Zuberbühler, 2006; McGraw, Zuberbühler & Noë, 2007). Our study group has been subject to other research projects in the past and all adult individuals are identifiable based on physical characteristics. During focal animal follows, we used a combination of all-occurrence and instantaneous sampling (Altmann, 1974). Data on the focal individual’s vocalizations (grunts, twitters, other vocalizations) were collected continuously. In two-minute intervals, we noted the behaviour of the focal individuals and the number of individuals present within 10 m. We refer to the individuals within 10 m as the focal individual’s subgroup, which for the purpose of our study is considered a proxy for the focal animal’s immediate social environment. Subgroup size ranged from 0 to 17 individuals (median = 4), excluding the focal individual. We did not record the age and sex composition of the subgroup.
Every 10 min, we noted the number of other primate species associated with the mangabey group. Another species was associated if we detected the presence of at least one individual of another species within 50 m from the focal individual (McGraw & Bshary, 2002). GPS coordinates were recorded automatically every 30 s with a Garmin Rhino 650 unit. We continuously recorded the soundscape around the focal individual (Sennheiser MKH-416 microphone, Marantz PMD660 recorder). From these audio recordings, we counted the number of monkey vocalizations audible to the coder and assigned them to either mangabeys (distinguishing grunts, twitters and others) or any of the associated primate species to calculate rates of vocalizations. Classification of vocalizations was done by ear from the audio recordings, based on descriptions in Range & Fischer (2004) and with the help of experienced local field assistants, who have worked in the study area for years. Vocalizations of the focal individuals were tagged during focal follows by giving spoken comments onto the recording. Rates of mangabey subgroup vocalizations in the soundscape were positively correlated with subgroup size (grunt: r = 0.22; twitter: r = 0.20; other: r = 0.34), but model diagnostics suggested that this was not problematic (see ESM). Hence, we refer to these vocalization rates in the soundscape as subgroup grunts, subgroup twitters and subgroup other vocalizations.
We used linear mixed models to address our questions. We created time-blocks of five minutes, for which we established the distance covered by the focal animal as response variable (numeric, hereafter: speed, i.e., distance covered per 5-min time-block). As our major predictor variables of interest we used whether or not the focal animal produced at least one grunt or other vocalization (binary), vocal rates of mangabeys as audible in the soundscape around the focal individual (grunts, twitters and other vocalizations, excluding the focal animal’s vocalizations, all numeric) and the rate of primate, non-mangabey vocalizations (numeric). In addition, we controlled for a number of variables that might influence movement speed, i.e., the average number of individuals within 10 m of the focal animal (numeric, hereafter: subgroup size), the sex of the focal individual (binary), and the number of associated primate species (numeric). Vocalizations of focal animals were too infrequent to allow calculating meaningful calling rates and hence were coded binary. For the same reason, we could not include twitter production of focal individuals as a predictor variable as no focal animal produced a twitter during the focal follows. We did not consider behaviour/activity as variable in our models, because pure travel behaviour was rare (2.3% of activity budget, as opposed to feeding, foraging, resting and socialising, see ESM for details). We incorporated an auto-correlation term to control for temporal dependence of data points (Fürtbauer et al., 2011) and fitted individual ID and calendar date as random intercepts.
Change points, i.e. points in space and time at which individuals modified the general direction into which they moved, were assessed following procedures described by Byrne et al. (2009). In brief, the change point test decomposes an individual track into smaller segments and examines whether a given track segment is aligned with systematically varied numbers of segments before and after it (Byrne et al., 2009). A more detailed description of the method can be found in Byrne et al. (2009) and examples of its application are Asensio et al. (2011), Janmaat, Ban & Boesch (2013) and Noser & Byrne (2014). We used the following parameters to calculate change points: q = 6, α = 0.05, N = 1,000 and a tolerance of 0.00002.
Our modelling approach was two-fold. First, we aimed to describe the co-variation between speed and the predictor variables within the same 5-min time blocks. Second, within a given time block we used our variables to predict speed in the following time block, i.e. future speed, while controlling for current speed. We followed the same approach for modelling probabilities of direction changes (presence or absence of ‘change points’) in current and future time blocks. Table 1 summaries the design. Our sample comprised 16 individuals (11 females, 5 males), encompassing 175 5-min data points totalling 14.6 h of focal observations (range: 0.25–1.75 h per individual).
Table 1. Outline of analysis strategy and summary of results.
Speed | Change points | |
---|---|---|
Current time block | χ142 = 36.59 | χ142 = 20.35 |
p = 0.0009 | p = 0.1194 | |
Future time block | χ142 = 26.60 | χ142 = 23.36 |
p = 0.0217 | p = 0.0546 |
All models were built in R 3.1.1 (R Core Team, 2014) with the lmer and glmer functions in the lme4 package (v. 1.1.11, Bates et al., 2015). Statistical significance was established using likelihood ratio tests (LRT, Dobson, 2002) comparing full models with their respective null model. These null models contained the same random effects as the full models and sex as fixed effect. Depending on the model, we also included the auto-correlation term and speed or direction change in the previous time block as additional terms in the null models. R2 values were computed following Nakagawa & Schielzeth (2013) using the MuMIn package (v.1.15.6, Bartoń, 2016). We tested several two-way interactions in each model, which were retained only if they improved model fit as determined by LRTs and were otherwise removed to allow interpretation of main effects (Mundry, 2011; Hector, von Felten & Schmid, 2010). Specifically, we included interactions between subgroup size and vocalizations of mangabeys (both focal individuals’ (grunt, other) and subgroups’ (grunt, twitter, other)), reasoning that effects of vocalizations may differ according to the number of individuals in proximity of the focal individual. We also included the two-way interaction between number of associated species and calling rate of associated species. We report and interpret results only for the major test predictor variables concerning vocal behaviour. Where appropriate, this includes reporting of interactions. More details on methods, analyses and checks of model assumptions can be found in the ESM.
This study was entirely observational and adhered to the legal requirements of Côte d’Ivoire and Switzerland, as well as to the Animal Behavior Society Guidelines for the Use of Animals in Research. Research permissions were granted by the Ministère de la Recherche Scientifique et Technique of Côte d’Ivoire.
Results
Travel speed
Both models for current and future travel speed were different from their respective null models (current: χ142 = 36.59, p = 0.0009, Rm2 = 0.36; future: χ142 = 26.60, p = 0.0217, Rm2 = 0.34; LRT; Table 1; full model results in Tables 2 and 3).
Table 2. Results of LMM testing variation in current speed.
Full model | Final model | LRT | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β ± se | t | β ± se | t | χ12 | p | |
Intercept | −0.35 ± 1.27 | −0.28 | −0.31 ± 1.27 | −0.25 | ||
Subgroup size | −0.07 ± 0.05 | −1.32 | −0.05 ± 0.05 | −0.99 | ||
Focal grunt (yes) | −0.15 ± 0.11 | −1.44 | −0.14 ± 0.11 | −1.33 | 1.76 | 0.1844 |
Focal other vocalization (yes) | −0.03 ± 0.13 | −0.19 | −0.05 ± 0.13 | −0.37 | 0.14 | 0.7107 |
Subgroup grunt | −0.07 ± 0.05 | −1.44 | −0.07 ± 0.05 | −1.38 | 1.90 | 0.1680 |
Subgroup other | 0.11 ± 0.05 | 2.45 | 0.10 ± 0.05 | 2.16 | ||
Subgroup twitter | −0.12 ± 0.05 | −2.28 | −0.11 ± 0.05 | −2.15 | 4.53 | 0.0333 |
Number of associated species | −0.03 ± 0.08 | −0.43 | −0.00 ± 0.08 | −0.06 | ||
Vocal rate of other species | −0.01 ± 0.05 | −0.19 | −0.03 ± 0.05 | −0.53 | ||
Sex (male) | 4.03 ± 2.27 | 1.78 | 3.99 ± 2.27 | 1.75 | 2.87 | 0.0901 |
Auto-correlation | −2.60 ± 0.14 | −18.53 | −2.61 ± 0.14 | −18.48 | 113.99 | 0.0000 |
IA subgroup size: focal grunt | 0.19 ± 0.11 | 1.68 | ||||
IA subgroup size: focal other | −0.23 ± 0.18 | −1.29 | ||||
IA subgroup size: subgroup grunt | 0.03 ± 0.05 | 0.56 | ||||
IA subgroup size: subgroup other | −0.16 ± 0.04 | −3.66 | −0.14 ± 0.04 | −3.41 | 11.18 | 0.0008 |
IA subgroup size: subgroup twitter | 0.03 ± 0.05 | 0.74 | ||||
IA associated species: vocal rate of other species | 0.10 ± 0.05 | 2.02 | 0.10 ± 0.05 | 2.02 | 4.01 | 0.0453 |
Note:
Reference levels of categorical variables are: focal grunt = ‘no,’ focal other = ‘no,’ and sex = ‘female.’ Test levels are given in parentheses. LRT, likelihood ratio test, IA, interaction.
Table 3. Results of LMM testing variation in future speed.
Full model | Final model | LRT | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β ± se | t | β ± se | t | χ12 | p | |
Intercept | 0.16 ± 0.71 | 0.22 | 0.20 ± 0.69 | 0.28 | ||
Subgroup size | −0.01 ± 0.07 | −0.12 | −0.03 ± 0.06 | −0.53 | ||
Focal grunt (yes) | −0.15 ± 0.14 | −1.05 | −0.11 ± 0.14 | −0.74 | 0.55 | 0.4575 |
Focal other vocalization (yes) | −0.31 ± 0.17 | −1.77 | −0.28 ± 0.17 | −1.65 | 2.70 | 0.1007 |
Subgroup grunt | 0.16 ± 0.06 | 2.63 | 0.15 ± 0.06 | 2.36 | ||
Subgroup other | −0.15 ± 0.06 | −2.45 | −0.15 ± 0.06 | −2.50 | 6.06 | 0.0139 |
Subgroup twitter | −0.02 ± 0.07 | −0.32 | −0.03 ± 0.06 | −0.41 | 0.16 | 0.6855 |
Number of associated species | −0.05 ± 0.10 | −0.49 | −0.05 ± 0.10 | −0.49 | 0.22 | 0.6354 |
Vocal rate of other species | 0.01 ± 0.07 | 0.07 | −0.01 ± 0.07 | −0.21 | 0.04 | 0.8378 |
Sex (male) | 0.67 ± 1.26 | 0.53 | 0.52 ± 1.24 | 0.42 | 0.18 | 0.6747 |
Control speed | 0.00 ± 0.06 | 0.02 | 0.02 ± 0.06 | 0.33 | 0.10 | 0.7461 |
Auto-correlation | −1.60 ± 0.13 | −12.76 | −1.60 ± 0.13 | −12.58 | 61.20 | 0.0000 |
IA subgroup size: focal grunt | −0.12 ± 0.15 | −0.79 | ||||
IA subgroup size: focal other | 0.00 ± 0.23 | 0.01 | ||||
IA subgroup size: subgroup grunt | −0.09 ± 0.06 | −1.41 | −0.13 ± 0.06 | −2.35 | 5.39 | 0.0203 |
IA subgroup size: subgroup other | −0.10 ± 0.06 | −1.70 | ||||
IA subgroup size: subgroup twitter | −0.02 ± 0.06 | −0.33 | ||||
IA associated species: vocal rate of other species | 0.08 ± 0.06 | 1.37 |
Note:
Reference levels of categorical variables are: focal grunt = ‘no,’ focal other = ‘no,’ and sex = ‘female.’ Test levels are given in parentheses. LRT, likelihood ratio test, IA, interaction.
Current travel speed
We found no strong relationships between vocalizations of focal animals and their current travel speed (grunts: β ± se = −0.142 ± 0.107, t = −1.332; other vocalizations: β ± se = −0.049 ± 0.132, t = −0.372; twitters: not tested, see methods).
In contrast, high rates of twitters in the soundscape (subgroup twitters) were associated with low current speed of the focal individual (β ± se = −0.108 ± 0.051, t = −2.146, Fig. 1). We found no such significant effect for subgroup grunts (β ± se = −0.068 ± 0.049, t = −1.383). For other subgroup vocalizations the effect of calling rate was mediated by subgroup size (interaction: β ± se = −0.140 ± 0.041, t = −3.410, Fig. 2), insofar as in smaller subgroups, higher call rates were associated with higher current speed, while the opposite was the case for larger subgroups.
Finally, the calling rate of other primate species also influenced current speed and this was mediated by the number of associated species (β ± se = 0.101 ± 0.050, t = 2.021): with fewer associated species, higher call rates were associated with lower current speed, while the opposite was the case for larger poly-specific groups consisting of many species (Fig. 3).
Future travel speed
As with current speed, we found no statistically significant effects of focal animal vocalizations on future travel speed (grunts: β ± se = −0.107 ± 0.143, t = −0.744; other vocalizations: β ± se = −0.280 ± 0.169, t = −1.652; twitters: not tested, see methods).
Focal individuals decreased future speed (i.e. travelled slower in the future) if the subgroup produced more other vocalizations (β ± se = −0.154 ± 0.062, t = −2.497, Fig. 4). Subgroup grunt rate was also associated with future speed, but this relationship depended on subgroup size (interaction: β ± se = −0.130 ± 0.055, t = −2.349, Fig. 5): in small subgroups, individuals travelled faster in the future if the subgroup produced grunts at higher rates, while the opposite was found for large subgroups. There was no statistically significant effect of subgroup twitter rate on future speed (β ± se = −0.026 ± 0.064, t = −0.406).
The calling rate of other primate species appeared to have no pronounced effect on future travel speed of focal individuals (β ± se = −0.014 ± 0.068, t = −0.208).
Direction changes
Regarding changes in direction, neither the ‘current direction changes’ nor the ‘future direction changes’ full model was significant at α = 0.05 (current: χ142 = 20.35, p = 0.1194, Rm2 = 0.28; future: χ142 = 23.36, p = 0.0546, Rm2 = 0.30; Table 1; full model results in Tables 4 and 5). Given the low p value of the future model, we continued to explore this model.
Table 4. Results of GLMM testing variation in current direction changes.
β ± se | z | |
---|---|---|
Intercept | −1.68 ± 0.34 | −4.89 |
Subgroup size | −0.78 ± 0.31 | −2.54 |
Focal grunt (yes) | −0.20 ± 0.49 | −0.41 |
Focal other vocalization (yes) | 0.97 ± 0.65 | 1.49 |
Subgroup grunt | 0.01 ± 0.26 | 0.05 |
Subgroup other | 0.02 ± 0.25 | 0.07 |
Subgroup twitter | 0.14 ± 0.27 | 0.51 |
Number of associated species | −0.14 ± 0.31 | −0.45 |
Vocal rate of other species | 0.32 ± 0.27 | 1.19 |
Sex (male) | 0.05 ± 0.61 | 0.08 |
IA subgroup size: focal grunt | 0.22 ± 0.60 | 0.36 |
IA subgroup size: focal other | 2.34 ± 1.04 | 2.24 |
IA subgroup size: subgroup grunt | 0.04 ± 0.30 | 0.12 |
IA subgroup size: subgroup other | −0.56 ± 0.28 | −1.98 |
IA subgroup size: subgroup twitter | −0.08 ± 0.30 | −0.26 |
IA associated species: vocal rate of other species | 0.36 ± 0.20 | 1.77 |
Note:
Reference levels of categorical variables are: focal grunt = ‘no,’ focal other = ‘no,’ and sex = ‘female.’ Test levels are given in parentheses. IA, interaction.
Table 5. Results of GLMM testing variation in future direction changes.
Full model | Final model | LRT | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β ± se | z | β ± se | z | χ21 | p | |
Intercept | −1.80 ± 0.38 | −4.72 | −1.76 ± 0.36 | −4.85 | ||
Subgroup size | −0.30 ± 0.33 | −0.91 | −0.43 ± 0.25 | −1.69 | ||
Focal grunt (yes) | 0.26 ± 0.52 | 0.51 | 0.46 ± 0.48 | 0.95 | 0.89 | 0.3444 |
Focal other vocalization (yes) | 0.79 ± 0.72 | 1.10 | 0.50 ± 0.63 | 0.79 | 0.61 | 0.4367 |
Subgroup grunt | −0.03 ± 0.28 | −0.11 | −0.10 ± 0.24 | −0.40 | 0.16 | 0.6917 |
Subgroup other | −0.40 ± 0.27 | −1.48 | −0.32 ± 0.25 | −1.30 | ||
Subgroup twitter | 0.51 ± 0.30 | 1.69 | 0.49 ± 0.25 | 1.94 | 3.86 | 0.0493 |
Number of associated species | −0.21 ± 0.28 | −0.75 | −0.31 ± 0.25 | −1.22 | 1.48 | 0.2238 |
Vocal rate of other species | 0.15 ± 0.28 | 0.54 | −0.03 ± 0.23 | −0.12 | 0.01 | 0.9064 |
Sex (male) | 0.53 ± 0.63 | 0.84 | 0.71 ± 0.61 | 1.17 | 1.36 | 0.2437 |
Control change point | 1.19 ± 0.51 | 2.33 | 1.31 ± 0.48 | 2.76 | 7.50 | 0.0062 |
IA subgroup size: focal grunt | −1.25 ± 0.68 | −1.84 | ||||
IA subgroup size: focal other | 1.46 ± 0.93 | 1.58 | ||||
IA subgroup size: subgroup grunt | 0.17 ± 0.28 | 0.61 | ||||
IA subgroup size: subgroup other | −0.79 ± 0.34 | −2.28 | −0.56 ± 0.29 | −1.94 | 4.44 | 0.0352 |
IA subgroup size: subgroup twitter | 0.04 ± 0.33 | 0.11 | ||||
IA associated species: vocal rate of other species | 0.33 ± 0.21 | 1.57 |
Note:
Reference levels of categorical variables are: focal grunt = ‘no,’ focal other = ‘no,’ and sex = ‘female.’ LRT, likelihood ratio test, IA, interaction.
Future direction changes
Focal animals’ vocalizations did not significantly predict the probability of a change in future travel direction (grunts: β ± se = 0.458 ± 0.482, z = 0.950; other vocalizations: β ± se = 0.499 ± 0.635, z = 0.786; twitters: not tested, see methods).
Individuals were more likely to change direction in the future if the rate of twitters in the subgroup increased (β ± se = 0.494 ± 0.255, z = 1.941, Fig. 6). The probability of an individual changing direction in the future also depended on the interaction between subgroup size and rate of other vocalizations in the subgroup (interaction: β ± se = −0.559 ± 0.288, z = −1.944, Fig. 7). In smaller subgroups, future direction changes were more likely with high rates of vocalizations compared to low vocalization rates. In larger subgroups, this pattern is reversed, such that direction changes in the future were more likely with low vocalization rates compared to high vocalization rates. There was no statistically significant effect of subgroup grunt rate on the probability of changing direction in the future (β ± se = −0.095 ± 0.241, t = −0.396).
As in the model of future speed, the calling rate of other primate species appeared to not have a pronounced effect on the probability of focal individuals changing direction in the future (calling rate of other species: β ± se = −0.027 ± 0.228, t = −0.118).
Discussion
Our results indicate that the travel speed and changes in direction of focal individuals co-varied with complex interactions of conspecific and heterospecific vocalizations. Notably, our results indicate that individual movement patterns were largely independent of the focal animal’s own vocal behaviour. In contrast, we found effects of the subgroup’s collective vocal behaviour and vocal rates of associated primate species, which were related to individual movement patterns.
Interestingly, differences in vocal rates were often not directly linked with differences in travel speed and direction changes, but were mediated by the social environment, i.e. subgroup size. The only vocal predictor of future speed that was independent of social factors was how many ‘other’ vocalizations were produced in the subgroup, which generally slowed down individuals. Most likely, this was a consequence of high rates of vocalizations produced during important social interactions, such as aggression and mating, which tend to take place while animals remain stationary.
Our results also suggest that the rate of twitters in the soundscape around the focal animal predicted whether or not this individual changed direction: individuals were more likely to change direction if twitters were more frequent as compared to when twitters were rare. Whether or not these direction changes led individuals towards the source of twitters is hitherto unknown and we do not have information about where these twitters originated from the focal individual’s perspective. Given the proposed function of twitters in foraging (Range & Fischer, 2004), it would be interesting to see whether these calls serve as food calls similar to those described in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), for example (Schel et al., 2013). In chimpanzees, it is thought that such food calls attract individuals to valuable food sources, presumably as a means to establish or maintain social bonds with group members (Schel et al. (2013), see also Clay, Smith & Blumstein (2012) for a review on alternative functions of food calls).
Similar to our results on future speed, we found that the effect of the subgroup’s ‘other vocalizations’ rate on the probability of changing future direction was modulated by subgroup size. As with changes in speed, it is most likely that these effects are a consequence of relevant social interactions nearby, for which ‘other vocalizations’ may be indicators and which may subsequently trigger changes in direction of focal individuals. Note, however, that our results on direction changes did not reach the conventional level of statistical significance and therefore have to be interpreted with caution.
A major focus of studies on animal travel is to look at how group movements are initiated and how cohesion is maintained (Boinski & Garber, 2000; Fichtel & Manser, 2010), yet close to nothing is known about how group movement and cohesion are regulated once individuals are on their way. While it is known that vocalizations can play a role in group cohesion (e.g., Robinson, 1981; Cheney, Seyfarth & Palombit, 1996; Fischer et al., 2001; Trillmich, Fichtel & Kappeler, 2004; Braune, Schmidt & Zimmermann, 2005; Ramos-Fernández, 2005) the results of our study suggest that individual movement patterns, i.e., changes in speed and direction, need to be addressed as a potential proximate mechanism as to how groups, which are made up of individuals, achieve and regulate cohesion (see also da Cunha & Byrne, 2009).
We propose that acceleration and deceleration of movements and adjustment of direction–in addition to initiating–is a domain that requires communication and our current findings support this view. In fact, fine-tuned regulation of group movements may be a common, hitherto largely overlooked, mechanism that is crucial in many group living species that depend on cohesion and occupy large home ranges. As such, vocally mediated movement regulation may be the default mechanism on a continuous scale with the more conspicuous initiation and termination of movements at the extremes.
Our hypothesis is that species that are constantly on the move, such as sooty mangabeys and other scramble foragers, may benefit specifically from a communication system that enables individuals to continuously regulate group movements as opposed to species for which group movements occur in discrete bouts for example to exploit clumped food resources (Kinnaird & O’Brien, 2000).
We might even expect that within-species variation exists as to what a coordination signal may mean. For example, frugivorous species are likely to travel in bouts during periods of high fruit availability and switch to continuous travelling in periods when food sources are dispersed, which is likely to exert different evolutionary pressures on a communication system. Sooty mangabeys exhibit such flexibility in their feeding ecology (Bergmüller, 1998, see also Janmaat, Byrne & Zuberbühler (2006)). Our data collection took place when food sources were dispersed, and it will be interesting to see how movements in this species are coordinated when resources are clumped (Janmaat, Byrne & Zuberbühler, 2006).
Our results also indicate that associations with other primate species and their vocalizations influence mangabey movements. Poly-specific associations among Taï monkeys are common (McGraw, Zuberbühler & Noë, 2007), offering mutually increased predator detection (Bergmüller, 1998; McGraw & Bshary, 2002). The cost/benefit ratio of these associations is high, given that they do not lead to increased food competition. Yet again, most data on communicative mechanisms so far demonstrate how associations are formed, rather than maintained and regulated (Heymann, 2011). Our results suggest that mangabeys adapt and coordinate their movements according to the presence and vocalizations of other primate species. These results suggest that the underlying communicative mechanisms of interspecific movement coordination may be more complex than previously thought.
In sum, our results represent an example of a potential vocal mechanism by which movements of individuals can be influenced and, by extension, groups can be coordinated. Our results suggest a complex picture of how movement patterns and vocalizations, both within and between species, are interlinked and mediated by the immediate social environment. Future playback experiments will elucidate whether the relationships we suggest are indeed of a causal nature.
Supplemental Information
Acknowledgments
We thank Richard Peho and Patterson for assistance in data collection, Anderson Bitty for logistic support and Julie Duboscq and the members of the Comparative Cognition Lab for discussions. We also wish to thank two reviewers and the editor for their constructive comments.
Funding Statement
This study was funded by the European Research Council (FP7/2007–2013, grant number 283871). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Additional Information and Declarations
Competing Interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Author Contributions
Christof Neumann conceived and designed the experiments, performed the experiments, analyzed the data, contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools, wrote the paper, prepared figures and/or tables, reviewed drafts of the paper.
Klaus Zuberbühler conceived and designed the experiments, contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools, wrote the paper, reviewed drafts of the paper.
Animal Ethics
The following information was supplied relating to ethical approvals (i.e., approving body and any reference numbers):
This study was entirely observational and adhered to the legal requirements of Côte d’Ivoire and Switzerland, as well as to the Animal Behavior Society Guidelines for the Use of Animals in Research.
Field Study Permissions
The following information was supplied relating to field study approvals (i.e., approving body and any reference numbers):
Research permissions were granted by the Ministère de la Recherche Scientifique et Technique of Côte d’Ivoire.
Data Deposition
The following information was supplied regarding data availability:
The raw data has been supplied as Supplemental Dataset Files.
References
- Altizer et al. (2003).Altizer S, Nunn CL, Thrall PH, Gittleman JL, Antonovics J, Cunningham AA, Dobson AP, Ezenwa V, Jones KE, Pedersen AB, Poss M, Pulliam JRC. Social organization and parasite risk in mammals: integrating theory and empirical studies. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics. 2003;34(1):517–547. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.34.030102.151725. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Altmann (1974).Altmann J. Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour. 1974;49(3):227–266. doi: 10.1163/156853974X00534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asensio et al. (2011).Asensio N, Brockelman WY, Malaivijitnond S, Reichard UH. Gibbon travel paths are goal oriented. Animal Cognition. 2011;14(3):395–405. doi: 10.1007/s10071-010-0374-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bartoń (2016).Bartoń K. MuMIn: Multi-Model Inference. R package version 1.15.6http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=MuMIn 2016
- Bates et al. (2015).Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software. 2015;67(1):1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bergmüller (1998).Bergmüller R. Nahrungsökologie der Rauchgrauen Mangabe (Cercocebus Torquatus Atys) [[Feeding Ecology of Sooty Mangabeys]]. Nürnberg: Diplomarbeit, Friedrich-Alexander-Universität; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Black (1988).Black JM. Preflight signalling in swans: a mechanism for group cohesion and flock formation. Ethology. 1988;79(2):143–157. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1988.tb00707.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Boinski & Garber (2000).Boinski S, Garber PA, editors. On the Move: How and Why Animals Travel in Groups. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Bousquet, Sumpter & Manser (2011).Bousquet CAH, Sumpter DJT, Manser MB. Moving calls: a vocal mechanism underlying quorum decisions in cohesive groups. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2011;278(1711):1482–1488. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.1739. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braune, Schmidt & Zimmermann (2005).Braune P, Schmidt S, Zimmermann E. Spacing and group coordination in a nocturnal primate, the golden brown mouse lemur (Microcebus ravelobensis): the role of olfactory and acoustic signals. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2005;58(6):587–596. doi: 10.1007/s00265-005-0944-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Byrne et al. (2009).Byrne RW, Noser R, Bates LA, Jupp PE. How did they get here from there? Detecting changes of direction in terrestrial ranging. Animal Behaviour. 2009;77(3):619–631. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.11.014. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cheney, Seyfarth & Palombit (1996).Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Palombit R. The function and mechanisms underlying baboon ‘contact’ barks. Animal Behaviour. 1996;52(3):507–518. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1996.0193. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Clay, Smith & Blumstein (2012).Clay Z, Smith CL, Blumstein DT. Food-associated vocalizations in mammals and birds: What do these calls really mean? Animal Behaviour. 2012;83(2):323–330. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.12.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Conradt & Roper (2005).Conradt L, Roper TJ. Consensus decision making in animals. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2005;20(8):449–456. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2005.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Couzin et al. (2005).Couzin ID, Krause J, Franks NR, Levin SA. Effective leadership and decision-making in animal groups on the move. Nature. 2005;433(7025):513–516. doi: 10.1038/nature03236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- da Cunha & Byrne (2009).da Cunha RGT, Byrne RW. The use of vocal communication in keeping the spatial cohesion of groups: intentionality and specific functions. In: Garber PA, Estrada A, Bicca-Marques JC, Heymann EW, Strier KB, editors. South American Primates. New York: Springer; 2009. pp. 341–363. (Developments in Primatology: Progress and Prospects). [Google Scholar]
- Dobson (2002).Dobson AJ. An Introduction to Generalized Linear Models. 2nd edition. Boca Raton: Chapman and Hall; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Elgar (1989).Elgar MA. Predator vigilance and group size in mammals and birds: a critical review of the empirical evidence. Biological Reviews. 1989;64(1):13–33. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.1989.tb00636.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fichtel & Manser (2010).Fichtel C, Manser M. Vocal communication in social groups. In: Kappeler PM, editor. Animal Behaviour: Evolution and Mechanisms. Heidelberg: Springer; 2010. pp. 29–54. [Google Scholar]
- Fischer et al. (2001).Fischer J, Hammerschmidt K, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM. Acoustic features of female chacma baboon barks. Ethology. 2001;107(1):33–54. doi: 10.1046/j.1439-0310.2001.00630.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fischer & Zinner (2011a).Fischer J, Zinner D. Communication and cognition in primate group movement. International Journal of Primatology. 2011a;32(6):1279–1295. doi: 10.1007/s10764-011-9542-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischer & Zinner (2011b).Fischer J, Zinner D. Communicative and cognitive underpinnings of animal group movement. In: Boos M, Kolbe M, Kappeler PM, Ellwart T, editors. Coordination in Human and Primate Groups. Heidelberg: Springer; 2011b. pp. 229–244. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fletcher (2007).Fletcher LE. Vibrational signals in a gregarious sawfly larva (Perga affinis): group coordination or competitive signaling? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2007;61(12):1809–1821. doi: 10.1007/s00265-007-0414-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fürtbauer et al. (2011).Fürtbauer I, Mundry R, Heistermann M, Schülke O, Ostner J. You mate, I mate: macaque females synchronize sex not cycles. PLoS ONE. 2011;6(10):e2298. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0026144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hector, von Felten & Schmid (2010).Hector A, von Felten S, Schmid B. Analysis of variance with unbalanced data: an update for ecology and evolution. Journal of Animal Ecology. 2010;79(2):308–316. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2009.01634.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heymann (2011).Heymann EW. Coordination in primate mixed-species groups. In: Boos M, Kolbe M, Kappeler PM, Ellwart T, editors. Coordination in Human and Primate Groups. Heidelberg: Springer; 2011. pp. 263–281. [Google Scholar]
- Janik & Slater (1998).Janik VM, Slater PJB. Context-specific use suggests that bottlenose dolphin signature whistles are cohesion calls. Animal Behaviour. 1998;56(4):829–838. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1998.0881. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Janmaat, Ban & Boesch (2013).Janmaat KRL, Ban SD, Boesch C. Chimpanzees use long-term spatial memory to monitor large fruit trees and remember feeding experiences across seasons. Animal Behaviour. 2013;86(6):1183–1205. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.09.021. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Janmaat, Byrne & Zuberbühler (2006).Janmaat KRL, Byrne RW, Zuberbühler K. Evidence for a spatial memory of fruiting states of rainforest trees in wild mangabeys. Animal Behaviour. 2006;72(4):797–807. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.12.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kinnaird & O’Brien (2000).Kinnaird MF, O’Brien TG. Comparative movement patterns of two semiterrestrial cercopithecine primates: the Tana River crested mangabey and Sulawesi crested black macaque. In: Boinski S, Garber PA, editors. On the Move. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2000. pp. 327–350. [Google Scholar]
- Krause & Ruxton (2002).Krause J, Ruxton GD. Living in Groups. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Majolo, de Bortoli Vizioli & Schino (2008).Majolo B, de Bortoli Vizioli A, Schino G. Costs and benefits of group living in primates: group size effects on behaviour and demography. Animal Behaviour. 2008;76(4):1235–1247. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.06.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McGraw & Bshary (2002).McGraw WS, Bshary R. Association of terrestrial mangabeys (Cercocebus atys) with arboreal monkeys: experimental evidence for the effects of reduced ground predator pressure on habitat use. International Journal of Primatology. 2002;23(2):311–325. doi: 10.1023/A:1013883528244. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McGraw, Zuberbühler & Noë (2007).McGraw WS, Zuberbühler K, Noë R. Monkeys of the Taï Forest: An African Primate Community. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Mumm, Urrutia & Knörnschild (2014).Mumm CAS, Urrutia MC, Knörnschild M. Vocal individuality in cohesion calls of giant otters, Pteronura brasiliensis. Animal Behaviour. 2014;88:243–252. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.12.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mundry (2011).Mundry R. Issues in information theory-based statistical inference–a commentary from a frequentist’s perspective. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2011;65(1):57–68. doi: 10.1007/s00265-010-1040-y. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Nakagawa & Schielzeth (2013).Nakagawa S, Schielzeth H. A general and simple method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods in Ecology and Evolution. 2013;4(2):133–142. doi: 10.1111/j.2041-210x.2012.00261.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Noser & Byrne (2014).Noser R, Byrne RW. Change point analysis of travel routes reveals novel insights into foraging strategies and cognitive maps of wild baboons. American Journal of Primatology. 2014;76(5):399–409. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palombit (1992).Palombit RA. A preliminary study of vocal communication in wild long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). II. Potential of calls to regulate intragroup spacing. International Journal of Primatology. 1992;13(2):183–207. doi: 10.1007/BF02547840. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Petit & Bon (2010).Petit O, Bon R. Decision-making processes: the case of collective movements. Behavioural Processes. 2010;84(3):635–647. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poole et al. (1988).Poole JH, Payne K, Langbauer WR, Jr, Moss CJ. The social contexts of some very low frequency calls of African elephants. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1988;22(6):385–392. doi: 10.1007/BF00294975. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team (2014).R Core Team . R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Radford (2004).Radford AN. Vocal coordination of group movement by green woodhoopoes (Phoeniculus purpureus) Ethology. 2004;110(1):11–20. doi: 10.1046/j.1439-0310.2003.00943.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ramos-Fernández (2005).Ramos-Fernández G. Vocal communication in a fission-fusion society: do spider monkeys stay in touch with close associates? International Journal of Primatology. 2005;26(5):1077–1092. doi: 10.1007/s10764-005-6459-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Range & Fischer (2004).Range F, Fischer J. Vocal repertoire of sooty mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus atys) in the Taï National Park. Ethology. 2004;110(4):301–321. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2004.00973.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson (1981).Robinson JG. Vocal regulation of inter- and intragroup spacing during boundary encounters in the titi monkey, Callicebus moloch. Primates. 1981;22(2):161–172. doi: 10.1007/BF02382607. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Schel et al. (2013).Schel AM, Machanda Z, Townsend SW, Zuberbühler K, Slocombe KE. Chimpanzee food calls are directed at specific individuals. Animal Behaviour. 2013;86(5):955–965. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.08.013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sterck, Watts & van Schaik (1997).Sterck EHM, Watts DP, van Schaik CP. The evolution of female social relationships in nonhuman primates. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1997;41(5):291–309. doi: 10.1007/s002650050390. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stewart & Harcourt (1994).Stewart KJ, Harcourt AH. Gorillas’ vocalizations during rest periods: signals of impending departure? Behaviour. 1994;130(1):29–40. doi: 10.1163/156853994X00127. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Strandburg-Peshkin et al. (2015).Strandburg-Peshkin A, Farine DR, Couzin ID, Crofoot MC. Shared decision-making drives collective movement in wild baboons. Science. 2015;348(6241):1358–1361. doi: 10.1126/science.aaa5099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trillmich, Fichtel & Kappeler (2004).Trillmich J, Fichtel C, Kappeler PM. Coordination of group movements in wild Verreaux’s sifakas (Propithecus verreauxi) Behaviour. 2004;141(9):1103–1120. doi: 10.1163/1568539042664579. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.