Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Aug 23.
Published in final edited form as: Int J Popul Stud. 2015;1(1):01007. doi: 10.18063/ijps.2015.01.007

Using panel data to examine pregnancy attitudes over time

Heini Väisänen 1,*, Rachel K Jones 2
PMCID: PMC4994192  NIHMSID: NIHMS796652  PMID: 27563690

Abstract

There is a lack of research examining changes in women's fertility attitudes over relatively short periods of time. The aim of this study was to determine whether and how women's attempts to get pregnant and their desire to avoid pregnancy changed over six months' time as well as which characteristics and circumstances were associated with these changes. Using multinomial regression, we analyzed two panels of data from a sample of approximately 3,000 U.S. adult women gathered within six months apart. Only 4% of the women were trying to get pregnant at both time points, but six percent went from trying to not or vice versa. Two-thirds reported a strong desire to avoid pregnancy at both points, but 9% transitioned from strong to not strong and an additional 7% transitioned from not strong to strong. Women who transitioned to a more serious romantic relationship were at increased risk of transitioning to trying to become pregnant and, not surprisingly, to a weaker pregnancy avoidance. Some of the variables we tested, including changes in employment status and race/ethnicity, were associated with one outcome but not the other. The results highlight the importance of taking a holistic perspective of women's lives when studying pregnancy intentions and in reproductive health care services such as contraceptive counseling. Context matters and it may change rapidly.

Keywords: fertility intentions, pregnancy avoidance, pregnancy planning, panel data

1. Introduction

About half of the pregnancies in the United States are unintended (Finer and Zolna, 2014). This figure has remained relatively stable for two decades and has inspired researchers to study topics such as the determinants of fertility intentions (McQuilan, Greil, Shreffler et al., 2015; Reed and Mcbroom, 1995; Gatny, Kusunoki, and Barber, 2014), which groups of women are more likely to experience an unintended pregnancy (Finer and Zolna, 2014; Finer and Henshaw, 2006), to what extent individuals fulfill their desired family size (Berrington and Pattaro, 2014; Hartnett, 2014; Morgan and Rackin, 2010; Miller, Rodgers, and Pasta, 2010), and how well their intentions to have children within a specified time period are met (Miller, Rodgers, and Pasta, 2010; Heaton, Jacobson, and Holland, 1999; White and McQuilan, 2006). However, only a handful of studies have used longitudinal data to examine other fertility and family planning behaviors (Jones, Tapales, Lindberg et al, 2015). Instead, many cross-sectional studies assume that these attitudes are fairly constant over time. Even rarer are studies which use prospective longitudinal data to assess changes and continuity in short-term pregnancy attitudes over time.

Based on cross-sectional studies, we know that at any given point in time around five percent of U.S. women reported that they are trying to get pregnant (Jones, Mosher, and Daniels, 2012; McQuilan, Greil, and Shreffler, 2011). These women are often married, non-White, and are less likely to have children than women who are not trying to get pregnant (McQuilan, Greil, and Shreffler, 2011). Although many studies have noted that a dichotomous assessment of trying versus not trying does not describe the variety of pregnancy intentions (McQuilan, Greil, and Shreffler, 2011; Morgan, 1982; Santelli, Rochat, Hatfield-Timajchy et al, 2003), there is little research examining which groups of women are less interested in avoiding pregnancy, whether and how these attitudes change over time and for whom they do so. Interestingly, many women who are not actively trying to become pregnant are also not actively trying to avoid it. One study found that a fifth of women who were not trying to become pregnant reported that it was only a little or not at all important to avoid pregnancy(Frost, Singh, and Finer, 2007). Similarly, McQuillan's (McQuilan, Greil, and Shreffler, 2011) study found that 23 percent of women were “okay either way” when asked about becoming pregnant.

There are few longitudinal studies that have examined changes in fertility intentions over time. However, some studies have used the National Survey of Families and Households (NSFH) to analyze change and stability in the desire to have children over a relatively long time period of six-years. Among a subsample of 1,440 respondents who had no prior births and were in their first marriage or never married at baseline, the majority of women and men were stable in their fertility intentions, though almost one in five reported a change in intentions between the first and second waves (Heaton, Jacobson, and Holland, 1999). A second study, restricted to those who wanted to have (more) children at baseline, also found that the majority of individuals were consistent in their fertility intentions (including following through with their wave 1 intention to have children by wave 2), but they also found that 15 percent transitioned to no longer intending to have children, and six percent became unsure (White and McQuilan, 2006). However, neither of these studies addressed the changes in women's decisions to actively try to become pregnant or avoid pregnancy in the short term.

Short-term fertility attitudes have been studied longitudinally using The Relationship Dynamics and Social Life (RDSL) survey, which collected weekly data from 1,003 women aged 18–19 at baseline and residing in a single Michigan county in 2008 and 2009. The data show that that while nine in ten of these women were strongly motivated to avoid pregnancy at each point in time, only seven in ten did so consistently over the two and a half year study period (Moreau, Hall, Trussell, et al, 2013). These attitudes can influence contraceptive use: women who place little or no importance on avoiding pregnancy use less effective methods and use methods inconsistently (Frost, Singh, and Finer, 2007; Moreau, Hall, Trussell et al, 2013). This study is one of the few examining short-term changes in pregnancy attitudes, but is unfortunately limited to only young women and is not nationally representative.

Given the relatively small number of studies that have used longitudinal data to examine family planning behaviors, several gaps remain. The focus on young women in most studies likely reflects the recognition that early adulthood is a period of change across many domains. Particularly relevant when it comes to issues of family planning is that the majority of pregnancies to adolescent and young adult women are unintended, and this population is also less likely to use contraception consistently. Accordingly, understanding changes in fertility motivations and behaviors among this population is important. Yet researchers should not assume stability, or fail to examine changes in the family planning behaviors and attitudes of adult women. Childbearing is more normative among adult women, and patterns of fertility and pregnancy attitudes are likely to be distinctly different, and perhaps more variable, than those of adolescents and young adults. For example, one recent study using longitudinal data found that pregnancy avoidance attitudes were strongly associated with consistent contraceptive use and that this attitude changed for a majority of adult women over an 18-month time period (Jones, Tapales, Lindberg et al, 2015). Our study is the first to prospectively examine changes in fertility intentions and pregnancy avoidance attitudes, and the factors associated with these outcomes, among a national sample of adult U.S. women. While our study period is limited to the relatively short time period of six months, we find that these outcomes change for non-negotiable proportions of women.

Following the theory of conjunctural action (TCA) (Johnson-Hanks, Bachrach, Morgan et al, 2011), we expect women to have different attitudes towards pregnancies at different stages of their lives, as past fertility and other life events shape the behavior of individuals. Furthermore, the theory implies that women's reproductive behavior differs depending on their social class: women with low education more often start childbearing before marriage or stable employment than women with high education. Thus, examining whether change in short-term fertility goals and attitudes is associated with socio-demographic characteristics and changes in these circumstances, is of interest. We tested whether pregnancy attitudes changed in response to change in relationship or employment status, or whether background characteristics such as age, education, number and age of children, and race/ethnicity were associated with these attitudes.

2. Data and Methods

Data for this analysis come from Continuity and Change in Contraceptive Use (CCCU) Study, which was administered online to a national sample of women aged 18–39. To best capture women at risk of pregnancy, the sample only included women who ever had vaginal sex with a man, were not pregnant, and who herself or whose main male sexual partner had not been sterilized. In late 2012, 11,365 women were invited to participate; 6,658 (59 percent) answered the four screening items; 4,634 were eligible and completed the survey. A subsequent survey was conducted with the same women six months later, and 69 percent participated. Women who dropped out were younger (average age 28 rather than 29 among those who participated), less educated (32 percent had a college degree compared to 46 percent among those who stayed), less often White (57 percent vs. 66 percent) and childless (44 percent vs 52 percent). In this paper we study the 3,041 women who participated in both waves and were not pregnant at Wave 2.

We examine two outcome measures: whether women were trying to get pregnant and how much they wanted to avoid pregnancy. All women were asked, “Which of the following best describes your current plans regarding having a(nother) baby?” One of the response categories was “I am trying to get pregnant now.” Respondents were classified according to “never tried,” “constantly trying,” “stopped trying” (women who first reported trying, but were not trying at follow-up)and the opposite cases as “started trying.”

Respondents were also asked, “How important is it to you to AVOID becoming pregnant now?” and provided with a 6-point scale where 1 indicated “not at all important” and 6 “very important.” Women reporting values 4-6 at both waves were classified as having “consistently strong” pregnancy avoidance, women reporting values 1-3 as “never strong,” women reporting values 4-6 at first wave but 1-3 at second wave were classified as “became weaker,” and the opposite as “became stronger.” Preliminary analyses explored several coding schemes and resulted in largely the same findings.

Our explanatory variables included, firstly, changes in union status, which was classified as: no change; stronger union (for those who got married, started cohabiting or dating); and union dissolution (including divorce, dissolution of cohabiting union and transitioning from dating to single).

Employment status had three categories: not employed, employed part-time (<35 hours/week) and employed full-time (≥35 hours/week). Change in employment status was described using categories: “more work” (transitioning from no job to part- or full-time; or from part-time to full-time) and “less work” (transitioning from a part- or full-time job into unemployment; or from full-time to part-time).The survey did not assess whether women who were not employed had been laid off, were on leave, or were not working by choice.

Our analyses also include the baseline characteristics of age, race/ethnicity, education, parity, and the age of the youngest child in the household. Sample distributions of all variables are provided in Table 1. Values are reported at each wave for time variant and at baseline for time invariant ones.

Table 1. Pregnancy attitudes and socio-demographic characteristics at baseline (and Wave 2 for time varying covariates), %.

Baseline Wave 2 Both waves N (baseline)
PREGNANCY ATTITUDES
Trying 6 8 4 192
Not trying 94 92 90 2827
Total 100 100 94 3019
Weak avoidance 23 26 16 697
Strong avoidance 77 74 68 2327
Total 100 100 84 3024

UNION STATUS
Married 45 46 44 1,378
Cohabiting 20 20 16 621
Dating 21 20 14 653
Single 13 14 9 389
Total 100 100 82 3,041

EMPLOYMENT
Not employed 35 34 28 1,030
Less than full-time 24 21 13 729
Full time 41 45 35 1,218
Total 100 100 76 2977

AGE
18–24 27 807
25–29 34 1,036
30–34 21 640
35–39 18 558
Total 100 3,041

PARITY
0 53 1,609
1 20 592
2 18 532
3 or more 10 299
Total 100 3,032

YOUNGEST CHILD IN HOUSEHOLD
No children in hh 51 1,562
Infant (0–12 months) 14 430
Toddler (1–3 yrs) 19 574
Kid (4–12 yrs) 12 355
Teen (13–19 yrs) 4 120
Total 100 3,041

RACE/ETHNICITY
White 65 1,978
Black 9 273
Hispanic 8 254
Other 18 536
Total 100 3,041
EDUCATION
Less than high school 5 145
High school 14 412
Some college 36 1,105
BA or higher 45 1,379
Total 100 3,041

We analyzed the data using descriptive statistics and multinomial regression. Firstly, independent variables were tabulated against the two outcome variables. Multinomial regression analyses with changes in trying (never trying as the base outcome); and pregnancy avoidance (consistently strong as the base outcome) as the outcome variables were conducted. The covariates that were not significant at 10% level were excluded from the final models. We also tested whether education interacted with any of the other characteristics, but the interactions were not statistically significant. The results were illustrated by calculating fitted probabilities using average marginal effects at representative values (Williams, 2012) for the outcome categories of interest; that is for having experienced a change in pregnancy avoidance or in trying to become pregnant. The probabilities were calculated for each explanatory variable by treating all respondents as though they had the characteristic of interest, say they experienced a union dissolution, leaving the values of all other variables as observed. The same calculation was subsequently conducted for each of the categories of the explanatory variable; that is also to “no change in relationship status” and “relationship became stronger”, for example. The average of these marginal effects became the probability of having experienced a change in pregnancy avoidance or in trying to become pregnant (Williams, 2012). We present the results as the predicted probabilities with 95 per cent confidence intervals.

3. Results

Four percent of women decided to start trying to get pregnant and two percent stopped trying without getting pregnant between baseline and follow-up studies (Table 2). Being in a romantic relationship that moved to “the next stage” was associated with starting to try to get pregnant more often (5 percent of women) than union dissolution (3 percent). Consistently working part-time was associated with starting (5 percent) and stopping trying (3 percent). Five to six percent of women who were aged 25 to 29 years, had high school education, had one child, had infants or toddlers, or were Black, started trying to get pregnant between the waves compared to two to four percent of women in the other categories of these covariates.

Table 2. The bivariate associations between the explanatory variables and trying (%).

Trying to become pregnant Never trying Consistently trying Started trying Stopped trying Total N
TOTAL 90 4 4 2 100 3,000

UNION STATUS p = 0.001
No change 89 5 4 2 100 2,466
Stronger union 93 1 5 1 100 280
Union dissolution 91 2 3 4 100 254

EMPLOYMENT p = 0.044
Full time 91 4 3 2 100 348
Part time 87 5 5 3 100 775
Not working 94 2 2 2 100 380
Less work 89 5 4 2 100 1,005
More work 91 4 4 1 100 481

AGE AT BASELINE p = 0.010
18–24 93 2 3 2 100 802
25–29 89 4 5 2 100 1,021
30–34 87 6 4 3 100 630
35–39 89 5 4 2 100 547

EDUCATION AT BASELINE p = 0.004
Less than high school 87 6 4 3 100 143
High school 85 5 5 4 100 401
Some college 90 4 4 3 100 1,090
College degree 91 4 4 1 100 1,366

PARITY (Wave II) p = 0.001
0 90 5 3 1 100 1,593
1 85 6 5 4 100 581
2 91 2 4 2 100 526
3 or more 92 2 5 1 100 300

YOUNGEST CHILD IN HH p = 0.001
No children in hh 90 5 4 2 100 1,540
Infant (0–12 months) 92 2 5 1 100 425
Toddler (1–3 yrs) 86 6 6 2 100 569
Kid (4–12 yrs) 89 3 3 4 100 348
Teen (13–19 yrs) 94 3 2 1 100 118

RACE/ETHNICITY p = 0.029
White 90 4 4 2 100 1,953
Black 88 3 5 4 100 267
Other, Non-Hispanic 91 3 4 2 100 252
Hispanic 89 7 4 1 100 528

Nine percent of women transitioned from strong to not strong avoidance and seven percent from not strong to strong (Table 3). Women who got married or started dating or cohabiting transitioned to weaker pregnancy avoidance more often (12 percent) than women who experienced a union dissolution (7 percent). Although women working part-time were most likely to transition in either direction when it came to trying, the same group was the least likely to report a change in pregnancy avoidance. Women in their late 20s and early 30s experienced changes in pregnancy avoidance more often than others confirming that when it comes to pregnancy attitudes, adult women experience more change than adolescents. Women with high school diploma or less more often shifted to a weaker avoidance than other women (12–13 percent vs 8–10 percent). Parous women shifted more often to weaker avoidance than childless women (8 percent vs 11 percent), and mothers of infants were more likely to transition into either direction than women with older children. Race/ethnicity was not significantly associated with this outcome.

Table 3. The bivariate associations between the explanatory variables and pregnancy avoidance (%).

Pregnancy avoidance Consistently strong Never strong Became weaker Became strong Total N
TOTAL 68 16 9 7 100 3,011

UNION STATUS p < 0.001
No change 66 17 9 7 100 2,477
Stronger union 73 9 12 6 100 279
Union dissolution 73 10 7 9 100 255

EMPLOYMENT p < 0.001
Full time 65 19 10 6 100 1,006
Part time 76 11 7 6 100 387
Not working 64 18 11 8 100 780
Less work 68 15 10 7 100 349
More work 72 12 8 8 100 478

AGE AT BASELINE p < 0.001
18–24 79 8 7 5 100 801
25–29 65 17 11 7 100 1,023
30–34 59 20 12 9 100 635
35–39 65 21 7 7 100 552

EDUCATION AT BASELINE p < 0.001
Less than high school 59 22 13 5 100 143
High school 58 19 12 11 100 405
Some college 69 15 8 8 100 1,097
College degree 70 15 10 5 100 1,366

PARITY (Wave II) p < 0.001
0 71 15 8 6 100 1,593
1 58 20 11 11 100 582
2 69 15 11 5 100 529
3 or more 67 16 11 7 100 303

YOUNGEST CHILD IN HH p < 0.001
No children in hh 70 16 8 6 100 1,542
Infant (0–12 months) 60 15 16 9 100 428
Toddler (1–3 yrs) 65 20 10 6 100 570
Kid (4–12 yrs) 68 15 7 10 100 353
Teen (13–19 yrs) 79 11 5 5 100 118

RACE/ETHNICITY p = 0.759
White 67 17 9 7 100 1,958
Black 65 16 12 7 100 270
Other, Non-Hispanic 72 14 8 6 100 249
Hispanic 67 15 9 8 100 534

Findings using the multivariate analyses were similar to the bivariate analyses although fewer differences were statistically significant. Change in employment status was excluded from the model estimating the likelihood of experiencing changes in trying to get pregnant, and race/ethnicity from the pregnancy avoidance model. None of the interaction effects between education and the other covariates were significant at 10 percent level, and were thus not included. See Supplementary Online Materials A Tables A1 and A2 for full results of the models. Fitted probabilities, which were calculated based on the models, are shown below in Figures 1 and 2.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Changes in trying, fitted probabilities (%)* with 95% confidence intervals.

* Calculated based on multinomial regression comparing outcomes never trying (reference), consistently trying, stopped trying, started trying. Tables including coefficients and p-values available on request.

Figure 2. Changes in avoidance, fitted probabilities (%)* with 95% confidence intervals.

Figure 2

* Calculated based on multinomial regression comparing outcomes consistently strong (reference), never strong, became weaker and became stronger. Tables including coefficients and p-values available on request.

Figure 1 shows the fitted probabilities for changes in trying to get pregnant based on the multinomial model. Again, transitioning into a stronger union or not changing one's union status was associated with starting rather than stopping trying; women in their late 20s were more likely to start than stop trying; and mothers of infants and toddlers were relatively likely to start trying to get pregnant. Those who had a college degree more often started than stopped trying. Being childless or having at least three children was associated with a higher probability to start trying. Hispanic and White women were less likely to stop trying and more likely to start than other racial or ethnic groups.

Figure 2 shows the fitted probabilities of experiencing a change in pregnancy avoidance based on the multinomial model. The directions of associations were similar to the model where transitions in trying to get pregnant were studied for most variables. Moving to the “next stage” in one's union was associated with a relatively high (14 percent) probability in transitioning into weaker pregnancy avoidance. Women in their late 20s and early 30s had a higher probability of transitioning into weaker avoidance than other women. Women whose youngest child was an infant had a markedly higher probability of transitioning into weaker pregnancy avoidance compared to women with older children.

Unlike in the model measuring changes in trying, women who had less than a high school education had relatively high probability of transitioning into a weaker avoidance (14 percent), but women with a college degree were also more likely to transition into a weaker than into a stronger avoidance (9 percent vs 5 percent). Women with at least two children had a higher probability of transitioning into a weaker than stronger avoidance. Constantly working full-time women were more often associated with transitioning into a weaker avoidance than into a stronger one (Figure 2).

4. Discussion

While strong pregnancy avoidance and not trying to get pregnant was the norm for women in our sample, our results show that pregnancy attitudes change for a non-negotiable minority of women over a relatively short period of time. Perhaps not surprisingly, pregnancy avoidance showed more movement than efforts to get pregnant. Pregnancy avoidance has a behavioral element; as many women who have a strong desire to avoid pregnancy are likely to engage in practices to prevent this from happening, but it is less exclusive than those women who were reportedly actively trying to get pregnant.

Women in the lowest level of education were relatively likely to transition to weaker pregnancy avoidance, but less often into trying to get pregnant, whereas women with at least college degree had both higher likelihood of transitioning into weaker avoidance and starting to try. This may reflect different strategies of planning childbearing. According to TCA, after having decided to start a family, women with higher education are more likely to change their behavior beyond just not using contraception, for instance, by optimizing the timing of intercourse. Women from less advantaged backgrounds may take a more informal approach to childbearing, for example, accepting a pregnancy even when it is unplanned or alternatively, stopping contraception to show commitment to their partner (Johnson-Hanks, Bachrach, Morgan et al, 2011). Thus, there may be a higher likelihood of reporting trying to get pregnant among those with higher education compared to those with lower education, even while both groups' report a similarly weaker pregnancy avoidance attitude. Interestingly, no significant interactions between education and other covariates were found, although TCA suggests otherwise. It may have been partly due to the small sample size in our study.

Some of the associations were not surprising, such as transitioning into a stronger union being associated with weaker avoidance and trying to get pregnant. However, given that employment situation is often associated with fertility intentions in the literature (Becker, 1991; Chibber, Biggs, Roberts et al, 2014), it is interesting that changes in hours worked did not have a clear association with pregnancy attitudes. It may be that women interpret such changes as favorable or unfavorable depending on their other life circumstances. In addition, as the data did not assess whether women who reported working no hours in the week prior to the interview were on leave, not working by choice or had been laid off, it may be that in some cases we did not capture the kind of change that affects pregnancy planning with this variable.

Young women (aged 18 to 24) were less likely to transition in any direction in their pregnancy attitudes compared to older women. These patterns might reflect that younger women are more often pursuing education, stable employment and relationships thus, motivated to postpone childbearing confirming our hypothesis that there is more fluctuation in these attitudes among older women By contrast, women in their late 20s often transitioned into weaker pregnancy avoidance and started trying, which suggests that this is seen as a preferred age to have children.

Women who had young child(ren) more often reported shifting to weaker pregnancy avoidance and transitioning into trying to get pregnant. These women may wish to have their children relatively closely spaced.

These results highlight the importance of taking a holistic perspective of women's lives when studying pregnancy attitudes. Since we know that these attitudes are associated with consistency in contraceptive use (Frost, Singh, and Finer, 2007; Moreau, Hall, Trussell et al, 2013), this should be taken into account when contraceptive counseling is given. As pregnancy attitudes may change rapidly, women should know how to adjust their contraceptive use accordingly. This result also has a methodological implication: cross-sectional studies may not capture the entire story of pregnancy attitudes, as these studies assume that these measures are fairly stable over time.

There were limitations in this study. Women who were lost to attrition between waves were younger and less educated than women who stayed. However, if we observe this much change even among our sample of women probably leading more stable lives, there is no reason to expect that the associations would be weaker in a less biased sample. Moreover, we lacked information of partner's characteristics which may affect pregnancy intentions (Chibber, Biggs, Roberts et al, 2014). A larger sample size would have permitted a more detailed examination between different types of transitions in attitudes. Although the partner's characteristics such as his occupation or age may have influenced respondents' pregnancy attitudes, this information was not collected and in turn, we were unable to control for these characteristics. Similarly, it may be that the association between changes in relationship status and the outcome depend on whether the women are in their first or subsequent union and on the duration of the partnership. However, this information was not collected either. Future studies on the topic should consider measuring and studying these characteristics.

The strengths on the study include the innovative study design exploring rarely studied associations between changes in women's lives and fertility intentions. Moreover, there are very few existing longitudinal studies at the national level measuring adult women's fertility attitudes prospectively.

Supplementary Material

Supplemental Files

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank participants of PAA 2015 Session 120: Fertility Intentions: Causes and Consequences for their comments on an earlier version of the paper. Heini Väisänen conducted the statistical analysis and wrote the first versions of ‘Data and Methods’ and ‘Results’ sections. Rachel K. Jones is the principal investigator in the research project that collected these data. She wrote the first version of the ‘Introduction’ section.

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest and Funding: There was no conflict of interest. Heini Väisänen was funded by the Economic and Social Research Council [Grant number ES/J00070/1]. The Continuity and Change in Contraceptive Use Study, on which this analysis was based, was supported by The JPB Foundation.

Author Contributions: Both authors contributed to the ‘Discussion’ section and edited all the sections of the study after putting together the first version of the manuscript.

Ethics Statement: The data of the study were collected by GfK, an online recruitment company, to administer the survey using its Knowledge Panel. The panel is comprised of approximately 50,000 individuals and is intended to be representative of the U.S. population. GfK obtains informed consent from all individuals, and because the study posed minimal risk, we were able to obtain expedited approval from the institutional review board of the Guttmacher Institute.

The data used in this study will be made publicly available through the Guttmacher Institute's Center for Population Research Innovation and Dissemination (http://www.guttmacher.org/popcenter/), tentatively in early 2017.

References

  1. Becker GS. A Treatise on the Family (Enlarged ed) Cambridge, Mass, London: Harvard University Press; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  2. Berrington A, Pattaro S. Educational differences in fertility desires, intentions and behaviour: A life course perspective. Advances in Life Course Research. 2014;21:10–27. doi: 10.1016/j.alcr.2013.12.003. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.alcr.2013.12.0037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Chibber KS, Biggs MA, Roberts SCM, et al. The role of intimate partners in women's reasons for seeking abortion. Women's Health Issues: Official Publication of the Jacobs Institute of Women's Health. 2014;24(1):e131–138. doi: 10.1016/j.whi.2013.10.007. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.whi.2013.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Finer LB, Henshaw SK. Disparities in rates of unintended pregnancy in the United States, 1994 and 2001. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health. 2006;38(2):90–96. doi: 10.1363/psrh.38.090.06. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1931-2393.2006.tb00065.x6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Finer LB, Zolna MR. Shifts in intended and unintended pregnancies in the United States, 2001 – 2008. American Journal of Public Health. 2014;104(S1):S43–S48. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2013.301416. http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/ajph.2013.3014162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Frost JJ, Singh S, Finer LB. Factors associated with contraceptive use and nonuse, United States, 2004. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health. 2007;39(2):90–99. doi: 10.1363/3909007. http://dx.doi.org/10.1363/3909007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Gatny H, Kusunoki Y, Barber J. Pregnancy scares and subsequent unintended pregnancy. Demographic Research. 2014;31(40):1229–1242. doi: 10.4054/DemRes.2014.31.40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Hartnett CS. White-Hispanic differences in meeting lifetime fertility intentions in the US. Demographic Research. 2014;30:1245–1276. doi: 10.4054/DemRes.2014.30.43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Heaton TB, Jacobson CK, Holland K. Persistence and change in decisions to remain childless. Journal of Marriage and Family. 1999;61(2):531–539. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/35376711. [Google Scholar]
  10. Johnson-Hanks JA, Bachrach CA, Morgan SP, et al. Understanding Family Change and Variation: Toward a Theory of Conjunctural Action. Springer Science & Business Media; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  11. Jones J, Mosher W, Daniels K. Current contraceptive use in the United States, 2006–2010, and changes in patterns of use since 1995. Washington DC: National Center for Health Statistics; 2012. Retrieved from http://198.246.124.22/nchs/data/nhsr/nhsr060.pdf. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Jones RK, Tapales A, Lindberg LD, et al. Using longitudinal data to understand changes In consistent contraceptive use. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health. 2015;47(3):131–139. doi: 10.1363/47e4615. http://dx.doi.org/10.1363/47e4615. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Kreyenfeld M. Uncertainties in female employment careers and the postponement of parenthood in Germany. European Sociological Review. 2010;26(3):351–366. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/esr/jcp02623. [Google Scholar]
  14. McQuillan J, Greil AL, Shreffler KM. Pregnancy intentions among women who do not try: Focusing on women who are okay either way. Maternal and Child Health Journal. 2011;15(2):178–187. doi: 10.1007/s10995-010-0604-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10995-010-0604-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. McQuillan J, Greil AL, Shreffler KM, et al. The importance of motherhood and fertility intentions among US women. Sociological Perspectives. 2015;58(1):20–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/07311214145343933. [Google Scholar]
  16. Miller WB, Rodgers JL, Pasta DJ. Fertility motivations of youth predict later fertility outcomes: A prospective analysis of National Longitudinal Survey of Youth Data. Biodemography and Social Biology. 2010;56(1):1–23. doi: 10.1080/19485561003709131. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19485561003709131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Moreau C, Hall K, Trussell J, et al. Effect of prospectively measured pregnancy intentions on the consistency of contraceptive use among young women in Michigan. Human Reproduction (Oxford, England) 2013;28(3):642–650. doi: 10.1093/humrep/des421. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/humrep/des421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Morgan SP. Parity-specific fertility intentions and uncertainty: The United States, 1970 to 1976. Demography. 1982;19(3):315–334. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Morgan SP, Rackin H. The correspondence between fertility intentions and behavior in the United States. Population and Development Review. 2010;36(1):91–118. doi: 10.1111/j.1728-4457.2010.00319.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Reed FW, Mcbroom WH. The impact of marriage on fertility intentions and related values. International Journal of Sociology of the Family. 1995;25(1):91–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Santelli JS, Rochat R, Hatfield-Timajchy K, et al. The measurement and meaning of unintended pregnancy. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health. 2003;35(2):94–101. doi: 10.1363/3509403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. White L, McQuillan J. No longer intending: The relationship between relinquished fertility intentions and distress. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2006;68(2):478–490. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2006.00266.x. [Google Scholar]
  23. Williams R. Using the margins command to estimate and interpret adjusted predictions and marginal effects. The Stata Journal. 2012;12(2):308–331. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplemental Files

RESOURCES