Abstract
Background
Insufficient sleep among school-aged children is a growing concern, as numerous studies have shown that chronic short sleep duration increases the risk of poor academic performance and specific adverse health outcomes. We examined the association between weekday nighttime sleep duration and 3 types of screen exposure: television, computer use, and video gaming.
Methods
We used age 9 data from an ethnically diverse national birth cohort study, the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study, to assess the association between screen time and sleep duration among 9-year-olds, using screen time data reported by both the child (n = 3269) and by the child's primary caregiver (n= 2770).
Results
Within the child-reported models, children who watched more than 2 hours of television per day had shorter sleep duration by approximately 11 minutes per night compared to those who watched less than 2 hours of television (β = −0.18; P < .001). Using the caregiver-reported models, both television and computer use were associated with reduced sleep duration. For both child- and parent-reported screen time measures, we did not find statistically significant differences in effect size across various types of screen time.
Conclusions
Screen time from televisions and computers is associated with reduced sleep duration among 9-year-olds, using 2 sources of estimates of screen time exposure (child and parent reports). No specific type or use of screen time resulted in significantly shorter sleep duration than another, suggesting that caution should be advised against excessive use of all screens.
Keywords: Sleep, Screentime, Media, Televison, Computer, Video games, Pediatrics, Bedtime
Introduction
Insufficient sleep among school-aged children is associated with an increased risk of poor academic performance,1–4 obesity,5–9 and depression.10–13 Furthermore, if short sleep duration continues throughout childhood, its associations with heightened depressive symptoms and low self-esteem strengthen.14 Inadequate sleep duration among young children and adolescents in the United States is common and growing.15 According to the 2014 National Sleep Foundation poll, 31% of school-aged children aged 6 to 11 years receive less than the recommended 9 hours of sleep.16
The overwhelming majority of studies indicate that screen time or electronic media use is associated with lower sleep duration, delayed bedtimes, or other sleep disturbances among school children.17,18 American children between ages 8 and 18 years spend an average of 7 hours per day in front of a screen.19,20 Results from the 2014 National Sleep Foundation poll found that 75% of school-aged children have at least 1 electronic device in their bedroom. This includes a television (45%), a music player (40%), a tablet or smartphone (30%), a video game (25%), or a computer (21%).16 A study by Calamaro et al21 found that children ages 6 to 11 years who have at least a television, a telephone, and a computer in their bedroom receive 45minutes less sleep, compared to those who have no technology devices in their bedroom. Congruently, Cespedes et al22 analyzed screen time in a longitudinal study of 7-year-olds and found that each additional hour of television watching per day was associated with 6 minutes of shorter sleep duration. Another study found that among children aged 6 to 16 years, watching television and using the computer for more than 2 hours per day were both significantly associated with short sleep duration, with the effect size being greater for computer use.23 Furthermore, among 10- and 11-year-olds, using a computer for 1 hour per day was associated with approximately 11 minutes less sleep per night on school nights over an 18-month period in another study.24 Beyond the use of individual screen types, the effects of aggregate screen time have been studied. Drescher et al25 measured the amount of time per day spent watching television, playing video or computer games, and using the Internet and found aggregate screen time to be significantly associated with shorter sleep duration among children younger than 13.3 years.
Previous scholars have hypothesized that interactive screen time, such as video gaming and Web surfing, is more likely to be disruptive to sleep than noninteractive screen time, such as watching television.26 One such study showed that elementary school children who played video games or used the Internet at bedtime had significantly shorter weekday sleep duration compared to those who did not use video games or surf the Internet before bed, whereas no such association was reported among those who watched television at bedtime compared to those who did not watch television at bedtime.27 However, there are very few studies that compare interactive and noninteractive screen time with regard to bedtime sleep duration, especially among school-aged children.23,26,27 Studies often only provide results for 1 or 2 types of screen time at a time or use a combined measure of overall screen time. Furthermore, a majority of the evidence on screen time and sleep duration among children focuses on television and computer use and often does not include video gaming.17 These results, among others, indicate that multiple types of media, ranging on a spectrum of interactivity, are associated with shorter sleep duration among school-aged children.28–30
In the present study, we analyzed the association between various types of screen time and average weekday sleep duration among a large national sample of 9-year-olds. Our data were novel in that we had both parent reports and child reports on screen time exposure and were able to separate out the different forms of screen types. We hypothesized that use of any form of electronic media would be negatively associated with sleep duration. Furthermore, we expected that the strength of the association would vary based on the level of interactivity of the screen type. More specifically, we hypothesized that interactive forms of screen time, such as computer use and video gaming, would be associated with shorter bedtime sleep duration compared to passive forms of screen time, such as watching television.
Methods
Participants
The data used in this study were collected in the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing (FFCW) Study, conducted jointly by Princeton University's Center for Research on Child Wellbeing and Center for Health and Wellbeing, and the Columbia Population Research Center and The National Center for Children and Families at Columbia University. The FFCW is a longitudinal cohort study that has followed approximately 5000 children, born between 1998 and 2000, since birth. Data were collected in 20 cities with populations of at least 200,000 across the United States. The sample was designed to include a high number of unmarried parents and racial minorities, along with a high proportion of low socioeconomic status. Reichman et al31 provide a complete description of the FFCW, including the research design and limitations.
Families were recruited at birth in hospitals, and mothers were screened for eligibility. The inclusion criteria to participate in the study entailed that (1) the mother intended to keep the focal child, (2) she was proficient enough in English or Spanish and healthy enough to answer the survey questions, and (3) the father was still alive. These inclusion criteria disqualified less than 5% of mothers. In addition, because of legal constraints, in approximately half of the hospitals, parents of the focal child had to be at least 18 years of age to be eligible to participate. However, this had only a minimal impact on the sample.31
Interviews were conducted at baseline with the mothers and fathers in person at the hospitals, and subsequent follow-up interviews were conducted over the telephone at 12, 36, and 60 months of age. The study was expanded for the age 9 wave of data and included interviews with the primary caregiver, the focal child, and the focal child's teacher, as well as in-home observations. At each wave, substantial information was collected regarding family structure and relationships, parent and child behavior, access to resources, physical and mental health and development, and home and school environment.
Measures
We used data from the primary caregiver (PCG) surveys and the child interview within the age 9 data. Sleep was reported as hours per night on weekdays by the PCG. General daily screen time, as reported by the PCG, was measured continuously from 0 to 16 hours per day for both television and computer usage separately. Television use was reported as average time on both weekdays and weekends, whereas computer time was nonspecific regarding weekdays and weekends. We chose to only analyze duration of television watching on weekdays, to agree with the data on weekday sleep, along with using general duration of computer use.
In the child interview, the focal child self-reported his or her average time on weekdays using the computer for school work, using the computer for chatting, playing video games, and watching television or movies. Response options included (1) “half an hour or less,” (2) “more than half an hour but less than an hour,” (3) “1–2 hours,” and (4) “more than 2 hours.” Based on our assessment of the distribution of screen time and on our preliminary analyses showing that the lower 3 levels of screen time were not differentially associated with sleep duration, we dichotomized the screen time categories into less than or more than 2 hours per day, for each type of screen time. This cutoff is in agreement with the 2-hour daily limit on media use recommended by the American Academy of Pediatrics.32
Additional covariates obtained from the mother's survey at baseline included sex, race, parental relationship, and mother's education. Sex was categorized as male or female; race was categorized as white non-Hispanic, black non-Hispanic, Hispanic, or other; parental relationship was categorized as married, cohabiting, visiting, friendly, hardly talk, never talk, or unknown father; and mother's education was categorized as less than high school, equivalent to high school, some or technical college, and college graduate. Within parental relationship, we consolidated visiting and friendly and hardly talk, never talk, and unknown father. We also combined the categories of less than high school and education equivalent to high school, within the mother's education variable. We chose to combine these categories based on the distribution of the data and our initial analyses.
Finally, we merged the data and omitted cases that did not contain full information for all variables of interest. We omitted 1629 and 2128 cases from the child-reported and PCG-reported models, respectively. We retained 66.7% of the baseline sample for the child-reported model and 56.6% for the PCG-reported model. Given the relatively large sample size, we justify using listwise deletion because most of the variable means were the same for the included sample as for the omitted sample.33 Furthermore, most of the missing data were from our dependent variable of sleep duration and our primary predictor variable of screen time use. Typically, there are limited benefits to imputing the outcome variable,34 and we do not want to introduce additional measurement error for the screen time exposure measure. Within the child-reported model, there were no differences between groups for sleep duration, any screen time variables, sex, or mother's education. Within the PCG-reported models, weekday nighttime sleep duration was approximately 12 minutes less for the omitted group (P < .001). The omitted group also watched approximately 17 minutes more television per weekday (P < .001). However, there were no differences found between groups for hours of computer use. Within both models, there were only minor differences found between groups in mother's race, mother's relationship with the father, and mother's education.
Statistical analyses
All data were analyzed using Stata version 13.1 (College Station, TX).Multiple linear regression models were used to determine associations between various forms of screen time and weekday sleep duration. We estimated 2 separate sets of nested regression models. The first set of models assessed data reported by the child at age 9 years (models 1 and 2),whereas the second set of models assessed data reported by the PCG (models 3 and 4).We chose to keep these analyses separate due to variations in how the questions were worded and, thus, coded. Furthermore, we wanted to compare the associations between child-reported screen time and sleep and PCG-reported screen time and sleep. Within each set of models, we created 2 models; we first estimated a regression with only the sleep and screen time variables and then a regression that adjusted for a range of potentially confounding covariates. For both sets of models, we maintained the same covariates in the full model. After estimating all regression models, we performed F tests on the final coefficients to compare variances. We also tested for interactions between screen time and both age and race/ethnicity.
Results
Descriptive statistics
Table 1 provides descriptive statistics of our analytic sample. Of the 4898 participating families at baseline, 3630 participated in the age 9wave, and 3269 provided complete information on all variables of interest in the first model. Among this sample, the average weekday sleep duration for 9-year-olds was approximately 9.0 hours (SD, 1.1). Of the focal children, 31.5% spent more than 2 hours watching television or movies per weekday, and 13.8% reported spending more than 2 hours per weekday playing video games, whereas fewer reported spending more than 2 hours per weekday working or chatting on the computer. Approximately half the sample of children were male (52.2%), and among the mothers, 20.7% were white, 50.5% were black, 25.5% were Hispanic, and 3.4% were of other races or ethnicities. Most of the children in the sample had their parents cohabiting (36.2%), friendly (33.9%), or married (22.8%). Furthermore, the majority of mothers (65.0%) had an education less than or equivalent to a high school diploma. The descriptive statistics for the caregiver-reported screen time models are quite similar but, due to a different sample size, are shown separately, in column 2 of Table 1.
Table 1.
Child-reported models (n = 3269) |
Caregiver-reported models (n = 2770) |
|
---|---|---|
Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | |
Weekday sleep duration (h) | 8.95 (1.11) | 9.00 (1.08) |
Television use per weekday (h) |
2.22 (1.62) | |
Computer use per weekday (h) |
0.81 (0.90) | |
Percentage | Percentage | |
Television use per weekday (h) | ||
>2 h watching television | 31.54 | |
<2 h watching television | 68.46 | |
Computer use per weekday (h) | ||
>2 h work on computer | 3.3 | |
<2 h work on computer | 96.7 | |
>2 h chatting | 2.54 | |
<2 h chatting | 97.46 | |
>2 h gaming | 13.77 | |
<2 h gaming | 86.23 | |
Child's sex | ||
Male | 52.22 | 53.03 |
Female | 47.78 | 46.97 |
Mother's race/ethnicity | ||
White, non-Hispanic | 20.71 | 24.87 |
Black, non-Hispanic | 50.47 | 47.18 |
Hispanic | 25.45 | 23.83 |
Other race/ethnicity | 3.36 | 4.12 |
Mother's relationship with father | ||
Married | 22.76 | 28.74 |
Cohabiting | 36.16 | 34.48 |
Friendly | 33.92 | 30.54 |
Hardly or never talk | 7.16 | 6.25 |
Mother's educational level | ||
≤ High school diploma | 65.00 | 57.87 |
Some college or technical college | 24.93 | 28.34 |
College graduate | 10.06 | 13.79 |
Child-reported screen time
Table 2 shows the results of our nested models on the child-report outcomes. In model 1, we find that more than 2 hours per day of chatting on the computer is associated with approximately 16 minutes less sleep per night (β = −0.27; P < .05) and more than 2 hours per day of television or movies is associated with approximately 11 minutes less sleep per night (β = −0.18; P < .001). We did not find a significant association between playing videogames and working on the computer for more than 2 hours per day and weekday nighttime sleep duration.
Table 2.
Model 1 | Model 2 | |
---|---|---|
Weekday sleep duration (h) |
Weekday sleep duration (h) |
|
Screen time (ref: <2 h) | ||
>2 h work on computer per day | −0.16 | −0.05 |
>2 h chatting per day | −0.27* | −0.24 |
>2 h gaming per day | −0.01 | 0.02 |
>2 h watching television per day | −0.18*** | −0.10* |
Child's sex (ref: female) | ||
Male | – | −0.01 |
Mother's race/ethnicity (ref: white) | ||
Black, non-Hispanic | – | −0.47*** |
Hispanic | – | −0.13* |
Other race/ethnicity | – | −0.36** |
Mother's relationship with father (ref: married) |
||
Cohabiting | – | −0.17** |
Friendly | – | −0.16** |
Hardly or never talk | – | −0.30*** |
Mother's educational level (ref: college graduate) |
||
≤ High school diploma/GED | – | −0.24** |
Some college or technical college | – | −0.09 |
Constant | 9.02*** | 9.60*** |
Observations | 3269 | 3269 |
R2 | 0.01 | 0.07 |
P < .05.
P < .01.
P < .001.
In model 2, we adjusted for sex, race/ethnicity, parental relationship, and mother's education, which attenuated the results. We find that more than 2 hours per day of television or movies is associated with 6 minutes less sleep per night (β=−0.10; P < .05). However, we did not find a significant association between chatting on the computer and shorter sleep duration in this model. After adjusting for covariates, there was no significant association found between playing video games or working on the computer and weekday sleep duration.
Race/ethnicity, parental relationship, and mother's education were all significant predictors of weekday reported sleep duration. In particular, children of white mothers were more likely to get more sleep than those of other ethnicities, children whose parents were married slept more than other maternal relationships, and children whose mothers had less than or equal to a high school education slept less than those whose mothers attended college.
Caregiver-reported screen time
Table 3 provides results of the nested models estimating the association between parent-reported screen time and parent-reported child's sleep duration. As shown in model 3, every additional hour of watching television or movies is associated with a decrease of approximately 5minutes in sleep duration (β=−0.09; P < .001). In addition, we find that every additional hour of computer use is associated with a decrease of approximately 7 minutes (β = −0.11; P < .001) in sleep duration. When covariates are added to the model (see model 4), every additional hour of television watching on a weekday is associated with 3 fewer minutes of sleep (β = −0.05; P < .001). The exact same magnitude of association was observed for every additional hour of computer use per day (β= −0.05; P < .05), which is equivalent to 3 fewer minutes of sleep per hour of computer use.
Table 3.
Model 3 | Model 4 | |
---|---|---|
Weekday sleep duration (h) |
Weekday sleep duration (h) |
|
Screen time (h) | ||
Duration of television per weekday | −0.09*** | −0.05*** |
Duration of computer use per weekday | −0.11*** | −0.05* |
Child's sex (ref: female) | ||
Male | – | −0.04 |
Mother's race/ethnicity (ref: white) | ||
Black, non-Hispanic | – | −0.44*** |
Hispanic | – | −0.12* |
Other race/ethnicity | – | −0.38*** |
Mother's relationship with father (ref: married) |
||
Cohabiting | – | −0.16** |
Friendly | – | −0.15* |
Hardly or never talk | – | −0.23* |
Mother's educational level (ref: college graduate) |
||
≤ High school diploma/GED | – | −0.25*** |
Some college or technical college | – | −0.11 |
Constant | 9.29*** | 9.73*** |
Observations | 2770 | 2770 |
R2 | 0.03 | 0.09 |
P < .05.
P < .01.
P < .001.
Among the covariates, those which significantly predicted weekday sleep duration were the same as those in model 2 (mother's race/ethnicity, parental relationship, and mother's education). In particular, children of white mothers, those married to the child's father, and those whose mother's had more education slept more.
Interaction effects
For both child-reported and caregiver-reported screen times, we also tested for interaction effects to see if the association between screen time and sleep duration varies by sex.
There was no significant interaction term by sex at the P < .05 level, although when the analyses were stratified by sex, some slight differences emerged. For example, among girls, the associations with sleep duration were only observed for those reporting 2 or more hours of chat time, and, among boys, the associations with sleep duration were only observed with 2 or more hours of television time (results not shown).
When we added an interaction term between screen time and race/ethnicity, we found a significant interaction with video game screen time and sleep, such that whites who played 2 or more hours of video games per day had significantly longer sleep durations than those who did not play video games. Among blacks and Hispanics, the coefficient associated with 2 or more hours of video games was not statistically significant. No interaction effect of race/ethnicity was observed for television or computer use and sleep duration.
When we estimated similar interaction terms with the caregiver-reported screen times, we found no interactions by either age or race/ethnicity.
Discussion
Our findings have shown significant associations between weekday nighttime sleep duration and the use of various screen types by 9-year-olds. Specifically, we found the most substantial reduction in sleep duration when participants reported using the computer, both in the child-reported (chatting) and the caregiver-reported (all computer use) models. However, within each model, an F test revealed that the coefficients for the screen time variables were not significantly different from each other. Therefore, we cannot make a conclusion in support of our hypothesis that certain types of screen time are more negatively associated with sleep duration than others or that more interactive screen types have a stronger association with reduced sleep duration than passive screen types. Furthermore, the effect sizes of our results suggested less sleep loss than previously examined in sleep restriction studies performed on children. In a study by Sadeh et al,35 sleep was restricted by 1 hour for 3 nights, whereas Fallone et al36 restricted sleep to 4 hours per night. Although the amount of sleep loss observed in the present study was not at the same level, our results still indicate a potentially detrimental degree of sleep loss. Furthermore, our results indicate that greater use of all types of media is associated with shorter sleep duration, regardless of the level interactivity involved in using the screen type. Finally, the evidence is suggestive that these patterns might vary by race/ethnicity or sex.
Several mechanisms have been hypothesized through which media use disrupts sleep.17,18 First, devoting hours in the day to screen time may decrease the amount of time available for sleep. Second, some media content may cause physiological arousal and impede sleep.37 Third, it has been shown in young adults that bright light from screens may suppress melatonin, delay sleep onset, and interfere with sleep cycles.38
Our results that both computer use and television watching are associated with shorter nighttime sleep duration are in agreement with multiple previous studies. For example, Garmy et al23 also found that spending more than 2 hours per day watching television or using the computer was associated with shorter sleep duration among school-aged children. Furthermore, BaHammam et al39 found that children who watched television or played computer games after 8 pm had significantly shorter sleep durations (16 and 15 minutes less, respectively).
Although we did not report a significant finding between video gaming and sleep duration, the inverse association between video gaming and sleep duration has been previously identified. Oka et al27 found that among Japanese school children, use of the Internet and video games had a larger negative association with sleep/wake parameters (bedtime, wake time, and sleep duration) than watching television. Excessive television watching and video gaming have also been shown by Tazawa and Okada40 to be associated with physical signs of sleep deprivation among 6- to 11-year-olds. However, Adam et al41 found that playing video games was not associated with shorter overall sleep duration. These differences may be due to factors such as changes in screen type preferences across age group hypothesized in the study of Drescher et al,25 which also reported no association before 13.3 years of age, but did find an association among children older than 13.3 years. Furthermore, the association between time video gaming and nighttime sleep duration has been shown to be greater on weekends than on weekdays among children.27,41
Our study provides several strengths and advances the literature on screen time and sleep. First, the FFCW Study allowed us to analyze a demographic that is often understudied in survey-based data. The nationwide data collection amply represented families with lower socioeconomic status, single parents, and racial minorities. Because low socioeconomic status and minority children are often put at a higher risk for insufficient sleep duration due to a wide range of factors,42 it is important to analyze data where they are well represented. Another strength of the present study is our use of 2 separate measures of screen time: child reported and parent reported. Because both measures had significant associations between reported screen time and sleep duration on the same sample, the results provided each other with a source of validation of the association.
The study has 4 limitations. First, we only had self-reported data available on both screen time and sleep. This presents concerns about the validity and reliability of the data. For example, 1 study found that among school-aged children, a comparison of child actigraphy data with parental report describes dissonant results on sleep duration.43 In addition, the parent-reported data may not be as accurate among parents who are not as observant of their child's sleep habits. This could be affected by various factors, including the number of children in the household and whether the parent and child share a bedroom. A second limitation is that this study analyzed cross-sectional data at one point in time rather than looking at repeated measures of screen time across multiple nights. As a result, we cannot infer strong conclusions about the causal associations between these 2 factors. Eggermont and Van den Bulck44 have also noted the possible bidirectionality of the association between sleep duration and technology. Third, because the participants of this study were 9-years-old between 2007 and 2009, the generalizability of their technology use to the present day is somewhat limited. With the rapid growth and change in the technology industry, it is difficult to know how this translates to today's 9-year-olds. Lastly, when testing for interaction effects, the sample sizes of the subgroups were quite small. For example, when we stratified for sex, only 46 girls and 37 boys reported 2 or more hours of chat time. Furthermore, the sample size for non-Hispanic white children who played 2 or more hours of video games per day (n=24 girls and 48 boys) was limited. Therefore, we are careful not to overinterpret the differences found by sex, and we are cautious about interpreting the unexpected direction of the interaction term for race/ethnicity. That said, larger prospective studies may be able to better address these possible interactions.
Future studies should further explore the associations examined in our research. Specifically, we want to know more about multitasking or simultaneous use of screens among school-aged children,45 as well as how it might affect their sleep. Furthermore, information on presleep habits such as nighttime/bedtime screen use could provide a more meaningful measure to examine, as compared to general daytime screen use.46 In addition, a follow-up study may investigate whether parenting style modifies the association between screen time and sleep duration,47,48 as well as the effects of screen time on other behavioral or health outcomes related to sleep.49–51 Lastly, future research should include newer forms of technology and media, including smartphones, tablets, and other portable devices.
Although future research could provide a better understanding of the association between sleep and screen exposure, the current evidence has strong implications for both parents and children. With inadequate sleep duration becoming a growing issue across the United States, it is imperative that parents are conscious of how their child's daytime activities may be linked to sleep habits.
Acknowledgments
Dr. Hale has received or is currently receiving research grant support from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute for Child Health and Human Development; The National Institute of Aging; the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases; and the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, and she has served as a consultant to a project funded by the National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences. She also sits on the Board of Directors for the National Sleep Foundation (NSF) and receives an honorarium from the NSF for her role as editor of this journal.
Funding for the analysis of this data was provided by the National Institutes of Health (R01 HD073352; Hale, PI). The authors are thankful for the range of sources of government and foundation funding for the Fragile Families Study data set, described in more detail on the http://www.fragilefamilies.princeton.edu/funders.asp. The funding agencies played no role in the development, analysis, reporting, or decision to submit this manuscript.
Footnotes
Disclosure
The other authors have nothing to disclose.
References
- 1.BaHammam A, Al-Faris E, Shaikh S, Saeed AB. Sleep problems/habits and school performance in elementary school children. Sleep Hypn. 2006;8(1):12. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Dewald JF, Meijer AM, Oort FJ, Kerkhof GA, Bogels SM. The influence of sleep quality, sleep duration and sleepiness on school performance in children and adolescents: a meta-analytic review. Sleep Med Rev. 2010;14(3):179–189. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2009.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Li S, Arguelles L, Jiang F, et al. Sleep, school performance, and a school-based intervention among school-aged children: a sleep series study in China. PLoS One. 2013;8(7):e67928. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0067928. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Gruber R, Laviolette R, Deluca P, Monson E, Cornish K, Carrier J. Short sleep duration is associated with poor performance on IQ measures in healthy school-age children. Sleep Med. 2010;11(3):289–294. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2009.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Van Cauter E, Knutson KL. Sleep and the epidemic of obesity in children and adults. Eur J Endocrinol. 2008;159(Suppl. 1):S59–S66. doi: 10.1530/EJE-08-0298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Cappuccio FP, Taggart FM, Kandala NB, et al. Meta-analysis of short sleep duration and obesity in children and adults. Sleep. 2008;31(5):619–626. doi: 10.1093/sleep/31.5.619. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Chen X, Beydoun MA, Wang Y. Is sleep duration associated with childhood obesity? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2008;16(2):265–274. doi: 10.1038/oby.2007.63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Landhuis CE, Poulton R, Welch D, Hancox RJ. Childhood sleep time and long-term risk for obesity: a 32-year prospective birth cohort study. Pediatrics. 2008;122(5):955–960. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-3521. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Sekine M, Yamagami T, Handa K, et al. A dose-response relationship between short sleeping hours and childhood obesity: results of the Toyama Birth Cohort Study. Child Care Health Dev. 2002;28(2):163–170. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2214.2002.00260.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Lin J-D, Tung H-J, Hsieh Y-H, Lin F-G. Interactive effects of delayed bedtime and family-associated factors on depression in elementary school children. Res Dev Disabil. 2011;32(6):2036–2044. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kelly RJ, El-Sheikh M. Reciprocal relations between children's sleep and their adjustment over time. Dev Psychol. 2014;50(4):1137–1147. doi: 10.1037/a0034501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Lehto JE, Uusitalo-Malmivaara L. Sleep-related factors: associations with poor attention and depressive symptoms. Child Care Health Dev. 2014;40(3):419–425. doi: 10.1111/cch.12063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Silva GE, Goodwin JL, Parthasarathy S, et al. Longitudinal association between short sleep, body weight, and emotional and learning problems in Hispanic and Caucasian children. Sleep. 2011;34(9):1197–1205. doi: 10.5665/SLEEP.1238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Fredriksen K, Rhodes J, Reddy R, Way N. Sleepless in Chicago: tracking the effects of adolescent sleep loss during the middle school years. Child Dev. 2004;75(1):84–95. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00655.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Iglowstein I, Jenni OG, Molinari L, Largo RH. Sleep duration from infancy to adolescence: reference values and generational trends. Pediatrics. 2003;111(2):302–307. doi: 10.1542/peds.111.2.302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Buxton OM, Chang A-M, Spilsbury JC, Bos T, Emsellem H, Knutson KL. Sleep in the modern family: protective family routines for child and adolescent sleep. Sleep Health: Journal of the National Sleep Foundation. 2015 doi: 10.1016/j.sleh.2014.12.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Hale L, Guan S. Screen time and sleep among school-aged children and adolescents: a systematic literature review. Sleep Med Rev. 2014;21:50–58. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2014.07.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Cain N, Gradisar M. Electronic media use and sleep in school-aged children and adolescents: a review. Sleep Med. 2010;11(8):735–742. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2010.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Rideout VJ, Foehr UG, Roberts DF. Generation M [superscript 2]: Media in the Lives of 8-to 18-Year-Olds. Henry J Kaiser Family Foundation; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Council on Communications and Media, Strasburger VC. Children, adolescents, obesity, and the media. Pediatrics. 2011;128(1):201–208. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-1066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Calamaro CJ, Yang K, Ratcliffe S, Chasens ER. Wired at a young age: the effect of caffeine and technology on sleep duration and body mass index in school-aged children. J Pediatr Health Care. 2012;26(4):276–282. doi: 10.1016/j.pedhc.2010.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Cespedes EM, Gillman MW, Kleinman K, Rifas-Shiman SL, Redline S, Taveras EM. Television viewing, bedroom television, and sleep duration from infancy to mid-childhood. Pediatrics. 2014;133(5):e1163–e1171. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-3998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Garmy P, Nyberg P, Jakobsson U. Sleep and television and computer habits of Swedish school-age children. J Sch Nurs. 2012;28(6):469–476. doi: 10.1177/1059840512444133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Nuutinen T, Ray C, Roos E. Do computer use, TV viewing, and the presence of the media in the bedroom predict school-aged children's sleep habits in a longitudinal study? BMC Public Health. 2013;13:684. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-13-684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Drescher AA, Goodwin JL, Silva GE, Quan SF. Caffeine and screen time in adolescence: associations with short sleep and obesity. J Clin Sleep Med. 2011;7(4):337–342. doi: 10.5664/JCSM.1182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Dworak M, Schierl T, Bruns T, Struder HK. Impact of singular excessive computer game and television exposure on sleep patterns and memory performance of school-aged children. Pediatrics. 2007;120(5):978–985. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-0476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Oka Y, Suzuki S, Inoue Y. Bedtime activities, sleep environment, and sleep/wake patterns of Japanese elementary school children. Behav Sleep Med. 2008;6(4):220–233. doi: 10.1080/15402000802371338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Heins E, Seitz C, Schuz J, et al. Bedtime, television and computer habits of primary school children in Germany. Gesundheitswesen. 2007;69(3):151–157. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-971061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Seo WS, Sung HM, Lee JH, et al. Sleep patterns and their age-related changes in elementary-school children. Sleep Med. 2010;11(6):569–575. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2010.03.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.de Jong E, Visscher TL, HiraSing RA, Heymans MW, Seidell JC, Renders CM. Association between TV viewing, computer use and overweight, determinants and competing activities of screen time in 4- to 13-year-old children. Int J Obes (Lond) 2013;37(1):47–53. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2011.244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Reichman NE, Teitler JO, Garfinkel I, McLanahan SS. Fragile families: sample and design. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2001;23(4):303–326. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Council on Communications and Media. Children, adolescents, and the media. Pediatrics. 2013;132(5):958–961. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-2656. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Patrician PA. Multiple imputation formissing data. Res Nurs Health. 2002;25(1):76–84. doi: 10.1002/nur.10015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Little RJA. Regression with Missing X's: a review. J Am Stat Assoc. 1992;87(420):1227–1237. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Sadeh A, Gruber R, Raviv A. The effects of sleep restriction and extension on school-age children: what a difference an hour makes. Child Dev. 2003;74(2):444–455. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.7402008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Fallone G, Acebo C, Arnedt JT, Seifer R, Carskadon MA. Effects of acute sleep restriction on behavior, sustained attention, and response inhibition in children. Percept Mot Skills. 2001;93(1):213–229. doi: 10.2466/pms.2001.93.1.213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Anderson CA, Bushman BJ. Effects of violent video games on aggressive behavior, aggressive cognition, aggressive affect, physiological arousal, and prosocial behavior: a meta-analytic review of the scientific literature. Psychol Sci. 2001;12(5):353–359. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Chang AM, Aeschbach D, Duffy JF, Czeisler CA. Evening use of light-emitting eReaders negatively affects sleep, circadian timing, and next-morning alertness. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2015;112(4):1232–1237. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1418490112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.BaHammam A, Bin Saeed A, Al-Faris E, Shaikh S. Sleep duration and its correlates in a sample of Saudi elementary school children. Singapore Med J. 2006;47(10):875–881. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Tazawa Y, Okada K. Physical signs associated with excessive television-game playing and sleep deprivation. Pediatr Int. 2001;43(6):647–650. doi: 10.1046/j.1442-200x.2001.01466.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Adam EK, Snell EK, Pendry P. Sleep timing and quantity in ecological and family context: a nationally representative time-diary study. J Fam Psychol. 2007;21(1):4–19. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.21.1.4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Hale L. The Oxford Handbook of Infant, Child, and Adolescent Sleep and Behavior. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2013. The Social Determinants of Child and Adolescent Sleep. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Holley S, Hill CM, Stevenson J. A comparison of actigraphy and parental report of sleep habits in typically developing children aged 6 to 11 years. Behav Sleep Med. 2010;8(1):16–27. doi: 10.1080/15402000903425462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Eggermont S, Van den Bulck J. Nodding off or switching off? The use of popular media as a sleep aid in secondary-school children. J Paediatr Child Health. 2006;42(7–8):428–433. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1754.2006.00892.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Pea R, Nass C, Meheula L, et al. Media use, face-to-face communication, media multitasking, and social well-being among 8- to 12-year-old girls. Dev Psychol. 2012;48(2):327–336. doi: 10.1037/a0027030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Calamaro CJ, Mason TB, Ratcliffe SJ. Adolescents living the 24/7 lifestyle: effects of caffeine and technology on sleep duration and daytime functioning. Pediatrics. 2009;123(6):e1005–e1010. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-3641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Brand S, Hatzinger M, Beck J, Holsboer-Trachsler E. Perceived parenting styles, personality traits and sleep patterns in adolescents. J Adolesc. 2009;32(5):1189–1207. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2009.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Spilsbury JC, Storfer-Isser A, Drotar D, Rosen CL, Kirchner HL, Redline S. Effects of the home environment on school-aged children's sleep. Sleep. 2005;28(11):1419–1427. doi: 10.1093/sleep/28.11.1419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Holley S, Hill CM, Stevenson J. An hour less sleep is a risk factor for childhood conduct problems. Child Care Health Dev. 2011;37(4):563–570. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2010.01203.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Magee L, Hale L. Longitudinal associations between sleep duration and subsequent weight gain: a systematic review. Sleep Med Rev. 2012;16(3):231–241. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2011.05.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Bagley EJ, Kelly RJ, El-Sheikh M. Longitudinal relations between children's sleep and body mass index: the moderating role of socioeconomic risk. Sleep Health. 2015;1(1):44–49. doi: 10.1016/j.sleh.2014.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]