Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2016 Aug 23;5:e12687. doi: 10.7554/eLife.12687

Probing the functional impact of sub-retinal prosthesis

Sébastien Roux 1,*, Frédéric Matonti 1,2, Florent Dupont 3,4, Louis Hoffart 1,2, Sylvain Takerkart 1, Serge Picaud 5, Pascale Pham 3,4, Frédéric Chavane 1,*
Editor: Doris Y Tsao6
PMCID: PMC4995098  PMID: 27549126

Abstract

Retinal prostheses are promising tools for recovering visual functions in blind patients but, unfortunately, with still poor gains in visual acuity. Improving their resolution is thus a key challenge that warrants understanding its origin through appropriate animal models. Here, we provide a systematic comparison between visual and prosthetic activations of the rat primary visual cortex (V1). We established a precise V1 mapping as a functional benchmark to demonstrate that sub-retinal implants activate V1 at the appropriate position, scalable to a wide range of visual luminance, but with an aspect-ratio and an extent much larger than expected. Such distorted activation profile can be accounted for by the existence of two sources of diffusion, passive diffusion and activation of ganglion cells’ axons en passant. Reverse-engineered electrical pulses based on impedance spectroscopy is the only solution we tested that decreases the extent and aspect-ratio, providing a promising solution for clinical applications.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.12687.001

Research Organism: Rat

eLife digest

One of the most common causes of blindness is a disorder called retinitis pigmentosa. In a healthy eye, the surface at the back of the eye – called the retina – contains cells called photoreceptors that detect light and convert it into electrical signals for the brain to process. In people with retinitis pigmentosa, these photoreceptor cells die off gradually, which leads to loss of vision.

The only treatment available for retinitis pigmentosa is to have an artificial retina implanted into the eye. The artificial retina consists of an array of tiny electrodes, which take over from the damaged photoreceptors and generate electrical signals. The person with the implant perceives these electrical signals as bright flashes called “phosphenes”. However, the phosphenes are too large and imprecise to provide the person with vision that is good enough for tasks such as walking unaided or reading.

To find out why artificial retinas produce such poor resolution, Roux et al. compared how a rat’s brain responds to either natural visual stimuli or activation of implanted an array of micro-electrodes. Both the micro-electrodes and the natural stimuli activated the same areas of the brain. However, the micro-electrodes produced larger and more elongated patterns of activation. This is because the electrical currents generated by the micro-electrodes diffused throughout the retinal tissue and activated other neurons besides those intended. To overcome this problem, Roux et al. tested different ways of stimulating the micro-electrodes in order to identify those that induce the desired patterns of brain activity. This approach – known as reverse engineering – did indeed improve the performance of the micro-electrode array.

The next step is to extend these findings, which were obtained in healthy rats, to non-human primates or animal models of retinitis pigmentosa to better understand the condition in humans. In addition, combining the current approach with other existing techniques should further improve the vision that can be achieved with artificial retinas.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.12687.002

Introduction

Blindness affects 45 million people around the world with an increase of 1 to 2 million people each year (Resnikoff et al., 2004). The two main retinal pathologies are age-related macular degeneration (AMD, Ambati and Fowler, 2012; Finger et al., 2011) and Retinitis Pigmentosa (RP, Bocquet et al., 2013). Although the genetic alterations and the mechanisms subtending photoreceptor death are well described, and therapeutic strategies are under clinical trials (Ferrari et al., 2013; Talcott et al., 2011), retinal degeneration inexorably leads to blindness (Tsujikawa et al., 2008). In this perspective, retinal prostheses provide a promising solution that remains to date a unique alternative for the patients. Restoring some visual perception using implants has been already achieved (Humayun et al., 2012; Shepherd et al., 2013; Zrenner et al., 2010) but still offers insufficient gains in visual acuity (Humayun et al., 2012; Zrenner et al., 2010); (Nanduri et al., 2012; Rizzo, 2003). Despite having fundamentally different designs and operating modes, the two main models of prostheses proposed to the patients with RP (Argus II epiretinal prosthesis and the subretinal alpha IMS microphotodiode array) restore some visual function although with a spatial resolution (Ahuja and Behrend, 2013; Humayun et al., 2012); (Stingl et al., 2013) that does not allow for the recognition of faces or autonomous locomotion. Improving the performances of such implants is thus a key strategic issue for further developments.

This problem is actually a general issue shared by other sensory prosthesis, such as the cochlear implants (Kral et al., 1998). This latter field, well in advance compared to retinal implants, has already demonstrated the importance of developing animal models for better understanding of the underlying physiological processes (Fallon and Shepherd, 2009; Miller et al., 2000). To improve the resolution and efficiency of retinal prosthesis, it is therefore necessary to launch appropriate models that allow probing precisely and quantitatively the functional impact of prosthetic activation. Pioneering animal studies have proposed to use cortical recordings to explore the efficiency of various patterns of retinal electrical stimulation (Chowdhury et al., 2008), including current steering methods (Jepson et al., 2014a; Matteucci et al., 2013) or the effect of the return electrode configuration (Cicione et al., 2012; Matteucci et al., 2013; Wong et al., 2009) and studied the temporal aspect of prosthetic vision (Elfar et al., 2009; Fransen et al., 2014; Jepson et al., 2014a, 2013, 2014b; Nadig, 1999; Schanze et al., 2003; Sekirnjak et al., 2008; Wilms et al., 2003). However, most were not designed to characterize and calibrate the functional activation of the visual system (Chowdhury et al., 2008; Eger et al., 2005; Mandel et al., 2013; Nadig, 1999; Schanze et al., 2003; Walter et al., 2005; Wong et al., 2009) nor probed the functional impact of implants through systematic comparison with visual activation (Chowdhury et al., 2008; Cicione et al., 2012; Eger et al., 2005; Fransen et al., 2014; Mandel et al., 2013; Matteucci et al., 2013; Schanze et al., 2003; Wong et al., 2009). Hence, none of these studies allowed to fully address the question of understanding and controlling the functional impact of retinal prosthesis.

To address this issue, we developed an acute animal model to quantitatively assess the functional impact of retinal prostheses by comparing the downstream activation of the visual system in response to visual versus artificial stimuli, using intrinsic optical imaging of the primary visual cortex (V1). To infer the hypothetic visual counterparts induced by electrical stimulation, we first established a quantitative mapping of the cartographic organization of the rat visual system. So far only Gias and colleagues (Gias et al., 2004) have provided a retinotopic description of the rat visual cortex using optical imaging. Here, we generalized this mapping to visual parameters that are the most important for prosthetic vision (position, size and intensity). Using this cartographic benchmark, we demonstrate that prosthetic stimulation generates a functional activation that occurs at the expected retinotopic location and amplitude. However, the aspect ratio and the extent of the activation are significantly larger than expected. This can be explained through a simple model with two sources of diffusion: an electrical passive diffusion and the activation of axons en passant from ganglion cells. To control the extent of cortical activation, we tested various patterns of electrical stimulation and showed that only reverse engineering of the electrical pulses to inject the desired electrical stimulation (Dupont et al., 2013; Pham et al., 2013) allowed focalizing the activation. This result provides a promising perspective that could be easily implemented for improving the visual acuity of already implanted patients.

Results

In order to investigate the functional impact of acutely implanted retinal prostheses, we have recorded the population activation of V1 of anesthetized rats using optical imaging (Eckhorn et al., 2006; Gias et al., 2004; Grinvald et al., 1999; Walter et al., 2005) (Figure 1A–B) and implanted micro-electrode array prosthesis (MEA, see Materials and methods) sub-retinally (Figure 1C). With such preparation, we quantified for each rat the visually as well as the artificially-evoked cortical population activations. Our results thus are derived from a systematic pairwise comparison of the artificial activation versus its counterpart visual activation (Figure 1A) in response to relevant key parameters (position, size, shape and intensity) across a large number of animals (N = 35). Hence, responses to visual stimuli were used to provide a functional benchmark to which prosthetic activations were systematically compared.

Figure 1. Experiment design.

Figure 1.

(A) Schematic view of the experimental setup with the camera and the visual pathway from the retina to V1 activated with normal visual stimuli (left) or with sub-retinal electrical stimulation using a MEA (right). Retinal ganglion cells’ (RGCs) axons leaving the retina and projecting to V1 via the LGN are schematized (black when non activated; in red for direct activation; in orange for activation of en passant fibers that could occur in the electrical stimulation case). Blue curves on the top of V1 schematize the expected spatial profile of cortical activation (symmetric for visual stimulation and asymmetric for electrical stimulation if axons en passant are activated). (B) Clear optical access through thinned bone over V1; scale bar: 2 mm. L: lateral; M: medial; A: anterior; P: posterior. (C) Image of the eye fundus with the 9 electrodes (top left) and the 17 electrodes (top right) MEA; scale bars: 500 microns. Note that the use of an additional magnifying lens induced optical artifacts (halos of light). Retinal OCT B-scan (bottom) of an implanted animal showing the MEA and intact retina (PE: pigmetary epithelium; *: shadowing of the external reference surrounding the 17 electrodes MEA).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.12687.003

Position

First, we investigated whether retinal prosthesis stimulation generates activation at the expected retinotopic position. To answer this question, we mapped the cortical retinotopic organization of the rat visual cortex (Gias et al., 2004) by flashing white squares in a 5x4 grid of 20° side (Figure 2A). For each animal, we computed polar maps for azimuth and elevation (Figure 2B,F for 2 different animals). Animals were also implanted sub-retinally with a MEA and its retinal position was identified using fundus imaging (Figure 2C,G), reported in the visual (A,E) and cortical domains (B,D,F,H), respectively. In the first animal (Figure 2 top row), we stimulated the whole MEA (wMEA), the size of which (1 mm) was in the range of the size of the stimuli used for visual retinotopic mapping (20°) (Hughes, 1979; Palagina et al., 2009). Figure 2D right shows the cortical activation generated by such electrical stimulation at ± 150 µA (red contour). For a better visualization, we provided all the maps without contour in Supplementary Figure 2—figure supplement 1 with scale bars expressed both in Z-score (left of the colorbar) and in DI/I (right of the colorbar). Note that Z-score and DI/I measures were highly and significantly correlated (median correlation coefficient r2 = 0.81 between all pixels of Z-score vs. DI/I maps with the corresponding [20-50-80] percentiles being [0.73--0.81--0.88]%, all pval = 1.40e−45, N = 9 rats, n = 225 maps). A 20° visual stimulus presented at this position (Figure 2A white square) generated activation at a similar position (Figure 2D left, white circle) but with an extent (white contour) that was much smaller than its electrical counterpart (red dashed line). We then tested single electrode stimulation (SE, see Materials and methods). Figure 2E–H shows an example in another animal for two individual electrodes each stimulated at ± 200 µA. The evoked activations for the two SE (Figure 2H middle and right maps, cyan and purple) were about the size of the 20° visual activation (left map, white) and their positions within the activation generated by the corresponding visual stimulus covering their retinal positions (Figure 2E). To generalize these observations, we computed the retino-cortical magnification factors by measuring the cortical distance between the centers of activation (white circles) elicited by visual stimuli displayed at different positions. Across 20 animals, the cortical distance between activations was plotted against the distance separating the visual stimuli (Figure 2I). We observed a linear increase of cortical distance with visual distance, at a rate of 22 µm/° (in accordance with the literature) (Gias et al., 2004). For all activations (visual and electrical), we used this value to estimate the error (in mm and equivalent degree of visual angle, °eq) between the position of the activity’s center-of-mass and its expected position within the retinotopic map. Visually evoked activations provided an approximation of the inherent variability of that measure (Figure 2J, gray dots). On average, the cortical error was 0.32 ± 0.22 mm for visual stimuli, 72% of the data points falling within the retinotopic representation of the 20° stimulus (<0.44 mm). The same measure was applied to electrically evoked activations for SE and wMEA stimulations. For SE, the estimation of the position of the evoked activity was quite accurate (error of 0.46 ± 0.27 mm), 55% of evoked responses falling within the retinotopic expected position. Note that these error values are actually over-estimated since in practice, the implant was not obligatory located at the exact same retinal position that corresponds to the visual stimulus. Finally, wMEA activations yielded responses with more variable and less accurate position than SE with an average error of 0.68 ± 0.47 mm, 38% of the evoked positions being within the expected representation.

Figure 2. Position.

The visual (A&E), retinal (C&G) and cortical (B,D&F,H) expected position and size of the MEA are compared to their corresponding visual stimuli. (A) Schematic view of the visual field showing the 20 positions (grid) of the visual stimuli used for retinotopic mapping. Optic disk: asterix; MEA: colored circles; nearest visual stimulus: white square (stands for all Figures). (B) V1 retinotopic polar map for azimuth (left) and elevation (right). Color hue and brightness code respectively for the retinotopic position and the strength of the response. Scale bar: 2 mm. (C) Image of the eye fundus with the implant. (D) Extent and center-of-mass (contour and circle respectively) of V1 activations generated by visual (white, right map) and stimulation at ± 150 µA wMEA (red, left map). The activation amplitude depicted in the colorbar is expressed both in Z-score and in DI/I (see Materials and methods). The solid line indicates the activation contour of the corresponding map and the superimposed dashed line corresponds to the compared condition; scale bar: 2 mm. (E-H) Same as in (A-D) in a different animal for 2 SE stimulations (cyan and purple; dashed circle: MEA position) at ± 200 µA; see Figure 6A for an example at low intensity in the same animal. (I) Retino-cortical magnification factor for azimuth computed over 20 animals (n = 177 displacements). Boxplots represent the median and interquartile range; whiskers represent ± 2.7σ or 99.3 coverage if data are normally distributed (any points outside are considered as outliers). (J) Retinotopic based positional cortical error for visual and electrical activations (right: mm; left: equivalent degrees of visual angle). Black dots correspond to visual counterparts of electrical activations. One-sided two-sample Wilcoxon rank sum test for paired data: pSE vs. wMEA=0.056, nwMEA = 21, nSE = 82, NwMEA = 6, NSE = 7. Wilcoxon rank sum test for paired data: *p=0.025, n = 13, N = 4 and ***p=5.35 10–5, n = 80, N = 6 (*p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, n = number of sample, N = number of rats).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.12687.004

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Raw maps.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

Raw z-score maps without overlaid contours corresponding to a sample of figures presented in the manuscript (see subtitles). Colorbars are expressed both in z-score as well as in DI/I.

Size

In the previous examples, we observed that the electrically-evoked activations are larger than expected (Figure 2D,H). We thus quantified this effect by systematically comparing the extents of the visually and artificially evoked activations (Figure 3). As shown in the example of Figure 3A, the extent of cortical activation linearly increased with visual stimulus size with an average slope of 81 µm/° when estimated at population level (Figure 3B, N = 7). In comparison, the active surface diameters of the SE and wMEA were of 0.05 and 0.65 mm (see Materials and methods) equivalent to 1 and 11° of visual angle (Hughes, 1979), respectively. Although the visual stimuli covered a larger region of the retina (20°, Figures 2D and 3C top right), we observed a systematic and highly significant increase of the cortical activation extent induced by wMEA stimulation in 94% of the cases (17/18 conditions, 10 animals; see Materials and methods and Figure 3D legend for details on statistical procedures), with an average increase of 2.9 ± 2.01 times the activation extent of the visual stimulation (Figure 3D, thin gray lines). Please note that for this analysis, all electrical stimulations were systematically paired with the closest visual stimulus used for retinotopic mapping (Figure 3C, bottom maps). In comparison, SE stimulation led to an average activation size of 0.9 ± 0.5 times their 20° visual counterparts (Figure 3D). Using the visual-size tuning function (Figure 3B), we extracted the equivalent visual stimulus sizes that would have evoked such cortical activations. These values reached on average 24.4 ± 10.6 and 32.2 ± 11.5° instead of 1 and 11° for the SE and wMEA configurations, respectively. Electrical stimulation hence led to extremely large diffusion of the activation, comparatively larger for SE than wMEA. Such differences could be explained by several factors including (i) the extent of wMEA activation being sometimes underestimated because of the limit of the imaged region of interest and (ii) the close proximity of the annular counter-electrode (reference) in wMEA constraining lateral diffusion of the current (Cicione et al., 2012; Pham et al., 2013; Wong et al., 2009). Our results unambiguously demonstrate that prosthetic retinal stimulation generates too large activation to obtain the necessary independence between electrodes; an inter-electrode spacing of at least 0.8 to 1.2 mm on the retina, at low- and high-intensity respectively, would be required to yield non-overlapping cortical activations.

Figure 3. Size.

Figure 3.

(A) V1 activation generated by visual stimuli of increasing size (value indicated above the maps). Center-of-mass: white circle; extent: white contour; equivalent ellipse orientation: white cross; scale bar: 2 mm. (B) Extent of cortical activation as a function of visual stimulus size pooled over 7 rats (linear fit: dashed black line). (C) Extent of cortical activations generated in 2 animals by SE (blue) and wMEA (red) stimulation (top maps) at a high current intensity (± 200 and 150 µA respectively) and their corresponding 20° visual stimulus (white, bottom maps). Centers of mass of the activation: colored circles; scale bar: 2 mm. (D) Size of cortical activation generated by visual (gray, N = 20 rats); SE (blue, N = 8 rats) and wMEA (red, N = 10 rats) stimulation. Wilcoxon rank sum test for paired data: **p=0.0019 (n = 54, N = 8), ***p=1.83 10–4 (n = 15, N = 10). An alternative ordinate of equivalent visual counterpart is given on the right. Gray thin lines link paired electrical stimulation to visual activation. Solid horizontal black line indicates the size of a 20° visual stimulus estimated from the fit in B. Note that we could not reveal any effect of the electrode-to-counter-electrode distance on activation size in the various SE configurations (not shown).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.12687.006

Shape

Next we compared the shape of cortical activations to visual and prosthetic stimulations, because it may help understanding the origin of the spread. When compared to visual stimulus (white), we generally observed elongated cortical activation for SE (Figure 4A blue) but not for wMEA (red) stimulation. In order to quantify this elongation, we computed the aspect ratio (AR) of activation contours at the level of the population (Figure 4B, see Materials and methods). Using pairwise visual-prosthetic comparison, we found a highly significant (see Figure 4B legend) increase in the aspect ratio of the evoked activations for SE condition compared to their visual counterparts (1.66 ± 0.43 vs. 1.35 ± 0.19) but none for wMEA. These elongations could be caused by the recruitment of ganglion cells’ axons en passant leading to oriented anisotropic cortical activations (Figure 1A right). However, why would SE activation yield stronger AR than wMEA? One explanation is that the ganglion cell neurons with axons that will be activated en passant, all converging radially towards the optic disk, are all located upstream to the electrode within a 'shadow cone' with a top angle that will (i) decrease with the distance between the implant and the optic disk location and (ii) increase with the size of the stimulated retinal surface (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). In a simple functional model (see Materials and methods), we combined the predictions of what should be the shape and size of retinal activations due to local and en passant activation. Isotropic 'direct' activations were modeled as a Gaussian activation around the site of stimulation (Figure 4—figure supplement 1 left column) for different electrode sizes (Figure 4—figure supplement 1, rows). Anisotropic en passant recruitment of ganglion cell axons was modeled as a shadow cone activation, i.e. all peripheral ganglion somata whose axons have been activated by the electrical stimulation (Figure 4—figure supplement 1, middle column, Figure 4—figure supplement 1A for details in the model, see Materials and methods). Via a weighted sum, we combined these two activations (right column and Figure 4C insets) with different ratios (Figure 4—figure supplement 1B: 0.5 & C: 1). From these predicted activations, we extracted similar parameters (size, position, elongation) as we did from our cortical recordings. Since our interest is to predict the effect of axon-en-passant activation on radial elongation, we used the following convention for the AR: the numerator is the length of activation along the radial axis and the denominator is the length of activation along a tangential axis (perpendicular to the radial axis). This model predicted that the resulting retinal activation should indeed be more elongated along radial axis for smaller cone angle (Figure 4C and Figure 4—figure supplement 1, AR > 1), this effect being stronger when we increase the relative contribution of en passant activation. The model further predicts that the retinal activation should become more elongated along a tangential axis for very large cone angle (AR < 1). Please note that these predictions are left unchanged if we apply a retino-cortical transformation to the simulated activations (see Figure 4—figure supplement 2C). To test for these predictions, we plotted the aspect-ratio of the activation across animals and experimental conditions as a function of the retinal 'shadow cone' angle formed by the stimulated surface (Figure 4D). Please note that, to account for any potential deformation of the evoked activity due to retino-cortical magnification factor (see Figure 4—figure supplement 2A) or physiological noise, we normalized all electrically-induced AR to their corresponding visual AR. In this plot we observed a similar decrease of the aspect ratio with the cone angle, confirming our predictions (Figure 4C). Our results thus suggest that the difference we observed between wMEA and SE is explained if we make the hypothesis that part of the functional activation of the visual system comes from of axons en passant recruitment. Note that our model suggests that the isotropy observed for wMEA activations simply results from the geometrical arrangement between the electrode size and the shadow cone. We thus conclude that wMEA activations are still suffering from contamination by the activation of fibers en passant. Hence SE simulation remains the best configuration to achieve the highest performance in spatial resolution. According to our simple model, the relative contribution of axons en passant to the global prosthetic activation should be of the same order of magnitude as direct activation (alpha = 1, see Materials and methods: model of retinal activation).

Figure 4. Shape.

(A) Shape of cortical activations generated in 2 animals by SE (blue) and wMEA (red) stimulation at high current intensity (top) and their corresponding 20° visual stimulus (white, bottom). (B) Aspect ratio (AR) of cortical activations (Wilcoxon rank sum test for paired data, ***p=1.06 10–4, n = 44, N = 7). (C) Predictions of the elongation of electrical activations as a function of the contribution of axons en passant and the distance to the optic disk. Insets correspond to a model of retinal activation due to direct isotropic activation plus passive electrical diffusion and anisotropic activation due to axons en passant recruitment for 3 different electrode sizes. The brightness codes the strength of the response. Center of the white dashed target: position of the optic disk; black circle: position and size of the MEA active surface; gray lines: 'shadow cone' angle sustained by the MEA active surface respective to the optic disk location; colored contour: size and shape of the global retinal activation for an axons en passant contribution of 1 (alpha, see Materials and methods). (D) Elongation of electrical activations relative to their corresponding visual activations (AR electrical/AR visual) as a function of the 'shadow cone' angle. (E) Cortical radial organization of prosthetic activations. Solid segments: orientation of cortical activations; dashed segments: optimal radial orientation towards the black disk; segment crossing; geometrical center; red dot: center-of-mass of cortical activations; Dark disk: cortical position that optimized radial organization; gray disk: median position of the optic disk. The blue lines connect the center-of-mass to the geometrical center of activations. Scale bar: 0.5 mm. Inset: distribution of median angular deviation expected by chance compare to our observation: blue segment. (F) Top: centered and reoriented deviations of the center-of-mass (blue disks) to the geometrical center (center of the representation), horizontal dashed axis corresponds to the orientation of the radial organization. Bottom: averaged, centered and reoriented SE (with AR > and < than 1.6, left and middle respectively) and wMEA maps (right). White circle: center-of-mass; additional dashed contour corresponds to a Z-score of −4.5.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.12687.007

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Model of retinal anisotropic activation.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

Model of retinal anisotropic activation due to axons en passant for 3 different electrode sizes (rows in B&C). (A) Modeling steps used to compute the activation of axons en passant (see Materials and methods). The brightness codes the strength of the response, all representations are scaled between 0 and 1. (B) Direct activation with passive electrical diffusion (left column). Center of the white dashed lines: position of the optic disk; black circles: position and size of the MEA active surface. Anisotropic activation due to axons en passant recruitment (middle column). Size and shape of the global retinal activation (colored contours, right column) for an axons en passant contribution of 0.5 (alpha, see Materials and methods). (C) same as in A for an axons en passant contribution of 1.
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. Model of retino-cortical transformation.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

To check whether retino-cortical transformation can introduce further bias to our predictions (Figure 4—figure supplement 1), we implemented a retino-cortical transformation based on the magnification measured in our data (A). This model simply generates a transformation using the formula RCM = 1 /(aR+b), where RCM is the retino-cortical magnification factor (mm/deg), R the retinal eccentricity (deg), a and b constants (along the horizontal meridian : a = 0.7; b = 30 & a = 0.4, b = 40 for the vertical dimension). In the Figure A, the resulting transformation is shown for a retinal pattern with circles of different diameter and eccentricity. (B) We show for 2 cone angles (blue and red) and ratio of axon-en-passant (same as Figure 4—figure supplement 1), what such transformation does. (C) From these maps, we calculated the cortical activation aspect ratio, normalized to the shape of activation without axon-en-passant (ratio = 0 not shown), plotted with the same convention as in Figure 4C.

Thus, the predictions that arise from this observation are: for small cone angle (i) the elongation of the cortical activation should be radially organized towards the representation of the optic disk and (ii) the activation should be anisotropic, attracted by the radial elongation towards more eccentric positions (i.e. away from the optic disk representation); lastly (iii) for large cone angle, cortical activation should be more elongated along a tangential axis. To test for these predictions, we pooled together all activation profiles that were sufficiently elongated, i.e. with an AR above 1.6, and plotted them in cortical space (Figure 4E). In this figure, each solid segment corresponds to the orientation of a cortical activation. The dark disk is the cortical position that optimized the radial arrangement of the observed orientations (see Materials and methods). Observed orientations of cortical activation deviated from an optimal radial organization (dashed lines) to this cortical position by only 13.3° (median value across N = 20 activations). Importantly, this cortical position is close to, and in the same direction as, the median position of the optic disk (gray disk). This latter one was estimated from 16 experiments whenever its mapping was possible. However, we could not use it systematically because of imprecision in its estimation (see Materials and methods). We then checked whether this result could occur by chance. For that purpose, we generated 1000 random distributions of orientations at the observed positions and, for each, looked for the cortical position that optimized radial arrangement (as described above). For this position, we computed the median angular deviation between the random orientations and the optimal radial arrangement to this position. This led to a Gaussian distribution of median angular deviation expected by chance in our particular configuration, with a mean of 23.6 ± 4.9° (Figure 4E inset). Our observation of 13.3° (red line) is significantly smaller than what would be expected by chance (p=0.02, see Materials and methods). To test for the second prediction of anisotropic activation that our model raised, we compared the center-of-mass of the activation (Figure 4E red dot) to the geometric center (dashed and solid line crossing). Indeed if the activity is attracted away from the blind-spot, the center-of-mass must be delocalized from the geometrical center, opposite to the position of the blind-spot. We plotted the position of the center-of-mass (red points in Figure 4E), and indeed observed that it was pushed away from the optic disk representation. Figure 4F present a centered, reoriented and zoomed view on all deviations of the center-of-mass (red disk) to the geometrical center (center of the representation) within the same reference frame (horizontal dashed axis being the radial organization). Center-of-masses all deviated away from the optic disk with an averaged angular deviation of −168° (opposite deviation being 180°). To better illustrate this deviation, we averaged all the maps under consideration in this analysis, after realignment along the radial axis (the axis linking the activation to the BS, here the BS is to the right) and centering (Figure 4F bottom left). We can see from this averaged map that there is indeed a radially elongated and anisotropic activation opposite to the representation of the blind spot (dotted contour represent activation higher than 4.5 z-score). For comparison, we made similar averaging on all the other maps in response to SE (with an aspect-ratio < 1.6) and wMEA. For SE activation with low AR, the result shows an activation that is more isotropic. For MEA activation, the averaged map shows, as predicted, a slight elongation along the tangential axis. Hence, in our experimental observations, the radial arrangement and deviation of the center-of-mass of the activation behave as expected from an activation of en-passant axons in the retina.

Intensity

Encoding different levels of luminance is an important aspect of prosthetic vision. We thus investigated the effect of stimulus intensity. Increasing luminance non-linearly increased the cortical activation size and amplitude (Figure 5A). Across the population (N = 8), we fitted the response amplitude (Figure 5C) with a Naka-Rushton function (Naka and Rushton, 1966) for the whole population (black dotted line), as well as for each animal individually (thin gray lines). The luminance of semi-saturation was 9.6 ± 2.9 cd/m2. We then compared these results with the intensity of electrical stimulation (Figure 5D–E) that similarly modulated the cortical activations (Figure 5B). Over 35 animals (wMEA: 10, SE: 25), we observed a gradual increase of response amplitude for both wMEA and SE electrical stimulation, well captured by Naka-Rushton fits (Figure 5D–E, thick dotted lines). The intensity of semi-saturation (Figure 5F) was significantly lower for SE than for wMEA (20.29 ± 3.25 µA vs. 52.66 ± 17.35 µA, see legend for details on statistical procedure). Figure 5G compares the exponent of the individual fits performed on the amplitude parameter for visual, SE and wMEA stimulation. Steep transitions were observed for wMEA (n = 5.58 ± 3.54) and for visual luminance (n = 6.05 ± 3.01) whereas more gradual transitions were observed for SE (n = 1.34 ± 0.6) which significantly differed from wMEA and visual stimuli (see Figure 5G legend). As a consequence, the operating range (see Materials and methods) of the intensity response function in visual stimulation was on average 9.2 ± 4.8 cd/m2, 82.5 ± 88.4 µA for wMEA and much broader for SE (182.6 ± 151.4 µA). This is further reflected in Figure 5H plotting the hypothetic equivalent luminance of electrical activations. We estimated the equivalent visual luminance that corresponds to the response amplitude evoked by electrical stimulation using the Naka-Rushton fit (Figure 5C). The functional operating range induced by current intensity manipulation allowed generating an artificial activation with an equivalent luminance that could theoretically be in the range of 2–50 cd/m2.

Figure 5. Intensity.

Figure 5.

(A) V1 activation generated by visual stimuli of increasing luminance (value indicated above maps). Center-of-mass: white circle; extent: white contour; equivalent ellipse orientation and size of activation contour: white cross; scale bar: 2 mm. (B) V1 activation generated by SE (top) and wMEA (bottom) in 2 different animals at high current intensity. Center-of-mass of visual and electrical activations are indicated with white and colored circles respectively and extent of electrical activation in colored contours; scale bar: 2 mm. (C) Amplitude of cortical activation as a function of visual stimulus luminance computed over 8 rats (population fit: dashed black line; individual fits: gray thin lines). (D) Amplitude of cortical activations generated by wMEA stimulation at high current intensity. Population fit: dark red dashed line; individual fits: orange thin lines (N = 10). (E) Amplitude of cortical activations generated by SE stimulation at high current intensity. Population fit: dark blue dashed line (N = 25). Note that individual fits (cyan thin lines) were only performed on N=6 animals (tested with 7 different levels of intensity) and not on the others (N=19, tested with only 2 different levels: 50 and 200 µA). (F) Constant of semi-saturation (c50) for visual (gray in cd/m2) and electrical activations (wMEA: red and SE: blue, in µA). Two-sample Wilcoxon rank sum test: **p=0.0017, nSE = NSE = 6, nwMEA = NwMEA = 10. (G) Exponent of the naka-rushton fits for visual (gray) and electrical activations (wMEA: red and SE: blue). Two-sample Wilcoxon rank sum test: ***pSE vs. visual=6.66 10–4 (nSE = NSE = 6, nvisual = Nvisual = 8); **pwMEA vs. SE=0.0075 (nSE = NSE = 6, nwMEA = NwMEA = 10); pwMEA vs. visual=0.896 (nwMEA = NwMEA = 10, nvisual = Nvisual = 8). (H) Amplitude-based correspondence for all electrical activations to their visual counterpart (wMEA: red and SE: blue) as a function of current intensity level (in equivalent cd/m2).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.12687.010

Focalization

The previous results show that artificial activations of the visual system with sub-retinal implants generate stimuli of appropriate position and scalable intensity, but whose size is on average 2.4 and 5.8 times larger than expected for wMEA and SE, respectively. This clearly impairs the functional efficiency of the implants. We therefore took advantage of our experimental design to seek solutions for controlling the spatial extent of the functional activation. Several candidate parameters have been investigated for SE stimulation (Figures 6 and 7, respectively blue vs. green colors): polarity (cathodic vs. anodic pulse first, Figure 6A–B), symmetry vs. asymmetry of the biphasic square pulse (McIntyre and Grill, 2000) (Figure 6C–D) and regular pulses vs. electrical impedance spectroscopy-based (IS) adaptation (Dupont et al., 2013) (Figure 7). In the example shown and over the population (Figure 6A–B), our results showed that the polarity of symmetric biphasic pulses did not significantly influence the extent of the cortical activation for the two intensity levels tested. However, the polarity was found to have a significant effect in asymmetric pulses delivered at 200 µA (Figure 6C–D). The combination of asymmetric with anodic-first stimulation (Figure 6C top right) generated smaller activation of 1.66 ± 0.71 mm corresponding to 17.12 ± 8.11°eq (N = 7), smaller than the three other combinations. However, this reduction was not systematic (70% of the cases compared to the symmetrical pulse) and small (20.43% ± 54.32, the corresponding [20-50-80] percentiles being [34.78–32.93–63.89]%). Please note that this negative result is the outcome of thorough manipulation of key parameters of the electrical stimulation and highlight the high variability of the changes induced. Our best result at this stage is obtained by combining the polarity and the asymmetry parameters.

Figure 6. Focalization.

Figure 6.

(A) SE activations generated by square pulses of different polarity (anodic first: blue; cathodic first: green) at 2 intensity levels (50 and 200 µA). Scale bar: 2 mm. (B) Effect of polarity and intensity on cortical extent for SE (anodic first: blue; cathodic first: green, N = 10). One-sided Wilcoxon rank sum test for paired data, p=0.3802 (n = 18, N = 10) and 0.0615 (n = 11, N = 10) for 50 and 200 µA respectively. (C) Individual example of asymmetrical (green) and symmetrical (blue) square pulses, same animal as in (A). (D) Effect of square pulse asymmetry (green) for two polarities on cortical extent for SE (N = 10). One-sided Wilcoxon rank sum test for paired data: **pCathoSym vs. CathoAsy=0.0052 (n = 13, N = 10); *pAnoAsy vs. CathoAsy=0.0469 (n = 6, N = 10).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.12687.011

Figure 7. Impedance spectroscopy adaptation.

(A) Example of electrode-tissue interface filtering on pulse shape (at 500 mVpp) with (green) and without (blue) IS based adaptation. Top: injected pulse; bottom: pulse shape reaching the tissue. (B) Individual example of IS adaptation (green, top maps) and reference (blue, bottom maps) for 2 voltage levels; scale bar: 2 mm. Note that for this protocol only, we switched from current to voltage injection. (C) Effect of IS adapted pulses on cortical extent for SE at 2 intensity levels (5 and 12 Vpp in voltage injection mode). IS adapted pulses: green; reference: blue, N = 4. One-sided Wilcoxon rank sum test for paired data: *p=0.0117 (n = 8, N = 4), **p=0.0078 (n = 7, N = 4).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.12687.012

Figure 7.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1. Principle of IS adaptation.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.

(A) The stimuli processing platform’s architecture integrates an Impedance Spectroscopy (IS) recording module; an Identification Algorithm module fitting IS data to an Electronic Equivalent Circuit (EEC); a Transfer Function Computing module and an Adapted Stimuli Shaping module computing adapted stimuli emitted by the Stimuli Generator. (B) Linear EEC used for this study. I: current; V: voltage; CPE: constant phase element (capacitance of the electrical double layer formed at the interface between a metallic electrode and an ionic solution); C: retinal membrane capacitance; R: resistance; bulk: subretinal fluid. (C) Mean duration (top) and frequency content (bottom) of reference (left) and IS adapted (right) pulse shapes recorded in the tissue (reference pulses: cathodic first symmetrical biphasic squared pulses). Duration: full width at half maximum (FWHM) of each recorded transients for the reference pulses (corresponding to the 3 abrupt pulse transitions) and of the 2 sustained phases for the adapted ones. Frequency: power of the Fourier transformed computed on the pulses temporal profiles.

One general issue encountered when injecting current or voltage in a tissue, and often ignored, is that the desired pattern to be injected can be strongly distorted by the non-ohmic properties of the electrode-tissue interface (Geddes, 1997; Pham et al., 2013). In a previous study (Pham et al., 2013), we indeed showed that the impedance phase and magnitude of the electrode-retina interface was found to be highly capacitive, yielding strong distortion of the applied stimulus (Figure 7A, left bottom) and lateral diffusion of the injected pattern. Our rationale was therefore to use the characterization of the physical properties of the electrode-tissue interface to calibrate through reverse engineering methods what needs to be injected to obtain the desired pattern in the tissue. To do so, we characterized the impedance spectrogram of selected SE of each implantation (Pham et al., 2013). Using an equivalent electronic circuit accounting for the observed non-ohmic behavior, we simulated (Figure 7—figure supplement 1B, see Materials and methods for details on the procedure) the adapted pattern of voltage injection (Figure 7A, top right) that will generate the desired pattern shape in the implanted tissue (Dupont et al., 2013) (Figure 7A, right bottom). Note that, for this protocol only, we switched from current to voltage injection and that the low voltage level used here was of the same order of magnitude as the high current level used in the previous protocols. For 100% and 88% of the low and high voltage level cases respectively (5 Vpp and 12 Vpp, Vpp: Volts peak-to-peak), the adapted pattern injected through SE decreased the extent of the activation (Figure 7B top maps & 7C). This significant decrease (see Figure 7C legend) varied on average from 36 ± 32% to 20.7 ± 29.2% at low and high voltage, respectively (the corresponding [20-50-80] percentiles were [11.78–27.50–59.05] and [1.89–6.56–39.67]%). Importantly, at high voltage it was not accompanied by a significant change in response amplitude (Wilcoxon rank sum test: p=0.546, n = 8, N = 4), suggesting that this reduction of the extent was not simply explained by a change in activation strength level, allowing for an independent control of the extent (size) and the amplitude (intensity) of the evoked activity.

Discussion

We have here described a detailed characterization of the functional impact of sub-retinal prosthesis at the V1 mesoscopic scale level of rats and proposed an original solution to control its extent. In detail, we have generated a precise mapping of the rodent cortical cartographical organization in response to position, size and intensity of visual stimuli. These cortical population maps were then used to establish a functional interpretation of the activations induced by electrical stimulation. The recording of robust and reliable electrically and visually evoked responses indicates that both types of information are efficiently transmitted to the primary visual cortex and prove the functional integrity of the retinal tissue after implantation, which was also anatomically confirmed by retinal imaging (OCT). Here we demonstrate that prosthetic stimulation generates a functional activation that occurs at the expected retinotopic location and amplitude but whose extent and aspect ratio are significantly larger than expected. To control the extent of cortical activation we tested various patterns of electrical stimulation and showed that supervised design of the electrical pulses -- taking into account the physical filtering properties of the electrode-retina interface -- allowed focalizing the activation.

The working hypothesis of this study is that population activation of the first cortical integration stage is correlated to underlying evoked percepts, as suggested by many studies (Chen et al., 2006; Ni, 2010; Palmer et al., 2007; Tehovnik and Slocum, 2006). Furthermore, the link between population activation in optical imaging and spiking discharge has long been established (Chen et al., 2012; Das and Gilbert, 1995; Shmuel and Grinvald, 1996; Toth et al., 1996). Lastly, a recent study showed that the spatial profile of cortical population activation in the awake monkey biases the shape of the evoked percept (Michel et al., 2013). Therefore, we believe that the current approach is valid for an initial description of the functional activation of retinal prosthesis. The next step will be to launch experiments in behaving non-human primates, to clarify, in a model closer to humans, the relationship between the properties of the population prosthetic activation and the behavioral evaluation of the animal’s percepts.

Accurate activation in position and intensity

Prosthetic activation yielded to cortical activations that were consistent with the V1 retinotopic organization. The degree of positional precision, higher in SE compared to wMEA, was of the same order of magnitude as the 20° visual stimuli. However, this measure was quite variable, as expected given the poor precision of the murine visual system (Euler and Wässle, 1995). Here we significantly extended previous investigations that have first used mesoscopic optical recordings to image the cortical features of the prosthetic activation but on a limited number of animals (Walter et al., 2005) and with no quantitative statistical analysis (Eckhorn et al., 2006; Walter et al., 2005). Our approach allowed providing a clear and quantitative description of the prosthetic activation.

We have also shown that increasing the level of current intensity increases the amplitude of the evoked cortical response with a sigmoidal profile similar to the one observed with visual luminance. Such non-linear profiles are typically well characterized by their threshold and slope (Naka and Rushton, 1966). The threshold differences observed in wMEA and SE stimulation configurations can trivially result from difference in active electrode surface. However, our experiments unveiled that SE offers a larger operating range (182 vs. 82 µA for wMEA) to cover a theoretical modulation range of about 50 cd/m2. This result demonstrates that small electrode sizes will be more appropriate to make fine manipulation of stimulus intensity. Importantly, our results also revealed that the increasing stimulus intensity increases both the response size and the response amplitude. This observation is crucial to consider since it will seriously challenge independent manipulation of those important parameters in prosthetic vision.

Distorted activation in size and shape

Our results demonstrate that the size elicited with standard parameters is approximately 2.4 to 5.8 times larger than expected for wMEA and SE, respectively. This increase can be partly explained by passive electrical diffusion in the implant-retina interface, as we previously suggest (Pham et al., 2013), but also by the activation of en passant ganglion axons. The spatial resolution of the prosthetic retinal activation measured in our experiments, about a millimeter of retinal space, is actually comparable to what has been reported in other animals (as estimated from Eger et al., 2005; Schanze et al., 2003) as well as in human studies (as estimated from Ahuja and Behrend, 2013; Humayun et al., 2012; Stingl et al., 2013) and extends beyond the retinal point spread function (Stett et al., 2000, 2007).

Are these results accentuated by the anesthetized state of the animal? Indeed, recent experiments showed that anesthetized mice have less surround inhibition than when awake (Vaiceliunaite et al., 2013). However, Vizuete et al (Vizuete et al., 2012) have shown that anesthesia affects not simply the inhibition but more probably the balance between excitation and inhibition (see also Chemla and Chavane (2016)). Hence, predicting how activation size of the population activity will be affected by anesthesia is not straightforward. Importantly, our study was devised to make a systematic comparison between conditions done under similar level of anesthesia (visual vs. electrical) and the polarity of the effect (the size increasing or decreasing) is not expected to reverse by changing the animal state.

For SE stimulation, large activations were also accompanied by an increase in the aspect ratio of the spatial profile of the cortical activation suggesting an asymmetric recruitment of the retinal tissue, for instance through activation of ganglion axons en passant (Nowak and Bullier, 1998a, 1998b). However, this increase in aspect ratio was not observed with wMEA. Using a simple model, we show that such difference in aspect ratio between stimulation conditions is actually to be expected because of differences in the spatial distribution of activation of axons en passant. Our model also allowed predicting that, to account for the observed spatial profiles, axons en passant should contribute to the same proportion than direct activation of the retina. In accordance with this prediction, we found that high aspect ratio activations distribute radially around the cortical representation of the blind spot and are asymmetrical, with center-of-masses displaced away from the blind spot representation. In contrast, Fransen et al. (Fransen et al., 2014) discard the possibility of direct RGCs activation through near infrared laser stimulation of photovoltaic subretinal array because of the absence of responses in the superior colliculus using inner retina synaptic blockers. However, several factors, including the low efficiency of photovoltaic transduction and the level of injected currents used in our study, might explain the differential recruitment of RGCs cell bodies and axons observed here using V1 population recordings.

Since most implantations occurred at more or less the same retinal eccentricity, our model predicts that activation size and aspect-ratio should be inversely related: the smaller the activation size (equivalent to a smaller cone angle for a fixed eccentricity), the larger the expected aspect ratio. Figure 8 indeed shows that the averaged sizes observed for standard electrical stimulations are systematically inversely correlated to their aspect ratio. Remarkably, all regular manipulations of the stimulation pattern moved the activation spatial profile along that inverse relationship. In contrast, visual activation does not display such correlation, the aspect-ratio being small and independent of activation size (Figure 8, gray dots). This relationship could therefore be taken as a diagnosis of the way electrical stimulation activates the retinal circuitry with lateral isotropic diffusion and anisotropic axons en passant activation. To mimic a functional visual activation, it is therefore needed to decrease both the extent and the aspect ratio of the prosthetic activation.

Figure 8. Differential effect of SE patterns.

Figure 8.

Averaged cortical size and aspect ratio (± sem: black error bars) elicited by visual stimuli, by the wMEA and by the different SE stimulation patterns delivered at high intensity levels. Gray circles: 10 and 20 deg visual stimuli (n = 7 and 75, respectively); cyan and red squares: high intensity symmetrical stimulation patterns for SE (n = 57) and wMEA (n = 10) respectively (both polarities); green and cyan circles: respectively IS adapted and non-adapted pulses delivered at 5 Vpp (n = 7, same order of magnitude as the other pulses delivered at high current intensity); green and cyan up triangles: respectively asymmetrical (n = 7) and symmetrical (n = 21) anodic first pulses delivered at ± 200 µA; green and cyan down triangles: respectively asymmetrical (n = 8) and symmetrical (n = 24) cathodic first pulses delivered at ± 200 µA. Lines link comparable conditions. The different SE patterns evoke activations that exhibit strong correlation between size and aspect ratio (gray shaded area), except for IS adapted stimuli which converge towards visual responses.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.12687.014

How to improve prosthetic activation?

Charge balanced biphasic pulses, delivered in voltage or current mode injection, are of common use in human (Ahuja and Behrend, 2013; Fujikado et al., 2007; Humayun et al., 2012; Klauke et al., 2011; Nanduri et al., 2012; Rizzo, 2003; Shepherd et al., 2013; Stingl et al., 2013; Wilke et al., 2011; Zrenner, 2013; Zrenner et al., 2010) or animal (Chowdhury et al., 2008; Cicione et al., 2012; Eckhorn et al., 2006; Eger et al., 2005; Elfar et al., 2009; Matteucci et al., 2013; Nadig, 1999; Schanze et al., 2003; Walter et al., 2005; Wong et al., 2009) studies of prosthetic vision. In the literature, we observed a large consensus concerning the use of cathodic pulse first for epiretinal stimulation (Ahuja and Behrend, 2013; Eckhorn et al., 2006; Eger et al., 2005; Elfar et al., 2009; Fried et al., 2006; Nanduri et al., 2012; Schanze et al., 2003; Walter et al., 2005) although not for subretinal, suprachoroidal and extraocular approaches (Chowdhury et al., 2008; Cicione et al., 2012; Eckhorn et al., 2006; Fujikado et al., 2007; Matteucci et al., 2013; Stingl et al., 2013; Wilke et al., 2011; Wong et al., 2009). It has been shown in vitro (Jensen et al., 2005) that the use of single cathodic pulse lowers the threshold of retinal ganglion cells (RGCs), lowers the latency of inner retina mediated RGCs response and targets more specifically RGCs cell bodies when compared to anodal pulses. As far as we know, only one study systematically examined the effect the polarity of charge balanced biphasic square pulses (Chowdhury et al., 2008). Using an extraocular device, Chowdhury et al. did not find any significant effect of the pulse polarity on the threshold of V1 electrically evoked potentials. Similarly, we did not find any effect of the polarity on the size of cortical activations. Altogether, anodic asymmetric pulses were the best among classical stimulations to restrict cortical activations, although not systematically. This result contradicts the prediction of the pioneer modeling work of McIntyre & Grill (McIntyre and Grill, 2000), which predicts more focal activation for cathodic asymmetrical pulses using a cable model. To conclude, basic manipulation of the electrical pulses did not yield a systematic reduction of the spatial extent of the activation. Furthermore, when compared to the aspect ratio (Figure 8), it turns out that all these stimulation configurations lead to the same inverse relationship suggesting that all activated the retinal network through diffusion and axons en passant.

Note that other strategies, such as current steering methods, could be investigated in future in vivo studies (Jepson et al., 2014a). These authors showed in in-vitro isolated retinal preparation, that it was possible to improve the spatial resolution of the implant up to the activation of a single RGC type with a single action potential resolution (Jepson et al., 2013; Sekirnjak et al., 2008).

To further scrutinize optimal stimulation parameters, we have also used a more 'supervised' approach by adapting the shape of the desired retinal stimulation through impedance spectroscopy to account for the filtering properties of the electrode-tissue interface (resistive and capacitive components) (Dupont et al., 2013; Pham et al., 2013). Our results show that electrical stimulation adaptation systematically and accurately decreased the activation spread while preserving their location and amplitude, when compared to their paired controls. Crucially, it was the only configuration we tested whose decrease in extent was not accompanied by an increase in aspect ratio (Figure 8, filled circles), similarly to functional visual stimulation. This strongly suggests that such stimulation reduces both the electrical diffusion extent (although not completely) and the axons en passant activation. What is the origin of the reduction of electrical diffusion? This could arise from the change of the shape of the injected current, that activate less high frequencies (Figure 7—figure supplement 1C) that increase the spread of the electrical diffusion and the current density magnitude (Pham et al 2013). By which mechanisms axons-en-passant would be less activated? Two mechanisms are possible. First, this could be a consequence of the reduction of the electrical diffusion. Since we are positioned subretinally, reducing the diffusion will favor activation of elements closer to the electrode (photoreceptors, bipolar cells…) compared to further away (ganglion cells). A second mechanism would originate from the fact that the duration of each adapted pulse (anodic or cathodic) in the tissue is much longer (1.2 ms on average) than the transients contained in the non-adapted pulses (0.2 ms on average). This can favor activation of cell bodies since axons have shorter chronaxie, generally some hundreds of microseconds, than cell bodies, generally several milliseconds (McIntyre and Grill, 2002; Nowak and Bullier, 1998b, 1998a; Ranck, 1975; Stern et al., 2015; Tehovnik and Slocum, 2009, Histed et al., 2009). This effect could be further amplified by the fact that the adapted pulse inside the tissue was also biphasic, cathodic phase first and asymmetrical in amplitude. According to the work of Mc Intyre & Grill (McIntyre and Grill, 2002), these are the conditions for which we can expect the threshold for activation to increase for axons and to decrease for the soma. It is thus expected that the adapted condition will lower the contribution of fiber-en-passant, since the amplitude of the current injected in the tissue was lower in adapted than in non-adapted mode. This is therefore a very promising solution getting closer to a more natural visual activation of the retinal network. Importantly, it is noteworthy to point out that this stimulation strategy can be easily embedded in already available commercial devices using small electronic circuits. Supervised adaptation of electrical pulse based on the electrical properties of the electrode-tissue interface is therefore a promising solution for retinal, but also probably, for cochlear prosthetic stimulations and more generally for all neuro-stimulation applications.

As a perspective, one may consider to combine the advantages of the different methods that have been shown, here and in other publications, to have an effect: adaptation, asymmetry of the pulse (McIntyre and Grill, 2001) and the spatial arrangement of stimulating and counterelectrodes (Jepson et al., 2014a). The possible independence of the mechanisms by which these methods decrease the activation size may allow an additive gain in spatial resolution.

Conclusion

Here we provide a clear quantitative functional description of retinal prosthetic activations by using a systematic comparison of artificial and natural activation of the rodent visual system. Using a similar approach, further investigations will have to probe the dynamical aspect of these prosthetic activation to better understand how to generate spatio-temporal activations in the visual pathway (Elfar et al., 2009; Fried et al., 2006) that are similar to the ones observed in response to natural stimuli. A further challenge will be to drive the stimulator to generate activity closer to the retina’s natural neural code (Nirenberg and Pandarinath, 2012), which may involve taking into account higher order correlations (Marre et al., 2012; Marre and Botella, 2014). All these strategic steps will greatly benefit from animal models such as the one used here. To conclude, our work demonstrates that pre-clinical studies are a necessary prerequisite to validate and improve the efficiency of retinal implants, equivalent to animal models that are the foundation of any drug development for human pharmacology. We thus expect that our study will pave the way for fast and significant improvements of the clinical benefits offered to implanted patients.

Materials and methods

Animal preparation

A total of 35 Brown Norway male rats (1.5–3 months old, 230–320 g) were anesthetized using an intraperitoneal injection of urethane. The experimental protocol was approved beforehand by the local Ethical Committee for Animal Research and all procedures complied with the French and European regulations on Animal Research (approval n°A12/01/13) as well as the guidelines from the Society for Neuroscience. Instead of a complete craniotomy, the bone was thinned with a drill until a clear optical access to V1 surface was obtained. Finally, transparent silicone was applied to the remaining bone and the preparation was covered with a glass slide.

Retinal implants

We used subretinal Micro Electrode Arrays (MEAs) manufactured at the CEA-LETI (Pham et al., 2013) (Grenoble, France). These planar MEAs of 1 and 1.2 mm diameter comprise respectively 9 and 17 (50 μm radius, 3 μm thick) platinum (Pt) contacts and a large annular Pt counter-electrode (CE) within a polyimide flexible substrate (Figure 1C top row). The electrical connection between the MEA and the stimulator (BioMEA, CEA-LETI) (Charvet et al., 2010) is made using an Omnetics 18-position nanominiature connector.

Sub-retinal implantation procedure

The MEAs were implanted between the pigmentary epithelium and the outer segment photoreceptor layer (Figure 1C bottom row). Eyedrops of oxybuprocaïn chlorohydrate were used to provide a local anesthesia and the pupil was dilated using atropine drops. The bulbar conjunctiva was removed on the top of the eyeball and a millimetric incision of the sclera was performed to access the subretinal space. A cannula was then carefully inserted and a controlled injection of balanced saline solution (BSS; from B. Braun Medical Inc.) was used to induce a retinal detachment by hydrodissection. The implants were then carefully inserted and advanced below the retina towards the desired position. Eye fundus and/or OCT imaging were then performed to check implantation quality as well as the absence of potential lesions. After each implantation a recovery period from 30 min to 1 hr was respected before starting data acquisition and the presence of visual evoked cortical activation was used to probe the functional integrity of the implanted retinal tissue.

Eye fundus and OCT

In some animals, the retina was imaged using the scanner 3D OCT-2000 (Topcon, Tokyo, Japan), allowing a complete assessment of the area around the MEA. Each acquisition combined both OCT and fundus imaging. The OCT has an axial resolution of 5 μm. The superluminescent diode light source used is centered at 840 nm with a bandwidth of 50 nm adapted for retinal imaging. The focus was adjusted manually on the retina above the MEA. The analysis was performed using 3D OCT-2000 software (Topcon, Tokyo, Japan) and consisted in the localization of the MEA on the corresponding B-Scan cross-sections. For fundus images, the OCT was combined with a camera (Nikon R_D90, Nikon Imaging Japan Inc.) that uses a white light flash with green filter, allowing color or red-free acquisitions. An additional +20 D magnifying lens was used to enlarge the field of view of the apparatus originally designed for measurements on humans. Note that the use of this lens induced optical artifacts in fundus (Figure 1C top row, halos of light). Finally, the positions of the implant and of the optic disk were back projected onto the visual stimulation screen using an ophthalmoscope coupled with a laser.

Data acquisition

We imaged 5*5 mm cortical windows using 2 different intrinsic imaging systems, the MiCAM ULTIMA imaging (SciMedia, 100*100 pixels, 33.3 Hz) and data-acquisition system as well as a Dalsa camera (Optical Imaging Inc, 340*340 pixels, 30 Hz) controlled by the VDAQ data-acquisition system (Optical Imaging). The brain was illuminated at 605 nm by 2 optic fibers. Each trial lasted 8 s and the beginning of each acquisition was triggered by the heartbeat. Five hundred milliseconds later (frame 0) the visual or electrical stimulus was displayed during 1 s. The blank condition consisted on a gray screen of 0.5 Cd/m2 of averaged luminance for visual stimulation and no current injection for the electrical stimulation. The trials were repeated 20 to 40 times per condition with an ISI of 3 s.

Visual stimulation

Stimuli were displayed monocularly at 60 Hz on a gamma corrected LCD monitor (placed at 21.6 cm from the animal eye plane) covering 100° (W) x 80° (H) of visual angle using the Elphy software (Elphy, Unic, Paris). In each trial, a gray screen with an averaged luminance of 0.5 Cd/m2 was displayed during 500 ms (frame 0); the visual stimulus was then presented during 1 s followed again by a gray screen of 0.5 Cd/m2 during 6.5 s. Three different protocols were then used. For retinotopic mapping, a white square (49 Cd/m2) of 20° side was displayed on a gray background (0.5 Cd/m2) at 20 different positions (5 horizontal*4 vertical) in a grid like manner (Figure 2A). For the size protocol, the same visual stimuli were displayed at 4 different positions. For each of these positions, the squared visual stimuli were displayed in different sizes. Several ranges of size were used [2 6 10 14 18°], [6 10 14 18 22°] and [3.2 9.6 16 22.4 28.8°]. In the last protocol, 20° side stimuli were also displayed at 4 different positions but with varying luminance levels ([2 13.75 25.5 37.25 49 Cd/m2]).

Electrical stimulation

Electrical stimuli were designed and injected using a BioMEA stimulator (CEA-LETI, Grenoble, France) (Charvet et al., 2010) in current injection mode. We used charge balanced squared pulses (1 ms per phase) of varying current intensity and polarity displayed at 80 Hz during 1 s. Such pulses were either injected through single electrodes (SE, for MEAs with 9 and 17 electrodes) or through all active electrodes (wMEA, for MEAs with 9 electrodes only) in monopolar mode. The diameters of the MEA active surface are of 50 and 650 µm for SE and wMEA, respectively which correspond to 1 and 11° according to Hughes (Hughes, 1979). Four different protocols were then used. In the first protocol, we tested the effect of increasing current intensity level [± 4 8 16 37.5 75 150 300 µA] (cathodic pulse first) for SE and wMEA. For the analysis, current levels ≤ 50 µA and ≥75 µA will be considered as low and high intensities, respectively. We next investigated the effect of stimulus intensity and polarity for SE only. Two current intensity levels (± 50 and ± 200 µA) and both polarities (cathodic or anodic pulse first) were used. For SE, we also tested the effect of pulse asymmetry and polarity at ± 200 µA. Instead of having symmetrical phase of 1 ms each, the asymmetrical pulses consisted of a first long phase of 1 ms at 40 µA followed by a short phase of 200 µs at 200 µA (asymmetrical ratio = 5). In total, 4 conditions were used in this protocol, 2 symmetrical and 2 asymmetrical pulses of opposite polarities. For the last protocol in SE, we switch from current to voltage injection mode to test the effect of Impedance Spectroscopy (IS) adapted stimuli as described below.

Impedance spectroscopy measurements and adapted stimulation

Here we present a voltage-mode stimulation platform with the capability to deliver a controlled voltage pattern to the stimulation targets by taking into account the electrode-tissue interface filtering properties (Dupont et al., 2013). This procedure requires first measuring the impedance of the electrodes (magnitude and phase) for a wide range of frequencies (Impedance Spectroscopy). These measures unveil the capacitive nature of the electrode-tissue interface (Geddes, 1997). Thus the actual current or voltage pattern injected in the tissue is strongly distorted and does not resemble the desired pattern (Figure 7A, left column). A solution proposed by Dupont et al. (Dupont et al., 2013) is therefore to estimate the equivalent electrical circuit that accounts for the observed spectrum and use it to reverse engineer what needed to be used to achieve the desired injected pattern (Figure 7A, right column). To achieve this, the experimental design integrates an IS recording module, an Identification Algorithm module fitting IS data to an Electronic Equivalent Circuit (EEC), a Transfer Function Computing module and an Adapted Stimuli Shaping module computing adapted stimuli emitted by the Stimuli Generator module (Figure 7—figure supplement 1A, patent CEA-LETI) (Dupont et al., 2013). In this study, the test bench is built with commercial devices. IS measurements are performed by injecting low AC voltage between 100 Hz and 200 kHz with a Bio-Logic potentiostat (SP240 type). Data are fitted to a chosen EEC (Figure 7—figure supplement 1B, see legend for a detailed description of the different components of the circuit) (McAdams and Jossinet, 2000, 1996; Ragheb and Geddes, 1990) using the potentiostat associated software (EC-Lab). A transfer function H linking VIMPLANT to VTISSUE is defined. To compute the adapted signal to be generated by the stimuli generator, we use the inverse transfer function H-1 estimation. However, this transfer function does not take into account the impedance’s non-linearity toward voltage levels. To address this issue, several IS measurements are performed between 1 mVpp and 4 Vpp (Vpp: Volts peak-to-peak). Each set of data is fitted onto the same EEC. The abacuses 'EEC parameters versus voltage' are then used to compute the adapted stimulus using voltage dedicated transfer functions per signal harmonics.

Data analysis

Stacks of images were stored on hard-drives for off-line analysis. The analysis was carried out with MATLAB R2014a (Math-Works) using the Optimization, Statistics, and Signal Processing Toolboxes. Data were first preprocessed to allow comparison between animals and conditions.

To allow comparison across conditions, sessions, animals and stimuli (visual vs electric) we performed temporal and spatial normalizations of optical imaging signals. We dealt beforehand with pixel-wise trial to trial variability. The averaged time-course of the outer border (2 pixels width) of the image area (outside the region of interest) was subtracted from each pixel to remove non-functional temporal noise pattern. We then performed from each trial a first temporal normalization by subtracting the mean and dividing by the standard deviation both estimated from the frame 0. Second we averaged over trials and applied a spatial normalization. Static maps were computed by averaging 1.5 to 2.5 s after stimulus onset (initial dip). Spatial z-score normalization was based on the averaged static map of the blank condition. Pixel by pixel, we subtracted from each static map the mean value of the blank and divided the outcome by the blank standard deviation over space. Significance of cortical activation was achieved by applying a threshold of −3.09 Z-score (activation contour) which corresponds to p-value of 0.01 (lower activations were not considered for further analysis). These activation contours were computed on a smoothed version of the maps (convolving the raw matrix with a 15x15 pixel flat matrix). To give a comparison with most optical imaging publications, we also provided on each map a second colorbar scale expressed in DI/I which was computed using the standard procedure as follows (see Reynaud et al., 2011): The first step consists of dividing each image of the stack by the mean of the first frames recorded before stimulus onset. In the second step, image stacks collected during stimulated trials are subtracted to those acquired during blank trials on a frame-by-frame basis.

The cortical center-of-mass was computed on the z-scored map. In order to quantify the extent and the shape of the activation contour, we computed the equivalent ellipse (Haralock and Shapiro, 1991). This provided the length and orientation of the equivalent ellipse minor and major axis as well as its equivalent diameter and geometric center.

In order to compute the retinotopic polar maps we computed for each pixel the visual activation map giving its response over the 5*4 visual positions. We then computed the visual center-of-mass (equivalent to a receptive field center). The abscissas or ordinates of each center-of-mass computed for each pixel across positions were plotted separately for azimuth and elevation respectively as polar map (Figure 2B, the colors representing the position across the 2 cardinal dimensions and the hue representing the intensity of the response for the center-of-mass position). Based on this retinotopic mapping, the expected cortical position representing each visual position was computed by taking the intersection of the 2 cardinal polar maps. For each visual or electricalevoked response, we computed the positional error as the distance between the retinotopic expected position (given by the retinoptopic position of the visual or electrical stimulus) and the center-of-mass of the corresponding activation (Figure 2J).

To compute cortical magnification factor, size or amplitude response functions, linear fits and Naka Rushton functions were used. For the intensity parameter, we computed the functional operational range (10–90%) for each animal individually as follow:

operationalrange=C50*91n C5091n

With C50 and n being the semi-saturation parameter and the exponent of the sigmoidal fit, respectively.

To investigate the spatial profile and the nature of retinal activation elicited by electrical stimulation of the MEA, we compute at the optic disk location the 'shadow cone' angle that sustains the MEA active surface as follow:

shadowconeangle=2*arcsin(xy)z

X being the MEA radius, y the distance between the MEA and the optic disk location (determined form eye fundus pictures). For each animal and stimulation condition (SE vs. wMEA), the angle values were corrected according to the real stimulated active surface diameter (z).

Radial organization and anisotropy of activations

To find the cortical position that optimized the radial arrangement of the elongated SE cortical activations, we made the following calculation. For each cortical positions (i, j), we calculated the median value of the differences between the angles formed by the segments that connect each cortical activation center to the cortical position in question (i, j) (e.g. expected radial arrangement, dashed segments in Figure 4E) and the observed angle of the elongated cortical activation (solid segments in Figure 4E). We then looked for the cortical position for which this difference was the smallest (i.e. the cortical position for which the observed orientation of the activations was the closest to the expected radial arrangement). This gave us the cortical position that optimized the radial arrangement for our specific configuration (black disk in Figure 4E). We thereafter use this position rather than the median position of the optic disk (quite close, gray disk in Figure 4E) because we believe it is more reliable for two reasons. First, since we positioned all cortical images within the same reference frame (aligned to the midline and to bregma), the cortical representation of the optic disk should be approximately located at similar location in the map. Second, the individual mapping of the optic disk would have added unnecessary noise because of its imprecision (and for some sessions we could not achieve a proper mapping). Indeed, to map the optic disk representation, we estimated its position on the screen using backward projections from an ophthalmoscope. Then we put it in correspondence to the visual stimulus of our retinotopic exploration (stimuli of 20x20°) it was falling within. The center of the cortical activation for this stimulus gave an estimation of where the optic disk should be represented cortically. This introduces a series of imprecision (estimation with the ophthalmoscope, backward projection, not centered with the visual stimulus, cortical activation) that would be detrimental to our computation.

To test whether the radial arrangement of the observed orientations of SE activations occurred by chance, we used a Monte-Carlo like simulation. The question is whether the difference between the predicted (optimal radial arrangement) and the observed angles of activation is smaller than the one expected by chance. For that purpose, we generated 1000 random distributions of orientations of cortical activation at the observed positions. For each of these random distributions, we looked for the cortical position that optimized radial arrangement with the method described above. For each specific distribution that was randomly generated, we thus obtained a value of the minimum angular deviation between the predicted and the observed orientations. We obtained likewise a distribution of the angular deviation expected by chance (Figure 4E inset), over which we used the area under the curve of our observation to estimate the p-value: p*=0.021 (note that the distribution was also very well captured by a Gaussian fit from which we can calculate the probability of finding the observed angular deviation, and obtained a comparable z-score of 2.09, p=0.018).

Model of retinal activation

A functional model was designed to predict spatial features of retinal activation elicited by electrical stimulation which depicted a 2D spatial layout of the retina, the central position corresponding to the optic disk. To predict the level, the shape and size of retinal activation, we have introduced two components of diffusion of activity that extend beyond the electrode surface: an isotropic Gaussian spread (G) and an 'en passant' activation (EP) of the ganglion cells’ axons in response to a stimulation of a given size and a given location in the retinal space. Isotropic 'direct' activation was modeled as a 'flat-top' Gaussian activation around the site of stimulation (see Figure 4—figure supplement 1):

IsoA(i,j)={1,  (ii0)2 +(jj0)2<Se((ii0)2 +(jj0)22(σS)2),  (ii0)2 +(jj0)2S

Where i,j are positions in space, i0,j0 the position of the electrode, S the size of the electrode and σ the spatial isotropic diffusion extending beyond the electrode.

Anisotropic en passant recruitment of ganglion cell axons was modeled as a shadow cone activation that would result from this 'direct' activation. To compute it, we multiply cone angle activation (Cone) with a sigmoid function that is 0 for distance between the blind spot and the electrode and 1 for distance further away (Sigma) and with a general attenuation for distances very far from the electrode (Attenuation):

EP(i,j)=AttSigAngDiff{Att(i,j)=e(((ii0)2 +(jj0)2S)22N2)Sig(i,j)=11+edOD(j0jod)S/4Cone(i,j)=eConeAng22σ2,   ifj>jc
where dOD(i,j)=(iiod)2 +(jjod)2 and ConeAng= |tan1(iiodjjod)|2sin1(Sj0jod)

Where Att is a general attenuation of the activation centered on the position of the electrode (N is the size of the matrix), Sig is a sigmoid function centered on the position of the electrode (iod, jod are position of the optic disk, dOD is the distance to the optic disk), Cone is a cone-like activation modeled as an angular (calculated as a normalized angle in ConeAng) Gaussian activation. The detail of all these computation steps is shown in Figure 4—figure supplement 1A.

We combined these two activations (Figure 4C insets) with different ratios (α) via a weighted sum (IsoA+α*EP) and extracted from this predicted profiles similar parameters (size, position, elongation) as we did from our cortical recordings. In our computation, N is 400, σ is 1, (iod, jod) is (200,100), (i0,j0) is (200, 150), S varies between 3 and 30, and α between 0.1 and 1.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1B and C shows the main steps of the computation (left column is IsoA, middle column is EP and right column is the weighted sum), for two ratios (0.5 and 1, B and C) and three electrodes size (5, 7 and 20, rows).

Statistics

To get rid of hypotheses concerning normal distribution and equal variance of data sets, as well as to be more confident in evaluating the significance of the results, we only used two-sided non-parametric Wilcoxon rank sum tests for two-sample or paired data (*p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001), except where otherwise stated (one-sided). The p values and sample sizes of the tests are given in the figure legends. N stands for the number of sample and N for the number of rats.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the Agence Nationale pour la Recherche (ANR08-TECS-018), the Fondation Berthe Fouassier (Fondation de France, 201120574) and the Fondation pour la Recherche Médicale (DPR20121125167). The authors would like to thank F Sauter and V Agache for providing the MEAs; R Guillemaud, JF Bêche and S Gharbi for their support in the implementation of the electrical stimulation; J-M Esprabens (TOPCON, France) for providing the OCT apparatus; J Degardin (Institut de la Vision, Paris); G Sadok (UNIC, Paris); I Balansard for expert veterinary advice and help throughout the project; A Duhoux for excellent technical help; AI Meso, B Kolomiets for help in revising the manuscript. We thank B Yvert, L Nowak and A Destexhe for insightful discussions on the electrical stimulation protocols that control the fiber-en-passant activation.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Agence Nationale de la Recherche ANR08-TECS-018 to Sébastien Roux, Serge Picaud, Pascale Pham, Frédéric Chavane.

  • Fondation pour la Recherche Médicale DPR20121125167 to Sébastien Roux, Frédéric Chavane.

  • Fondation de France Fondation Berthe Fouassier, 201120574 to Sébastien Roux.

Additional information

Competing interests

SP: Founder and consultant for Pixium Vision.

The other authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Author contributions

SR, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

FM, Acquisition of data, Drafting or revising the article.

FD, Designing Impedance Spectrocopy protocols, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data.

LH, Revising early stage of the manuscript, Acquisition of data.

ST, Analysis and interpretation of data, Revising early stage of the manuscript.

SP, Participation in early stages of the conception of the experiment, Revising early stage of the manuscript.

PP, Designing Impedance Spectrocopy protocols, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

FC, Conception and design, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: The experimental protocol was approved beforehand by the local Ethical Committee for Animal Research and all procedures complied with the French and European regulations on Animal Research (approval number A12/01/13) as well as the guidelines from the Society for Neuroscience.

References

  1. Ahuja AK, Behrend MR. The Argus™ II retinal prosthesis: factors affecting patient selection for implantation. Progress in Retinal and Eye Research. 2013;36:1–23. doi: 10.1016/j.preteyeres.2013.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ambati J, Fowler BJ. Mechanisms of age-related macular degeneration. Neuron. 2012;75:26–39. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bocquet B, Lacroux A, Surget MO, Baudoin C, Marquette V, Manes G, Hebrard M, Sénéchal A, Delettre C, Roux AF, Claustres M, Dhaenens CM, Rozet JM, Perrault I, Bonnefont JP, Kaplan J, Dollfus H, Amati-Bonneau P, Bonneau D, Reynier P, Audo I, Zeitz C, Sahel JA, Paquis-Flucklinger V, Calvas P, Arveiler B, Kohl S, Wissinger B, Blanchet C, Meunier I, Hamel CP. Relative frequencies of inherited retinal dystrophies and optic neuropathies in Southern France: assessment of 21-year data management. Ophthalmic Epidemiology. 2013;20:13–25. doi: 10.3109/09286586.2012.737890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Charvet G, Rousseau L, Billoint O, Gharbi S, Rostaing JP, Joucla S, Trevisiol M, Bourgerette A, Chauvet P, Moulin C, Goy F, Mercier B, Colin M, Spirkovitch S, Fanet H, Meyrand P, Guillemaud R, Yvert B. BioMEA: a versatile high-density 3D microelectrode array system using integrated electronics. Biosensors and Bioelectronics. 2010;25:1889–1896. doi: 10.1016/j.bios.2010.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Chemla S, Chavane F. Effects of GABAA kinetics on cortical population activity: computational studies and physiological confirmations. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2016;115:2867–2879. doi: 10.1152/jn.00352.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Chen Y, Geisler WS, Seidemann E. Optimal decoding of correlated neural population responses in the primate visual cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2006;9:1412–1420. doi: 10.1038/nn1792. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Chen Y, Palmer CR, Seidemann E. The relationship between voltage-sensitive dye imaging signals and spiking activity of neural populations in primate V1. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2012;107:3281–3295. doi: 10.1152/jn.00977.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chowdhury V, Morley JW, Coroneo MT. Development of an extraocular retinal prosthesis: evaluation of stimulation parameters in the cat. Journal of Clinical Neuroscience. 2008;15:900–906. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2007.08.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cicione R, Shivdasani MN, Fallon JB, Luu CD, Allen PJ, Rathbone GD, Shepherd RK, Williams CE. Visual cortex responses to suprachoroidal electrical stimulation of the retina: effects of electrode return configuration. Journal of Neural Engineering. 2012;9:036009. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/9/3/036009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Das A, Gilbert CD. Long-range horizontal connections and their role in cortical reorganization revealed by optical recording of cat primary visual cortex. Nature. 1995;375:780–784. doi: 10.1038/375780a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Dupont F, Beche JF, Condemine C. Devices for electrical stimulation of a biological tissue and method for calibrating same. US20130317563 A1 United States Patent. 2013
  12. Eckhorn R, Wilms M, Schanze T, Eger M, Hesse L, Eysel UT, Kisvárday ZF, Zrenner E, Gekeler F, Schwahn H, Shinoda K, Sachs H, Walter P. Visual resolution with retinal implants estimated from recordings in cat visual cortex. Vision Research. 2006;46:2675–2690. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.01.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Eger M, Wilms M, Eckhorn R, Schanze T, Hesse L. Retino-cortical information transmission achievable with a retina implant. Biosystems. 2005;79:133–142. doi: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2004.09.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Elfar SD, Cottaris NP, Iezzi R, Abrams GW. A cortical (V1) neurophysiological recording model for assessing the efficacy of retinal visual prostheses. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2009;180:195–207. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2009.02.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Euler T, Wässle H. Immunocytochemical identification of cone bipolar cells in the rat retina. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1995;361:461–478. doi: 10.1002/cne.903610310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fallon JB, Irvine DR, Shepherd RK. Cochlear implant use following neonatal deafness influences the cochleotopic organization of the primary auditory cortex in cats. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2009;512:101–114. doi: 10.1002/cne.21886. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Ferrari MP, Mantelli F, Sacchetti M, Antonangeli MI, Cattani F, D'Anniballe G, Sinigaglia F, Ruffini PA, Lambiase A. Safety and pharmacokinetics of escalating doses of human recombinant nerve growth factor eye drops in a double-masked, randomized clinical trial. BioDrugs. 2014;28:275–283. doi: 10.1007/s40259-013-0079-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Finger RP, Fimmers R, Holz FG, Scholl HPN. Incidence of blindness and severe visual impairment in germany: projections for 2030. Investigative Opthalmology & Visual Science. 2011;52:4381–4389. doi: 10.1167/iovs.10-6987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Fransen JW, Pangeni G, Pardue MT, McCall MA. Local signaling from a retinal prosthetic in a rodent retinitis pigmentosa model in vivo. Journal of Neural Engineering. 2014;11:e12687. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/11/4/046012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fried SI, Hsueh HA, Werblin FS. A method for generating precise temporal patterns of retinal spiking using prosthetic stimulation. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2006;95:970–978. doi: 10.1152/jn.00849.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fujikado T, Morimoto T, Kanda H, Kusaka S, Nakauchi K, Ozawa M, Matsushita K, Sakaguchi H, Ikuno Y, Kamei M, Tano Y. Evaluation of phosphenes elicited by extraocular stimulation in normals and by suprachoroidal-transretinal stimulation in patients with retinitis pigmentosa. Graefe's Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology. 2007;245:1411–1419. doi: 10.1007/s00417-007-0563-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Geddes LA. Historical evolution of circuit models for the electrode-electrolyte interface. Annals of Biomedical Engineering. 1997;25:1–14. doi: 10.1007/BF02738534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gias C, Hewson-Stoate N, Jones M, Johnston D, Mayhew JE, Coffey PJ. Retinotopy within rat primary visual cortex using optical imaging. NeuroImage. 2005;24:200–206. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Grinvald A, Shoham D, Shmuel A, Glaser D, Vanzetta I, Shtoyerman E, Slovin H, Wijnbergen C, Hildesheim R, Arieli A. Modern Techniques in Neuroscience Research. 1999. In-vivo optical imaging of cortical architecture and dynamics; pp. 893–969. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Haralock RM, Shapiro LG. Computer and Robot Vision. Boston, MA: Addison-Wesley Longman Publishing; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  26. Histed MH, Bonin V, Reid RC. Direct activation of sparse, distributed populations of cortical neurons by electrical microstimulation. Neuron. 2009;63:508–522. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hughes A. A schematic eye for the rat. Vision Research. 1979;19:569–588. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(79)90143-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Humayun MS, Dorn JD, da Cruz L, Dagnelie G, Sahel JA, Stanga PE, Cideciyan AV, Duncan JL, Eliott D, Filley E, Ho AC, Santos A, Safran AB, Arditi A, Del Priore LV, Greenberg RJ, Argus II Study Group Interim results from the international trial of Second Sight's visual prosthesis. Ophthalmology. 2012;119:779–788. doi: 10.1016/j.ophtha.2011.09.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Jensen RJ, Ziv OR, Rizzo JF. Responses of rabbit retinal ganglion cells to electrical stimulation with an epiretinal electrode. Journal of Neural Engineering. 2005;2:S16–S21. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/2/1/003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Jepson LH, Hottowy P, Mathieson K, Gunning DE, Dąbrowski W, Litke AM, Chichilnisky EJ. Spatially patterned electrical stimulation to enhance resolution of retinal prostheses. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014a;34:4871–4881. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2882-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Jepson LH, Hottowy P, Mathieson K, Gunning DE, Dabrowski W, Litke AM, Chichilnisky EJ. Focal electrical stimulation of major ganglion cell types in the primate retina for the design of visual prostheses. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:7194–7205. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4967-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Jepson LH, Hottowy P, Weiner GA, Dabrowski W, Litke AM, Chichilnisky EJ. High-fidelity reproduction of spatiotemporal visual signals for retinal prosthesis. Neuron. 2014b;83:87–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.04.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Klauke S, Goertz M, Rein S, Hoehl D, Thomas U, Eckhorn R, Bremmer F, Wachtler T. Stimulation with a wireless intraocular epiretinal implant elicits visual percepts in blind humans. Investigative Opthalmology & Visual Science. 2011;52:449–455. doi: 10.1167/iovs.09-4410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kral A, Hartmann R, Mortazavi D, Klinke R. Spatial resolution of cochlear implants: the electrical field and excitation of auditory afferents. Hearing Research. 1998;121:11–28. doi: 10.1016/S0378-5955(98)00061-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Mandel Y, Goetz G, Lavinsky D, Huie P, Mathieson K, Wang L, Kamins T, Galambos L, Manivanh R, Harris J, Palanker D. Cortical responses elicited by photovoltaic subretinal prostheses exhibit similarities to visually evoked potentials. Nature Communications. 2013;4:1980. doi: 10.1038/ncomms2980. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Marre O, Amodei D, Deshmukh N, Sadeghi K, Soo F, Holy TE, Berry MJ. Mapping a complete neural population in the retina. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:14859–14873. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0723-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Marre O, Botella-Soler V, Simmons KD, Mora T, Tkačik G, Berry MJ II. High accuracy decoding of dynamical motion from a large retinal population. PLoS Computational Biology. 2015;11:e12687. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Matteucci PB, Chen SC, Tsai D, Dodds CWD, Dokos S, Morley JW, Lovell NH, Suaning GJ. Current steering in retinal stimulation via a quasimonopolar stimulation paradigm. Investigative Opthalmology & Visual Science. 2013;54:4307–4320. doi: 10.1167/iovs.13-11653. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. McAdams ET, Jossinet J. Problems in equivalent circuit modelling of the electrical properties of biological tissues. Bioelectrochemistry and Bioenergetics. 1996;40:147–152. doi: 10.1016/0302-4598(96)05069-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. McAdams ET, Jossinet J. Nonlinear transient response of electrode-electrolyte interfaces. Medical & Biological Engineering & Computing. 2000;38:427–432. doi: 10.1007/BF02345012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. McIntyre CC, Grill WM. Selective microstimulation of central nervous system neurons. Annals of Biomedical Engineering. 2000;28:219–233. doi: 10.1114/1.262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. McIntyre CC, Grill WM. Finite element analysis of the current-density and electric field generated by metal microelectrodes. Annals of Biomedical Engineering. 2001;29:227–235. doi: 10.1114/1.1352640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. McIntyre CC, Grill WM. Extracellular stimulation of central neurons: influence of stimulus waveform and frequency on neuronal output. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2002;88:1592–1604. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.88.4.1592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Michel MM, Chen Y, Geisler WS, Seidemann E. An illusion predicted by V1 population activity implicates cortical topography in shape perception. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:1477–1483. doi: 10.1038/nn.3517. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Miller CA, Robinson BK, Rubinstein JT, Abbas PJ, Runge-Samuelson CL. Auditory nerve responses to monophasic and biphasic electric stimuli. Hearing Research. 2001;151:79–94. doi: 10.1016/S0300-2977(00)00082-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Nadig MN. Development of a silicon retinal implant: cortical evoked potentials following focal stimulation of the rabbit retina with light and electricity. Clinical Neurophysiology. 1999;110:1545–1553. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(99)00027-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Naka KI, Rushton WA. S-potentials from luminosity units in the retina of fish (Cyprinidae) The Journal of Physiology. 1966;185:587–599. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1966.sp008003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Nanduri D, Fine I, Horsager A, Boynton GM, Humayun MS, Greenberg RJ, Weiland JD. Frequency and amplitude modulation have different effects on the percepts elicited by retinal stimulation. Investigative Opthalmology & Visual Science. 2012;53:205–214. doi: 10.1167/iovs.11-8401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Ni AM, Maunsell JH. Microstimulation reveals limits in detecting different signals from a local cortical region. Current Biology. 2010;20:824–828. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.02.065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Nirenberg S, Pandarinath C. Retinal prosthetic strategy with the capacity to restore normal vision. PNAS. 2012;109:15012–15017. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1207035109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Nowak LG, Bullier J. Axons, but not cell bodies, are activated by electrical stimulation in cortical gray matter. I. Evidence from chronaxie measurements. Experimental Brain Research. 1998a;118:477–488. doi: 10.1007/s002210050304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Nowak LG, Bullier J. Axons, but not cell bodies, are activated by electrical stimulation in cortical gray matter. II. Evidence from selective inactivation of cell bodies and axon initial segments. Experimental Brain Research. 1998b;118:489–500. doi: 10.1007/s002210050305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Palagina G, Eysel UT, Jancke D. Strengthening of lateral activation in adult rat visual cortex after retinal lesions captured with voltage-sensitive dye imaging in vivo. PNAS. 2009;106:8743–8747. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900068106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Palmer C, Cheng SY, Seidemann E. Linking neuronal and behavioral performance in a reaction-time visual detection task. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:8122–8137. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1940-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Pham P, Roux S, Matonti F, Dupont F, Agache V, Chavane F. Post-implantation impedance spectroscopy of subretinal micro-electrode arrays, OCT imaging and numerical simulation: towards a more precise neuroprosthesis monitoring tool. Journal of Neural Engineering. 2013;10:e12687. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/10/4/046002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Ragheb T, Geddes LA. Electrical properties of metallic electrodes. Medical & Biological Engineering & Computing. 1990;28:182–186. doi: 10.1007/BF02441775. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Ranck JB. Which elements are excited in electrical stimulation of mammalian central nervous system: a review. Brain Research. 1975;98:417–440. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(75)90364-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Resnikoff S, Pascolini D, Etya'ale D, Kocur I, Pararajasegaram R, Pokharel GP, Mariotti SP. Global data on visual impairment in the year 2002. Bulletin of the World Health Organization. 2004;82:844–851. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Reynaud A, Takerkart S, Masson GS, Chavane F. Linear model decomposition for voltage-sensitive dye imaging signals: application in awake behaving monkey. NeuroImage. 2011;54:1196–1210. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Rizzo JF, Wyatt J, Loewenstein J, Kelly S, Shire D. Perceptual Efficacy of Electrical Stimulation of Human Retina with a Microelectrode Array during Short-Term Surgical Trials. Investigative Opthalmology & Visual Science. 2003;44:5362–5369. doi: 10.1167/iovs.02-0817. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Schanze T, Greve N, Hesse L. Towards the cortical representation of form and motion stimuli generated by a retina implant. Graefe's Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology. 2003;241:685–693. doi: 10.1007/s00417-003-0746-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Sekirnjak C, Hottowy P, Sher A, Dabrowski W, Litke AM, Chichilnisky EJ. High-resolution electrical stimulation of primate retina for epiretinal implant design. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:4446–4456. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5138-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Shepherd RK, Shivdasani MN, Nayagam DA, Williams CE, Blamey PJ. Visual prostheses for the blind. Trends in Biotechnology. 2013;31:562–571. doi: 10.1016/j.tibtech.2013.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Shmuel A, Grinvald A. Functional organization for direction of motion and its relationship to orientation maps in cat area 18. Journal of Neuroscience. 1996;16:6945–6964. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-21-06945.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Stern S, Agudelo-Toro A, Rotem A, Moses E, Neef A. Chronaxie measurements in patterned neuronal cultures from rat hippocampus. PLoS One. 2015;10:e12687. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0132577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Stett A, Barth W, Weiss S, Haemmerle H, Zrenner E. Electrical multisite stimulation of the isolated chicken retina. Vision Research. 2000;40:1785–1795. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(00)00005-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Stett A, Mai A, Herrmann T. Retinal charge sensitivity and spatial discrimination obtainable by subretinal implants: key lessons learned from isolated chicken retina. Journal of Neural Engineering. 2007;4:S7–16. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/4/1/S02. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Stingl K, Bartz-Schmidt KU, Besch D, Braun A, Bruckmann A, Gekeler F, Greppmaier U, Hipp S, Hörtdörfer G, Kernstock C, Koitschev A, Kusnyerik A, Sachs H, Schatz A, Stingl KT, Peters T, Wilhelm B, Zrenner E. Artificial vision with wirelessly powered subretinal electronic implant alpha-IMS. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2013;280:e12687. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.0077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Talcott KE, Ratnam K, Sundquist SM, Lucero AS, Lujan BJ, Tao W, Porco TC, Roorda A, Duncan JL. Longitudinal study of cone photoreceptors during retinal degeneration and in response to ciliary neurotrophic factor treatment. Investigative Opthalmology & Visual Science. 2011;52:2219–2226. doi: 10.1167/iovs.10-6479. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Tehovnik EJ, Slocum WM. Phosphene induction by microstimulation of macaque V1. Brain Research Reviews. 2007;53:337–343. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Tehovnik EJ, Slocum WM. Depth-dependent detection of microampere currents delivered to monkey V1. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:1477–1489. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06695.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Toth LJ, Rao SC, Kim DS, Somers D, Sur M. Subthreshold facilitation and suppression in primary visual cortex revealed by intrinsic signal imaging. PNAS. 1996;93:9869–9874. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.18.9869. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Tsujikawa M, Wada Y, Sukegawa M, Sawa M, Gomi F, Nishida K, Tano Y. Age at onset curves of retinitis pigmentosa. Archives of Ophthalmology. 2008;126:337–340. doi: 10.1001/archopht.126.3.337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Vaiceliunaite A, Erisken S, Franzen F, Katzner S, Busse L. Spatial integration in mouse primary visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2013;110:964–972. doi: 10.1152/jn.00138.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Vizuete JA, Pillay S, Diba K, Ropella KM, Hudetz AG. Monosynaptic functional connectivity in cerebral cortex during wakefulness and under graded levels of anesthesia. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience. 2012;6:e12687. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Walter P, Kisvárday Zoltán F., Alteheld N, Rossler G, Stieglitz T, Eysel UT, Görtz M, Kisvarday ZF. Cortical activation via an implanted wireless retinal prosthesis. Investigative Opthalmology & Visual Science. 2005;46:1780–1785. doi: 10.1167/iovs.04-0924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Wilke R, Gabel V-P, Sachs H, Bartz Schmidt K-U, Gekeler F, Besch D, Szurman P, Stett A, Wilhelm B, Peters T, Harscher A, Greppmaier U, Kibbel S, Benav H, Bruckmann A, Stingl K, Kusnyerik A, Zrenner E. Spatial resolution and perception of patterns mediated by a subretinal 16-electrode array in patients blinded by hereditary retinal dystrophies. Investigative Opthalmology & Visual Science. 2011;52:5995–6003. doi: 10.1167/iovs.10-6946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Wilms M, Eger M, Schanze T, Eckhorn R. Visual resolution with epi-retinal electrical stimulation estimated from activation profiles in cat visual cortex. Visual Neuroscience. 2003;20:543–555. doi: 10.1017/S0952523803205083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Wong YT, Chen SC, Seo JM, Morley JW, Lovell NH, Suaning GJ. Focal activation of the feline retina via a suprachoroidal electrode array. Vision Research. 2009;49:825–833. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.02.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Zrenner E, Bartz-Schmidt KU, Benav H, Besch D, Bruckmann A, Gabel V-P, Gekeler F, Greppmaier U, Harscher A, Kibbel S, Koch J, Kusnyerik A, Peters T, Stingl K, Sachs H, Stett A, Szurman P, Wilhelm B, Wilke R. Subretinal electronic chips allow blind patients to read letters and combine them to words. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 2011;278:1489–1497. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.1747. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Zrenner E. Fighting blindness with microelectronics. Science Translational Medicine. 2013;5:e12687. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3007399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
eLife. 2016 Aug 23;5:e12687. doi: 10.7554/eLife.12687.019

Decision letter

Editor: Doris Y Tsao1

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your work entitled "Probing the functional impact of sub-retinal prosthesis" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been favorably evaluated by David Van Essen (Senior editor) and three reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

Retinal prostheses are a potentially useful therapy for blindness, but they currently provide only fairly low acuity. Roux et al. have developed an in vivo rodent preparation in order to compare visual and prosthetic activation of downstream visual areas, specifically visual cortex. They find that the cortical projective field, i.e., the cortical area activated by stimulation of single or multiple electrodes, is larger than the projective field of visual stimuli covering a similar extent of the retina. The authors then attempted to identify potential causes of and solutions to this problem, focusing primarily on spread of the electrical pulse through tissue and en passant activation of RGC axons coursing to the optic nerve. They find that by measuring the impedance of the tissue-electrode interface and correcting for this effect, they can somewhat reduce the lateral spread of excitation in cortex. This use of adaptation, particularly in tandem with other published techniques, may aid in enhancing the acuity of retinal prostheses.

Overall, the paper represents an important potential contribution to the field of retinal prostheses, but the data need to be analyzed more carefully, and the sections on cone angle and on approach to reduce lateral spread need major clarification.

Essential revisions:

1) The presentation of the cone angle model was virtually impenetrable. If the distortion is due to the radial organization of ganglion cell fibers, then the asymmetry should always be elongated radially. Was this true?

2) One expectation of en passant activation is not only an elongated RF but also an anisotropic one. They show that AR scales with eccentricity, but not whether the increased AR is asymmetric to the more eccentric side of the RF center.

3) In Figure 4 the authors show that decreasing the cone angle decreases the aspect ratio (AR). Therefore, reducing the lateral spread of activation via impedance spectroscopy adaptation should reduce the cone angle at a given point, in turn increasing the aspect ratio (AR), yet they find both parameters are reduced. It is not clear how shaping the waveforms as the authors have done would reduce en passant activation of RGC axons. Is their electrical stimulation intended to activate RGCs or photoreceptors?

4) These measurements were performed in anesthetized animals, which are reported to have less lateral inhibition in cortex. Likewise, it is unclear how the activated cortical areas correlate with acuity in awake animals.

5) The authors found multiple effects that could reduce the lateral spread of cortical activation. Do they expect or have they tested whether these effects are additive? Likewise, they cite other studies showing that interactions between adjacent electrodes can be used to narrow areas of excitation, analogous to lateral inhibition. It would be worth discussing whether these methods are compatible and how it might aid in further improving acuity with these prostheses.

6) The imaging data examples are patchy and not of the best quality, and in some cases the fitted contours do not obviously relate to the observed pattern. Moreover, the overlaid contours in many instances make it difficult to resolve the activated areas. Figure 7B illustrates both concerns, and as it pertains to the central advance the authors claim, it is not especially convincing. Nor is it clear which conditions are being shown in these images, as contours corresponding to four different conditions are overlaid on just two images. It would also be helpful to see not only Z scores but also signal magnitudes (deltaR/R) when assessing these data.

[Editors' note: further revisions were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for submitting your article "Probing the functional impact of sub-retinal prosthesis" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by two peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and David Van Essen as the Senior Editor. The following individuals involved in review of your submission have agreed to reveal their identity: Christian Casanova (Reviewer #3).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

The reviewers think that overall the authors have addressed most of our concerns through clarifications and additional analysis, and are mostly satisfied. However, there are a few small remaining concerns:

1) Cortical magnification factor also creates anisotropy in visual receptive fields, and a worthwhile comparison would be to those of the visual projective fields in these areas, which would be the ultimate desired target of any correction.

2) It is still unclear how the aspect ratio would be lower than 1 – if there were no en passant activation, a perfectly circular patch of retina would be excited. Is this a result of cortical magnification factor? The treatment of cone angle, e.g. in Figure 4C, remains unclear.

3) We appreciate the utility of Z-scores, but for the sake of others in the field who more commonly use deltaR/R and to facilitate others' comparison with their own studies, it would be useful to provide these in addition, to offer a sense of the response magnitudes observed.

eLife. 2016 Aug 23;5:e12687. doi: 10.7554/eLife.12687.020

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) The presentation of the cone angle model was virtually impenetrable.

Indeed, we did not describe the model in previous version of the manuscript; we now add a thorough description of the model both in the Results section (subsection “Shape”) and the Methods section (subsection “Model of retinal activation”). We also improved the supplementary figure (Figure 4—figure supplement 1) that describes the model, in link with the Methods section.

If the distortion is due to the radial organization of ganglion cell fibers, then the asymmetry should always be elongated radially. Was this true?

We thank the reviewers for this excellent remark. We went back on the data and analyzed the radial organization of the activations. For that purpose we kept only the cortical activations that displayed a high aspect ratio (to select those for which the elongation due to activation of axons en passant was clear). This analysis actually verified the prediction of the reviewers: we do have a significant radial organization of the activations towards the representation of the blind spot (see Figure 4E). See our explanations in the manuscript (subsection “Shape”, second paragraph).

2) One expectation of en passant activation is not only an elongated RF but also an anisotropic one. They show that AR scales with eccentricity, but not whether the increased AR is asymmetric to the more eccentric side of the RF center.

This is a second very interesting prediction we did not tested. We are really grateful to the reviewers for these two excellent remarks that allowed us to dissect in more detail the activation profile and greatly improved the manuscript. To test for that second prediction, we compared the position of the center-of-mass to the geometrical center; the latter is only influenced by the geometrical elongation of the activation at threshold level, the first is influenced by the spatial profile of the activity. The prediction is that the center-of-mass will be attracted towards more eccentric regions of the activation (this is confirmed by our model, see Author response image 1).

Author response image 1. Predictions of the distance between the center-of-mass (COM) and the geometrical center of electrical activations(normalized to implant size) as a function of the contribution of axons en passant and the distance to the optic disk (from the same model presented in Figure 4C).

Author response image 1.

Values >0 correspond to distances away from the BS, when compared to the geometrical center (<0 is towards the BS).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.12687.015

This is indeed what we observed systematically (see new Figure 4E and F): all centers of masses are displaced towards more eccentric retinotopic locations. This is further confirmed when averaging (after rotation and re-centering) the shape of the cortical activations for all conditions with high-aspect ratio for which we observe that the spatial profile is asymmetric, with strong activation away from the blind spot (to the left), and with a cone shape (see inset in Figure 4F). In comparison, asymmetry and cone shape are not observed for all the other activation with low aspect- ratio. Again we thank the reviewer for this very insightful comment (subsection “Shape”, second paragraph).

3) In Figure 4 the authors show that decreasing the cone angle decreases the aspect ratio (AR). Therefore, reducing the lateral spread of activation via impedance spectroscopy adaptation should reduce the cone angle at a given point, in turn increasing the aspect ratio (AR), yet they find both parameters are reduced. It is not clear how shaping the waveforms as the authors have done would reduce en passant activation of RGC axons. Is their electrical stimulation intended to activate RGCs or photoreceptors?

The reviewers point to an important point that was unclear in previous version of the manuscript. As we stated in the Discussion of previous version, we believe that Impedance Spectroscopy adaptation is acting “both [on] the electrical diffusion extent (although not completely) and the axons en passant activation”. Hence, this can explain how it decreases both the spread of activation (e.g. cone angle) and the AR by acting on these two mechanisms. We now further describe in the text what could be the reason for a decrease in the spread of activation. The evidence is coming from spectral analysis of the pulses: Adapted pulses have energy exclusively concentrated below 800Hz, whereas non-adapted pulses have energy ringing up to 4KHz (Figure 7—figure supplement 1C). We have shown in Pham et al. (Pham et al., 2013), for good implantations) that high frequencies increase both the spread of the electrical diffusion and the current density magnitude (Figure 9A-B-C in Pham et al. 2013).

Next, we think that there are (at least) two mechanisms to account for the decrease of activation of RGC axons: First, since we are positioned subretinally, reducing the diffusion, for a given eccentricity and electrode size, will favor activation of elements closer to the electrode (photoreceptors, bipolar cells…) compared to further away (ganglion cells).

A second mechanism is that the stimulus shape itself can participate to lower the ratio of axons-en-passant activation. It is now well documented that axons have shorter chronaxie, generally some hundreds of microsecond, than cell bodies, generally several milliseconds (Histed et al., 2009; McIntyre and Grill, 2002; Nowak and Bullier, 1998a; 1998b; Ranck, 1975; Stern et al., 2015; Tehovnik and Slocum, 2009). If we look at the shape of the current injected in the tissue using regular non-adapted pulses (Figure 7A left row), we see that the duration of each cathodic or anodic pulse lasts about 0.23ms (Figure 7—figure supplement 1C, width at half-height), hence favoring activation of axons. Our result (e.g. a ratio of en passantrecruitment of about the same contribution as direct activation) is thus not surprising. In contrast, in the adaptation condition, the duration of each (cathodic or anodic) pulse inside the tissue was much longer (Figure 7—figure supplement 1C, 1.2ms), biphasic, cathodic phase first and asymmetrical in amplitude. According to the work of Mc Intyre (McIntyre and Grill, 2002), these are the conditions for which an increase in the threshold for activation of axons and a decrease for the soma are expected (see citation below*). Furthermore, since the amplitude of the current injected in the tissue was low compare to the reference condition, it is thus expected that the adapted condition will comparatively lower the contribution of fiber-en-passant.

We now include this argumentation in the Discussion of the paper (subsection “How to improve prosthetic activation?”, third paragraph) that helped clarifying this important issue. Please note that we coordinated this answer with renowned experts in the field (Lionel Nowak, Alain Destexhe and Blaise Yvert).

(*) “When an asymmetrical charge-balanced biphasic cathodic phase firststimulus waveform was used, there was a decrease in the threshold for activation of the local cell and an increase in the threshold to activate either passing fiber.”

4) These measurements were performed in anesthetized animals, which are reported to have less lateral inhibition in cortex.

Since there are not so many publications on this important topic, we believe that the reviewers point to the paper of Vaiceliunaite et al. (Vaiceliunaite et al., 2013) on mouse with isoflurane/urethane anesthesia. These authors describe in V1 neurons less surround inhibition, in other words, neurons have a maximal response for smaller stimuli when awaked than when anesthetized. This could imply that cortical activation will be larger in anesthetized condition than in the awake. However, in their paper, we can see that the overall response amplitude for large stimulus in the awake condition is still significant (see their Figure 3G & J, Vaiceliunaite et al., 2013). So, if we try to deduce from these single neuron activities the extent of cortical neuronal population significantly activated, it is not clear that the size will change, however, probably, the shape will (more peaky when awaked). This is reinforced by the evidence, in the rat, that both excitation and inhibition are affected by anesthesia (Vizuete et al., 2012) suggesting that surround suppression decrease could be accompanied by a drop in excitatory level as well. In a recent model, we investigated how the population network response is affected by anesthesia (Chemla and Chavane, 2016), and we confronted our prediction to experimental measure. Our conclusion was that the whole excitatory/inhibitory balance was indeed disrupted. When taken globally, this suggests that it is straightforward to make a simple prediction of how anesthesia will change the activation extent. A direct measure is warranted. We actually did a series of experiments on the monkey using optical imaging (Chemla and Chavane, 2016), comparing the response dynamic in awake vs. anesthetized conditions (where we confirm the general decrease in response amplitude documented in the mouse and rat in the two previous articles). From one of these experiments, not used in the article, we presented a local stimulus (1° in diameter, optimized in spatial and temporal frequency) in the same monkey while he was awake, and when he was anesthetized using midazolam (potentiates GABAA, see Author response image 2). In this experiment, we show that the population response is actually smaller in the anesthetized condition (reduction of 14% of the equivalent diameter of the ellipse formed by the contour taken at 75% of the response maximum, hence lowers absolute threshold for the anesthetized condition). As a conclusion, it is not trivial to predict how the cortical spatial profile of activation will be affected by anesthesia. However, this was an important point to clarify.

Author response image 2. V1 cortical activation measured with voltage sensitive dye imaging in non-human primate, elicited by local drifting gratings in awake (left) and anesthetized (right, midazolam) condition.

Author response image 2.

Red and white contours represent 75 and 50% of maximal activation; scale bar: 1 mm. Drowsy state of the animal (right) was induced by intranasal administration of 0.2 ml midazolam (7-chloro-imidazo, 0.1 mg/kg). Midazolam is a short-acting hypnotic-sedative drug with anxiolytic and amnestic properties. From the benzodiazepines class of tranquilizer drugs, the pharmacologic effects of midazolam are mediated through a majority of GABAA receptors (see Sigel 2002 for a review).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.12687.016

Importantly, the main rationale of our study was to make a systematic comparison between conditions done under similar level of anesthesia (visual vs.electrical). It would be very surprising to observe (and we actually cannot see any mechanism that would subtend it) an inversion of the effects when shifting from anesthesia to awake state (i.e. reduction of response size from condition A to condition B would become increased by changing the animal state). We added a paragraph in the Discussion to comment this point (subsection “Distorted activation in size and shape:”, second paragraph).

Likewise, it is unclear how the activated cortical areas correlate with acuity in awake animals.

In the previous version of the manuscript, we already discussed the relation between population activity and evoked percepts: “The working hypothesis of this study is that population activation of the first cortical integration stage is related and proportional to underlying evoked percepts, as suggested by many studies (Chen et al., 2006; Ni, 2010; Palmer et al., 2007; Tehovnik and Slocum, 2006). […] The next step will be to launch experiments in behaving non-human primates, to clarify in a model closer to humans, the relationship between the properties of the population prosthetic activation and the report of the animal’s percepts.”

However, the reviewer points to a supplementary and important issue: is the size of the activation directly linked to the size of the percept? This is what is expected in first simple assumption: a large activation in the optical imaging map will generate a large retinotopic spiking region that can be decoded as high probability of a large stimulus being present in the outside world. In a recent study, the group of E. Seidemann actually showed, using optical imaging, that the shape of the cortical activation in V1 bias accordingly the shape of the evoked percept (Michel et al., 2013), suggesting that there is a link between the spatial profile of the activation and the percept. Further evidence comes from the publications from Tehovnik and Shiller group using cortical stimulation. Schiller et al. (Schiller et al., 2011) indeed show that by increasing stimulation intensity of the cortex, there is a tendency for monkeys to perceive the stimulus size larger. This should be taken in consideration with the result of a previous publication, where Tolias et al. (Tolias et al., 2005) have shown that increasing the stimulus intensity increases the size of the cortical activation. These are two evidences that the spatial profile of cortical activation in V1 is most probably linked to the size of the percept. We now add this in the Discussion (second paragraph), arguing in favor of pursuing these studies in awake animals, using behavioral tasks, in order to further demonstrate this important link.

5) The authors found multiple effects that could reduce the lateral spread of cortical activation. Do they expect or have they tested whether these effects are additive?

We haven’t tested whether the effects are additive unfortunately. The two parameters that seem to affect the activation extent from our study are adaptation and asymmetry.

Note that the adaptation itself affected the asymmetry of the pulses in the tissue in the relative amplitude between the two phases but not in duration. However, duration of the asymmetry could also contribute in increasing the threshold of activation in axons (see (McIntyre and Grill, 2000), and our discussion on “focalization”). This is the reason why we tested it; unfortunately the effect was not systematic. Interestingly, diffusion in itself should not be much affected by asymmetry, because the main factor is the distance of the electrode to the retinal tissue (Pham et al., 2013).

Hence asymmetry in pulse duration could be a parameter that acts on the threshold of activation of fiber-en-passant through a complementary and sufficiently independent mechanism to generate an effect that will be additive to the one generated by the adaptation protocol. We now add comments on this in the last paragraph of the subsection “How to improve prosthetic activation?”

Likewise, they cite other studies showing that interactions between adjacent electrodes can be used to narrow areas of excitation, analogous to lateral inhibition. It would be worth discussing whether these methods are compatible and how it might aid in further improving acuity with these prostheses.

We agree with the reviewers. The spatial arrangement of stimulation electrodes (passive or active return electrode close to stimulating electrode) is a key factor that would be worth testing. The current will be more localized between the stimulating and the return electrode. This is another independent parameter that could be combined with adaptation of pulse.

So we would end up with three potential independent factors to be combined: adaptation, asymmetry, and spatial arrangement of the electrodes. To test this, however, would require a high combination of tests. We now add comments on this in (subsection “How to improve prosthetic activation?”, last paragraph).

Please note at this stage that we did not do justice to the difficulty of the adaptation experiment. For these latter, the experimental bench was composed of an assemblage of commercial devices (signal generator (Agilent 33250A) + impedance spectroscopy potentiostat (Bio-Logic SP200) and the estimation of each adapted pulse for each electrode and each voltage level was performed externally using Matlab (see Dupont, 2014). This generated important delays in the experiments that prevent testing a large set of parameters. Further experimentations to test the combination of adaptation, asymmetry and spatial arrangement, to be performed on same animals, will necessitate setting up an experimental bench where adaptation is performed in real-time. This implies a series of further experiments that we are trying to set but that are not trivial. All those propositions are underlining the complexity of the approach, because of the large number of parameters to explore and the amount of experiments to be performed for a proper statistical study. This was probably not fully clear in our original manuscript and we clarify it now in the first paragraph of the subsection “Focalization”).

However, we believe that our study is yet an important step for the field of retinal prosthesis because (i) it is the first that tried to explore systematically a sub-part of the parameter space, (ii) critically takes into account for the bias generated by the electrode- tissue interface properties by direct measure of the injected current and (iii) propose an original inverse engineering approach for the control of the desired stimulation.

6) The imaging data examples are patchy and not of the best quality, and in some cases the fitted contours do not obviously relate to the observed pattern. Moreover, the overlaid contours in many instances make it difficult to resolve the activated areas. Figure 7B illustrates both concerns, and as it pertains to the central advance the authors claim, it is not especially convincing. Nor is it clear which conditions are being shown in these images, as contours corresponding to four different conditions are overlaid on just two images. It would also be helpful to see not only Z scores but also signal magnitudes (deltaR/R) when assessing these data.

The reviewer is right. We probably oversimplified the figures by putting too many information in a single map. Second, the gray scale color-code we chose was probably not the best color-code to see variations of the activation in the map. Lastly, we forgot to mention that the contour was calculated on a smoothed version of the maps (now added in the Methods, subsection “Data analysis”, second paragraph). We now provide a single map for each condition and overlap contours to ease comparison, a secondary DI/I scale, and use another color- code that we believe provide more perceptible gradation of activity level. Regarding DI/I vs. z-score, we agree that DI/I is a scale of reference so important to keep for comparison. However, please note that the z-score measure was devised to ensure proper comparison of maps across conditions (see Jancke et al., 2004). The importance of this calculation is grounded because of experimental noise and variation across experiments, the baseline and the amplitude of the DI/I are highly variable compare to the z-score values (see Figure 2D and H and even more in Figure 3A and C). Comparison between these maps would be thus impossible. The z-score maps provided a common statistical metric that was crucial to ensure comparative analysis.

[Editors' note: further revisions were requested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

The reviewers think that overall the authors have addressed most of our concerns through clarifications and additional analysis, and are mostly satisfied. However, there are a few small remaining concerns:

1) Cortical magnification factor also creates anisotropy in visual receptive fields, and a worthwhile comparison would be to those of the visual projective fields in these areas, which would be the ultimate desired target of any correction.

The reviewers are right: magnification factor can induce anisotropy in population receptive fields. This is actually exactly the reason why, on Figure 4D, we show the aspect ratio (AR) of the electrically-induced cortical activations normalized by their visual-counterparts. This allowed correcting for anisotropy due to cortical magnification factor, but also variability of the activations’ shape (due to vasculature artifacts for instance).

To further answer the reviewers’ concerns we looked at two aspects of this transformation:

A) We checked whether there is any systematic anisotropy of the visual activations due to magnification factor that could be taken into account in our analysis. The prediction drawn by retino-cortical transformations is that, at eccentric locations, cortical activations should get elongated tangential to the radial axis (see the simulated “cortical space” in Author response image 3A). We analyzed whether such effect existed systematically in our data. The results (see Author response image 3B) show that the elongations of visual responses are too variable to be used as a predictor. Indeed only activations with high AR (long segment) do show some tangential organization as predicted. However, if we restrict our analysis to high AR, this would drastically decrease the data points. Therefore, we believe that the ratio of the electrical AR to visual AR is the simplest yet reliable measure to do.

Author response image 3.

Author response image 3.

(A) Simulated deformation of cortical activations (right) to circular retinal regions located at various eccentricities and angles (left), the center of the gray pattern corresponding to the optic disk location. For this simulation, we used our estimation of the retino-cortical magnification factor. (B) Orientations of visual activations on the cortical surface. Length of the segments is proportional to the aspect ratio of the activation. Gray disk represents the median blind spot representation, scale bar: 0.5mm.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.12687.017

To avoid confusing the readers, we propose not to mention this in the article. We now simply argue in the Results section: “Please note that, to account for any potential deformation of the evoked activity due to retino-cortical magnification factor (see Figure 4—figure supplement 1) or physiological noise, we normalized all electrically-induced AR to their corresponding visual AR”.

B) Second, we checked directly on our model whether the retino-cortical transformations could further affect our initial prediction. This is shown in the figure below, that we propose to add as a supplementary figure. The retino-cortical transform does not change the result qualitatively: In panel A we show the mapping transformation we used for our model. In panel B we give 4 examples of the transform for different activation sizes and ratios between direct activation and axons-en-passant. In panel C, we show the same calculation as we did in Figure 4C, but for the cortical prediction. Here, to achieve a similar analysis as in Figure 4D, we show the normalized aspect ratio (normalized to what was obtained when we removed the contribution of axon-en-passant) as a function of implant size. Qualitatively, the result is not changing when applying this transformation (if we don’t normalize the results are also not affected qualitatively).

The result is nice and reassuring but we prefer to keep it as a supplementary figure, since this part of the model is adding unnecessary complications and parameters for an effect which is already present, and originate at the retinal level. We now add in the “Please note that these predictions are left unchanged if we apply a retino-cortical transformation to the simulated activations (see Figure 4—figure supplement 1).”

2) It is still unclear how the aspect ratio would be lower than 1 – if there were no en passant activation, a perfectly circular patch of retina would be excited. Is this a result of cortical magnification factor? The treatment of cone angle, e.g. in Figure 4C, remains unclear.

Our apologies if it remained unclear. First, we realized we actually did not explain the convention we chose to compute the AR in the model. Since we wanted to extract elongation along the radial axis, we used as numerator the length of the activation along the radial axis and as denominator, the length of the activation along a tangential axis. We wrote in the revised manuscript: “Since our interest is to predict the effect of axon-en-passant activation on radial elongation, we used the following convention for the AR: the numerator is the length of activation along the radial axis and the denominator is the length of activation along a tangential axis (perpendicular to the radial axis).”

Second, in our model, the aspect ratio is getting lower than 1 when both the cone angle and axons en passant recruitment are large (Figure 4C). When the cone angle become large (for instance when the whole MEA is stimulated very close to the blind spot), the radial diffusion of activity tangential to the radial axis becomes more important than the radial activation. This leads to an elongation tangential to the one observed with small cone angle, hence an AR lower than 1. In our simulation, this inversion is observed around 40°, a value for which the tangent of the cone angle (i.e. tangential activation) becomes larger than the cosine (radial activation). This effect is strengthened by axon-en passant contribution that will accentuate the diffusion radially and tangentially. Thanks to the reviewer comment, we further analyzed our data to verify whether this prediction was indeed verified experimentally. We now include a third averaged cortical activation in Figure 4F for the case of wMEA activations (*). In this averaged cortical map, we indeed see that the activation in response to wMEA is, as predicted by the model, more elongated along an axis orthogonal to the one for SE.

We added the following to the revised manuscript: “The model further predicts that the retinal activation should become more elongated along a tangential axis for very large cone angle (AR<1).”

“Thus, the predictions that arise from this observation are: for small cone angle (i) the elongation of the cortical activation should be radially organized towards the representation of the optic disk for small cone angle and (ii) the activation should be anisotropic, attracted by the radial elongation towards more eccentric positions (i.e. away from the optic disk representation); lastly (iii) for large cone angle, cortical activation should be more elongated along a tangential axis.”

“For comparison, we made similar averaging on all the other maps in response to SE (with an aspect-ratio < 1.6) and wMEA. For SE activation with low AR, the result shows an activation that is more isotropic. For MEA activation, the averaged map shows, as predicted, a slight elongation along the tangential axis.”

We also clarified the presentation of Figure 4C.

(*) Please note that we also improved those maps, they are now oriented, not according to the orientation of their main elongated axis as presented in previous revision, but according to the radial axis linking activation to BS, the BS being on the right of the figure.

3) We appreciate the utility of Z-scores, but for the sake of others in the field who more commonly use deltaR/R and to facilitate others' comparison with their own studies, it would be useful to provide these in addition, to offer a sense of the response magnitudes observed.

To further clarify this point, and in addition to Figure 4—figure supplement 1 of our previous revision, we computed the correlation coefficient r2 between all pixels of Z-score vs DI/I maps over 225 maps (which correspond to all the data recorded on the animals presented in Supplementary Figure S2: N=9 rats, n=225 conditions). We found a highly significant*** correlation (see Author response image 4A, all 225 pval= 1.40e-45) between these two measures with a median r2 of 0.81 and the [20-50-80] percentiles being [0.73-- 0.81--0.88]% . Thus, the two types of maps are highly spatially correlated. As representative examples, we provide in the Author response image 4B the DI/I version (with a double scale bar expressed in DI/I and in Z-score values) of the first maps shown in Figure 2D, H and Figure 2—figure supplement 1D, H. We added these values into the text p:5, l:142.Hence, since there is a very good correlation, we believe that the z-scored maps shown with a colorbar for the z-score value (left of the colorbars) together with the corresponding DI/I min and max values (right of the colorbars) provide the reader with a very good handle to compare our data with results obtained in other studies.

Author response image 4. Correlation between DI/I and Z-score values.

Author response image 4.

(A) Histogram of highly significant*** r2 correlation coefficients between all pixels of Z-score vs DI/I maps (median r2= 0.81, N=9 rats, n=225 maps, all pval= 1.40e-45). (B) Representative examples of DI/I maps (same as the one presented in Figure 2 D, H and Figure 2—figure supplement 1D, H).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.12687.018


Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

RESOURCES