Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2016 Jul 21;5:e16135. doi: 10.7554/eLife.16135

Dopaminergic neurons write and update memories with cell-type-specific rules

Yoshinori Aso 1,*, Gerald M Rubin 1,*
Editor: Liqun Luo2
PMCID: PMC4987137  PMID: 27441388

Abstract

Associative learning is thought to involve parallel and distributed mechanisms of memory formation and storage. In Drosophila, the mushroom body (MB) is the major site of associative odor memory formation. Previously we described the anatomy of the adult MB and defined 20 types of dopaminergic neurons (DANs) that each innervate distinct MB compartments (Aso et al., 2014a, 2014b). Here we compare the properties of memories formed by optogenetic activation of individual DAN cell types. We found extensive differences in training requirements for memory formation, decay dynamics, storage capacity and flexibility to learn new associations. Even a single DAN cell type can either write or reduce an aversive memory, or write an appetitive memory, depending on when it is activated relative to odor delivery. Our results show that different learning rules are executed in seemingly parallel memory systems, providing multiple distinct circuit-based strategies to predict future events from past experiences.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16135.001

Research Organism: D. melanogaster

Introduction

Animals use memories of past events to predict the future. In some cases, an animal is best served by making a prediction based solely on their most recent experience. In others, a series of experiences is integrated to make a probabilistic prediction, discounting an event experienced only once. How are such different strategies implemented in the brain? One idea is that individual components of a memory—often called engrams—are simultaneously stored in distinct sub-circuits whose outputs can then be combined upon recall to affect behavior. These sub-circuits would vary in their rules for writing, updating and retaining these engrams, having differences in synaptic plasticity and circuit properties (Hikosaka et al., 2014). Experiments aimed at uncovering the mechanisms by which different forms of memory are established and maintained, and then coherently coordinated to drive behavior, are facilitated by using a model system in which the relevant cells and circuits can be identified and manipulated either individually or in specific combinations. In this report, we describe experiments performed in such a model system, the olfactory circuitry of Drosophila melanogaster.

Neuronal circuits for learning associations often share a common architecture: a large array of anatomically similar neurons that represent the sensory environment converge onto a much smaller number of output neurons (Luo, 2015, Dean et al., 2010) (Figure 1A). Punishment or reward activates modulatory neurons that in turn cause changes in the synaptic weight matrix between the neurons representing the sensory cues and the output neurons, resulting in memory formation. The mushroom body (MB) shares this architecture and is the major site of associative learning in Drosophila (Heisenberg et al., 1985; de Belle and Heisenberg, 1994; Dubnau et al., 2001; McGuire et al., 2003; Heisenberg, 2003). Odor identity is represented by a pattern of sparse activity in the ~2000 Kenyon cells (KCs) (Laurent and Naraghi, 1994; Perez-Orive et al., 2002), whose parallel axons form the lobes of the MB. The dendrites of MB output neurons (MBONs) and terminals of dopaminergic input neurons (DANs) tile the KC axons; the extent of the arbors of individual MBONs and DANs overlap precisely, defining 15 distinct compartmental units that tile the MB lobes of adult Drosophila (Tanaka et al., 2008; Aso et al., 2014a).

Figure 1. Learning rules in one MB compartment.

(A) Conceptual diagram of memory circuit organization. The parallel axonal fibers of the Kenyon cells represent odor stimuli, modulatory dopaminergic inputs induce plasticity at KC to MBON synapses in distinct compartments (represented by the shaded rectangles) along the length of these axons and MB output neurons from the compartments read out memory. See Aso et al. (2014a) for more details. (B) Left: Design of the optogenetic olfactory arena. See Figure 1—figure supplement 1 and Supplementary file 1 for details. Right: Diagram illustrating odor paths in the arena. Flies are confined in the circular arena at the center (3-mm high and 10-cm diameter). Video 1 illustrates the pattern of airflow. (C) Diagram of the expression pattern of the split-GAL4 line MB320C driving CsChrimson-mVenus (blue) in PPL1-γ1pedc; see Aso et al. (2014a) and www.janelia.org/split-gal4 for primary image data for this and other split-GAL4 lines. (D) Top: Training protocols. For odor delivery, valves were open for 60 s. For training, thirty 1 s pulses of red light (627 nm peak and 34.9 µW/mm2 at the position of the flies) were applied over 60 s starting 5 s after valve opening. Experiments were done reciprocally: In one group of flies, odor A and B were 3-octanol and 4-methylcyclohexanol, respectively, while in a second group of flies, the odors were reversed. All flies went through the same initial training and test protocol, and then the flies, without removal from the arena, went through one of the four diagramed training and test protocols. Bottom: Time course of the performance index (PI) during first test period (from 4–6 min of the experiment; left) and second test period (from 10–12 min of the experiment; right). The PI is defined as [(number of flies in the odor A quadrants) - (number of flies in odor B quadrants)]/(total number of flies). The average PI of reciprocal experiments is shown. The overall PI, which is reported in Figure 1F and Figures 2 and 3 was calculated by averaging the PIs from the final 30 s of each test period (indicated by the black horizontal line on the time axis). In the left panel, thin lines show individual reciprocal experiments and the thick line the mean of all experiments. In the right panel the mean with error bars representing the SEM are shown. Control genotypes did not show any significant memory in the same training protocols: (1) no driver control, pBDPGAL4 in attP2/20xUAS-CsChrimson in attP18 (PI = 0.07, SEM = 0.037, N = 14); and (2) no effector control, MB320C/w1118 (PI = −0.01, SEM = 0.031, N = 10). (E) A single frame of Video 2 showing the position of odor-conditioned flies at the end of the 1-min test period. Lines show trajectory of four flies. Video 2 shows the behavior of flies in the area for the full 1 min test period. (F) Inter-stimulus-interval (ISI) curve. A single training was done for each experiment. The relative timing of a 10 s delivery of odor A and a 10 s period in which three 1 s light pulse were delivered was varied. The diagram on top illustrates the cases of ISI = −10 s, 0 s and +10 s corresponding to the light pulses starting −10 s, 0 s, or +10 s after the initiation of odor delivery, respectively. The data points and error bars show the mean and SEM for MB320C/CsChrimson-mVenus. N = 10–14. Asterisk indicates significance from 0: *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001; n.s., not significant.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16135.002

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Diagram of the olfactory behavioral apparatus.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

See Supplementary file 1 for details.

While these compartmental units share a similar general structure, there are important anatomical and functional differences between them. Each compartment contains only one of the three major classes of KCs: γ, α′/β′ and α/β. Anatomical data suggest that each MBON samples from ~90 to ~2000 KCs in one or two compartments, while each KC forms en passant synapses on 5–6 types of MBONs along its axon in the lobes (Aso et al., 2009, 2014a). During associative learning, each DAN is thought to modulate KC-MBON synapses only in its target compartment(s) (Hige et al., 2015; Cohn et al., 2015). Thus, the information encoded by KC activity might contribute to multiple distinct engrams by compartmental specific modulation of KC-MBON en passant synapses. A large body of previous work has established that the KCs, MBONs and DANs innervating individual compartments are differentially involved in forming memories with different valence—that is, appetitive and aversive memories—and with different stabilities (Schwaerzel et al., 2003; Zars et al., 2000; Isabel et al., 2004; Blum et al., 2009; Krashes et al., 2009; Claridge-Chang et al., 2009; Aso et al., 2010; Sejourne et al., 2011; Trannoy et al., 2011; Liu et al., 2012; Burke et al., 2012; Aso et al., 2012; Placais et al., 2013; Pai et al., 2013, Aso et al., 2014b; Lin et al., 2014; Bouzaiane et al., 2015; Ichinose et al., 2015; Yamagata et al., 2015; Owald et al., 2015; Huetteroth et al., 2015). However little is known about the rules for writing and updating memory in each compartment. By using the anatomical map of the MB and cell type specific drivers we reported previously (Aso et al., 2014a, 2014b) in conjunction with newly developed behavioral assays, we have been able to establish that different compartments can employ vastly different learning rules.

Video 1. Separation of airflow in the four quadrants of arena.

Download video file (6.6MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16135.004

Ammonium chloride smoke was introduced in two opposing quadrants allowing the borders of airflow in the circular arena to be seen. Valves opened at the beginning of the movie. After a ~2 s delay, smoke reached the peripheral of arena, a further ~3 s was required to fill the arena. At a flow rate of 400 mL/min, replacing the 23.5 mL of air in the arena takes ~3.5 s. Elapsed time after valve opening is shown in the upper right.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16135.004

Video 2. Conditioned odor avoidance after training with PPL1-γ1pedc.

Download video file (4.5MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16135.005

An example of conditioned odor avoidance behaviors after training using optogenetic activation of PPL1-γ1pedc. The movie shows the 1-min test period (corresponding to the 5 to 6 min time range in Figure 1D left). Colored lines follow the trajectories of four flies. Notice that flies approaching the borders between compartments readily cross into the B quadrants with the control odor, but avoid crossing into the A quadrants with the trained odor.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16135.005

Results and discussion

Experimental design

Punitive or rewarding stimuli such as electric shock, heat, cold, bitter taste and sugar generally activate a complex pattern of DANs (Riemensperger et al., 2005; Mao and Davis, 2009; ; Liu et al., 2012; Tomchik, 2013; Galili et al., 2014; Das et al., 2014; Kirkhart and Scott, 2015). Believing that a reduction in complexity will be essential to understand the roles played by DAN inputs to different MB compartments, we utilized intersectional split-GAL4 drivers to express CsChrimson, a red-shifted channelrhodopsin, in specific cell types (see Materials and methods). While activation of CsChrimson in these driver lines provides a stimulus that is unlikely to occur naturally, it allowed us to separately examine the memory components induced by individual DAN cell types.

We developed an olfactory arena that allows fine temporal control of both odor delivery and DAN activation using optogenetics (Figure 1B, Video 1 and Figure 1—figure supplement 1; detailed construction documents are included as a Supplementary file 1). In this arena, freely moving flies can be repeatedly trained and tested without the manual handling or temperature changes required in previous assays, thereby minimizing variability that might obscure subtle behavioral effects. These methods allowed us to systematically examine the properties of memories induced in different MB compartments, including: (1) the temporal pairing requirements of odor presentation and DAN activation; (2) the amount of training required for memory formation; (3) retention time; (4) weakening of the conditioned response induced by either DAN activation in the absence of odor presentation or by odor presentation in the absence of DAN activation; (5) the ability to learn new associations; and (6) the capacity to store multiple memories.

Assessing the learning rules in one compartment

Dopamine signaling to KCs has been implicated in both learning and forgetting (Schwaerzel et al., 2003; Schroll et al., 2006, Kim et al., 2007; Tomchik and Davis, 2009; Gervasi et al., 2010; Qin et al., 2012; Placais et al., 2012; Berry et al., 2012; Boto et al., 2014; Shuai et al., 2015). However, it has not been determined if a single DAN cell type can drive both processes within the same compartment. Pairing one of two odors with activation of PPL1-γ1pedc, results in robust aversive memory to the paired odor (Aso et al., 2010; Figure 1C–E and Video 2). That memory is fully retained after 10 min (blue line in Figure 1D) but has largely decayed by 24 hr (Figure 3B). Presentation of the odors alone a few minutes after training resulted in a modest reduction in the conditioned response (orange line in Figure 1D). A second activation of the same DAN a few minutes after training in the absence of odor can almost completely abolish the conditioned response (red line in Figure 1D). Recent imaging data of the γ4 MBON (Cohn et al., 2015) suggest that this reduction most likely results from restoration of the response of the MBON to the odor; that is, erasure of the memory. If, after the first training, contingencies are reversed such that the other odor is presented paired with DAN activation, the first memory is reduced and a memory of the new association is formed (purple line in Figure 1D). Taken together, these data indicate that the same DANs can write a new memory or reduce an existing conditioned response, enabling the flexibility to rapidly change the associations formed between a conditioned stimulus (CS), the odor, and an unconditioned stimulus (US) represented by dopamine release.

In classical conditioning, both the rate of learning and the valence of the resultant memory depend on the relative timing between the CS and the US (Christian and Thompson, 2003; Gerber et al., 2014). Our ability to precisely control DAN stimulation and odor presentation enabled us to examine the CS-US timing relationship (Figure 1F). We found that when PPL1-γ1pedc stimulation fell within a 30-s time window following the onset of a 10-s odor presentation, an aversive memory was formed (negative PI, Figure 1F). Interestingly, we observed that DAN stimulation that precedes odor presentation by 20 to 60 s induced an appetitive memory (positive PI, Figure 1F). These observations are consistent with the notion that it is the predictive aspect of CS-US timing that matters. When the timing is such that the CS predicts a subsequent aversive US, animals learn to avoid the CS. However, animals can also learn that the CS predicts the end of an aversive US, and are consequently attracted to the CS (Tanimoto et al., 2004). It has been suggested that this timing dependency could result from the dynamics of the biochemical signaling cascade acting downstream of dopamine receptors (Yarali et al., 2012).

Pairing activation of PPL1-γ1pedc with odor has been shown to depresses the subsequent spiking rate of the MBON from the γ1pedc compartments in response to the trained odor (Hige et al., 2015). In behavioral assays, optogenetic activation of this MBON was shown to attract flies (Aso et al., 2014b). Taken together, these observations suggest that DAN activation paired with an odor produces an aversive behavioral response to that odor by decreasing the MBON’s attractive output. Thus our data can be most easily explained if this single DAN can bi-directionally alter the strength of KC-MBON synapses depending on the presence and relative timing of odor-driven KC activity; a full testing of this model awaits additional physiological measurements.

Comparing rules for writing memory across compartments

In order to compare the parameters of learning in different MB compartments, we selected a set of additional split-GAL4 drivers that express at similar and high levels in different DAN cell types (diagrammed in Figure 2A; primary imaging data documenting expression patterns can be found at http://www.janelia.org/split-gal4). In addition, the three DAN cell types, which express CsChrimson using the MB320C and MB099C drivers, have been shown to have similar spiking responses to CsChrimson activation (Hige et al., 2015). In three of six cases, we chose drivers expressing in a combination of two cell types because we found that activation of only a single DAN cell type did not produce a sufficiently robust memory (Figure 2—figure supplements 1,2). We confirmed that the lines used in our optogenetic experiments (Figure 2A) showed comparable memory formation when trained with electric shock or sugar reward (Figure 2—figure supplement 3). Together, the selected drivers innervate 11 of the 15 MB compartments. Below we describe the results obtained in a number of different learning assays by activating these split-GAL4 drivers.

Figure 2. Rules for writing memory.

(A) Diagram of the DANs contained in split-GAL4 driver lines, which have been color-coded to facilitate comparison with the plots shown in the subsequent panels. Expression patterns of these drivers, including full confocal stacks, can be found at www.janelia.org/split-gal4. In all experiments, the drivers were crossed with 20xUAS-CsChrimson-mVenus in attP18. (B) Differential effect of training length among DANs. Left: Diagram of the experimental design. Immediate memories formed after paring a 60-s odor presentation with thirty 1-s CsChrimson-activating light pulses (Test 1) were compared with those obtained with a 10-s odor presentation paired with three 1-s light pulses (Test 2). Center: A 60-s training period resulted in significantly better memory performance compared with a 10-s training for MB043C, MB213B and MB315C+MB109B (data from ISI = 0 s for MB099C and ISI =+ 10 s for others drivers were used to provide maximum memory formation; see ISI curves in Figure 2—figure supplement 4). We also observed increased learning with 60-s versus 10-s training (PI of 0.72 versus 0.15) when using R58E02-GAL4 (Liu et al., 2012), a strong GAL4 driver expressed in ~90 PAM cluster DANs that includes all of the ~50 DANs that have expression in MB043C, MB213B and MB315C+MB109B. To facilitate comparison of PI magnitudes, the sign of the PI in this and subsequent panels was reversed for DANs that induced aversive memory (MB320C, MB099C and MB630B). The bottom and top of each box represents the first and third quartile, and the horizontal line dividing the box is the median. The whiskers represent the 10th and 90th percentiles. N = 8–16. Right: Comparison of the effect of training time on memory formation induced by activation of different DANs. Ratios of the mean PI obtained with short training and individual PIs obtained with long training are shown for each driver. Asterisk indicates significance of depicted pairs after comparing all pairs. (C) Comparison of learning after single and repetitive training using the three drivers MB320C, MB099C and MB630B. Either a single training with memory test after 1 min (immediate memory; left) or 10 trainings separated by 15 min resting intervals and then memory tests after 1 (middle) or 4 (right) days were used. Significant aversive 1-day memory was seen with all drivers, while 4-day memory was observed only with MB099C and MB630B. MB320C failed to show 4-day memory despite displaying the most robust immediate memory, while MB630B did not induce significant immediate memory. N = 8–12. Asterisk indicates significance of comparison of indicated pairs in B and from 0 in C: *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16135.006

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Combinatorial roles of DANs in memory formation.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Top: Diagram of the expression patterns of PAM-γ5 (MB315C) and PAM-β′2a (MB109B) as well as that obtained by combining them. As illustrated in the circuit diagram, each of these DANs innervates one of two compartments that are spanned by a common MBON. Bottom: Activating the combined drivers induced significantly higher appetitive memory than obtained by activating either driver alone. N = 12–16. (B) Top: Expression patterns of CsChrimson-mVenus (green) and the reference pattern of anti-Brp (magenta) in the brain and ventral nerve cord of the indicated split-GAL4 drivers. The expression pattern of the combined driver was nearly the simple sum of individual drivers MB109B and MB315C. Bottom: Higher magnification views of the region including the MB lobes in one brain hemisphere are shown without the reference anti-Brp pattern. (C) Training ten times with 15-min rest intervals (10x spaced training) resulted in significant 4-day aversive memory when using the three drivers, MB099C, MB060B and MB065B that express in the combination of the γ2, α′1, α′2 and α2 compartments (PPL1-γ2α′1 and PPL1-α′2α2). MB060B and MB065B also express in α3 and α′3 (PPL1-α3 and PPL1-α′3). Drivers expressing either in just α′2 plus α2 (MB058B) or just in γ2 plus α′1 (MB296B), failed to induce 4-day memory. N = 6–10. Asterisk indicates significance from 0: *p<0.05; ***p<0.001.
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Additional drivers that induced weak, but significant, memory.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

pBDPGAL4 is an enhancerless GAL4 driver used as a control. MB296B expresses in PPL1-γ2α′1, a subset of the expression pattern of MB099C, and MB063B expresses in PAM-β1, a subset of the expression pattern of MB213B (Aso et al. 2014a). These drivers show significant memory compared to the control genotype, but much weaker than the combination lines (MB099C and MB213B; see Figure 2B) used in our main experiments. MB312C (Aso et al. 2014a) expresses in a combination of PAM-γ4 and PAM-γ4 < γ1γ2. N = 16–20.
Figure 2—figure supplement 3. Controls for genetic background.

Figure 2—figure supplement 3.

(A) Diagram of the key split-GAL4 lines used in this study. (B) MB320C, MB099C and MB630B were tested as heterozygotes with UAS-CsChrimson for their ability to form aversive memory with electric shock (twelve 1.25 s pulses of 60V delivered in a shock tube) as the US rather than optogenetic stimulation and then tested in our standard arena. Both immediate memory with a single training (n = 10–12) and 4-day memory (n = 8) with 10X spaced training were assessed. (C) Similarly, MB043C, MB213B and MB315+MB109B were tested for immediate (n = 10–12) and 1-day (n = 8) appetitive memory using sugar reward as the US. The results show that all lines show roughly similar levels of memory formation, indicating that the failure to form some types of memory by optogenetic stimulation of a specific DAN is unlikely to be due to differences in genetic background between lines.
Figure 2—figure supplement 4. Inter stimulus interval curves.

Figure 2—figure supplement 4.

Inter stimulus interval curves were measured as in Figure 1G. MB099C showed significant aversive memory only at ISI = 0 s; ISI = −10 s and +10 s were significant without correction for multiple comparisons. MB043C, MB213B and MB315C+MB109B all showed significant appetitive memory at ISI = +10 s and +30 s; MB315C+MB109B also showed significant appetitive memory at ISI = 0. Thus, for appetitive memory, the period when dopamine signaling resulted in the most robust memory formation was shifted slightly later relative to odor presentation; such a response profile might be an adaptation to the time required for converting ingested food to a nutritional reward signal. The lower PIs observed relative to those shown in Figure 3B presumably result from the need to use a 10-s (rather than a 60-s) training time in these experiments. N = 8–12.
Figure 2—figure supplement 5. A conceptual model of memory dynamics in parallel memory units.

Figure 2—figure supplement 5.

The top and bottom panels show hypothetical retention curves following three different extents of training in two memory units: memory unit one has a fast acquisition rate and fast decay dynamics (top) and memory unit 2 has a slow acquisition rate and slow decay dynamics. Memory retention at certain time t is a function of the initial magnitude of memory following training, I, and the memory stability, S (Ebbinghaus, 1885). Note that while the initial memory score (I) depends on the amount of training, memory stability (S) is constant in each memory unit and is not altered by the amount of training. Based on this and previous studies (Hige et al., 2015; Aso et al., 2012), we propose that to a first approximation individual MB compartments can be modeled as distinct memory units with different baseline acquisition rates and memory stabilities, which can be set independently. For aversive memory, we found memory stability and acquisition rate appear negatively correlated (Figure 2 and Figure 3B). However, stable appetitive memory can be induced by brief training (Figure 3B) (Yamagata et al., 2015; Huetteroth et al., 2015).

Longer and repetitive training has been shown to induce stronger and more persistent memory across animal phyla (Frost et al., 1985; Tully et al., 1994). Consistent with those observations, we found that a 10-s training generally induced memory less effectively than a 60-s training in our immediate memory assay (Figure 2B). We also found that the optimal temporal relationship of DAN activation and odor presentation for memory formation was similar, but not identical, for DANs innervating different MB compartments (Figure 2—figure supplement 4).

Long-term aversive memory in flies requires repetitive electric shock conditioning with resting intervals, so-called spaced training (Tully et al., 1994). We found that two sets of DANs, PPL1-α3 alone (MB630B) or the combination of PPL1-γ2α′1 and PPL1-α′2α2 (MB099C) can induce 1-day and 4-day aversive memory after spaced training (Figure 2C), suggesting that the effects of spaced training can be implemented in individual compartments. Making memory formation dependent on repetitive training might be beneficial by allowing an animal to ignore spurious one-time events. Recent work has shown that the γ2α′1 compartments play key roles in both sleep regulation and long-term memory (Sitaraman et al., 2015; Haynes et al., 2015). The observation that co-activation of PPL1-γ2α′1 and other DANs synergistically prolongs memory retention (Aso et al., 2012) raises the possibility that PPL1-γ2α′1 might act broadly to facilitate memory consolidation by promoting sleep after learning.

We found that a particular DAN’s ability to induce the formation of immediate, 1-day and 4-day memories is not correlated. For example, immediate memory after a single pairing with activation of PPL1-α3 (MB630B) was barely detectable, although multiple activations resulted in 4-day memory (Figure 2C). In contrast, PPL1-γ1pedc (MB320C) activation resulted in robust immediate memory acquisition after a single round of training, but its activation failed to induce 4-day memory even after extensive spaced training (Figure 2C). These results imply the stability of memory is an intrinsic property of the MB compartment, rather than a consequence of the training protocol. In this view, repetitive training with naturalistic stimuli that activate many DAN cell types would recruit additional compartments with slower acquisition rates and the behaviorally assayed retention of memory would reflect the combined memories formed in different compartments (Figure 2—figure supplement 5). It remains an open question whether short-term memories are converted into long-term memories as biochemical changes in the same synapses or whether these memories are formed separately and in parallel. For olfactory learning in Drosophila, our data are consistent with a model in which memory formation and consolidation can occur independently and in parallel in individual MB compartments; this view does not exclude the possibility that network activity facilitates memory consolidation.

Comparing rules for updating memory

We found that the memories induced in different compartments have different stabilities, displaying different dynamics of spontaneous memory decay over a 1-day period (Figure 3A–B). Memories in each compartment also differed in the extent to which they were reduced by a second presentation of the trained odor without reinforcement (Figure 3C).

Figure 3. Rules for updating memory.

(A) Diagram of the DANs contained in split-GAL4 driver lines, which have been color-coded to facilitate comparison with the plots shown in the subsequent panels. In all experiments, the drivers were crossed with 20xUAS-CsChrimson-mVenus in attP18. (B) Memory decay after 1 d. Left: Flies were trained with a 60-s odor delivery during which thirty 1-s pulses of red light were given, and then tested either immediately (Test 1) or after 1 d (Test 2). Center: To facilitate comparison of PI magnitudes, the sign of the PI in this and subsequent panels was reversed for DANs that induced aversive memory (MB320C, MB099C and MB630B). The bottom and top of each box represents the first and third quartile, and the horizontal line dividing the box is the median. The whiskers represent the 10th and 90th percentiles. N = 8–18. Right: Comparison of memory retention times induced by activation of different DANs. Ratios of the mean PI measured at 1d and individual PIs immediately after training are shown for each driver. Asterisk indicates significance of depicted pairs after comparing all pairs. (C) Decrease in the conditioned response by unpaired odor exposure. After the first training and test (Test 1), flies were exposed to both odors without optogenetic DAN activation and then retested (Test 2). In panels CE, Test 1 and Test 2 were performed on the same group of flies and thus the ratios of individual data points are plotted in the rightmost graphs. N = 10–18. (D) Decrease in the conditioned response by unpaired DAN activation. After first training and test (Test 1), flies were exposed to light to activate DANs (thirty 1-s pulses) without odor delivery, and then retested (Test 2). N = 10–17. (E) Flexibility to learn a new association. After first training and test (Test 1), flies were trained with the opposite pairing of odor and DAN activation and tested for their ability to learn the new pairing (Test 2). N = 10–16. For most DANs, the ability to learn the second association was not significantly impaired by the first training. The exception was MB043C, which expresses in the DAN innervating the α1 compartment. (F) Memory capacity. Flies were trained and tested with two pairs of odors. Odor A and B were pentyl acetate and 3-octanol, respectively. Odor C and D were ethyl lactate and 4-methylcyclohexanol, respectively. N = 8–12. For MB320C (compartments γ1 and pedc) only the most recently trained odor was retained, an effective memory capacity of one. For MB043C (α1 compartment), flies were able to remember both comparisons, demonstrating a memory capacity of at least two. Asterisk indicates significance: n.s. not significant, *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16135.012

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. One-day memory is resistant to unpaired DAN activation.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

Unlike with immediate memory that was induced by a single training (Figure 3D), memory induced by spaced training with MB099C or MB630B and tested after 1 d does not show a significant reduction after DAN activation. N = 15.

Likewise, DAN activation without odor presentation significantly reduced immediate memory (Figure 3D) for four of the five sets of DANs tested. These two effects might be mechanistically linked as odor presentation alone can result in activation of a subset of dopaminergic neurons (Riemensperger et al., 2005; Mao and Davis, 2009).

In both the case of presentation of odor without dopamine and of dopamine without odor, the association the fly had previously learned is not confirmed. It would make sense for a memory to be diminished when the contingency upon which it is based is found to be unreliable. Consistent with this idea, repetitive spaced training with these same DANs can induce 1-day memory that is resistant to DAN activation (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). The differences we observed between compartments suggest that they weigh the importance of the reliability of the correlation between CS and US differently.

The α1 compartment differed from the other compartments we tested in that it was resistant to memory reduction by DAN activation (Figure 3D). This compartment plays a key role in long-term appetitive memory of nutritious foods (Yamagata et al., 2015; Huetteroth et al., 2015) and has an unusual circuit structure: its MBON (MBON-α1) appears to form synapses on the dendrites of the DAN that innervates the α1 compartment (PAM-α1) forming a recurrent circuit necessary for long-term memory formation (Ichinose et al., 2015). The α1 compartment also showed the least ability to replace an older association with a new one (Figure 3E). This observation suggests that the initial memory may not be affected by the second training, resulting in co-existing appetitive memories for both odors. Indeed, flies were able to retain associations between each of two odors and PAM-α1 (MB043C) activation, while only the most recently learned association was remembered with PPL1-γ1pedc (MB320C) activation (3F). The higher memory capacity of the α1 compartment is not due to generalization, since training with one odor pair did not affect the innate odor preference observed with a different, untrained odor pair. Thus two distinct strategies for updating memories appear to be used in different MB compartments: (1) writing a new memory, while diminishing the old memory; or (2) writing a new memory, while retaining the old memory.

Processing of conflicting memories

Our results suggest that memory formation in each compartment is largely parallel and independent, with compartmental specific rules for updating memories. Such a model of independent memory storage should allow appetitive and aversive memories to be simultaneously formed for the same odor in different compartments. We tested this idea by simultaneously activating DANs to α1 and γ1pedc while exposing flies to an odor (Figure 4A). When flies were tested immediately after training, the odor was strongly aversive, but the same odor became appetitive after 1 day. These results are most easily explained by simultaneous formation of an aversive memory in γ1pedc and an appetitive memory in α1, with rapid decay of the memory in γ1pedc and slow decay in α1 resulting in a shift in valence of the conditioned response over time. However, the fact that we observed strongly aversive immediate memory, rather than an intermediate response, suggests that the MB network non-linearly integrates these conflicting signals. The known feedforward connection between γ1pedc and α1 provides a possible circuit mechanism (Figure 5B; Aso et al. 2014a). Recent studies (Kaun et al., 2011; Das et al., 2014; Aso et al., 2014b) provide further examples most easily explained by parallel induction of conflicting memories of different decay rates. We also found that wild type flies are capable of efficiently switching odor preference when they had conflicting sequential experiences of sugar reward followed by shock punishment with the same odor (Figure 4B).

Figure 4. Processing of conflicting memories.

Figure 4.

(A) Flies expressing CsChrimson in PPL1-γ1pedc (VT045661-LexA in JK22C x 13xLexAop2-CsChrimson-tdTomato in attP18), or in PAM-α1 (MB043C x 20xUAS-CsChrimson-mVenus in attP18), or in both PPL1-γ1pedc and PAM-α1 (by combining all four transgenes) were starved for 48 hr and then trained with a 60-s odor delivery during which thirty 1-s pulses of red light were given, and then tested either immediately or after 1 d. N = 8–12. (B) Wild type flies were starved for 48 hr and then trained using 2-min exposures to odor A with sucrose and then to odor B without sucrose and tested for odor preference after 1d (Test 1). Following the first memory test, flies were trained with a 1-min exposure of odor A (the odor previously paired with sugar) and electric shock (twelve 1.25 s pulses of 60V) in the olfactory arena and then with odor B without shock. Odor preference was measured immediately after the second training (Test 2). For comparison, wild type flies starved for same period were conditioned with electric shock and tested immediately (Test 3). N = 8.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16135.014

Figure 5. Summary of distinct rules for learning and updating memory.

Figure 5.

(A) Summary table of distinct learning rules induced by different DAN cell types. Criteria were qualitatively judged to be +, ++, +++ or n.d. (not determined) based on the data presented in Figures 2,3. (B) Diagram summarizing the feed forward network within the MB lobes (compartments shown as boxes) and the convergence of MBONs in common target zones in other brain areas (shown as ovals). These circuit motifs might provide a path through which memories distributed in different MB compartments might be integrated (Aso et al., 2014a). Not shown in this diagram are cases where MBONs target the dendrites of DANs, a circuit motif that is known to occur (Aso et al., 2014a; Ichinose et al., 2015) and could also promote communication between compartments.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16135.015

Concluding remarks

Our results demonstrate that different MB compartments use distinct rules for writing and updating memories of odors (Figure 5A). By analyzing individual memory components–or engrams–induced by local dopamine release, we found that the interpretation of a common odor representation carried by sparse KC activity to multiple compartments could be modified differently in each of those compartments. We do not know the mechanisms that generate these distinct learning rules. They could arise from differences in the dopamine release properties of different DAN cell types or from local differences in the biochemical response to dopamine signaling in each MB compartment. For example, KCs express four distinct dopamine receptors (Crocker et al., 2016), which might be deployed differently in each compartment. Or they could originate from circuit properties: we know from anatomical (Tanaka et al., 2008; Aso et al., 2014a), behavioral (Ichinose et al., 2015) and functional imaging (Boto et al., 2014; Cohn et al., 2015; Owald et al., 2015) studies that MB compartments can communicate through connections between their extrinsic neurons, the DANs and MBONs, as well as by a layered network within the MB (Figure 5B). In the mammalian brain, associative memories are also stored as distributed and parallel changes with partially overlapping functions (Herry and Johansen, 2014; Hikosaka et al., 2014; Tonegawa et al., 2015); for example, different populations of dopaminergic neurons develop representations of a visual objects’ value with distinct learning rules (Kim et al., 2015). We expect many of the underlying strategies and mechanisms may be shared between flies and other species. Our work provides a foundation for experiments aimed at understanding the molecular and circuit mechanisms by which distributed memory components are written with distinct rules and later integrated to guide memory-based behaviors.

Materials and methods

Fly strains

Crosses of split-GAL4 lines for DANs (Aso et al., 2014a) and 20xUAS-CsChrimson-mVenus in attP18 (Klapoetke et al., 2014) were kept on standard cornmeal food supplemented with retinal (0.2 mM all-trans-retinal prior to eclosion and then 0.4 mM) at 22°C at 60% relative humidity in the dark. Female flies were sorted on cold plates at least 1 d prior to the experiments and 4–10 d old flies were used for experiments. The new split-GAL4 driver, MB630B was designed based on confocal image databases (http://flweb.janelia.org) (Jenett et al., 2012), BrainBase (http://brainbase.imp.ac.at), and constructed by inserting VT026773-p65ADZp in attP40 and R72B05-ZpGAL4DBD in attP2 as described previously (Pfeiffer et al., 2010). VT045661-LexA was constructed as described previously (Pfeiffer et al., 2010) and injected into JK22C (Knapp et al., 2015). The confocal images of expression patterns are available online (http://www.janelia.org/split-gal4). For driving CsChrimson by both MB109B and MB315C, 20xUAS-CsChrimson-mVenus in attP18 was first combined with MB315C, and then crossed with MB109B.

Optogenetic olfactory arena

The olfactory arena for optogenetics experiments was designed based on the four-field olfactometer (Pettersson, 1970; Vet et al., 1983) and was briefly described in previous reports (Aso et al., 2014b; Hige et al., 2015). The overview of the assay is described in Figure 1—figure supplement 1 and a detailed description of the apparatus is provided in Supplementary file 1; the Janelia TechTransfer office (techtransfer@janelia.hhmi.org) will provide complete construction documentation and CAD files upon request.

Behavioral assay

Groups of approximately 20 females of 4–10 d post-eclosion were trained and tested at 25°C at 50% relative humidity in a dark chamber. The flow rate of input air from each of the four arms was maintained at 100 mL/min throughout the experiments by mass-flow controllers, and air was extracted from the central hole at 400 mL/min. Odors were delivered to the arena by switching the direction of airflow to the tubes containing diluted odors using solenoid valves. The odors were diluted in paraffin oil (Sigma–Aldrich): 3-octanol (OCT; 1:1000; Merck) and 4-methylcyclohexanol (MCH; 1:750; Sigma–Aldrich), Pentyl acetate (PA: 1:5000; Sigma–Aldrich) and ethyl lactate (EL: 1:5000; Sigma–Aldrich). Shock and sugar conditioning was performed as previously described by using tubes with sucrose absorbed Whatman 3 MM paper or copper grids (Figure 2—figure supplement 3) (Aso et al., 2012; Liu et al 2012). For the experiments in Figure 4B, a sheet of copper grid was placed at the bottom of arena. For appetitive memory assays, flies were starved for 24–48 hr on 1% agar. Videography was performed at 30 frames per second and analyzed using Fiji (Schindelin et al., 2012). Statistical comparisons were performed (Prism; Graphpad Inc, La Jolla, CA 92037) using the Kruskal Wallis test followed by Dunn's post-test for multiple comparison, except those in Figure 1F, Figure 2C and Figure 2—figure supplement 4 which used Wilcoxon signed-rank test with Bonferroni correction to compare from zero.

Acknowledgements

We thank Igor Negrashov, Steven Sawtelle, Peter Polidoro, William Rowell, Jinyang Liu, Alice Robie, Kristin Branson, Chuntao Dan, Roman Huber and Hiromu Tanimoto for help in establishing the olfactory arena. Brandi Sharp, James McMahon, Lori Laughrey, Teri Ngo and the Janelia Fly Facility helped in fly husbandry. Rebecca Vorimo, Allison Sowell and the FlyLight Project Team performed brain dissections and histological preparations. VT026773-p65ADZp was a gift from Barry J Dickson. Heather Dionne made new molecular constructs. We thank Gowan Tervo, Joshua Dudman, Ulrike Heberlein, TJ Florence, Yichun Shuai, Kit Longden, Adam Hantman, Daisuke Hattori, Larry Abbott, Richard Axel, Chuntao Dan, Krystyna Keleman, Toshihide Hige, Glenn Turner, Toshiharu Ichinose, Nobuhiro Yamagata and Hiromu Tanimoto for stimulating discussions and for comments on earlier drafts of the manuscript.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grant:

  • Howard Hughes Medical Institute to Yoshinori Aso, Gerald M Rubin.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Author contributions

YA, Conceived and designed the study, Acquired and analyzed the data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article, Wrote the article.

GMR, Conceived and designed the study, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article, Wrote the article.

Additional files

Supplementary file 1. Design and parts list of the olfactory behavioral apparatus.

The first page shows a side view of the apparatus with a parts list. On the second page, a 3D model of the apparatus is shown which can be rotated and the visualization of each part can be individually turned on or off.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16135.016

elife-16135-supp1.pdf (1.6MB, pdf)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16135.016

References

  1. Aso Y, Grübel K, Busch S, Friedrich AB, Siwanowicz I, Tanimoto H. The mushroom body of adult Drosophila characterized by GAL4 drivers. Journal of Neurogenetics. 2009;23:156–172. doi: 10.1080/01677060802471718. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aso Y, Hattori D, Yu Y, Johnston RM, Iyer NA, Ngo T-TB, Dionne H, Abbott LF, Axel R, Tanimoto H, Rubin GM. The neuronal architecture of the mushroom body provides a logic for associative learning. eLife. 2014a;3:e16135. doi: 10.7554/eLife.04577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Aso Y, Herb A, Ogueta M, Siwanowicz I, Templier T, Friedrich AB, Ito K, Scholz H, Tanimoto H. Three dopamine pathways induce aversive odor memories with different stability. PLoS Genetics. 2012;8:e16135. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1002768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Aso Y, Sitaraman D, Ichinose T, Kaun KR, Vogt K, Belliart-Guérin G, Plaçais P-Y, Robie AA, Yamagata N, Schnaitmann C, Rowell WJ, Johnston RM, Ngo T-TB, Chen N, Korff W, Nitabach MN, Heberlein U, Preat T, Branson KM, Tanimoto H, Rubin GM. Mushroom body output neurons encode valence and guide memory-based action selection in Drosophila. eLife. 2014b;3:e16135. doi: 10.7554/eLife.04580. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Aso Y, Siwanowicz I, Bräcker L, Ito K, Kitamoto T, Tanimoto H. Specific dopaminergic neurons for the formation of labile aversive memory. Current Biology. 2010;20:1445–1451. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.06.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Berry JA, Cervantes-Sandoval I, Nicholas EP, Davis RL. Dopamine is required for learning and forgetting in Drosophila. Neuron. 2012;74:530–542. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Blum AL, Li W, Cressy M, Dubnau J. Short- and long-term memory in Drosophila require cAMP signaling in distinct neuron types. Current Biology. 2009;19:1341–1350. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Boto T, Louis T, Jindachomthong K, Jalink K, Tomchik SM. Dopaminergic modulation of cAMP drives nonlinear plasticity across the Drosophila mushroom body lobes. Current Biology. 2014;24:822–831. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.03.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bouzaiane E, Trannoy S, Scheunemann L, Plaçais PY, Preat T. Two independent mushroom body output circuits retrieve the six discrete components of Drosophila aversive memory. Cell Reports. 2015;11:1280–1292. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2015.04.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Burke CJ, Huetteroth W, Owald D, Perisse E, Krashes MJ, Das G, Gohl D, Silies M, Certel S, Waddell S. Layered reward signalling through octopamine and dopamine in Drosophila. Nature. 2012;492:433–437. doi: 10.1038/nature11614. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Christian KM, Thompson RF. Neural substrates of eyeblink conditioning: acquisition and retention. Learning & Memory. 2003;10:427–455. doi: 10.1101/lm.59603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Claridge-Chang A, Roorda RD, Vrontou E, Sjulson L, Li H, Hirsh J, Miesenböck G. Writing memories with light-addressable reinforcement circuitry. Cell. 2009;139:405–415. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2009.08.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cohn R, Morantte I, Ruta V. Coordinated and Compartmentalized Neuromodulation Shapes Sensory Processing in Drosophila. Cell. 2015;163:1742–1755. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.11.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Crocker A, Guan XJ, Murphy CT, Murthy M. Cell-type-specific transcriptome analysis in the Drosophila mushroom body reveals memory-related changes in gene expression. Cell Reports. 2016;15:1580–1596. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2016.04.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Das G, Klappenbach M, Vrontou E, Perisse E, Clark CM, Burke CJ, Waddell S. Drosophila learn opposing components of a compound food stimulus. Current Biology. 2014;24:1723–1730. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.05.078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. de Belle JS, Heisenberg M. Associative odor learning in Drosophila abolished by chemical ablation of mushroom bodies. Science. 1994;263:692–695. doi: 10.1126/science.8303280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dean P, Porrill J, Ekerot CF, Jörntell H. The cerebellar microcircuit as an adaptive filter: experimental and computational evidence. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2010;11:30–43. doi: 10.1038/nrn2756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dubnau J, Grady L, Kitamoto T, Tully T. Disruption of neurotransmission in Drosophila mushroom body blocks retrieval but not acquisition of memory. Nature. 2001;411:476–480. doi: 10.1038/35078077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Ebbinghaus H. Memory: A Contribution to Experimental Psychology. New York: Dover Publications; 1885. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Frost WN, Castellucci VF, Hawkins RD, Kandel ER. Monosynaptic connections made by the sensory neurons of the gill- and siphon-withdrawal reflex in Aplysia participate in the storage of long-term memory for sensitization. PNAS. 1985;82:8266–8269. doi: 10.1073/pnas.82.23.8266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Galili DS, Dylla KV, Lüdke A, Friedrich AB, Yamagata N, Wong JY, Ho CH, Szyszka P, Tanimoto H. Converging circuits mediate temperature and shock aversive olfactory conditioning in Drosophila. Current Biology. 2014;24:1712–1722. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.06.062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gerber B, Yarali A, Diegelmann S, Wotjak CT, Pauli P, Fendt M. Pain-relief learning in flies, rats, and man: basic research and applied perspectives. Learning & Memory. 2014;21:232–252. doi: 10.1101/lm.032995.113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gervasi N, Tchénio P, Preat T. PKA dynamics in a Drosophila learning center: coincidence detection by rutabaga adenylyl cyclase and spatial regulation by dunce phosphodiesterase. Neuron. 2010;65:516–529. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Haynes PR, Christmann BL, Griffith LC. A single pair of neurons links sleep to memory consolidation in Drosophila melanogaster. eLife. 2015;4 doi: 10.7554/eLife.03868. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Heisenberg M, Borst A, Wagner S, Byers D. Drosophila mushroom body mutants are deficient in olfactory learning. Journal of Neurogenetics. 1985;2:1–30. doi: 10.3109/01677068509100140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Heisenberg M. Mushroom body memoir: from maps to models. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2003;4:266–275. doi: 10.1038/nrn1074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Herry C, Johansen JP. Encoding of fear learning and memory in distributed neuronal circuits. Nature Neuroscience. 2014;17:1644–1654. doi: 10.1038/nn.3869. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hige T, Aso Y, Modi MN, Rubin GM, Turner GC. Heterosynaptic plasticity underlies aversive olfactory learning in Drosophila. Neuron. 2015;88:985–998. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hikosaka O, Kim HF, Yasuda M, Yamamoto S. Basal ganglia circuits for reward value-guided behavior. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2014;37:289–306. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-013924. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Huetteroth W, Perisse E, Lin S, Klappenbach M, Burke C, Waddell S. Sweet taste and nutrient value subdivide rewarding dopaminergic neurons in Drosophila. Current Biology. 2015;25:751–758. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.01.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Ichinose T, Aso Y, Yamagata N, Abe A, Rubin GM, Tanimoto H. Reward signal in a recurrent circuit drives appetitive long-term memory formation. eLife. 2015;4 doi: 10.7554/eLife.10719. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Isabel G, Pascual A, Preat T. Exclusive consolidated memory phases in Drosophila. Science. 2004;304:1024–1027. doi: 10.1126/science.1094932. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Jenett A, Rubin GM, Ngo TT, Shepherd D, Murphy C, Dionne H, Pfeiffer BD, Cavallaro A, Hall D, Jeter J, Iyer N, Fetter D, Hausenfluck JH, Peng H, Trautman ET, Svirskas RR, Myers EW, Iwinski ZR, Aso Y, DePasquale GM, Enos A, Hulamm P, Lam SC, Li HH, Laverty TR, Long F, Qu L, Murphy SD, Rokicki K, Safford T, Shaw K, Simpson JH, Sowell A, Tae S, Yu Y, Zugates CT. A GAL4-driver line resource for Drosophila neurobiology. Cell Reports. 2012;2:991–1001. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2012.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kaun KR, Azanchi R, Maung Z, Hirsh J, Heberlein U. A Drosophila model for alcohol reward. Nature Neuroscience. 2011;14:612–619. doi: 10.1038/nn.2805. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kim HF, Ghazizadeh A, Hikosaka O. Dopamine neurons encoding long-term memory of object value for habitual behavior. Cell. 2015;163:1165–1175. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.10.063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kim YC, Lee HG, Han KA. D1 dopamine receptor dDA1 is required in the mushroom body neurons for aversive and appetitive learning in Drosophila. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:7640–7647. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1167-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kirkhart C, Scott K. Gustatory learning and processing in the Drosophila mushroom bodies. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:5950–5958. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3930-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Klapoetke NC, Murata Y, Kim SS, Pulver SR, Birdsey-Benson A, Cho YK, Morimoto TK, Chuong AS, Carpenter EJ, Tian Z, Wang J, Xie Y, Yan Z, Zhang Y, Chow BY, Surek B, Melkonian M, Jayaraman V, Constantine-Paton M, Wong GK, Boyden ES. Independent optical excitation of distinct neural populations. Nature Methods. 2014;11:338–346. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Knapp JM, Chung P, Simpson JH. Generating customized transgene landing sites and multi-transgene arrays in Drosophila using phiC31 integrase. Genetics. 2015;199:919–934. doi: 10.1534/genetics.114.173187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Krashes MJ, DasGupta S, Vreede A, White B, Armstrong JD, Waddell S. A neural circuit mechanism integrating motivational state with memory expression in Drosophila. Cell. 2009;139:416–427. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2009.08.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Laurent G, Naraghi M. Odorant-induced oscillations in the mushroom bodies of the locust. Journal of Neuroscience. 1994;14:2993–3004. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-05-02993.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Lin S, Owald D, Chandra V, Talbot C, Huetteroth W, Waddell S. Neural correlates of water reward in thirsty Drosophila. Nature Neuroscience. 2014;17:1536–1542. doi: 10.1038/nn.3827. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Liu C, Plaçais PY, Yamagata N, Pfeiffer BD, Aso Y, Friedrich AB, Siwanowicz I, Rubin GM, Preat T, Tanimoto H. A subset of dopamine neurons signals reward for odour memory in Drosophila. Nature. 2012;488:512–516. doi: 10.1038/nature11304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Luo L. Principles of Neurobiology. New York: Garland Science; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  45. Mao Z, Davis RL. Eight different types of dopaminergic neurons innervate the Drosophila mushroom body neuropil: anatomical and physiological heterogeneity. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2009;3 doi: 10.3389/neuro.04.005.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. McGuire SE, Le PT, Osborn AJ, Matsumoto K, Davis RL. Spatiotemporal rescue of memory dysfunction in Drosophila. Science. 2003;302:1765–1768. doi: 10.1126/science.1089035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Owald D, Felsenberg J, Talbot CB, Das G, Perisse E, Huetteroth W, Waddell S. Activity of defined mushroom body output neurons underlies learned olfactory behavior in Drosophila. Neuron. 2015;86:417–427. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Pai TP, Chen CC, Lin HH, Chin AL, Lai JS, Lee PT, Tully T, Chiang AS. Drosophila ORB protein in two mushroom body output neurons is necessary for long-term memory formation. PNAS. 2013;110:7898–7903. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1216336110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Perez-Orive J, Mazor O, Turner GC, Cassenaer S, Wilson RI, Laurent G. Oscillations and sparsening of odor representations in the mushroom body. Science. 2002;297:359–365. doi: 10.1126/science.1070502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Pettersson J. An aphid sex attractant. I. Biological studies. Insect Systematics & Evolution. 1970;1:63–73. doi: 10.1163/187631270X00357. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. Pfeiffer BD, Ngo TT, Hibbard KL, Murphy C, Jenett A, Truman JW, Rubin GM. Refinement of tools for targeted gene expression in Drosophila. Genetics. 2010;186:735–755. doi: 10.1534/genetics.110.119917. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Plaçais PY, Trannoy S, Friedrich AB, Tanimoto H, Preat T. Two pairs of mushroom body efferent neurons are required for appetitive long-term memory retrieval in Drosophila. Cell Reports. 2013;5:769–780. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2013.09.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Plaçais PY, Trannoy S, Isabel G, Aso Y, Siwanowicz I, Belliart-Guérin G, Vernier P, Birman S, Tanimoto H, Preat T. Slow oscillations in two pairs of dopaminergic neurons gate long-term memory formation in Drosophila. Nature Neuroscience. 2012;15:592–599. doi: 10.1038/nn.3055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Qin H, Cressy M, Li W, Coravos JS, Izzi SA, Dubnau J. Gamma neurons mediate dopaminergic input during aversive olfactory memory formation in Drosophila. Current Biology. 2012;22:608–614. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.02.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Riemensperger T, Völler T, Stock P, Buchner E, Fiala A. Punishment prediction by dopaminergic neurons in Drosophila. Current Biology. 2005;15:1953–1960. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.09.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Schindelin J, Arganda-Carreras I, Frise E, Kaynig V, Longair M, Pietzsch T, Preibisch S, Rueden C, Saalfeld S, Schmid B, Tinevez JY, White DJ, Hartenstein V, Eliceiri K, Tomancak P, Cardona A. Fiji: an open-source platform for biological-image analysis. Nature Methods. 2012;9:676–682. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Schroll C, Riemensperger T, Bucher D, Ehmer J, Völler T, Erbguth K, Gerber B, Hendel T, Nagel G, Buchner E, Fiala A. Light-induced activation of distinct modulatory neurons triggers appetitive or aversive learning in Drosophila larvae. Current Biology. 2006;16:1741–1747. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Schwaerzel M, Monastirioti M, Scholz H, Friggi-Grelin F, Birman S, Heisenberg M. Dopamine and octopamine differentiate between aversive and appetitive olfactory memories in Drosophila. Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23:10495–10502. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-33-10495.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Shuai Y, Hirokawa A, Ai Y, Zhang M, Li W, Zhong Y. Dissecting neural pathways for forgetting in Drosophila olfactory aversive memory. PNAS. 2015;112:E6663–E6672. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1512792112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Sitaraman D, Aso Y, Jin X, Chen N, Felix M, Rubin GM, Nitabach MN. Propagation of homeostatic sleep signals by segregated synaptic microcircuits of the Drosophila mushroom body. Current Biology. 2015;25:2915–2927. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.09.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Séjourné J, Plaçais PY, Aso Y, Siwanowicz I, Trannoy S, Thoma V, Tedjakumala SR, Rubin GM, Tchénio P, Ito K, Isabel G, Tanimoto H, Preat T. Mushroom body efferent neurons responsible for aversive olfactory memory retrieval in Drosophila. Nature Neuroscience. 2011;14:903–910. doi: 10.1038/nn.2846. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Tanaka NK, Tanimoto H, Ito K. Neuronal assemblies of the Drosophila mushroom body. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2008;508:711–755. doi: 10.1002/cne.21692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Tanimoto H, Heisenberg M, Gerber B. Experimental psychology: event timing turns punishment to reward. Nature. 2004;430:983. doi: 10.1038/430983a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Tomchik SM, Davis RL. Dynamics of learning-related cAMP signaling and stimulus integration in the Drosophila olfactory pathway. Neuron. 2009;64:510–521. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.09.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Tomchik SM. Dopaminergic neurons encode a distributed, asymmetric representation of temperature in Drosophila. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:2166–2176. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3933-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Tonegawa S, Liu X, Ramirez S, Redondo R. Memory engram cells have come of age. Neuron. 2015;87:918–931. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Trannoy S, Redt-Clouet C, Dura JM, Preat T. Parallel processing of appetitive short- and long-term memories in Drosophila. Current Biology. 2011;21:1647–1653. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Tully T, Preat T, Boynton SC, Del Vecchio M. Genetic dissection of consolidated memory in Drosophila. Cell. 1994;79:35–47. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(94)90398-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Vet LEM, Lenteren J, Heymans M, Meelis E. An airflow olfactometer for measuring olfactory responses of hymenopterous parasitoids and other small insects. Physiological Entomology. 1983;8:97–106. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3032.1983.tb00338.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  70. Yamagata N, Ichinose T, Aso Y, Plaçais P-Y, Friedrich AB, Sima RJ, Preat T, Rubin GM, Tanimoto H. Distinct dopamine neurons mediate reward signals for short- and long-term memories. PNAS. 2015;112:578–583. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1421930112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Yarali A, Nehrkorn J, Tanimoto H, Herz AV. Event timing in associative learning: from biochemical reaction dynamics to behavioural observations. PLoS One. 2012;7:e16135. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0032885. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Zars T, Fischer M, Schulz R, Heisenberg M. Localization of a short-term memory in Drosophila. Science. 2000;288:672–675. doi: 10.1126/science.288.5466.672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
eLife. 2016 Jul 21;5:e16135. doi: 10.7554/eLife.16135.018

Decision letter

Editor: Liqun Luo1

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "Dopaminergic neurons write and update memories with cell-type-specific rules" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor, Liqun Luo, and K VijayRaghavan as the Senior Editor. The following individuals involved in review of your submission have agreed to reveal their identity: Josh Dubnau (Reviewer #1); Yi Zhong (Reviewer #2); Leslie C Griffith (Reviewer #3).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

All three reviewers, as well as the Reviewing Editor, are highly enthusiastic about the significance, and most of the critiques can be addressed by textual changes. However, the first critique of Reviewer 1 regarding back-crossing represents substantial amount of work if the current data were collected from flies that had not been back-crossed to a common genetic background. We hope that these flies were indeed back-crossed and it was an omission in the methods. If they had not been, after much discussion among the Reviewers and Reviewing Editor, we think that it is necessary to perform additional controls to exclude the possibility that the differences reported in Figures 3,2 are due to different backgrounds of flies harboring individual transgenes.

[We note that in the Aso et al., 2014 paper, you used many conditional manipulations (e.g. shibire). So even though each Gal4 line or split may be on a unique background, you had a within genotype temperature shift manipulation so we could see the acute impact of that cell type. Here this is not the case, and there are direct comparisons between lines and conclusions drawn in a comparative sense between the cell types.]

Specifically, we suggest that you perform standard electric shock/sugar training paradigms with each of the key strains (Chrimson included), and examine short term and long term memory. If each of these strains you use has comparable conditioned responses to electric shock and sugar, we can be more certain that the differences with light-induced training are due to cell-type specific Chr expression, not something in the strain background.

Reviewer #1:

The MB are a highly studied neural center of associative olfactory memory and learning in flies. A large body of prior work establishes that odor driven CS inputs are represented as sparse patterns of activity within the large ensemble of MB Kenyon cslls. US driven dopaminergic neuron inputs are thought to converge onto MB Kenyon cells in the MB lobes, which is the site of Kenyon cell synapses with output neurons. Recent work has established that the different sub-types of dopaminergic neurons (DANs) and output neurons (MBONs) are each restricted within zones of the MB lobes. The anatomical organization of the MB Kenyon cell sub-types also implies that the DAN/MBON zones are restricted to sub-types of Kenyon cells. Layered onto this anatomical model is a large body of work that demonstrates that memory is processed through a series of genetically dissectible phases, that rely on different sub-sets of the MB Kenyon cells. This manuscript investigates very interesting questions about how the anatomical organization of the DANs and MBONs into sub-zones provide functional modules upon which each of the memory phases after appetitive vs aversive memories are built. The questions being asked are important and the world view that the authors propose is beautiful. So on a deep level, I am highly enthusiastic. Unfortunately, however, I have several foundational critiques that undermine the major conclusions of this study. This is really unfortunate, because I think that if properly done, this could be a really important piece of work. Below I outline in roughly descending order of importance, the issues that I have with this study in its current form.

1) If my read of the Methods section is accurate, the authors have made no attempt to control the genetic backgrounds in any of their experiments. This is a fundamental and in my view fatal flaw. There is a long and rich history of this, and it is well established that for all quantitative behavioral traits, genetic background needs to be controlled between all groups for which differences in phenotype are used to draw conclusions. In this case, the authors have used Gal4, and Gal4 reagents that were generated on an outbred background. So every single line has a founder effect, which means that the genetic background of every single strain used is completely unique. As a result, all bets are off in terms of how we interpret differences in magnitude of performance, kinetics of memory decay, and even olfactory acuity and locomotion behaviors that underlie the memory performance. Any and all of the 'rules' that are discovered here that govern how activation of a given DAN might contribute to short term or long-term memory, or to its extinction, etc., could just as easily be due to differences in the kinetics and processing of memory between genetic backgrounds. If I had to guess from my own personal experience, I would wager that a decent fraction of observed quantitative differences between different DANs will fail to reproduce when the genetic backgrounds are equilibrated. We simply cannot fill the literature with this sort of doubt.

2) The authors state up front that strong optogenetic activation of a single class of neuron may not reflect real physiological US inputs. Although some reviewers may have an issue with that, I actually have no conceptual problem with it: I buy the argument that this is a good framework by which to pick apart the rules that govern plasticity at each DAN:KC:MBON zone. However, it is essential to do this sort of analysis in parallel with 'real' learning in which a real US is used (sugar reward, electric shock or some other aversive stimulus). And then within the context of a real US, it is important to query the contribution of that DAN to performance. Otherwise, the insight that we gain from activating a DAN in place of the US is severely reduced.

Critiques that need to be addressed with text revisions:

3) The major dopamine receptor that is established to mediate the US inputs is the type 1 receptor, DopR. Mutations in DopR can be rescued by expression only in MB (Kim et al) and in the case of aversive memory, expression of this receptor only in MB γ neurons is sufficient to fully restore all stages of memory. Given this, how do the authors interpret the fact that activation of DANs that are restricted to zones outside γ lobes are able to induce aversive memories? Is there a different Dopamine receptor at play? Or should we conclude that DANs also co-release other transmitters that are not acting through a dopamine receptor? I am not arguing that the authors need to resolve this issue experimentally, but they cannot ignore this.

4) Long term memory in this field has an operational meaning that is mis-applied here. When this field uses this term, we mean memory that requires new protein synthesis. In general, it has been required for any new behavioral assay that one use inhibitors of protein synthesis and/or establish a requirement for the CREB gene and/or show a difference between massed versus spaced training. Here, the authors use a spaced training protocol, and then refer to a long lived trace as long-term memory. But we really don't know whether this is the same consolidated form of memory that others have discussed. Moreover, the text confounds the impact of multiple training sessions on acquisition versus consolidation.

5) The accepted historical nomenclature for Pavlovian literature is flaunted badly. Extinction has a specific meaning in the literature, and it does not involve presentation of the US absent the CS. So activation of DANs after training should not be called extinction, and it shouldn't be said to extinguish memory. And even in the cases where CS+ is presented post training, it is not clear that the decrement in performance is due to extinction. If I read the methods correctly, this is a degradation of avoidance under continuous odor exposure. So this easily could be due to (e.g.) sensory adaptation. Extinction has specific definitions, and there are behavioral tests for whether extinction has taken place. Similarly, the term 'relieve learning' is not really correct. I realize that this term has been used by Tanimoto et al. But the larger literature on Pavlovian learning has already established terms: conditioned excitation and conditioned inhibition. Relief learning is a rediscovery of CI and CE.

6) The discussion of parallel vs. sequential memory formation is superficial. Isabel et al. and Blum et al. do not impact this issue in the way the authors have implied, and I don't think the findings in this paper bear on this discussion in a compelling way.

In sum, I very much like the outlook and the over-all approach. But this manuscript does not provide compelling data to support the main conclusions. I realize that the bar I am setting would require (e.g.) back-crossing all lines to the same strain and then repeating most or all of the experiments. But this bar has been set, appropriately, by a consensus in this field that conclusions that are drawn from behavioral comparisons with uncontrolled genetic backgrounds do not withstand the test of time. We all live by this standard.

Reviewer #2:

In this manuscript, the authors systematically analyzed the impacts of artificial activation of several DAN subsets on olfactory memory formation, retention, extinction, extinguishment and reverse. The majority of their discoveries are new while a few observations are confirmative of previously published reports. All observations presented in this manuscript together help to clarify the roles of DANs in different memory processes and memory components and build a systematic understanding of involvement of the dopaminergic system in memory. This manuscript is suitable for publication in eLife if the following concerns are addressed:

1) The statement concerning "extinguish" needs to be clarified. To show whether the first odor memory is extinguished, the authors may want to perform a third odor test (test the preference between the first odor and a third odor).

2) The manuscript included 1d and 4d odor memories. It would be of interests to determine whether these memories are long-term one or not through feeding of flies with CXM.

3) The grammatical mistakes should be corrected, such as "each innervate distinct MB compartments" and "a series of experiences is integrated to make a probabilistic prediction".

4) Some technical terms, such as "Drosophila" and Gal4 names, should be written in the right format.

Reviewer #3:

This paper is an interesting and important contribution to our understanding of how dopamine can influence behavior. The last few years have seen a number of high profile papers that suggest that particular subsets of DAergic neurons can have specialized roles in either temporal or functional domains. This paper steps back and takes a very broad view of this issue looking at most of the DAergic neurons and testing their roles in multiple temporal scales in the encoding and extinguishing of appetitive and aversive memories.

The results are important for the field in two ways. First, they demonstrate that essentially all DAergic neurons have some specialist qualities. The previously described "forgetting neurons" and "STM" and "LTM" neurons are not outliers, but rather reflect a basic feature of the system. Second, they provide very strong evidence to buttress the idea that there are multiple parallel streams of memory formation. Short-term memories are not simply converted to long-term memories, but rather they are independently encoded. This paper is well-suited for this eLife format since these data are directly tied to the previous Aso papers and the tools they generated.

My only comments regard putting these data in context of older findings:

1) The figure legends have way too much text. Descriptions of data not presented in a figure should be in the Results section.

2) A subset of PPL1 neurons are activated by odors (Mao & Davis 2009). How does this affect the interpretation of Figure 3C (odor extinction) and Figure 3D (US extinction)?

3) Yarali & Gerber (2010) found that TH-gal4 neurons are not necessary for relief learning. Does this study conflict with these prior findings (i.e., in finding that gamm1-pedc is sufficient for relief learning), or do you propose that there are both DAN and non-DAN mediated relief learning pathways?

eLife. 2016 Jul 21;5:e16135. doi: 10.7554/eLife.16135.019

Author response


All three reviewers, as well as the Reviewing Editor, are highly enthusiastic about the significance, and most of the critiques can be addressed by textual changes. However, the first critique of Reviewer 1 regarding back-crossing represents substantial amount of work if the current data were collected from flies that had not been back-crossed to a common genetic background. We hope that these flies were indeed back-crossed and it was an omission in the methods. If they had not been, after much discussion among the Reviewers and Reviewing Editor, we think that it is necessary to perform additional controls to exclude the possibility that the differences reported in Figures 3,2 are due to different backgrounds of flies harboring individual transgenes.

[We note that in the Aso et al., 2014 paper, you used many conditional manipulations (e.g. shibire). So even though each Gal4 line or split may be on a unique background, you had a within genotype temperature shift manipulation so we could see the acute impact of that cell type. Here this is not the case, and there are direct comparisons between lines and conclusions drawn in a comparative sense between the cell types.]

Specifically, we suggest that you perform standard electric shock/sugar training paradigms with each of the key strains (Chrimson included), and examine short term and long term memory. If each of these strains you use has comparable conditioned responses to electric shock and sugar, we can be more certain that the differences with light-induced training are due to cell-type specific Chr expression, not something in the strain background.

We performed the suggested experiments. These data, which confirm that the strains have comparable conditioned responses, have been added as Figure 2—figure supplement 3.

Reviewer #1:

The MB are a highly studied neural center of associative olfactory memory and learning in flies. A large body of prior work establishes that odor driven CS inputs are represented as sparse patterns of activity within the large ensemble of MB Kenyon cslls. US driven dopaminergic neuron inputs are thought to converge onto MB Kenyon cells in the MB lobes, which is the site of Kenyon cell synapses with output neurons. Recent work has established that the different sub-types of dopaminergic neurons (DANs) and output neurons (MBONs) are each restricted within zones of the MB lobes. The anatomical organization of the MB Kenyon cell sub-types also implies that the DAN/MBON zones are restricted to sub-types of Kenyon cells. Layered onto this anatomical model is a large body of work that demonstrates that memory is processed through a series of genetically dissectible phases, that rely on different sub-sets of the MB Kenyon cells. This manuscript investigates very interesting questions about how the anatomical organization of the DANs and MBONs into sub-zones provide functional modules upon which each of the memory phases after appetitive vs aversive memories are built. The questions being asked are important and the world view that the authors propose is beautiful. So on a deep level, I am highly enthusiastic. Unfortunately, however, I have several foundational critiques that undermine the major conclusions of this study. This is really unfortunate, because I think that if properly done, this could be a really important piece of work. Below I outline in roughly descending order of importance, the issues that I have with this study in its current form.

1) If my read of the Methods section is accurate, the authors have made no attempt to control the genetic backgrounds in any of their experiments. This is a fundamental and in my view fatal flaw. There is a long and rich history of this, and it is well established that for all quantitative behavioral traits, genetic background needs to be controlled between all groups for which differences in phenotype are used to draw conclusions. In this case, the authors have used Gal4, and Gal4 reagents that were generated on an outbred background. So every single line has a founder effect, which means that the genetic background of every single strain used is completely unique. As a result, all bets are off in terms of how we interpret differences in magnitude of performance, kinetics of memory decay, and even olfactory acuity and locomotion behaviors that underlie the memory performance. Any and all of the 'rules' that are discovered here that govern how activation of a given DAN might contribute to short term or long-term memory, or to its extinction, etc., could just as easily be due to differences in the kinetics and processing of memory between genetic backgrounds. If I had to guess from my own personal experience, I would wager that a decent fraction of observed quantitative differences between different DANs will fail to reproduce when the genetic backgrounds are equilibrated. We simply cannot fill the literature with this sort of doubt.

We agree that difference in genetic background can influence behavioral phenotypes, especially for long-term memory. However, we note that the split-GAL4 stocks that we used are much more similar in background than the stocks that the reviewer correctly notes have proved problematic in the past. Those stocks were often genetic mutations or enhancer trap GAL4 lines in widely different genetic backgrounds. While the split-GAL4 lines we used were not back-crossed, these lines are in similar genetic backgrounds: the AD and DBD DNA constructs used in the split-GAL4 lines were injected into the same set of recipient stocks and inserted into the same landing sites, and the X chromosome in all split-GAL4 lines was exchanged with that of a common stock. Also, unlike in many previous experiments that studied the behavioral effects of mutations, we do not test these split-GAL4 lines as homozygotes, but as a population of the heterozygous progeny of a cross between a specific split-GAL4 line and a common 20xUAS-CsChrimson-mVenus stock, thus masking the effect of any recessive genetic differences between the split-GAL4 stocks.

2) The authors state up front that strong optogenetic activation of a single class of neuron may not reflect real physiological US inputs. Although some reviewers may have an issue with that, I actually have no conceptual problem with it: I buy the argument that this is a good framework by which to pick apart the rules that govern plasticity at each DAN:KC:MBON zone. However, it is essential to do this sort of analysis in parallel with 'real' learning in which a real US is used (sugar reward, electric shock or some other aversive stimulus). And then within the context of a real US, it is important to query the contribution of that DAN to performance. Otherwise, the insight that we gain from activating a DAN in place of the US is severely reduced.

We agree that studying the effects of blocking specific DANs after presentation of a “real” US (shock or sugar) will be needed to fully understand associative learning. However, interpreting the results of such experiments is complicated by the fact that shock or sugar activate multiple DANs (Kirkhart, C. & Scott, K. 2015; Mao, Z. & Davis, R. L. 2009; Liu, C. et al. 2012) and almost certainly, given previous studies (Aso et al., 2012) and the results we present in this paper, induce partially redundant memories in multiple compartments in parallel. Thus making conclusions about the roles of individual DANs by studying the behavioral effects of their inactivation is not straightforward and would, at a minimum, require construction of strains that would allow different, specific combinations of DANs to be inactivated together. These experiments are technically challenging and beyond the scope of the current study. Indeed, avoiding the complications of redundancy was a major motivation of our current study’s experimental design: inducing memory in defined compartment(s) by optogenetic stimulation and then assaying memory dynamics using behavioral assays.

Critiques that need to be addressed with text revisions:

3) The major dopamine receptor that is established to mediate the US inputs is the type 1 receptor, DopR. Mutations in DopR can be rescued by expression only in MB (Kim et al) and in the case of aversive memory, expression of this receptor only in MB γ neurons is sufficient to fully restore all stages of memory. Given this, how do the authors interpret the fact that activation of DANs that are restricted to zones outside γ lobes are able to induce aversive memories? Is there a different Dopamine receptor at play? Or should we conclude that DANs also co-release other transmitters that are not acting through a dopamine receptor? I am not arguing that the authors need to resolve this issue experimentally, but they cannot ignore this.

In Figure 2D, we showed that PPL1-α3 can induce 1-day and 4-day memory after 10x spaced training. These data fit well with previous reports that rutabaga rescue in α/β KCs can restore LTM (Blum 2010), but appear at first glance to be inconsistent with the results reported in Qin et al. 2012 that showed full restoration of both immediate and 1-day memory in DopR1 mutants by driving DopR1 in γ KCs but not in α/β KCs. There are many possible explanations for such lack of agreement, including the use of other dopamine receptors in α/β Kenyon cells. Indeed, α/β and γ Kenyon cells have been reported to express high levels of four different dopamine receptors (Crocker et al., 2016). We added a statement about the presence of multiple dopamine receptors to the text.

4) Long term memory in this field has an operational meaning that is mis-applied here. When this field uses this term, we mean memory that requires new protein synthesis. In general, it has been required for any new behavioral assay that one use inhibitors of protein synthesis and/or establish a requirement for the CREB gene and/or show a difference between massed versus spaced training. Here, the authors use a spaced training protocol, and then refer to a long lived trace as long-term memory. But we really don't know whether this is the same consolidated form of memory that others have discussed. Moreover, the text confounds the impact of multiple training sessions on acquisition versus consolidation.

We have modified the text to use the simple operational description of “4-day” memory throughout, rather than “long-term” memory, which we agree implies specific attributes that were not assessed by our experiments.

5) The accepted historical nomenclature for Pavlovian literature is flaunted badly. Extinction has a specific meaning in the literature, and it does not involve presentation of the US absent the CS. So activation of DANs after training should not be called extinction, and it shouldn't be said to extinguish memory. And even in the cases where CS+ is presented post training, it is not clear that the decrement in performance is due to extinction. If I read the methods correctly, this is a degradation of avoidance under continuous odor exposure. So this easily could be due to (e.g.) sensory adaptation. Extinction has specific definitions, and there are behavioral tests for whether extinction has taken place. Similarly, the term 'relieve learning' is not really correct. I realize that this term has been used by Tanimoto et al. But the larger literature on Pavlovian learning has already established terms: conditioned excitation and conditioned inhibition. Relief learning is a rediscovery of CI and CE.

We removed the terms “extinction” and “relief learning” from the text. Instead we simply describe our experiments in operational terms.

6) The discussion of parallel vs. sequential memory formation is superficial. Isabel et al. and Blum et al. do not impact this issue in the way the authors have implied, and I don't think the findings in this paper bear on this discussion in a compelling way.

We have modified this section of the text and removed these two citations. We agree that our data to not resolve the issue of parallel vs. sequential memory formation and now simply say that our results “provide support for” parallel learning.

We also added an additional experiment that we feel bears directly on the issue of parallel memory (new Figure 4).

Reviewer #2:

In this manuscript, the authors systematically analyzed the impacts of artificial activation of several DAN subsets on olfactory memory formation, retention, extinction, extinguishment and reverse. The majority of their discoveries are new while a few observations are confirmative of previously published reports. All observations presented in this manuscript together help to clarify the roles of DANs in different memory processes and memory components and build a systematic understanding of involvement of the dopaminergic system in memory. This manuscript is suitable for publication in eLife if the following concerns are addressed:

1) The statement concerning "extinguish" needs to be clarified. To show whether the first odor memory is extinguished, the authors may want to perform a third odor test (test the preference between the first odor and a third odor).

We removed the terms “extinction” and “relieve learning” from the text. Instead we simply describe our experiments in operational terms.

2) The manuscript included 1d and 4d odor memories. It would be of interests to determine whether these memories are long-term one or not through feeding of flies with CXM.

We have modified the text to use the simple operational description of “4-day” memory throughout, rather than “long-term” memory, which we agree implies specific attributes that were not assessed by our experiments.

3) The grammatical mistakes should be corrected, such as "each innervate distinct MB compartments" and "a series of experiences is integrated to make a probabilistic prediction".

Done. (“Series” is a singular noun.)

4) Some technical terms, such as "Drosophila" and Gal4 names, should be written in the right format.

Done.

Reviewer #3:

This paper is an interesting and important contribution to our understanding of how dopamine can influence behavior. The last few years have seen a number of high profile papers that suggest that particular subsets of DAergic neurons can have specialized roles in either temporal or functional domains. This paper steps back and takes a very broad view of this issue looking at most of the DAergic neurons and testing their roles in multiple temporal scales in the encoding and extinguishing of appetitive and aversive memories.

The results are important for the field in two ways. First, they demonstrate that essentially all DAergic neurons have some specialist qualities. The previously described "forgetting neurons" and "STM" and "LTM" neurons are not outliers, but rather reflect a basic feature of the system. Second, they provide very strong evidence to buttress the idea that there are multiple parallel streams of memory formation. Short-term memories are not simply converted to long-term memories, but rather they are independently encoded. This paper is well-suited for this eLife format since these data are directly tied to the previous Aso papers and the tools they generated.

My only comments regard putting these data in context of older findings:

1) The figure legends have way too much text. Descriptions of data not presented in a figure should be in the Results section.

We moved some text from figure legends into Results as suggested.

2) A subset of PPL1 neurons are activated by odors (Mao & Davis 2009). How does this affect the interpretation of Figure 3C (odor extinction) and Figure 3D (US extinction)?

We agree that the results of Mao & Davis (2009) showing that odor presentation alone can result in activation of a subset of dopaminergic neurons (different to those that we studied here) raises the possibility that activated dopaminergic neurons might play a role in the phenotype we report in Figure 3C. We have added a comment to that effect in the text.

3) Yarali & Gerber (2010) found that TH-gal4 neurons are not necessary for relief learning. Does this study conflict with these prior findings (i.e., in finding that gamm1-pedc is sufficient for relief learning), or do you propose that there are both DAN and non-DAN mediated relief learning pathways?

This apparent conflict could be explained by redundancy among DANs, since neither TH-GAL4 nor DDC-GAL4 include all the DANs to MB.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Supplementary file 1. Design and parts list of the olfactory behavioral apparatus.

    The first page shows a side view of the apparatus with a parts list. On the second page, a 3D model of the apparatus is shown which can be rotated and the visualization of each part can be individually turned on or off.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16135.016

    elife-16135-supp1.pdf (1.6MB, pdf)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16135.016

    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES