Abstract
Functional imaging studies in human reliably identify a trio of scene-selective regions, one on each of the lateral [occipital place area (OPA)], ventral [parahippocampal place area (PPA)], and medial [retrosplenial complex (RSC)] cortical surfaces. Recently, we demonstrated differential retinotopic biases for the contralateral lower and upper visual fields within OPA and PPA, respectively. Here, using functional magnetic resonance imaging, we combine detailed mapping of both population receptive fields (pRF) and category-selectivity, with independently acquired resting-state functional connectivity analyses, to examine scene and retinotopic processing within medial parietal cortex. We identified a medial scene-selective region, which was contained largely within the posterior and ventral bank of the parieto-occipital sulcus (POS). While this region is typically referred to as RSC, the spatial extent of our scene-selective region typically did not extend into retrosplenial cortex, and thus we adopt the term medial place area (MPA) to refer to this visually defined scene-selective region. Intriguingly MPA co-localized with a region identified solely on the basis of retinotopic sensitivity using pRF analyses. We found that MPA demonstrates a significant contralateral visual field bias, coupled with large pRF sizes. Unlike OPA and PPA, MPA did not show a consistent bias to a single visual quadrant. MPA also co-localized with a region identified by strong differential functional connectivity with PPA and the human face-selective fusiform face area (FFA), commensurate with its functional selectivity. Functional connectivity with OPA was much weaker than with PPA, and similar to that with face-selective occipital face area (OFA), suggesting a closer link with ventral than lateral cortex. Consistent with prior research, we also observed differential functional connectivity in medial parietal cortex for anterior over posterior PPA, as well as a region on the lateral surface, the caudal inferior parietal lobule (cIPL). However, the differential connectivity in medial parietal cortex was found principally anterior of MPA. We suggest that there is posterior–anterior gradient within medial parietal cortex, with posterior regions in the POS showing retinotopically based scene-selectivity and more anterior regions showing connectivity that may be more reflective of abstract, navigationally pertinent and possibly mnemonic representations.
Keywords: retinotopy, population receptive fields, scene-selectivity, resting-state functional connectivity, memory
Introduction
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies investigating scene-perception in human reliably and repeatedly identify a trio of spatially distinct scene-selective regions, one on each of the lateral [occipital place area (OPA) or transverse occipital sulcus (TOS)] (Kravitz et al., 2011; Nasr et al., 2011; Dilks et al., 2013; Silson et al., 2015), ventral [parahippocampal place area (PPA)] (Epstein and Kanwisher, 1998) and medial [retrosplenial complex (RSC)] (Epstein, 2005, 2008; Aminoff et al., 2007) cortical surfaces.
Despite their shared category-selectivity, a division of labor between these regions has been suggested recently, with OPA and PPA reportedly more involved in the visual representation of scenes (MacEvoy and Epstein, 2007; Silson et al., 2015) and landmark coding (Marchette et al., 2015), respectively, whereas RSC is implicated in spatial memory (MacEvoy and Epstein, 2007; Marchette et al., 2015), navigation (Epstein and Higgins, 2007), topographical orientation (Kim et al., 2015), remembering previous events (Gilmore et al., 2016) and the anchoring of internal spatial representations (Marchette et al., 2014).
Recently, the presence and influence of retinotopic information has been demonstrated within both OPA (Nasr et al., 2011; Silson et al., 2015, 2016) and PPA (Arcaro et al., 2009; Silson et al., 2015). For example, we demonstrated differential representations of the contralateral lower and upper visual fields within OPA and PPA, respectively (Silson et al., 2015). These data suggest not only that computations within both OPA and PPA are likely constrained by their underlying visual field representations, but also, that each area is likely adapted to visual features that occur in the lower and upper visual fields, respectively (Silson et al., 2015). The magnitude of these biases, coupled with the consistency across individuals compelled us to ask whether RSC similarly exhibits systematic retinotopic biases. Unlike OPA and PPA, previous work provides conflicting evidence for the presence of retinotopic information within RSC. Although larger response magnitudes were reported for scenes presented contralaterally, fMRI adaptation was found not to depend on visual field position, indicating perhaps, the presence of receptive fields that span the vertical meridian (MacEvoy and Epstein, 2007) and suggesting RSC plays a more prominent role in spatial memory rather than scene perception per se (MacEvoy and Epstein, 2007). Others have reported a strong representation of the far periphery of the contralateral visual field within a similarly located region of medial parietal cortex identified through mental navigation (Huang and Sereno, 2013).
Here, we combined detailed mapping of both population receptive fields (pRF) and scene-selectivity with independently acquired resting-state functional connectivity analyses to explore scene and retinotopic processing in medial parietal cortex. Across participants, medial scene-selectivity was identified reliably within the posterior bank of ventral parieto-occipital sulcus (POS) and did not extend into retrosplenial cortex (Morris et al., 2000; Maguire, 2001; Epstein, 2008). We propose referring to this visually defined scene-selective region as the medial place area (MPA). Our analyses of pRF responses demonstrates a high degree of retinotopic sensitivity within MPA. Both left and right MPA exhibited significant biases for the contralateral visual field coupled with large receptive field sizes. Further, we confirmed this preference for contralateral space in MPA in an independent event-related quadrant experiment. Beyond its retinotopic sensitivity, our resting-state analyses highlight that MPA is more closely associated with the ventral than lateral surface. Dividing PPA into posterior and anterior subdivisions revealed a region of high differential connectivity, adjacent but anterior of scene- and retinotopically selective MPA. Taken together, these data suggest a posterior–anterior gradient of representation exists within medial parietal cortex, whereby the posterior bank of POS represents scene information retinotopically and anterior portions of medial parietal cortex mediate more spatial, navigational, and potentially mnemonic processes.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Sixteen participants (nine male, mean age = 31 years) completed retinotopic mapping (eight runs) and functional localizer (two runs) scans. Ten of these participants (six female, mean age = 28 years) also completed an additional event-related experiment with scenes presented in the four quadrants of the visual field. A separate group of 48 participants (33 female, mean age = 25 years) completed resting-state functional connectivity sessions. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and gave written informed consent in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki. The National Institutes of Health Institutional Review Board approved the consent and protocol. This work was supported by the Intramural Research program of the National Institutes of Health – National Institute of Mental Health Clinical Study Protocol 93-M-0170, NCT00001360.
fMRI Retinotopic Mapping, Functional Localizer and Event-Related Sessions
Participants were scanned on either a research-dedicated GE 3 Tesla Signa scanner or a research-dedicated Siemens 7 Tesla Magnetom scanner in the Clinical Research Centre on the National Institutes of Health campus (Bethesda, MD, USA). Across scanners, oblique slices were oriented approximately parallel to the base of the temporal lobe and extended posteriorly through all of visual cortex. All participants completed at least eight runs of pRF mapping as well as two runs of an additional category-selective functional localizer. Participants in the event-related sessions completed six event-related runs as well as two runs of an additional category-selective functional localizer.
3T Scanning Parameters
Partial volumes of the occipital and temporal cortices were acquired using an eight-channel head coil (21 slices; 2 mm × 2 mm × 2 mm; 10% interslice gap; TR 2 s; TE 30 ms; matrix size, 96 × 96; FOV 192 mm).
7T Scanning Parameters
Partial volumes of the occipital and temporal cortices were acquired using a 32-channel head coil (42 slices; 1.2 mm × 1.2 mm × 1.2 mm; 10% interslice gap; TR 2 s, TE 27 ms; matrix size, 170 × 170; FOV 192 mm).
Visual Stimuli and Tasks
Population Receptive Field Mapping
Population receptive field mapping sessions were conducted at either 3T (11 participants) or 7T (5 participants) field strengths. During pRF mapping sessions a bar aperture traversed gradually through the visual field, whilst revealing random scene fragments. During each 36 s sweep the aperture made 18 evenly spaced steps, one every 2 s (1TR), to traverse the entire screen. During each bar position (1TR) five scene fragments were displayed in rapid succession (400 ms per image). Across the 18 aperture positions all 90 possible scene images were displayed once. Thus, in any single sweep, each scene occurred only once, reducing the likelihood that participants mentally ‘fill-in’ in the underlying image, a problem that can arise if a single background image is revealed gradually. A total of eight sweeps were performed in each run (four orientations, two directions). Specifically, the bar aperture progressed in the following order for all (8) runs: Left–Right, Bottom Right–Top Left, Top–Bottom, Bottom Left–Top Right, Right–Left, Top Left–Bottom Right, Bottom–Top, and Top Right–Bottom Left. The bar stimuli covered a circular aperture (20° diameter 7T; 15° diameter 3T. During runs, participants performed a color detection task at fixation, indicating via button press when the white fixation dot changed to red. Color fixation changes occurred semi-randomly, with approximately two color changes per sweep.
Category-Selective Functional Localizers
Functional localizer runs were conducted at either 3T (11 participants) or 7T (5 participants) field strengths. All participants completed two runs in order to localize scene- and face-selective areas. These scans employed an on/off design (scenes/faces) with 16 alternating blocks (16 s) of 20 stimuli (5° × 5°) presented while participants performed a one-back task. Within a block, 20 stimuli were presented for 300 ms each, separated by a 500 ms inter-stimulus interval. Additionally five participants completed independent localizer scans employing a multiblocked design. In these sessions, images from eight categories including faces, buildings, scenes (manmade/natural, open/closed), and man-made and natural objects, were presented in 16-s blocks with an 8 s blank fixation period separating blocks. Each category was presented twice per run, and the order of presentation was counterbalanced across participants and runs.
Event-Related Scene Quadrant Presentation
Event-related scanning sessions were conducted at either 3T (six participants) or 7T (four participants) field strengths. Participants performed an attention-demanding task at fixation whilst whole-scene images were presented randomly in one of the four quadrants of the visual field (top left, top right, bottom left, and bottom right). Stimuli subtended 6.5° × 6.5° and were centered 6.5° from the central fixation cross into one of the quadrants of the visual field. Participants maintained fixation throughout. Scene stimuli were scaled to subtend the same visual angle of the screen during both 3T and 7T scans. As a scene was presented, one arm of the fixation cross, either the horizontal or the vertical, increased in length. Participants were required to identify, via button response, the longer fixation arm. Stimulus presentation and fixation cross changes occurred simultaneously. Within each run, each scene (n = 24) appeared at each location (n = 4) for 400 ms, with a jittered (4–12 s) interstimulus fixation period; thus, each run contained 96 trials. The order of presentations and fixation arm extensions was randomized within each run. Participants completed six runs of the event-related experiment.
Population receptive field and functional localizer fMRI preprocessing. All data were analyzed using the Analysis of Functional NeuroImages (AFNI) software package1 (Cox, 1996). All functions and programs are readily available in the current version: AFNI binary version February 10, 2016. Prior to functional localizer, event-related and pRF analyses, all images for each participant were motion corrected to the first volume of the first run, after removal of the appropriate ‘dummy’ volumes (8) to allow stabilization of magnetization. Post motion-correction data were smoothed with a 2 mm full-width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel for both 3T and 7T localizer runs and event-related runs.
Localizer Analysis
To identify scene- and face-selective regions of interest (ROIs), significance maps of the brain were computed in each participant by performing a correlation analysis between the assumed hemodynamic response function and the activation time courses thresholded at p < 0.0001 (uncorrected). Significance maps were then projected onto surface reconstructions of the gray–white matter boundary of individual hemispheres. Only contiguous clusters of nodes exceeding the above threshold were defined as scene- or face-selective. The anatomical locations of these clusters were then inspected in order to define ROIs consistent with previously published work (Sayres and Grill-Spector, 2008; Schwarzlose et al., 2008; Kravitz et al., 2011; Nasr et al., 2011; Marchette et al., 2014, 2015; Silson et al., 2015).
Event-Related Analysis
For event-related runs, performing t-tests between each condition (top left, bottom left, top right, and bottom right) and baseline generated significance maps. The β-values for each condition were extracted from voxels within each ROI and averaged.
Population Receptive Fields Mapping Analysis
Population Receptive Field analyses of unsmoothed data were conducted in AFNI, using a pRF implementation for the AFNI distribution (Silson et al., 2015). The model divides the stimulated field of view into an X, Y grid with 200 samples across its height and width, and for each position in that X, Y grid, sigma (pRF size) values are simultaneously sampled at the same resolution, but over a default range of 0 to half the size of the field of view (sampled at 100 even intervals). These default parameters result in 4 million possible pRF’s (all possible combinations of X, Y location and sigma). Given the position of the stimulus in the visual field at every TR, the estimated time series for a pRF of a given location (X, Y) and size (sigma) can be estimated by convolving these estimates with a 2-D stimulus time series, which contains binary masks of the stimulus location at each TR. Both Simplex and Powell optimization algorithms are utilized to find the best time series/parameter sets (X, Y, and sigma) by minimizing the least-squares error of the predicted time series measured against the acquired time series in each voxel. The model outputs for each voxel the X, Y location representing the center of the receptive field; sigma, which represents the diameter (size) of the receptive field; and R2, which corresponds to the explained variance of the fit and can be used to statistically threshold these data.
Visual field coverage plots, which are built by combining the best Gaussian receptive field model for each voxel within an ROI, can be computed from these data. The coverage plots for a given ROI are an aggregation of these Gaussians. Assuming a strong central tendency in the centers of the receptive fields, a linear aggregation (e.g., summation) will result in a coverage plot appearing as a single large Gaussian. In our analyses a max operator is used. This creates a coverage plot that reflects, at each point, the maximum pRF value from all of the receptive field models within an ROI (Winawer et al., 2010). Thus, the coverage plot reflects the maximum envelope of all the Gaussians within an ROI. While this allows for a non-symmetric shape, the edges of that plot will often evidence a Gaussian falloff as few pRFs define the edges.
Delineation of Visual Field Maps
To rule out the possibility that our MPA ROI was simply an extension of retinotopy present within antecedent visual areas, we identified V1, V2d, V2v, V3d, V3v, and hV4 in all participants. To identify visual field maps in individual participants, the representations of polar angle and eccentricity were visualized on surface reconstructions of both hemispheres and inspected. Surface reconstructions of the gray and white matter boundary of individual participant hemispheres were made using the Freesurfer4 autorecon script2. Retinotopically organized maps were visible and present in all tested hemispheres. Notwithstanding subtle inter-participant variability, the main features of the maps, in particular the reversals in visual field representation at the vertical and horizontal meridians were consistent across participants. In accordance with previous reports (Engel et al., 1994; Sereno et al., 1995; DeYoe et al., 1996; Larsson and Heeger, 2006; Wandell et al., 2007) retinotopic visual field maps were delineated using the following criteria: (1) the polar angle representations displayed reversals. That is, the representations of polar angle in neighboring visual areas were mirror reversals of one another, with a reversal in the representation along their shared boundary; (2) the polar angle and eccentricity components within each visual area were organized largely orthogonal to one another.
Resting-State Functional Connectivity
3T scanning parameters
Volumes of the whole brain were acquired using a 32-channel head coil. Multi-echo EPI scans were collected with the following parameters: TEs = 14.9, 28.4, 41.9 ms, TR = 2 s, ASSET acceleration factor = 2, flip-angle = 65°, bandwidth = 250.000 kHz, FOV = 24 cm × 24 cm, acquisition matrix = 64 × 64, resolution = 3.4 mm × 3.4 mm × 3.4 mm, slice gap = 0.3 mm, 34 slices per volume covering the whole brain. Respiratory and cardiac traces were recorded. For resting-state MRI runs, the first 30 volumes were discarded. In each participant two independent 20-min rest periods (eyes closed) were acquired.
Resting state fMRI preprocessing
Preprocessing of the fMRI data was performed using AFNI. Each echo was processed independently prior to optimal combination of the data from each TE (see below). Slice-time correction was applied (3dTShift) and signal outliers were attenuated [3dDespike (Jo et al., 2013)]. Motion correction parameters were estimated relative to the first volume of the middle TE (28.4 ms), and registered to the structural scan (3dSkullStrip, 3dAllineate). These registration parameters were then applied in one step (3dAllineate) and the data were resampled to 3 mm isotropic resolution. The three TEs were combined as described below.
The optimal echo time for imaging the BOLD effect is where the TE is equal to T2∗. Because T2∗ varies across the brain, single echo images are not optimal to see this variation. By acquiring multiple echoes, this enables the calculation of the “optimal” T2∗ weighted average of the echoes, which allows one to recover signals in dropout areas and improves contrast-to-noise ratio (Posse et al., 1999; Poser et al., 2006; Kundu et al., 2014; Evans et al., 2015).
After optimal combination, we applied the basic ANATICOR (Jo et al., 2010) procedure to yield nuisance time series for the ventricles and local estimates of the BOLD signal in white matter. All nuisance time-time series [six parameters of motion, local white matter, ventricle signal, and six physiological noise regressors (AFNI: RetroTS)] were detrended with fourth order polynomials. For rest data, these regressors, along with a series of sine and cosine functions to remove all frequencies outside the range (0.01–0.25 Hz) were regressed out in a single regression step (AFNI program 3dTproject).
Time points with motion greater than 0.3 mm were removed from the data [scrubbing, see Power et al. (2012)] and replaced with values obtained via linear interpolation in time. These cleaned time series were aligned to the standard mesh for subsequent analysis (@Suma_AligntoExperiment). No smoothing was applied to these data.
Resting state analysis
Category-selective ROIs for resting state analysis were derived from the group data (n = 16). Using the contrasts described above, these ROIs [PPA, OPA, MPA, fusiform face area (FFA), and Occipital face area (OFA)] were restricted to the top 300 category selective nodes in each region to account for large differences in ROI size. This thresholding procedure revealed anterior and posterior components of PPA, which were considered separately (aPPA and pPPA) in some analyses, resulting in the following ROI set (MPA, OPA, pPPA, aPPA, FFA, and OFA).
Results
Scene-Selectivity in Medial Parietal Cortex
Initially, scene-selective regions, one on each of the lateral (OPA), ventral (PPA), and medial (RSC) cortical surfaces were identified, where possible, in each participant (OPA = 16 bilaterally, PPA = 16 bilaterally, RSC = 13 left hemisphere and RSC = 15, right hemisphere) using the contrast of scenes > faces (p < 10-4, uncorrected). The locations of our OPA, PPA and RSC ROIs (Figure 1) are entirely consistent with previous literature (Epstein and Kanwisher, 1998; Epstein, 2005, 2008; Kravitz et al., 2011; Nasr et al., 2011; Baldassano et al., 2013; Silson et al., 2015, 2016).
As others have noted (Nasr et al., 2011), despite the commonly used name, ‘RSC,’ the scene-selective activation in medial parietal cortex appears to lie largely outside retrosplenial cortex proper. Indeed, RSC is identified frequently within the posterior bank of ventral POS, in close proximity to, but importantly spatially distinct from, the peripheral representations of both primary visual cortex (V1) and V2d (Epstein, 2005, 2008; Nasr et al., 2011; Baldassano et al., 2013; Stevens et al., 2015). However, human retrosplenial cortex is located largely within the callosal sulcus, extending onto the cingulate gyrus primarily posteroventrally, specifically the isthmus of the cingulate gyrus (Morris et al., 2000; Maguire, 2001; Epstein, 2008). Given that the peak of the scene-selective activation is largely contained within the POS and that at a standard threshold (for example see Sayres and Grill-Spector, 2008; Kravitz et al., 2010, 2011) the group activation does not extend onto the cingulate gyrus, we think that the continued use of ‘retrosplenial’ to refer to this region is potentially misleading. Instead, we suggest referring to this visually defined scene-selective region as the medial place area (MPA).
To determine to what extent this localization of scene-selectivity outside of retrosplenial cortex is dependent on the statistical threshold we employed, we systematically varied this threshold (Figure 2). At lower thresholds, a small peak of activation does emerge on the cingulate gyrus, potentially corresponding to BA29/30, but it is dorsal to the isthmus and only extends into the callosal sulcus, where most of retrosplenial cortex is contained, at very low thresholds (Figures 2D,E). To investigate individual variability in the spatial extent of the MPA, we calculated in how many participants the scene-selective activation extended onto the cingulate gyrus and into the commonly drawn BA 29/30. In the left hemisphere, activation in BA29/30 was only present in 2/13 participants and in 1/15 participants in the right hemisphere. Finally, to investigate to what extent the specific contrast we employed (scenes vs. faces) impacts the spatial extent of the medial parietal scene-selectivity, we compared the extent of scene-selective activation in a subset of our participants (n = 5) who participated in a separate experiment allowing us to define scene-selective activation by the contrast scenes vs. objects. This alternative contrast identified a similarly located region within the POS (see Supplementary Figure S1). Importantly, in all participants the spatial extent of the ROI based on scene vs. objects did not extend anteroventrally in the direction of restrosplenial cortex beyond the boundary defined by scenes vs. faces.
Retinotopic Profile of MPA
Historically, scene-selective regions in medial parietal cortex have been implicated in the processing and mediating of relatively high-level visual and spatial processes such as spatial memory (MacEvoy and Epstein, 2007; Marchette et al., 2015), navigation (Epstein and Higgins, 2007), topographical orientation (Kim et al., 2015), remembering past events (Gilmore et al., 2016), and the anchoring of internal spatial representations (Marchette et al., 2014), rather than thought of as sensitive to visual field position, as would be predicted of a retinotopically responsive region.
To explore the retinotopic profile of MPA, we initially calculated, in each participant, the mean time-series of all voxels within both left and right MPA and then averaged across participants. Notwithstanding subtle differences in the pRF’s across voxels, which will exhibit different time-courses, the existence of retinotopically sensitive responses following such averaging demonstrates the retinotopic sensitivity of this region at a gross level. These time-courses are shown for the right hemisphere of a representative participant and at the group level in Figure 3.
Inspection of Figure 3 reveals a number of interesting features. First, the fact that MPA voxels exhibit sensitivity to our simple and restricted stimuli (bar containing a restricted fragment of a scene), shows that scene features, without detailed context, are sufficient to drive this region. Second, in both single participant and group time-series, eight peaks of activity are clearly evident, which reflect the eight sweeps our pRF mapping stimulus made through the visual field and thus through the pRF’s of MPA voxels (Figure 3A). Third, the stimuli at the top of Figure 3A, shows position in the visual field corresponding to the peak of our group level time-series during each sweep (adjusted temporally to account for the lag in hemodynamic response). The region of visual space over which these bar positions overlap maximally, reflecting the positions in the visual field that right MPA is most sensitive to are shown in (Figure 3B). Importantly, although this region covers portions of the visual field both above and below the horizontal meridian, they are restricted exclusively to the left (contralateral) visual field. Finally, the width of each peak of activity is wide, spanning much of the width of each sweep (dashed vertical lines in Figure 3A), which is suggestive of large receptive fields within MPA (although it is also possible that this reflects a population of neurons with small receptive fields with limited spatial overlap). Crucially, if MPA were retinotopically insensitive, such a systematic pattern of activity would be unlikely to occur.
Next, we sought to characterize MPA’s receptive field profile in a number of ways that mirror our previous characterizations of retinotopy within both OPA and PPA (Silson et al., 2015, 2016). Initially, visual field coverage plots (Winawer et al., 2010; Silson et al., 2015) were computed for each participant and then averaged across participants (Figure 4). These plots were computed from pRF values derived from all nodes within the functionally localized MPA ROIs, without thresholding of the pRF values themselves. Both left and right MPA exhibit clear biases for the contralateral visual field, with more pRF’s centered within, and more representation of, the contralateral visual field in both hemispheres (see proportion of pRF centers per visual quadrant inset in Figure 4A). Such contralaterally biased representations mirror those present in both left and right OPA and PPA, respectively (MacEvoy and Epstein, 2007; Arcaro et al., 2009; Kravitz et al., 2013; Silson et al., 2015). Unlike its lateral and ventral counterparts, however, MPA does not exhibit a consistently clear bias for a single visual quadrant in either hemisphere. Indeed, representations of the contralateral visual field were largely equivalent about the horizontal meridian in both regions (Figure 4A). We tested for any biases for a single quadrant of the visual field using repeated-measures ANOVA with factors Hemisphere (Left, Right), Visual Field (Ipsilateral, Contralateral) and Vertical Position (Upper, Lower). There was a main effect of Hemisphere [F(1,13) = 18.17, p < 0.001], reflecting on average stronger responses in the right than left hemisphere. The main effect of Visual Field was also significant [F(1,13) = 10.45, p < 0.007], reflecting on average stronger responses in the contralateral visual field within both regions. Further, there was a significant Hemisphere by Visual Field interaction [F(1,13) = 4.72, p < 0.49], reflecting on average a stronger contralateral bias in the right than left hemisphere. However, there was no main effect of Vertical Position [F(1,13) = 0.791, p = 0.39] and no interactions involving Vertical Position reflecting on average the largely equal representations within the upper and lower visual fields within both regions. These data demonstrate that both left and right MPA exhibit a contralateral bias, but no bias for a specific quadrant of the visual field.
To further quantify these biases, we calculated contralateral (contralateral minus ipsilateral pRF value) and elevation (contralateral upper minus contralateral lower pRF value) bias measurements in each participant and ROI. Consistent with our previous characterizations of OPA and PPA (Silson et al., 2015), MPA exhibited significant contralateral biases (p < 0.05, relative to a zero bias assumption) in both hemispheres (Figure 4B). In contrast however, the elevation biases within MPA were not significant in either hemisphere (Figure 4C).
To confirm these retinotopic biases within MPA, independently, we measured responses in MPA during a condition-rich event-related experiment. Participants viewed 24 whole scene images presented randomly into one of the four quadrants of the visual field, whilst performing an orthogonal task at fixation (Figure 5A). In agreement with our pRF mapping, univariate analyses (Figure 5B) demonstrated that both left and right MPA exhibited stronger responses for stimuli presented in the contralateral over ipsilateral visual fields (see Supplementary Figure S2 for average β-values for all conditions collapsed across hemispheres). Again contralateral and elevation bias measurements (computed as above) were calculated for each participant and region. Both left and right MPA exhibited significantly stronger responses (p < 0.05, relative to zero bias assumption) to stimuli in the contralateral visual field, confirming this organizational principle within MPA. Elevation biases in MPA, however, were not significant for either hemisphere (Figure 5C).
Finally, we examined whether MPA could be identified based solely on retinotopy. In particular, we asked whether the quality of our pRF estimates (explained variance of the pRF model) could localize this region. Importantly, across participants we observed reliable and statistically significant pRF responses within the posterior and ventral bank of POS in both hemispheres (Figure 6). Indeed, this region either encompassed entirely or overlapped substantially with our MPA ROIs in many participants, but importantly, was separated spatially from both antecedent visual areas (V1 and V2d) and retinotopic maps found in more dorsal regions of POS, such as V6, V6Ad, and V6Av (Pitzalis et al., 2006, 2015; Cardin et al., 2012). The spatial overlap between scene-selective MPA and retinotopic sensitivity in both hemispheres is shown for a representative participant and at the group level in Figure 6.
Taken together, our characterization of MPA pRF’s suggests that, MPA represents predominantly the contralateral visual field. Unlike its lateral and ventral surface counterparts, however, MPA does not exhibit a consistent bias for a single quadrant of the visual field, but rather, exhibits an apparent full hemifield representation.
Resting-State Reveals Differential Connectivity among Scene-Selective Regions
To explore further the relationship between MPA and scene-selective regions on both lateral (OPA) and ventral surfaces (PPA), we analyzed functional imaging data collected from a separate group of 48 participants whilst at rest.
Initially, we examined whether MPA could be identified on the basis of differential connectivity with scene- vs. face-selective regions on both the ventral and lateral surfaces, considered independently. Our principal localization of MPA was based on the direct contrast of scenes > faces, for consistency therefore, we computed differential connectivity measurements for (i) PPA vs. FFA (ventral surface contrast) and (ii) OPA vs. OFA (lateral surface contrast). A region overlapping our MPA ROI was clearly identifiable on the basis of differential connectivity between PPA and FFA in both hemispheres (Figure 7A), but not, between OPA and OFA (Figure 7B), suggesting that MPA is more strongly associated with PPA than OPA.
Posterior–Anterior Gradient Within Medial Parietal Cortex
Previous work has suggested that medial scene-selective regions show stronger functional connectivity with anterior PPA (aPPA) than posterior PPA (pPPA; Baldassano et al., 2013). Given these prior results, we analyzed our resting-state data with respect to pPPA and aPPA (see Materials and Methods), by computing for each node, the differential connectivity between aPPA vs. pPPA. This differential connectivity analysis, illustrated in Figure 8, revealed a number of noteworthy features. First, significant differential aPPA-pPPA connectivity was found within medial parietal cortex, but was largely localized to a region directly adjacent and anterior of MPA in the anterior bank of ventral POS and extending into the precuneus in both hemispheres (Figures 8A,B). Second, highly differential aPPA-pPPA connectivity was also found on the lateral surface, overlapping considerably with the caudal inferior parietal lobule (cIPL) ROI, taken from the Eickhoff–Zilles PGP probabilistic cytoarchitectonic map (Eickhoff et al., 2005, based on Caspers et al., 2006, 2008; Figures 8C,D) consistent with prior work (Baldassano et al., 2013). In contrast, however, lateral portions of visual cortex including OPA and face-selective regions OFA and FFA were significantly more associated with pPPA than aPPA (Figures 8C,D). The spatial adjacency but limited overlap between scene-selective MPA in the posterior bank of POS and the more anterior precuneus region showing significant association with aPPA, suggests a posterior–anterior gradient exists within medial parietal cortex and the POS.
To investigate the functional connectivity in medial parietal cortex further, we created a connectivity-defined ROI (CON) based on the differential connectivity between aPPA vs. pPPA. Importantly, we defined this connectivity ROI on the basis of differential connectivity data acquired during one rest period and subsequently applied this ROI to independent data acquired during a separate rest period within each participant (see Materials and Methods).
Next, to compare the connectivity profiles of the selectivity-defined MPA with the more anterior CON, we directly contrasted the connectivity patterns for these two regions in both hemispheres (Figure 9A). Consistent with our pRF analyses, MPA exhibited stronger connectivity than CON with visual cortical areas, in particular pPPA and OPA, which have been shown previously to evidence strong retinotopy (Silson et al., 2015, 2016). In contrast, the CON ROI showed stronger functional connectivity than MPA with the precuneus (medially), aPPA (ventrally) and with the inferior parietal lobule (laterally) that overlapped with cIPL (Figure 9B). Stronger CON connectivity was also present within anterior temporal, as well as, orbitofrontal and superior frontal cortical locations (Figure 9B).
To characterize the functional connectivity profiles of CON and MPA further, we focused on all scene-related ROIs (MPA, CON, pPPA, aPPA, OPA, and cIPL) and first computed all pair-wise time course correlations (Figure 10). In general, there are positive correlations between all these ROIs in both hemispheres. However, CON shows very low correlation with both pPPA and OPA (regions that are generally considered to represent the visual properties of scenes). Next we analyzed the relative strength of connectivity of MPA and CON by computing differential correlations between our medial parietal ROIs (MPA and CON) and scene-related regions on the (i) ventral surface (pPPA, aPPA), and (ii) lateral surface (OPA and cIPL) in both hemispheres. For each surface we conducted separate two-way repeated-measures ANOVAs with Hemisphere (Left, Right) and Seed ROI (MPA, CON).
For the ventral surface we observed a significant main effect of Hemisphere [F(1,47) = 21.53, p =< 0.0001], reflecting on average greater differential connectivity with aPPA and pPPA in the left compared to right hemisphere. The main effect of Seed was also significant [F(1,47) = 64.51, p = 2.28-10], reflecting on average greater differential connectivity for CON than MPA. Further, there was a significant Hemisphere by Seed interaction [F(1,47) = 59.73, p = 6.49-10], indicating a stronger difference between MPA and CON in the right than left hemisphere (Figure 11A).
For the lateral surface, there was no significant effect of Hemisphere [F(1,47) = 2.52, p = 0.12]. The main effect of Seed was significant [F(1,47) = 123.79, p = 9.14-15], reflecting on average greater differential connectivity with OPA and cIPL for CON over MPA. There was also a significant Hemisphere by Seed interaction [F(1,47) = 4.99, p = 0.03], again indicating a stronger difference between MPA and CON in the right than left hemisphere (Figure 11B). Thus, within the scene-related regions, CON shows a relatively stronger association than MPA with aPPA and cIPL.
Overall, our functional connectivity data highlight (i) differential patterns of connectivity among scene-selective regions with MPA more strongly associated with PPA ventrally than OPA laterally, (ii) significant differential connectivity with aPPA over pPPA localized to a region of medial parietal cortex anterior of MPA in both hemispheres and iii) stronger association of the connectivity-defined medial parietal ROI (CON), compared to MPA, with regions often associated with the memory network, including the precuneus, orbitofrontal cortex, and the anterior temporal lobe (Ranganath and Ritchey, 2012).
Scene-Selectivity and Retinotopy in MPA and the Connectivity-Defined Region
Finally, we compared these two regions within medial parietal cortex in terms of both scene-selectivity and retinotopy. Initially, we examined the relationship between these two functional properties within the MPA alone. Within our group-based MPA ROIs we calculated the correlation between each nodes scene-selectivity index and the explained variance of the pRF model (Figure 12A). This analysis revealed a significant positive correlation (r = 0.41, R2 = 0.16, p < 0.01), suggesting, that in general, nodes within MPA that show greater scene-selectivity also tend to show higher retinotopic sensitivity. These data provide no evidence for separate nodes that are selectively sensitive to either type of visual information (scene-selectivity and retinotopy) alone and suggest that both types of visual information are capable of being represented by the same underlying population of nodes. Second, we examined the scene-selectivity exhibited by both MPA and the CON. To avoid any circularity in this analysis our MPA ROI was redefined, not in terms of its scene-selectivity as before, but in terms of its retinotopic sensitivity. That is, we selected only nodes within the POS that exhibited significant retinotopic responses and then calculated the average scene-selectivity exhibited by both regions. Consistent with our original definition, MPA was found to exhibit significantly greater scene-selectivity than CON [t(14) = 2.62, p < 0.01] (Figure 12B). Finally, we calculated the contralateral bias exhibited by both MPA (defined by its scene-selectivity) and CON. Again, MPA exhibited a significantly greater contralateral bias [t(14) = 2.04, p < 0.05], although of note, CON also exhibited a numerical bias to the contralateral visual field (Figure 12C). These data support the conclusion that MPA is more sensitive than CON to both retinotopic location and visual scene information and suggest a gradient of visual responsiveness across the medial parietal surface.
Discussion
In this study, we combined pRF and category-selectivity mapping with independently acquired resting-state analyses to explore scene and retinotopic processing in medial parietal cortex. We identify a region of scene-selectivity within the posterior and ventral banks of POS that we term the MPA. Our pRF data not only highlight strong spatial sensitivity within this region, but also, the presence of significant contralateral visual field biases, coupled with large pRFs [mean (se) 7.01° (0.52°)]. Our resting state analyses highlight differential patterns of connectivity between MPA and PPA over OPA. Moreover, a region directly anterior of MPA exhibited significant differential connectivity with anterior over posterior PPA divisions, and strong connectivity with cIPL, suggesting a posterior–anterior gradient exists within medial parietal cortex from retinotopic and scene-related representations more posteriorly to more abstract, navigationally relevant and potential mnemonic representations more anteriorly.
Scene-Selectivity in Medial Parietal Cortex
Although originally labeled ‘RSC’ (Epstein, 2005), we, along with others (Nasr et al., 2011; Marchette et al., 2014), find the medial scene-selective region to be located largely within the ventral and posterior bank of POS and generally, spatially separate from both retrosplenial cortex proper (Morris et al., 2000) and the representations of the far periphery in V1 and V2d.
The precise boundaries of human retrosplenial cortex are actively debated in the literature, with conflicting opinions as to its spatial extent with respect to the cingulate gyrus (Maddock, 1999; Morris et al., 2000; Vogt et al., 2000; Maguire, 2001). Retrosplenial cortex corresponds to Brodmann areas 29/30, which encircle the posterior and ventral portion of the corpus callosum (Zilles and Palomero-Gallagher, 2001; Vann et al., 2009), and are in turn partially encircled by the posterior cingulate (BA 23 and 31; Maguire, 2001; Epstein, 2008). Although Brodmann regions have been converted to Talairach space for visualization on cortical surfaces, a number of inaccuracies in the mapping from Brodmann to Talairach have been reported (Vogt et al., 1995) making the accuracy of this mapping difficult to interpret. A detailed analysis of the anatomical location of retrosplenial cortex (Morris et al., 2000) suggests that, like in macaque, its dorsal component is buried within the callosal sulcus, with only the posteroventral portion extending onto the medial surface overlapping the isthmus of the cingulate gyrus (Morris et al., 2000; Maguire, 2001). This anatomical location is both slightly ventral of the tip of the calcarine sulcus and also anterior of the ventral portion of the POS (see Figure 2B in Morris et al., 2000; Figure 1A in Vann et al., 2009 and Shine et al., 2016).
Importantly, the location of scene-selectivity we report here is located largely within the ventral and posterior bank of the POS – it is not only dorsal of the anterior tip of the calcarine sulcus, but also, dorsal and posterior to the isthmus of the cingulate gyrus (Morris et al., 2000; Maguire, 2001), suggesting that it does not extend into retrosplenial cortex proper. A similar spatial separation (estimated at ∼1 cm) between retrosplenial cortex and medial scene-selectivity has been reported previously (Nasr et al., 2011). Such spatial separation from retrosplenial cortex suggests, as others have noted (Nasr et al., 2011; Marchette et al., 2014), that the continued use of ‘retrosplenial’ to refer to this medial scene-selective region may cause unnecessary confusion. We propose adopting a similar nomenclature to that used for PPA (Epstein and Kanwisher, 1998) and more recently OPA (Nasr et al., 2011; Dilks et al., 2013) and argue for the use of the MPA to refer to this visually defined scene-selective region of medial parietal cortex.
Posterior to Anterior Gradient Within Medial Parietal Cortex
We demonstrate a posterior–anterior gradient across medial parietal cortex in terms of both retinotopic properties and functional connectivity (Supplementary Figure S3).
Retinotopy within Medial Parietal Cortex
Our pRF analyses demonstrate that MPA evidences strongly retinotopic voxels, which exhibit significant contralateral biases despite large receptive field sizes. Our analysis based solely on the strength of pRF responses highlighted a region within the ventral and posterior bank of POS that overlapped substantially with our MPA ROI in both hemispheres. In both cases, this region was spatially separate from representations of the far periphery in early visual cortex and from visual field maps present in dorsal POS (Pitzalis et al., 2006; Cardin et al., 2012; Pitzalis et al., 2015), suggesting that these responses do not simply reflect spatial blurring of retinotopy from either antecedent or dorsal retinotopic regions. If this region were simply an extension of retinotopy within V1 and V2d it would represent the very far periphery, which was not stimulated in our current pRF mapping paradigm. Importantly, the retinotopy we demonstrate here was only present within posterior and ventral portions of medial parietal cortex, showing a tight coupling to the location of scene-selective MPA.
Functional Connectivity within Medial Parietal Cortex
The stronger connectivity with aPPA compared with pPPA in medial parietal cortex anterior to scene-selective MPA is consistent with previous reports (Baldassano et al., 2013). However, whilst previous work emphasized some overlap between this region of differential connectivity and scene-selective regions, we find that the strongest differential connectivity with aPPA falls directly adjacent and anterior of MPA, with limited overlap in either hemisphere. The striking spatial adjacency between our scene- and retinotopically sensitive MPA, within the posterior banks of ventral POS, and the more anterior region of strong differential connectivity with aPPA, suggests a gradient in the representation of scene-related visual information within medial parietal cortex along a posterior–anterior axis. Indeed, our direct comparison of MPA vs. our CON demonstrates stronger associations between MPA and posterior visual and category-selective regions, which have been shown previously to be highly retinotopic (Sayres and Grill-Spector, 2008; Silson et al., 2015, 2016), whereas significant CON associations were found within the cIPL and precuneus, as well as anterior temporal, superior frontal, and orbitofrontal areas, respectively.
Our finding of a region anterior of scene-selective MPA that shows strong associations with regions likely involved in high-level spatial-, navigational- and memory-related processes is consistent with a number of recent reports (Marchette et al., 2014; Gilmore et al., 2016). For example, Marchette et al. (2014) utilized searchlight analyses to identify regions of the brain from which imagined facing direction could be decoded successfully. In the right hemisphere, this region was located within the anterior bank of ventral POS and in the left hemisphere this region was anterior and slightly dorsal, of both the anterior bank of POS and RSC (we term MPA; Marchette et al., 2014). Moreover, Gilmore et al. (2016) compared the cortical responses for remembered over imagined events, which highlighted, among other regions, ventral and anterior portions of POS. The proposed gradient within medial parietal cortex, based on the differential connectivity with anterior and posterior PPA divisions is also compatible with a very recent proposal for two scene processing networks derived from functional connectivity data (Baldassano et al., 2016). In this framework, the first network comprises OPA and pPPA and is thought to be more related to processing visual features, whereas the second network consists of cIPL, aPPA, and the RSC (we term MPA). Taking this framework into account, our data suggest that the visually selective MPA is likely associated with the first network, whereas the more anterior region in precuneus (CON) is more associated with the second network.
Differential Functional Connectivity among Scene-Selective Regions
The disproportionately strong relationship between MPA and PPA reported here is consistent with previous resting-state connectivity results (Kim et al., 2015), demonstrating that PPA exhibits the strongest connectivity when seeding RSC (we term MPA) in healthy controls, but not a patient with topographical disorientation (Kim et al., 2015). Moreover, the relatively weak association we observe between MPA and OPA is similar to the weak association reported previously for RSC (MPA) and other category-selective regions including LOC and FFA (Kim et al., 2015). These data suggest that MPA is more closely associated with PPA on the ventral surface than with OPA on the lateral surface, despite sharing a common selectivity profile.
Retinotopy within MPA
Although significant responses to contralaterally (over ipsilaterally) presented scenes have been reported previously within this region (MacEvoy and Epstein, 2007), a systematic evaluation of pRFs has hitherto not been conducted. Indeed, the suggestion that this region is largely insensitive to visual field position and may contain receptive fields that span the vertical meridian were made largely on the basis of larger ipsilateral responses in this region relative to either OPA or PPA and fMRI adaptation that was tolerant to positional changes (MacEvoy and Epstein, 2007). Here, we demonstrate that activity within MPA is modulated systematically by visual field position and that receptive fields of MPA voxels are not only more abundant in the contralateral visual field, but also, that the representations are largely confined therein - although some representation of the ipsilateral visual field is expected given the large size of MPA pRF’s and the assumption of circular RFs in our model (Silson et al., 2015). A previous study (Huang and Sereno, 2013), reported a representation of the far periphery, coupled with an upper field representation within a medial region similar in location to MPA. We too demonstrate peripheral representations within MPA, but do not observe a consistent upper visual bias across either our pRF or event-related analyses. Differences in the methods for both localization of MPA and retinotopy could underlie these discrepancies. For instance, the previous report identified their medial parietal ROI using a mental navigation task and employed a retinotopic mapping paradigm whereby coherent scene clips from a television series were revealed gradually (Huang and Sereno, 2013).
Clear and orderly progressions of the visual field within MPA were not visible consistently in our data, despite the strong retinotopic sensitivity shown by MPA as a whole. MPA’s large receptive fields may prohibit accurate estimates of polar angle in this region under the current pRF paradigm as voxels will respond to a number of visual field positions. Additionally, we do not observe clear clustering within MPA for different quadrants of the visual field (e.g., upper or lower). Again, the large receptive fields within MPA could underpin this. Importantly, however, these data cannot rule out the possibility that a map(s) of the visual field within MPA could be delineated with more sophisticated mapping paradigms at higher resolution. MPA exhibits large pRF sizes are suggestive of more global computations allowing neurons within MPA to pool visual scene information across large areas of the contralateral visual field. The large size of MPA pRF’s likely underpin the more complete hemifield representation exhibited by MPA, compared to the largely quadrant representations within OPA (contralateral lower) and PPA (contralateral upper), respectively (Silson et al., 2015). Although currently not known, it is possible that MPA receives input from dorsal visual field maps within POS (V6A, V6Ad, and V6Av), which all represent the contralateral hemifield, rather than direct input from OPA, which represents predominantly the contralateral lower field (Silson et al., 2015, 2016). A largely complete hemifield representation within MPA suggests further that MPA plays a role in mediating scene-relevant visual information between OPA on the lateral surface (lower visual field biased) and PPA on the ventral surface (upper visual field biased), whose visual field representations only overlap at the fovea (Silson et al., 2015). Finally, our comparison of scene-selectivity and retinotopy within MPA demonstrates a significant positive correlation suggesting that the same underlying neural populations are capable of encoding both scene-relevant visual features and their retinotopic location, rather than being two separate neural populations within MPA.
Conclusion
Together, our data highlight that scene-selective MPA, in the posterior and ventral banks of POS, evidences strongly retinotopic voxels, contains large receptive fields and represents predominantly the contralateral visual field. MPA is also differentially connected to ventral (PPA) over lateral (OPA) scene-selective regions. Dividing PPA into posterior and anterior portions revealed a region of medial parietal cortex directly adjacent and anterior to MPA that exhibited highly significant aPPA associations. The spatial adjacency within medial parietal cortex, exhibited by this region and our scene- and retinotopically sensitive MPA, suggests that scene-related visual information undergoes a transformation from strongly retinotopic and visually grounded in posterior and ventral portions of POS, to more abstract, navigationally- and memory-relevant more anteriorly.
Author Contributions
ES jointly designed the study, collected and analyzed the retinotopic, event-related and functional localizer fMRI data, and wrote the manuscript. AS collected and analyzed the resting-state functional connectivity data and wrote the manuscript. CB jointly designed the study and wrote the manuscript.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
The authors are grateful to Dr. Iris Groen for useful discussions about the data reported in the current manuscript.
Funding. This work was supported by the Intramural Research Program (ZIAMH002909) of the National Institutes of Health – National Institute of Mental Health Clinical Study Protocol 93-M-0170, NCT00001360.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00412
References
- Aminoff E., Gronau N., Bar M. (2007). The parahippocampal cortex mediates spatial and nonspatial associations. Cereb. Cortex 17 1493–1503. 10.1093/cercor/bhl078 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arcaro M. J., McMains S. A., Singer B. D., Kastner S. (2009). Retinotopic organization of human ventral visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 29 10638–10652. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2807-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baldassano C., Beck D. M., Fei-Fei L. (2013). Differential connectivity within the parahippocampal place area. Neuroimage 75 228–237. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.073 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baldassano C., Esteva A., Beck D. M., Fei-Fei L. (2016). Two distinct scene processing networks connecting vision and memory. bioRxiv 15 571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brodmann K. (1909). Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Groshirnrinde. Leipzig: Barth. [Google Scholar]
- Cardin V., Sherrington R., Hemsworth L., Smith A. T. (2012). Human V6: functional characterisation and localisation. PLoS ONE 7:e47685 10.1371/journal.pone.0047685 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caspers S., Eickhoff S. B., Geyer S., Scheperjans F., Mohlberg H., Zilles K., et al. (2008). The human inferior parietal lobule in stereotaxic space. Brain Struct. Funct. 212 481–495. 10.1007/s00429-008-0195-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caspers S., Geyer S., Schleicher A., Mohlberg H., Amunts K., Zilles K. (2006). The human inferior parietal cortex: cytoarchitectonic parcellation and interindividual variability. Neuroimage 33 430–448. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.054 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cox R. W. (1996). AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 29 162–173. 10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeYoe E. A., Carman G. J., Bandettini P., Glickman S., Wieser J., Cox R., et al. (1996). Mapping striate and extrastriate visual areas in human cerebral cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93 2382–2386. 10.1073/pnas.93.6.2382 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dilks D. D., Julian J. B., Paunov A. M., Kanwisher N. (2013). The occipital place area is causally and selectively involved in scene perception. J. Neurosci. 33 1331–1336. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4081-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eickhoff S. B., Stephan K. E., Mohlberg H., Grefkes C., Fink G. R., Amunts K., et al. (2005). A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. Neuroimage 25 1325–1335. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engel S. A., Rumelhart D. E., Wandell B. A., Lee A. T., Glover G. H., Chichilnisky E. J., et al. (1994). fMRI of human visual cortex. Nature 369 525 10.1038/369525a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Epstein R. (2005). The cortical basis of visual scene processing. Vis. Cogn. 12 954–978. 10.1080/13506280444000607 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Epstein R., Kanwisher N. (1998). A cortical representation of the local visual environment. Nature 392 598–601. 10.1038/33402 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Epstein R. A. (2008). Parahippocampal and retrosplenial contributions to human spatial navigation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12 388–396. 10.1016/j.tics.2008.07.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Epstein R. A., Higgins J. S. (2007). Differential parahippocampal and retrosplenial involvement in three types of visual scene recognition. Cereb. Cortex 17 1680–1693. 10.1093/cercor/bhl079 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans J. W., Kundu P., Horovitz S. G., Bandettini P. A. (2015). Separating slow BOLD from non-BOLD baseline drifts using multi-echo fMRI. Neuroimage 105 189–197. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.10.051 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilmore A. W., Nelson S. M., McDermott K. B. (2016). The contextual association network activates more for remembered than for imagined events. Cereb. Cortex 26 611–617. 10.1093/cercor/bhu223 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang R. S., Sereno M. I. (2013). Bottom-up retinotopic organization supports top-down mental imagery. Open Neuroimag. J. 7 58–67. 10.2174/1874440001307010058 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jo H. J., Gotts S. J., Reynolds R. C., Bandettini P. A., Martin A., Cox R. W., et al. (2013). Effective preprocessing procedures virtually eliminate distance-dependent motion artifacts in resting state FMRI. J. Appl. Math. 2013 9 10.1155/2013/935154 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jo H. J., Saad Z. S., Simmons W. K., Milbury L. A., Cox R. W. (2010). Mapping sources of correlation in resting state FMRI, with artifact detection and removal. Neuroimage 52 571–582. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.246 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim J. G., Aminoff E. M., Kastner S., Behrmann M. (2015). A neural basis for developmental topographic disorientation. J. Neurosci. 35 12954–12969. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0640-15.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kravitz D. J., Kriegeskorte N., Baker C. I. (2010). High-level visual object representations are constrained by position. Cereb. Cortex 20 2916–2925. 10.1093/cercor/bhq042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kravitz D. J., Peng C. S., Baker C. I. (2011). Real-world scene representations in high-level visual cortex: it’s the spaces more than the places. J. Neurosci. 31 7322–7333. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4588-10.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kravitz D. J., Saleem K. S., Baker C. I., Ungerleider L. G., Mishkin M. (2013). The ventral visual pathway: an expanded neural framework for the processing of object quality. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17 26–49. 10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kundu P., Santin M. D., Bandettini P. A., Bullmore E. T., Petiet A. (2014). Differentiating BOLD and non-BOLD signals in fMRI time series from anesthetized rats using multi-echo EPI at 11.7 T. Neuroimage 102(Pt 2), 861–874. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larsson J., Heeger D. J. (2006). Two retinotopic visual areas in human lateral occipital cortex. J. Neurosci. 26 13128–13142. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1657-06.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacEvoy S. P., Epstein R. A. (2007). Position selectivity in scene-and object-responsive occipitotemporal regions. J. Neurophysiol. 98 2089–2098. 10.1152/jn.00438.2007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maddock R. J. (1999). The retrosplenial cortex and emotion: new insights from functional neuroimaging of the human brain. Trends Neurosci. 22 310–316. 10.1016/S0166-2236(98)01374-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maguire E. (2001). The retrosplenial contribution to human navigation: a review of lesion and neuroimaging findings. Scand. J. Psychol. 42 225–238. 10.1111/1467-9450.00233 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marchette S. A., Vass L. K., Ryan J., Epstein R. A. (2014). Anchoring the neural compass: coding of local spatial reference frames in human medial parietal lobe. Nat. Neurosci. 17 1598–1606. 10.1038/nn.3834 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marchette S. A., Vass L. K., Ryan J., Epstein R. A. (2015). Outside looking in: landmark generalization in the human navigational system. J. Neurosci. 35 14896–14908. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2270-15.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris R., Paxinos G., Petrides M. (2000). Architectonic analysis of the human retrosplenial cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 421 14–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nasr S., Liu N., Devaney K. J., Yue X., Rajimehr R., Ungerleider L. G., et al. (2011). Scene-selective cortical regions in human and nonhuman primates. J. Neurosci. 31 13771–13785. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2792-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pitzalis S., Fattori P., Galletti C. (2015). The human cortical areas V6 and V6A. Vis. Neurosci. 32:E007 10.1017/S0952523815000048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pitzalis S., Galletti C., Huang R. S., Patria F., Committeri G., Galati G., et al. (2006). Wide-field retinotopy defines human cortical visual area V6. J. Neurosci. 26 7962–7973. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0178-06.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poser B. A., Versluis M. J., Hoogduin J. M., Norris D. G. (2006). BOLD contrast sensitivity enhancement and artifact reduction with multiecho EPI: parallel-acquired inhomogeneity-desensitized fMRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 55 1227–1235. 10.1002/mrm.20900 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Posse S., Wiese S., Gembris D., Mathiak K., Kessler C., Grosse-Ruyken M. L., et al. (1999). Enhancement of BOLD-contrast sensitivity by single-shot multi-echo functional MR imaging. Magn. Reson. Med. 42 87–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Power J. D., Barnes K. A., Snyder A. Z., Schlaggar B. L., Petersen S. E. (2012). Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage 59 2142–2154. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ranganath C., Ritchey M. (2012). Two cortical systems for memory-guided behaviour. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13 713–726. 10.1038/nrn3338 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sayres R., Grill-Spector K. (2008). Relating retinotopic and object-selective responses in human lateral occipital cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 100 249–267. 10.1152/jn.01383.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwarzlose R. F., Swisher J. D., Dang S., Kanwisher N. (2008). The distribution of category and location information across objectselective regions in human visual cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105 4447–4452. 10.1073/pnas.0800431105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sereno M. I., Dale A. M., Reppas J. B., Kwong K. K. (1995). Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Science 268 889–893. 10.1126/science.7754376 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shine J. P., Valdés-Herrera J. P., Hegarty M., Wolbers T. (2016). The human retrosplenial cortex and thalamus code head direction in a global reference frame. J. Neurosci. 36 6371–6381. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1268-15.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silson E. H., Chan A. W. Y., Reynolds R. C., Kravitz D. J., Baker C. I. (2015). A retinotopic basis for the division of high-level scene processing between lateral and ventral human occipitotemporal cortex. J. Neurosci. 35 11921–11935. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0137-15.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silson E. H., Groen I. I., Kravitz D. J., Baker C. I. (2016). Evaluating the correspondence between face-, scene-, and object-selectivity and retinotopic organization within lateral occipitotemporal cortex. J. Vis. 16 14–14. 10.1167/16.6.14 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevens W. D., Tessler M. H., Peng C. S., Martin A. (2015). Functional connectivity constrains the category-related organization of human ventral occipitotemporal cortex. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36 2187–2206. 10.1002/hbm.22764 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vann S. D., Aggleton J. P., Maguire E. A. (2009). What does the retrosplenial cortex do? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10 792–802. 10.1038/nrn2733 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vogt B. A., Absher J. R., Bush G. (2000). Human retrosplenial cortex: where is it and is it involved in emotion? Trends Neurosci. 23 195–197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vogt B. A., Nimchinsky E. A., Vogt L. J., Hof P. R. (1995). Human cingulate cortex: surface features, flat maps, and cytoarchitecture. J. Comp. Neurol. 359 490–506. 10.1002/cne.903590310 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wandell B. A., Dumoulin S. O., Brewer A. A. (2007). Visual field maps in human cortex. Neuron 56 366–383. 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winawer J., Horiguchi H., Sayres R. A., Amano K., Wandell B. A. (2010). Mapping hV4 and ventral occipital cortex: the venous eclipse. J. Vis. 10:1 10.1167/10.5.1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zilles K., Palomero-Gallagher N. (2001). Cyto-, myelo-, and receptor architectonics of the human parietal cortex. Neuroimage 14 S8–S20. 10.1006/nimg.2001.0823 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.