Significance
When determining responsibility for harmful actions, people often consider whether the actor behaved intentionally. The spread of surveillance cameras, “on-officer” recording devices, and smart-phone video makes it increasingly likely that such judgments are aided by video replay. Yet, little is known about how different qualities of the video, such as replay speed, affect human judgment. We demonstrate that slow motion replay can systematically increase judgments of intent because it gives viewers the false impression that the actor had more time to premeditate before acting. In legal proceedings, these judgments of intent can mean the difference between life and death. Thus, any benefits of video replay should be weighed against its potentially biasing effects.
Keywords: intent, time, slow motion, egocentrism, law
Abstract
To determine the appropriate punishment for a harmful action, people must often make inferences about the transgressor’s intent. In courtrooms and popular media, such inferences increasingly rely on video evidence, which is often played in “slow motion.” Four experiments (n = 1,610) involving real surveillance footage from a murder or broadcast replays of violent contact in professional football demonstrate that viewing an action in slow motion, compared with regular speed, can cause viewers to perceive an action as more intentional. This slow motion intentionality bias occurred, in part, because slow motion video caused participants to feel like the actor had more time to act, even when they knew how much clock time had actually elapsed. Four additional experiments (n = 2,737) reveal that allowing viewers to see both regular speed and slow motion replay mitigates the bias, but does not eliminate it. We conclude that an empirical understanding of the effect of slow motion on mental state attribution should inform the life-or-death decisions that are currently based on tacit assumptions about the objectivity of human perception.
When harm has been observed, judgment generally follows. Even when someone has indisputably committed a guilty act, judgments of responsibility depend, in large part, on inferences about whether the actor did so with an intending mind. In everyday social judgments, perceptions of intent may mean the difference between concluding that an act was aggressive or calculated rather than clumsy or impulsive. In legal proceedings, it may mean the difference between lethal injection and a lesser sentence. A central concern of the courts, as well as of public opinion, is to adjudicate this mental state inference, and both are increasingly relying on video replay to do so.
With the continued spread of surveillance cameras in major cities and small towns, the growing adoption of “on-officer” recording systems by police forces around the world, and the billions of bystanders carrying fully operational cameras in their pockets, it is increasingly likely that any given behavior will be recorded on video. When that behavior causes harm, video replays may surface in the public via social media or be introduced in courtrooms as evidence. Because video affords repeated viewings, it can augment the limits of human attention, visual processing, and memory. Because video can be slowed down, it also provides the ostensible benefit of giving people “a better look” at real-time events that happened quickly or in a chaotic environment. Although slow motion replay may be intuitively appealing for this reason, we demonstrate that slow motion replay, compared with regular speed replay, produces systematic differences in judgments of intent.
The life-or-death consequences of these differences are exemplified in a case that appeared before the Supreme Court of Pennsylvania, stemming from a 2009 murder trial in which prosecutors presented slow motion surveillance video of John Lewis fatally shooting a Philadelphia police officer during an armed robbery. The prosecution’s case convinced jurors that the shooting was premeditated, warranting a charge of first-degree murder and possible death by lethal injection, rather than reflexive, warranting a charge of second-degree murder and life in prison. On appeal, the defense argued that the slowed tape artificially stretched the relevant time period, creating a “false impression of premeditation.” The prosecution responded that the jury saw both regular speed and slow motion video, and could not be biased because jurors were fully informed (by a superimposed digital display) that Lewis shot the officer approximately 2 s after seeing him at the door (1). (The Supreme Court of Pennsylvania affirmed the trial court's decision that slow motion was more probative than prejudicial, and denied Lewis’s initial appeal. His June 18, 2014 execution date was stayed to allow for the exhaustion of appeals. At the time of this writing, Lewis is on death row.)
Weighing the admissibility of the slow motion video, one justice asked, “What can you see in this case only in the slower version that you couldn't see in the fast version?” (2). We suggest that what you can see only in the slower version is more time, or, more specifically, an actor who seems to have had more time to form and act on an intention. Two features of human judgment suggest that impressions of the duration over which real-time events unfolded are likely to be affected by the speed of video replay. First, duration estimations vary across people, situations, level of distraction, and estimation procedures (3), indicating that the mind’s timing mechanisms are susceptible to the influence of incidental factors. Second, even when people are aware that an incidental factor (e.g., slow motion) has the potential to influence their judgment, they often do not correct sufficiently (4–7). We therefore predicted that slowing a video would cause people to perceive that the events in question unfolded over more time, making people more likely to infer that the actor had formulated and carried out an intentional action. To test this prediction, we used an experimental paradigm in which we showed viewers the same events at regular speed or in slow motion, and asked them to indicate how much time they felt the actor had to execute his actions and whether (or to what degree) they thought the action was intentional. We focus on the formulation of intentional action that is most relevant to the legal setting, namely, whether a given action was “willful, deliberate, and premeditated” (1).
Study 1
Study 1 modeled the legal context for judgments of intent. Participants imagined themselves as jurors in a criminal trial and saw real surveillance video (with an accurate digital display of elapsed time) depicting 5 s of an attempted robbery that ended with the assailant shooting a store clerk. Participants were randomly assigned to watch the clip at “regular speed” or “slow motion” (2.25 times slower than regular speed). [This rate is similar to the rate at which the video in the John Lewis trial was slowed (footnote 3 in ref. 1).] Participants then indicated whether the person with the gun shot “with the intention to kill the victim”; the extent to which the action was performed with willful, deliberate, and premeditated intent to kill; and how much time it felt like the shooter had to assess the situation before he fired (SI Materials and Methods).
As predicted, participants who saw the slow motion (vs. regular speed) video felt that the action was performed with more of a willful, deliberate, and premeditated intent to kill [t(469.91) = 2.85, P = 0.005, d = 0.26] and were more likely to conclude that the person with the gun shot with the intention to kill [χ2(1, n = 489) = 6.60, P = 0.010, φ = 0.12; Table 1]. The statistically significant increase in “yes” votes to the intentional killing question is also of practical significance. In bootstrapped simulations of 1,000 12-person juries (sampled from each condition of our data), 39 juries composed of participants who saw the video at regular speed would be unanimous in saying that the defendant shot with the intention to kill, compared with 150 juries composed of participants who saw the video in slow motion (odds ratio = 4.35). This simulation does not account for jury deliberation; however, ceteris paribus, slow motion video quadrupled the odds that jurors would begin the deliberation phase ready to convict. Slow motion viewers also felt the shooter had more time than did regular speed viewers [t(480.67) = 5.54, P < 0.001, d = 0.50], an increase that statistically accounts for the relationship between video speed and judgments of intent (indirect effect 95% confidence interval [CI; 0.16, 0.46]; SI Materials and Methods).
Table 1.
Study | Condition | Clock time | Perceived time | Intent (continuous) | Intent (dichotomous) |
Study 1 (n = 489) | Regular speed | 55.11 (29.38) | 73.81 (27.47) | 77.3% (0.42) | |
Slow motion | 69.18 (26.73) | 80.36 (23.13) | 86.2% (0.35) | ||
Study 2 (n = 580) | Regular speed | 21.85 (21.19) | 39.32 (25.49) | ||
Paused | 26.51 (23.41) | 41.54 (27.54) | |||
Slow motion | 28.05 (25.04) | 47.08 (28.38) | |||
Study 3 (n = 410) | Time nonsalient | ||||
Regular speed | 3.70 (1.73) | 52.25 (29.23) | 72.94 (28.37) | 76.0% (0.43) | |
Slow motion | 5.25 (2.22) | 67.77 (28.51) | 83.99 (22.43) | 82.0% (0.39) | |
Time salient | |||||
Regular speed | 3.11 (1.04) | 54.83 (29.81) | 77.97 (25.82) | 72.2% (0.45) | |
Slow motion | 3.36 (0.93) | 62.15 (26.54) | 83.37 (23.70) | 83.0% (0.38) | |
Study 4 (n = 905) | Regular speed | 56.01 (27.98) | 72.57 (28.92) | 70.7% (0.46) | |
Both speeds | 63.92 (27.46) | 76.17 (26.51) | 71.9% (0.45) | ||
Slow motion | 67.97 (24.52) | 77.51 (24.26) | 76.5% (0.43) |
Clock time is estimated number of seconds. Perceived time and Intent (continuous) are on 0–100 scales. Intent (dichotomous) is the percentage indicating that the action was intentional as opposed to unintentional (study 1) or the percentage indicating a verdict of first-degree murder as opposed to second-degree murder (studies 3 and 4).
SI Materials and Methods
The original study materials that participants saw, and the raw data from all studies, are available at the Open Science Framework repository (https://osf.io/uzs3a/). We determined all sample sizes in advance and, for each study, did not run any analyses until after the entire sample was collected.
Study 1.
Sample recruitment and exclusions.
We posted an advertisement for this study on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk and kept the study open until we had collected 500 complete responses. The consent form specified that participants could withdraw consent at any time by exiting an incomplete survey; therefore, only the participants who completed the survey in its entirety provided the ongoing consent necessary for us to analyze their data (in this and all subsequent studies). Responses from those participants who completed the entire survey constitute the 500 observations provided in the data file. Of these participants, four reported technical difficulties, four reported having taken the survey before, and three suggested that we exclude their data because they had not paid enough attention. The final sample we used in all analyses therefore consisted of 489 participants (age range: 18–76 y, mean = 31.93, SD = 9.54; 33.9% female).
Results.
Mediation analyses.
We used the PROCESS procedure provided by Hayes (13) to test a mediation model with video condition as the independent variable (dummy coded as 0 = regular speed, 1 = slow motion), dichotomous measure of intent as the dependent variable (0 = no, 1 = yes), and perceived time as the mediator. For this and subsequent descriptions of mediation analyses, we use the path labels depicted as “model 4” by Hayes (ref. 13, p. 445): Path a refers to the effect of the independent variable on the mediator, path b refers to the effect of the mediator on the dependent variable, path ab refers to the indirect effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable through the mediator, and path c′ refers to the direct effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable.
Participants who watched the slow motion video thought that the actor had more time than did those participants who watched the regular speed video (a = 14.07), and the more time that participants thought the actor had, the more intentional they judged his action (b = 0.20). A bias-corrected bootstrap CI for the indirect effect based on 10,000 bootstrap samples did not include zero (ab = 0.29, 95% CI [0.16, 0.46]). There was not reliable evidence that slow motion influenced judgments of intent independent of its effect on perceived time (c′ = 0.34, P = 0.177).
We next analyzed responses on the continuous measure of intent. Those participants watching the video in the slow motion condition felt more strongly that the action was performed with a willful, deliberate, and premeditated intent to kill than did those participants watching the video in the regular speed condition [t(469.9) = 2.85, P = 0.005, d = 0.26]. We ran the same mediation model specified above, but with this continuous measure of intent as the dependent variable. A bias-corrected bootstrap CI for the indirect effect based on 10,000 bootstrap samples did not include zero (ab = 4.96; 95% CI [3.01, 7.35]). There was not reliable evidence that slow motion influenced judgments of intent independent of its effect on perceived time (c′ = 1.59, P = 0.466).
In short, participants who saw the video in slow motion felt that the shooter had more time than did those participants who saw it at regular speed, which statistically accounts for the relationship between slow motion and both measures of intent that we collected.
Outcome beliefs.
We asked participants to indicate whether they believed the victim lived or died, regardless of what they read in the scenario. If participants’ beliefs about the outcome of the shooting differed by experimental condition, this difference might provide an alternative mechanism for explaining an effect of replay speed on intentions, because outcome severity has been positively linked to intent (14, 15). The clip shown to participants ended immediately after the gun fired, so participants may have been uncertain as to the resolution of events they witnessed. Although we (truthfully) informed participants that the victim died from the gunshot, we wanted to ensure that any potential doubt about that information was distributed similarly across conditions. Overall, even though the mere inclusion of the question may have induced some doubt, 72.4% of participants reported believing that the victim had, in fact, died from the gunshot. Importantly, this percentage did not differ significantly by condition [slow motion = 73.7%, regular speed = 71.1%; χ2(1, n = 489) = 0.417, P = 0.519].
Study 2 Pilot.
Sample recruitment and exclusions.
Participants were visitors to the Museum of Science and Industry in Chicago. With the approval of the museum, research assistants asked admitted visitors in one area of the museum if they would be willing to participate in a short study in exchange for small chocolates and the knowledge that they were helping to advance science. We aimed to collect 100 valid responses over a 3-d period, but were only able to collect 42 during the first 3 d. Before analyzing the data, we arranged to recruit again over a second 3-d period 2 wk later, during which time we were able to collect data from 102 additional participants. No participants withdrew from this study. Responses from those participants who completed the entire survey constitute the 144 observations provided in the data file. Of these participants, 13 reported having seen the video before. The final sample we used in all analyses therefore consisted of 131 participants (age range: 16–71 y, mean = 33.37, SD = 14.51; those participants under 18 y of age participated with parental consent, two participants failed to indicate their age; 55.0% female, one participant failed to indicate sex).
Procedure.
Research assistants escorted interested participants to a quiet room with computer terminals. The study was presented using MediaLab software. Participants watched the same video reported in study 2 of the main text, at regular speed or in slow motion, and indicated the extent to which (i) the player was trying to hit the other player in the helmet, (ii) the player planned to hit the other player in the helmet, (iii) the player had the ability to avoid hitting the other player in the helmet, and (iv) the player’s action was intentional. The first three questions were presented in randomized order, and were always followed by the fourth measure. All responses were made on separate scales ranging from 0 (Not at all) to 7 (Very much).
Results.
The four measures of intentionality were highly correlated (Cronbach’s α = 0.90), and were not significantly affected by their randomized positions in the questionnaire. Consistent with our prediction, slow motion viewers saw the actor as planning more, trying more, and having a greater ability to avoid, and his action as more intentional, than did regular speed viewers (Table S3).
Table S3.
Study | Condition | Intentional | Planning | Trying | Ability to avoid |
Study 2 pilot (n = 131) | Regular speed | 2.77 (2.35) | 2.45 (2.18) | 2.49 (2.20) | 3.34 (2.02) |
Slow motion | 4.44 (2.11) | 3.91 (2.06) | 4.40 (2.14) | 4.53 (1.91) | |
Regular speed vs. slow motion | |||||
Test statistic | t(129) = 4.21 | t(129) = 3.90 | t(129) = 5.01 | t(129) = 3.42 | |
P value | P < 0.001 | P < 0.001 | P < 0.001 | P < 0.001 | |
Effect size | d = 0.75 | d = 0.69 | d = 0.88 | d = 0.60 |
All measures are scales ranging from 0 (Not at all) to 7 (Very much).
Study 2.
Sample recruitment and exclusions.
We posted an advertisement for this study on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk and left the study open until we had collected 650 complete responses. Responses from those participants who completed the entire survey constitute the 653 observations provided in the data file. Of these participants, 33 reported technical difficulties, 43 reported having taken the survey before, and 3 reported both. The final sample we used in all analyses therefore consisted of 580 participants (age range: 18–68 y, mean = 33.04, SD = 10.04; 42.1% female). One participant failed to answer all measures of intentionality and was therefore excluded from any analysis of the intentionality composite.
Participants indicated whether they had seen the clip before, reported how familiar they were with football, and listed any particular teams they supported after completing the demographic measures.
Results.
Contrasts.
As noted in the main text, a planned contrast indicated that participants in the slow motion condition felt that the action was marginally more intentional than did participants in the paused condition [t(378.10) = 1.93, P = 0.054, d = 0.20]; however, participants in the slow motion condition did not think that the actor had more time than did participants in the paused condition [t(378.95) = 0.62, P = 0.536]. Compared with participants in the regular speed condition, participants in the paused condition thought that the actor had more time to act, [t(367.71) = 2.03, P = 0.043, d = 0.21], but did not think his action was more intentional [t(370.01) = 0.82, P = 0.414].
Mediation analyses.
We used the PROCESS procedure as described in study 1 to test a mediation model with video condition as the independent variable (dummy coded as 0 = regular speed, 1 = slow motion), composite measure of intent as the dependent variable, and perceived time as the mediator. Participants who watched the slow motion video thought that the actor had more time than did those participants who watched the regular speed video (a = 6.32), and the more time that participants thought the actor had, the more intentional they judged his action to be (b = 0.71). A bias-corrected bootstrap CI for the indirect effect based on 10,000 bootstrap samples did not include zero (ab = 4.48; 95% CI [1.32, 7.82]). There was not reliable evidence that slow motion influenced judgments of intent independent of its effect on perceived time (c′ = 3.28, P = 0.131).
Study 3.
Sample recruitment and exclusions.
We posted an advertisement for this study on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk and left the study open until we had collected 500 complete responses. Responses from those participants who completed the entire survey constitute the 500 observations provided in the data file. Of these participants, 17 reported technical difficulties, 69 reported having taken the survey before, 4 suggested that we exclude their data because they had not paid enough attention, 10 reported being ineligible to serve on a jury in the United States, and 10 reported more than one reason for exclusion. The final sample we used in all analyses therefore consisted of 410 participants (age range: 19–74 y, mean = 34.56, SD = 11.56; 43.7% female). One participant failed to answer the continuous measure of intent and was therefore excluded from any analysis of that measure.
Results.
We ran separate ANOVAs on each of our dependent variables with time salience (yes vs. no) and video speed (regular vs. slow motion) conditions as independent variables. For the clock-time measure, there was a main effect of the time salience condition [F(1, 406) = 62.03, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.133], a main effect of the speed condition [F(1, 406) = 32.62, P < 0.001, = 0.074], and an interaction between the two [F(1, 406) = 16.79, P < 0.001, = 0.040]. Slow motion (vs. regular speed) participants gave significantly higher estimates of clock time in the time nonsalient conditions [simple effect F(1, 406) = 48.89, P < 0.001], but not in the time nonsalient conditions [simple effect F(1, 406) = 1.28, P = 0.258].
For the perceived time measure, there was no main effect of the time salience condition [F(1, 406) = 0.29, P = 0.592, = 0.001], a main effect of the speed condition [F(1, 406) = 16.11, P < 0.001, = 0.038], and no interaction between the two [F(1, 406) = 2.08, P = 0.150, = 0.005].
For the continuous intent measure, there was no main effect of the time salience condition [F(1, 405) = 0.78, P = 0.378, = 0.002], a main effect of the speed condition [F(1, 405) = 10.80, P = 0.001, = 0.026], and no interaction between the two [F(1, 405) = 1.28, P = 0.258, = 0.003].
For the dichotomous intent measure, there was no main effect of the time salience condition [F(1, 406) = 0.12, P = 0.725, = 0.000], a main effect of the speed condition [F(1, 406) = 4.14, P = 0.043, = 0.010], and no interaction between the two [F(1, 406) = 0.347, P = 0.556, = 0.001]. Therefore, these results do not provide any evidence to support the conjecture that making clock time salient would eliminate any potentially biasing effects of slow motion.
Mediation analyses.
We used the PROCESS procedure as described in study 1 to test a mediation model with video condition as the independent variable (dummy coded as 0 = regular speed, 1 = slow motion), the dichotomous measure of verdict decisions as the dependent variable (0 = second-degree murder, 1 = first-degree murder), perceived time as the mediator, and estimated clock time as a covariate. Controlling for estimated clock time, participants who watched the slow motion video thought that the actor had more time than did those participants who watched the regular speed video (a = 6.37), and the more time that participants thought the actor had, the more likely they were to deliver a verdict of first-degree murder (b = 0.04). A bias-corrected bootstrap CI for the indirect effect based on 10,000 bootstrap samples did not include zero (ab = 0.28; 95% CI [0.04, 0.54]). There was not reliable evidence that slow motion influenced judgments of intent independent of its effect on perceived time (c′ = −0.003, P = 0.993).
We ran the same mediation model specified above, but with the continuous measure of intent as the dependent variable. A bias-corrected bootstrap CI for the indirect effect based on 10,000 bootstrap samples did not include zero (ab = 3.10; 95% CI [0.50, 6.02]). There was not reliable evidence that slow motion influenced judgments of intent independent of its effect on perceived time (c′ = 2.76, P = 0.215).
In short, controlling for estimates of clock time, participants who saw the video in slow motion felt that the shooter had more time than those participants who saw it at regular speed, which statistically accounts for the relationship between slow motion and both measures of intent that we collected.
Clock time vs. perceived time.
We ran separate mediation models to disambiguate the effects of clock time and perceived time on judgments of intent. Perceived time (indirect effect = 0.51, 95% CI [0.25, 0.81]) and clock time (indirect effect = 0.30, 95% CI [0.13, 0.54]) each mediate the relationship between video speed and verdict decisions. When entered into a parallel mediation model, perceived time significantly mediates the relationship between video speed and verdict decisions, even when controlling for the indirect effect of clock time (indirect effect = 0.50, 95% CI [0.24, 0.80]). In contrast, when controlling for the indirect effect of perceived time, clock time does not mediate the relationship between video speed and verdict decisions (indirect effect = 0.12, 95% CI [−0.08, 0.35]). A pairwise comparison between the mediators confirms that the indirect effect via perceived time is larger than is the indirect effect via clock time (difference between indirect effects = 0.39, 95% CI [0.05, 0.77]). This pattern of results suggests that slow motion influenced judgments of intent in this experiment through its effect on the perceived, rather than actual, time.
Time salient vs. time nonsalient.
We next explored in more detail the impact of time salience on time perception and judgments of intent. On appeal of the Lewis trial, the appellant argued that slow motion “created a false impression of how much time elapsed.” The Commonwealth argued against this claim by noting that the time was displayed in the video itself and that the jury had been reminded several times of the actual window of time that elapsed in the relevant portion of the video. (Note that our operationalization of time salience was designed to encompass both of these points.) We interpret this statement as an assertion that, because time was made salient, slow motion would not change jurors’ impressions of elapsed time, and hence the slow motion video could not unduly influence their judgments of intent.
To test this hypothesis, we estimated a first-stage moderated mediation model, where the effect of slow motion on perceived time was moderated by making time salient and the effect of perceived time on first-degree murder verdicts was not moderated by any variables. The effect of slow motion on perceived time was not significantly moderated by time salience (video speed * time salience interaction term = −8.20, 95% CI [−19.39, 2.99]). The index of moderated mediation (IMM) (13) had a bootstrap CI that included zero (IMM = −0.36, 95% CI [−0.88, 0.14]). Results of this analysis indicate that when time is not made salient, estimates of perceived time mediate the impact of slow motion on verdict decisions (indirect effect = 0.44, 95% CI [0.19, 0.81]), which replicates our mediation results in all other studies in which time is not made salient. However, even when time is made salient, there remains a significant indirect effect (indirect effect = 0.07, 95% CI [0.001, 0.20]). In sum, whereas making time salient does reduce an error in estimates of clock time, it does not change the subjective feeling that the actor had more time to premeditate before acting; because perceptions of intent are mediated through perceived time, slow motion video continues to influence judgments of intent even when people are fully aware of, and can accurately estimate, the objective amount of time that the actor had.
Study 4.
Sample recruitment and exclusions.
We posted an advertisement for this study on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk and left the study open until we had collected 1,000 complete responses. Responses from those participants who completed the entire survey constitute the 1,002 observations provided in the data file. Of these participants, 28 reported technical difficulties, 48 reported having taken the survey before, 10 suggested that we exclude their data because they had not paid enough attention, 21 reported being ineligible to serve on a jury in the United States, and 10 reported more than one reason for exclusion. The final sample we used in all analyses therefore consisted of 905 participants (age range: 18–86 y, mean = 35.80, SD = 11.62; 49.1% female). One participant failed to answer the continuous measure of intent and was therefore excluded from any analysis of that measure.
Results.
Contrasts.
We ran separate one-way ANOVAs on each of our dependent variables, followed by linear and quadratic contrasts. For the perceived time measure, there was a significant between-groups effect [F(2, 902) = 15.53, P < 0.001], a significant linear trend [t(584.24) = 5.56, P < 0.001], and a nonsignificant quadratic trend [t(590.60) = 1.01, P = 0.313]. For the continuous intent measure, there was a marginally significant between-groups effect [F(2, 901) = 2.74, P = 0.065], a significant linear trend [t(574.51) = 2.26, P = 0.024], and a nonsignificant quadratic trend [t(616.04) = 0.61, P = 0.545]. For the dichotomous intent measure, there was a nonsignificant between-groups effect [F(2, 902) = 1.42, P = 0.241], a nonsignificant linear trend [t(592.49) = 1.61, P = 0.109], and a nonsignificant quadratic trend [t(602.38) = 0.54, P = 0.588]. (Levene’s test indicated unequal variances on these three measures, so degrees of freedom were adjusted down in the contrast models.)
As noted in the main text, these contrasts indicate that, on balance, responses from participants in the both speeds condition fell between the (relatively lower) responses of those participants who saw only the regular speed and the (relatively higher) responses of those participants who saw only the slow speed. We also conducted contrasts to compare the effect of the both speeds condition to the regular speed condition only, and (separately) to the slow motion condition only. Compared with participants in the regular speed condition, participants in the both speeds condition thought that the actor had more time to act [t(599.58) = 3.50, P < 0.001], but did not think his action was significantly more willful, deliberate, and premeditated [t(591.52) = 1.59, P = 0.112] and were not more likely to convict him of first-degree murder [t(599.93) = 0.32, P = 0.748]. Compared with participants in the slow speed condition, participants in the both speeds condition thought that the actor had (marginally) less time to act [t(600.04) = 1.92, P = 0.055], but did not think his action was significantly less willful, deliberate, and premeditated [t(602.59) = 0.65, P = 0.518] and were not less likely to convict him of first-degree murder [t(604.77) = 1.29, P = 0.196].
Mediation analyses.
We used the PROCESS procedure as described in study 1 to test a mediation model for the basic slow motion vs. regular speed comparison with video condition as the independent variable (dummy coded as 0 = regular speed, 1 = slow motion), dichotomous measure of intent as the dependent variable, and perceived time as the mediator. Participants who watched the slow motion video thought that the actor had more time than did those participants who watched the regular speed video (a = 11.96), and the more time that participants thought the actor had, the more intentional they judged his action (b = 0.03). A bias-corrected bootstrap CI for the indirect effect based on 10,000 bootstrap samples did not include zero (ab = 0.41; 95% CI [0.257, 0.593]). There was not reliable evidence that slow motion influenced judgments of intent independent of its effect on perceived time (c′ = −0.11, P = 0.587).
We ran the same mediation model specified above, but with the continuous measure of intent as the dependent variable. A bias-corrected bootstrap CI for the indirect effect based on 10,000 bootstrap samples did not include zero (ab = 6.24; 95% CI [3.97, 8.88]). There was not reliable evidence that slow motion influenced judgments of intent independent of its effect on perceived time (c′ = −1.31, P = 0.500).
Follow-Up Studies.
Sample recruitment and exclusions.
We posted advertisements for each follow-up study on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk and left the study open until we had collected 600 (follow-up studies 1 and 2) or 800 (follow-up study 3) complete responses. In follow-up study 1, of the 600 participants who completed the survey, 10 reported technical difficulties, 31 reported having taken the survey before, 4 suggested that we exclude their data because they had not paid enough attention, and 2 reported more than one reason for exclusion. The final sample we used in all analyses therefore consisted of 557 participants (age range: 18–76 y, mean = 33.73, SD = 11.24; 46.7% female).
In follow-up study 2, of the 599 participants who completed the survey, 8 reported technical difficulties, 28 reported having taken the survey before, 4 suggested that we exclude their data because they had not paid enough attention, and 1 reported more than one reason for exclusion. The final sample we used in all analyses therefore consisted of 560 participants (age range: 18–69 y, mean = 35.21, SD = 11.33; 51.8% female). One participant failed to answer the dichotomous measure of intent, and was therefore excluded from any analysis of that measure.
In follow-up study 3, of the 799 participants who completed the survey, 21 reported technical difficulties, 29 reported having taken the survey before, 13 suggested that we exclude their data because they had not paid enough attention, 27 reported being ineligible to serve on a jury in the United State, and 60 reported more than one reason for exclusion. The final sample we used in all analyses therefore consisted of 715 participants (age range: 19–74 y, mean = 35.55, SD = 11.93; 51.5% female).
Results.
For each follow-up study, we report the means and SDs for each measure, along with indicators of statistically significant differences between conditions, in Table S4.
Table S4.
Study | Condition | Perceived time | Intent (continuous) | Intent (dichotomous) |
Follow-up study 1 (n = 557) | Regular speed | 57.74 (29.56) | 75.64 (26.95) | 82.0% (0.39) |
Both speeds | 61.83 (28.26) | 76.48 (26.45) | 83.2% (0.38) | |
Regular speed vs. both speeds | ||||
Test statistic | t(555) = 1.67 | t(555) = 1.67 | χ2(1) = 0.12 | |
P value | P = 0.095 | P = 0.712 | P = 0.730 | |
Effect size | d = 0.14 | d = 0.03 | φ = 0.02 | |
Follow-up study 2 (n = 560) | Regular speed | 57.67 (30.70) | 77.17 (28.64) | 81.7% (0.39) |
Both speeds | 66.93 (26.45) | 79.15 (24.37) | 87.0% (0.34) | |
Regular speed vs. both speeds | ||||
Test statistic | t(554.7) = 3.83 | t(553.6) = 0.88 | χ2(1) = 3.04 | |
P value | P < 0.001 | P = 0.377 | P = 0.081 | |
Effect size | d = 0.33 | d = 0.08 | φ = 0.07 | |
Follow-up study 3 (n = 715) | Regular speed | 57.77 (29.80) | 74.87 (29.15) | 73.7% (0.44) |
Both speeds | 66.61 (26.08) | 77.98 (25.46) | 77.1% (0.42) | |
Regular speed vs. both speeds | ||||
Test statistic | t(700.1) = 4.22 | t(699.8) = 1.52 | χ2(1) = 1.13 | |
P value | P < 0.001 | P = 0.129 | P = 0.288 | |
Effect size | d = 0.32 | d = 0.12 | φ = 0.04 |
Perceived time and Intent (continuous) are on 0–100 scales. Intent (dichotomous) is the percentage indicating a verdict of first-degree murder as opposed to second-degree murder. When Levene’s test indicated unequal variances, degrees of freedom were adjusted.
Meta-analyses.
For each dependent variable, we calculated the standardized effect size and inverse variance weight separately for each study. For binary variables, the standardized effect size was the logged-odds ratio, and for continuous variables, the standardized effect size was the standardized mean difference (a summary of the formulae used is provided in table 3.2 of ref. 12). We used inverse variance weight (as opposed to sample size) to weight each study because it provides a better estimate of the precision of the effect size. Detailed results from each study are provided in Tables S5 and S6.
Table S5.
Perceived time (standardized mean difference) | Willful, deliberate, premeditated (standardized mean difference) | First- vs. second-degree murder (logged-odds ratio) | ||||||
Study | N regular speed | N both speeds | Effect size | Inverse variance weight | Effect size | Inverse variance weight | Effect size | Inverse variance weight |
Study 4 | 297 | 306 | 0.285 | 149.21 | 0.130 | 150.14 | 0.058 | 30.84 |
Follow-up study 1 | 284 | 273 | 0.142 | 138.85 | 0.031 | 139.18 | 0.077 | 19.98 |
Follow-up study 2 | 289 | 270 | 0.324 | 138.02 | 0.075 | 139.72 | 0.411 | 17.88 |
Follow-up study 3 | 357 | 358 | 0.316 | 176.56 | 0.114 | 178.46 | 0.181 | 33.06 |
Participants in the regular speed condition watched the video three times at regular speed. Participants in the both speeds condition watched the video once at regular speed and then twice in slow motion.
Table S6.
Statistic | Perceived time (standardized mean difference) | Willful, deliberate, premeditated (standardized mean difference) | First- vs. second-degree murder (logged-odds ratio) |
Effect (SE) | 0.270 (0.041) | 0.090 (0.041) | 0.165 (0.099) |
Z score | 6.62 | 2.21 | 1.66 |
P value | <0.001 | 0.027 | 0.096 |
Total weighted effect size is calculated using the inverse variance weights from Table S5.
Mediation analyses.
We analyzed each study separately to assess the indirect effect of the both speeds condition (compared with the regular speed condition) on the continuous and dichotomous measures of intent (separately) via perceived time using the PROCESS macro (13). This approach is preferred to analyzing the indirect effect of the studies together, because four studies is not enough to estimate random intercepts and slopes reliably across the studies.
Three of the four studies provide significant evidence of the mediating role of perceived time on both the continuous measure and dichotomous measure of intent. We can combine the four bootstrapped effect sizes (using bootstrapped SEs to calculate inverse variance weight) with the same meta-analytic principles as above. Whereas a Z-test is recommended due to the nonnormal distributions of indirect effects, we can get a first-order approximation of the strength of the effect with the meta-analytic method. Detailed results from each study are provided in Tables S7 and S8.
Table S7.
Study | N | A path coefficient (SE) | B path coefficient (SE) | Indirect effect 95% CI | C′ path coefficient (SE) |
Study 4 | 602 | 7.85 (2.26) | 0.559 (0.034) | [1.95, 7.00] | −0.793 (1.89) |
Follow-up study 1 | 557 | 4.10 (2.45) | 0.346 (0.036) | [−0.183, 3.28] | −0.582 (2.11) |
Follow-up study 2 | 560 | 9.26 (2.43) | 0.344 (0.037) | [1.56, 5.11] | −1.20 (2.12) |
Follow-up study 3 | 715 | 8.84 (2.09) | 0.526 (0.031) | [2.46, 7.07] | −1.54 (1.75) |
Each mediation model was evaluated separately, with each indirect effect calculated and each SE estimated via bootstrap using the PROCESS macro. The individual indirect effects were weighted by a factor of 1/(SE2) (i.e., inverse variance weighting) to produce a first-order estimate of the mean indirect effect size: mean effect size = 3.05, SE = 0.510, Z = 5.98, P < 0.001.
Table S8.
Study | N | A path coefficient (SE) | B path coefficient (SE) | Indirect effect 95% CI | C′ path coefficient (SE) |
Study 4 | 603 | 7.90 (2.26) | 0.041 (0.004) | [0.141, 0.524] | −0.277 (0.206) |
Follow-up study 1 | 557 | 4.10 (2.45) | 0.029 (0.004) | [−0.018, 0.281] | −0.050 (0.236) |
Follow-up study 2 | 559 | 9.32 (2.43) | 0.026 (0.004) | [0.115, 0.395] | 0.155 (0.250) |
Follow-up study 3 | 715 | 8.84 (2.09) | 0.041 (0.004) | [0.192, 0.564] | −0.228 (0.201) |
Each mediation model was evaluated separately, with each indirect effect calculated and each SE estimated via bootstrap using the PROCESS macro. The individual indirect effects were weighted by a factor of 1/(SE2) (i.e., inverse variance weighting) to produce a first-order estimate of the mean indirect effect size: mean effect size = 0.241, SE = 0.041, Z = 5.85, P < 0.001.
For completeness, we ran the same mediation models on the meta-analytic results from studies 1, 3, and 4, each of which showed independent evidence that perceived time mediated the effect of video speed (regular speed vs. slow motion) on both the continuous measure and dichotomous measure of intent. Detailed results from each study are provided in Table S9.
Table S9.
Measure | N | A path coefficient (SE) | B path coefficient (SE) | Indirect effect 95% CI | C′ path coefficient (SE) |
Intent (continuous) | |||||
Study 1 | 489 | 14.07 (2.54) | 0.353 (0.038) | [3.05, 7.33] | 1.59 (2.18) |
Study 3 | 408 | 11.82 (2.83) | 0.468 (0.037) | [2.86, 8.54] | 2.51 (2.15) |
Study 4 | 598 | 11.91 (2.15) | 0.524 (0.036) | [3.99, 8.79] | −1.307 (1.91) |
Intent (dichotomous) | |||||
Study 1 | 489 | 14.07 (2.54) | 0.020 (0.004) | [0.155, 0.455] | 0.340 (0.252) |
Study 3 | 410 | 11.63 (2.83) | 0.044 (0.005) | [0.254, 0.817] | 0.067 (0.282) |
Study 4 | 599 | 11.96 (2.15) | 0.034 (0.004) | [0.253, 0.592] | −0.112 (0.207) |
Each mediation model was evaluated separately, with each indirect effect calculated and each SE estimated via bootstrap using the PROCESS macro. The individual indirect effects were weighted by a factor of 1/(SE2) (i.e., inverse variance weighting) to produce a first-order estimate of the mean indirect effect size. For Intent (continuous): mean effect size = 5.53, SE = 0.711, Z = 7.77, P < 0.001. For Intent (dichotomous): mean effect size = 0.362, SE = 0.053, Z = 6.78, P < 0.001. Coefficients in study 3 do not control for participant estimates of actual time.
Jury Simulation Analyses.
We used a variation on the jury bootstrapping simulation described in study 1 to illustrate the practical impact of slow motion video on judgments of intent, using the binomial distribution (based on the meta-analytic effect size) instead of bootstrap simulations. We performed separate analyses comparing all regular speed with all both speeds conditions (across study 4 and follow-up studies 1–3) and all regular speed with all slow motion conditions (across studies 1, 3, and 4).
Regular speed vs. both speeds.
The mean logged-odds ratio in Tables S5 and S6 is an estimate of the difference in proportions of a first-degree murder categorization between the regular speed and both speeds conditions; that is, one of the proportions must be fixed to calculate the other. The inverse variance-weighted average of the regular speed condition proportion is 0.775. Given a logged-odds ratio of 0.164 and a regular speed condition proportion of 0.775, the both speeds condition proportion must be 0.802. With these proportions, we calculated (using the probability mass function of the binomial distribution) the estimated number of juries out of 1,000 from each condition that would have 12 of 12 jurors choosing a verdict of first-degree murder (regular speed condition = 47.0, both speeds condition = 71.2). The implied odds ratio is 1.55, a 55% increase. The estimate is similar if the both speeds condition proportion is fixed instead (both speeds condition inverse variance-weighted average = 0.806, implied unanimous jury odds ratio = 1.55).
Regular speed vs. slow motion.
The mean logged-odds ratio in Tables S10 and S11 is an estimate of the difference in proportions of a first-degree murder categorization between the regular speed and slow motion conditions. The inverse variance-weighted average of the regular speed condition proportion is 0.739. Given a logged-odds ratio of 0.439 and a regular speed condition proportion of 0.739, the slow motion condition proportion must be 0.815. With these proportions, we again calculated (using the probability mass function of the binomial distribution) the estimated number of juries out of 1,000 from each condition that would have 12 of 12 jurors choosing a verdict of first-degree murder (regular speed condition = 26.6, slow motion condition = 85.6). The implied odds ratio is 3.42, a 242% increase. The estimate is similar if the slow motion condition proportion is fixed instead (slow motion condition inverse variance-weighted average = 0.820, implied unanimous jury odds ratio = 3.32).
Table S10.
Perceived time (standardized mean difference) | Willful, deliberate, premeditated (standardized mean difference) | First- vs. second-degree murder (logged-odds ratio) | ||||||
Study | N regular speed | N both speeds | Effect size | Inverse variance weight | Effect size | Inverse variance weight | Effect size | Inverse variance weight |
Study 1 | 242 | 247 | 0.501 | 118.53 | 0.258 | 121.23 | 0.611 | 17.35 |
Study 3 | 211 | 199 | 0.407 | 100.35 | 0.321 | 100.88 | 0.502 | 16.87 |
Study 4 | 297 | 302 | 0.454 | 145.98 | 0.185 | 148.85 | 0.299 | 28.84 |
Participants in the regular speed condition watched the video three times at regular speed. Participants in the slow motion condition watched the video three times in slow motion.
Table S11.
Statistic | Perceived time (standardized mean difference) | Willful, deliberate, premeditated (standardized mean difference) | First- vs. second-degree murder (logged-odds ratio) |
Effect (SE) | 0.456 (0.052) | 0.246 (0.052) | 0.439 (0.126) |
Z score | 8.72 | 4.73 | 3.49 |
P value | <0.001 | <0.001 | <0.001 |
Total weighted effect size is calculated using the inverse variance weights from Table S10.
Study 2
Study 2 used a sports video to test whether this slow motion intentionality bias generalizes beyond rare and horrific criminal actions to more mundane transgressions. Participants watched a National Football League (NFL) player execute a prohibited “helmet-to-helmet” tackle. Although the tackle itself was clearly intentional, it was unclear whether the player intended to strike the opposing player’s helmet with his own. Because legal definitions of intent (focusing on premeditation) are not necessarily the same as lay definitions [which typically incorporate components of desire, belief, intention, and awareness into assessments of intentionality (8)], we included face-valid measures of each to determine which aspects of intentionality are affected by video speed. In addition, study 2 aims to provide further insight into our proposed subjective-time mechanism. An artifact of comparing slow motion video with regular speed video is that slow motion viewers are exposed to the stimuli for more time. It is therefore possible that the slow motion bias results from the greater amount of time that perceivers have to make inferences about the actor’s intent. To examine this alternative, study 2 included a “paused” condition that played the video at regular speed but with a still-frame pause inserted to equate its duration to the duration of the slow motion video.
Three key findings emerged from study 2. First, judgments of the various intentionality-related constructs were highly correlated (all dyadic r ≥ 0.57, P < 0.001; Table S1) and produced similar effects when examined independently (Table S2), suggesting that observers do not finely differentiate these constructs when judging the intent behind a harmful action. We therefore collapsed these measures into an overall composite (α = 0.95). Second, this composite replicated the findings from study 1: Participants felt that the action was more intentional if they saw it in slow motion than if they saw it at regular speed [t(389.54) = 2.87, P = 0.004, d = 0.29; Table 1], and this effect was mediated by perceptions that the actor had more time to assess the situation (t(383.88) = 2.66, P = 0.008, d = 0.27, indirect effect 95% CI [1.23, 7.73]). Third, increasing the duration of the video by pausing it did not have the same effect on intentionality judgments as did increasing the duration of the video by slowing it down: Participants felt that the action was marginally more intentional if they saw it in slow motion than if they saw it with an inserted pause [t(378.10) = 1.93, P = 0.054, d = 0.20].
Table S1.
Measure | Planning | Ability to avoid | Intentional | Willful, deliberate, premeditated |
Trying | 0.892*** | 0.594*** | 0.876*** | 0.864*** |
Planning | — | 0.583*** | 0.885*** | 0.882*** |
Ability to avoid | — | — | 0.636*** | 0.578*** |
Intentional | — | — | — | 0.900*** |
Correlation is significant at ***P < 0.001. The five measures of intentionality formed a highly reliable scale (Cronbach’s α = 0.95).
Table S2.
Study | Condition | Intentional | Planning | Trying | Ability to avoid | Willful, deliberate, premeditated |
Study 2 (n = 580) | Regular speed | 37.50 (30.00) | 34.95 (27.89) | 36.29 (28.82) | 49.90 (27.36) | 38.14 (28.68) |
Paused | 41.35 (31.26) | 36.92 (30.45) | 39.02 (31.12) | 50.78 (27.53) | 39.66 (30.04) | |
Slow motion | 45.51 (32.71) | 42.72 (31.15) | 44.83 (32.25) | 57.09 (27.91) | 45.28 (32.33) | |
Regular speed vs. slow motion | ||||||
Test statistic | t(391.74) = 2.54 | t(389.29) = 2.61 | t(389.65) = 2.78 | t(393.85) = 2.59 | t(388.48) = 2.33 | |
P value | P = 0.011 | P = 0.009 | P = 0.006 | P = 0.010 | P = 0.021 | |
Effect size | d = 0.26 | d = 0.26 | d = 0.28 | d = 0.26 | d = 0.24 |
All measures are scales ranging from 0 (Not at all) to 100 (Very much). Levene’s test indicated unequal variances, so degrees of freedom were adjusted.
Study 3
Study 3 further investigated the underlying mechanism of this effect by testing whether perceived time, and its subsequent effect on judgments of intent, could be debiased by highlighting the actual amount of clock time that the actor had. An objectivist account of the bias we have demonstrated presumes that viewers have some objective cognitive knowledge about what can occur in a given window of clock time and that any threat of a slow motion bias would be eliminated by ensuring accurate clock-time estimates. This account appears to be the one that the court had in mind after hearing the Lewis appeal when it ruled that slow motion could not have biased the jury because “the time that transpired was displayed on the tape and was repeatedly pointed out to the jury” (1). In contrast, our proposed subjectivist account recognizes that the same amount of clock time can feel shorter or longer under different conditions (3) and predicts that viewers will be more likely to infer intent when they feel like an actor had more time rather than less time, even when they are fully aware of (and can accurately estimate) the objective amount of time that the actor had. We therefore directly measured estimates of objective clock time in addition to subjective perceptions of time.
To test between the objectivist and subjectivist accounts, study 3 added a time salience manipulation to the study 1 procedure. In the “time nonsalient” conditions that replicate study 1, the clock time (via a digital time stamp on the video) was present but not highlighted. In the new “time salient” conditions, participants were repeatedly reminded that they could “see from the digital time stamp” that “approximately three seconds elapsed.” We predicted that the difference in the amount of time it felt like the actor had, and hence the slow motion intentionality bias, would persist even when the actual amount of time was highlighted. To increase ecological validity further, we changed the dichotomous measure (from study 1) to ask outright whether participants would convict the actor of first- or second-degree murder if they were jurors in this trial.
Results revealed an interaction between video speed and time salience on clock-time estimates [F(1, 406) = 16.79, P < 0.001, = 0.040]. When clock time was available but not made salient, participants in the slow motion (vs. regular speed) condition estimated that more clock time had elapsed [F(1, 406) = 48.89, P < 0.001, = 0.107]. In contrast, when clock time was made salient, clock-time estimates in the two conditions did not differ significantly [F(1, 406) = 1.28, P = 0.258, = 0.003; Table 1]. Although making clock time salient successfully decreased the bias in estimates of clock time, it did not significantly affect the difference in estimates of the time it felt like the shooter had. Consistent with the subjectivist account, participants in the slow motion (vs. regular speed) conditions felt like the shooter had more time to assess the situation before he fired [F(1, 406) = 16.11, P < 0.001, = 0.038]; the interaction with time salience was not significant [interaction F(1, 406) = 2.08, P = 0.150, = 0.005].
Critically, subjective perceptions of time, not estimated clock time, drove judgments of intent. Participants in the slow motion (vs. regular speed) conditions felt that the action was performed with more of a willful, deliberate, and premeditated intent to kill [F(1, 405) = 10.80, P = 0.001, = 0.026], and more slow motion viewers than regular speed viewers returned a verdict of first-degree murder [F(1, 406) = 4.14, P = 0.043, = 0.010]. The interaction with time salience was not significant for the continuous measure of intent [interaction F(1, 405) = 1.28, P = 0.259, = 0.003] or the dichotomous measure of intent [interaction F(1, 406) = 0.35, P = 0.556, = 0.001]. Subjective perceptions of time mediated the effect of slow motion on first-degree murder verdicts, even when controlling for clock-time estimates (indirect effect 95% CI [0.04, 0.54]).
Study 4
Studies 1 and 3 were designed to reflect judgments routinely made by real jurors, but a notable divergence is that jurors may see multiple versions of a video at different playback speeds, whereas our participants saw one speed exclusively. In fact, in evaluating the potentially prejudicial impact of slow motion in the Lewis case, the justices cited the presentation of both video speeds as a mitigating influence (1). To approximate further the actual evidence a jury is likely to see during trial, and, by extension, to examine another potential bias-reduction strategy, study 4 added a “both speeds” condition in which participants first saw the regular speed version, followed by the slow speed version. We predicted that responses from those individuals who saw both video speeds would fall between the (relatively lower) responses of those individuals who saw only the regular speed and the (relatively higher) responses of those individuals who saw only the slow speed.
To test this prediction, we ran linear and quadratic contrasts. The linear trend was significant for the perceived time measure [t(584.24) = 5.56, P < 0.001] and the willful, deliberate, and premeditated intent to kill measure [t(574.51) = 2.26, P = 0.024]; it was not significant for the first- vs. second-degree murder measure [t(592.49) = 1.61, P = 0.109; Table 1]. None of the quadratic contrasts was significant, which supports the evidence for a linear trend (SI Materials and Methods). Although these patterns are generally in line with our prediction, Table 1 reveals some variation in the effect across measures, whereby viewing both speeds is closer to the slow speed condition on the continuous measure of intent, closer to the regular speed condition on the binary measure of intent, and between the two on the measure of perceived time (results of all pairwise comparisons are provided in SI Materials and Methods).
To clarify the mixed evidence from study 4, we ran three additional studies to isolate the comparison for the practical question of interest, namely, whether seeing the video at both speeds results in more perceived intent than seeing the video only at regular speed. The first experiment was an exact replication of study 4 (omitting the slow motion condition), and the other two experiments were variants on study 1 (replacing the slow motion condition with the both speeds condition). In a meta-analysis that included results from study 4 and these three follow-ups, the both speeds condition led to significantly higher judgments than the regular speed condition on the measure of perceived time (mean effect size = 0.270, SE = 0.041, Z = 6.62, P < 0.001) and the continuous measure of intent (mean effect size = 0.090, SE = 0.041, Z = 2.21, P = 0.027) and to marginally higher judgments on the dichotomous measure of intent (mean effect size = 0.165, SE = 0.099, Z = 1.66, P = 0.096). Perceived time consistently mediated the effect of viewing condition on both judgments of intent (SI Materials and Methods).
Empirical Summary
To gauge the practical magnitude of the effect of viewing condition on the dichotomous measure of intent, and the potential bias-reducing impact of seeing both regular and slow versions, we performed separate meta-analyses on (i) regular speed vs. slow motion conditions and (ii) regular speed vs. both speeds (i.e., regular and then slow) conditions. A bootstrap analysis (based on the meta-analytic effect size) revealed that, compared with simulated juries who saw only the regular speed video, the odds of a unanimous first-degree murder verdict were 3.42-fold higher among juries who saw only the slow version, and 1.55-fold higher among juries who saw both versions. These results demonstrate that giving viewers the opportunity to view both speeds reduces the intentionality bias, but does not eliminate it.
Discussion
With more moments of our lives being caught on video comes the increasing likelihood that retrospective judgments of those moments, in courts of public opinion and courts of law, will be influenced by video replays. In courts of law, whether an accused party had “time enough so that the defendant can and does fully form an intent to kill and is conscious of that intention” (9) is a question of fact that juries are responsible for determining. In the cases that we studied, perceived time was indeed at the crux of participants’ inferences about this fact. For example, once the store clerk appeared, did the shooter have time for forethought or did he fire reflexively? Although timing will be irrelevant or uncontroversial in some behavioral sequences submitted for the court’s inspection, the question of whether an actor had a “long enough” window to assess and prepare to inflict the harm is likely to be central in many disputes.
The present investigation cannot determine whether slow motion replay makes viewers more or less accurate in judging premeditation in these situations, but it does demonstrate that slow motion can systematically increase perceptions of premeditation itself. Considering that viewers’ inferences about intentions were sensitive to the amount of time they felt the actor had, we suspect that slow motion is one of many ways in which the temporal dynamics of an event could be modulated. From a lawyer’s narrative pacing to the selection, arrangement, and timing of still photographs presented to a jury, many processes in a trial could potentially affect perceptions of how much time elapsed and, consequently, could affect the perceived intent of an actor. If jurors perceive video as a particularly “objective” representation of true events, its biasing potential may be especially pernicious.
In the case of video, our results do offer some initial insight into how to mitigate the bias that results from the artificial distortion of temporal dynamics. Contrary to the intuition of the justices in the Lewis appeal, reminding viewers of the actual elapsed time was not sufficient (in our studies) to prevent them from feeling that the actor had more time to act, and hence inferring that his action was more intentional and more deserving of a first-degree murder conviction (study 3). However, consistent with the intuition of the justices, showing the action at both regular speed and in slow motion was somewhat, albeit not completely, effective in reducing the impact of slow motion on first-degree murder convictions (study 4).
In its decision on the Lewis appeal, the court acknowledged that, “In a sense, all slow motion and freeze frame video distorts reality,” and that “such distortion may enhance the jury’s understanding or it may do the opposite” (1, citing ref. 10). Therefore, in determining whether, and under what conditions, slow motion evidence should be admissible in court, its potential benefits must be weighed against its potential costs. Although we agree that slow motion may enhance the jury’s understanding of the actions in question, including the actus reus and any mitigating or aggravating contextual events, our results underscore that under some conditions it may “do the opposite” for the jury’s understanding of actors’ mental states (mens rea). The relative impact of this tradeoff under various potentially moderating conditions, including the number, order, and exact speed of replays, remains open for investigation, but even the possibility of such a tradeoff demands empirical attention. For instance, at a certain point of “superslow motion,” actors may appear to be moving at nonhumanly slow speeds and seem less likely to possess any mental states, including intentions (11). It seems imperative that an empirical understanding of the factors that contribute to assessments of intent inform the life-or-death decisions that are currently based, in part, on the intuitions of lawmakers and their tacit assumptions about the objectivity of human perception.
Materials and Methods
Ethics Statement.
All studies were approved by the Institutional Review Board at The University of Chicago or Northwestern University. All participants read and provided informed consent before completing the studies.
Open Science Statement.
The original study materials that participants saw, and the raw data from all studies, are available at the Open Science Framework repository (https://osf.io/uzs3a/). We determined all sample sizes in advance and, for each study, did not run any analyses until after the entire sample was collected.
Recruitment, Participants, and Exclusions.
We recruited participants from all experiments using Amazon Mechanical Turk (limited to the United States) and paid either $0.50 (studies 1 and 2) or $0.60 (studies 3 and 4) in exchange for their participation. Across the four main studies, 2,384 participants met our criteria for inclusion [study 1: n = 489, mean age (Mage) = 31.9 y (9.5), 323 male, 166 female; study 2: n = 580, Mage = 33.0 y (10.0), 336 male, 244 female; study 3: n = 410, Mage = 34.6 y (11.6), 231 male, 179 female; study 4: n = 905, Mage = 35.8 y (11.6), 461 male, 444 female]. We set screening criteria to disallow access to anyone who had completed any previous study in this line of work or whose browser was incapable of playing the video (full details are provided in SI Materials and Methods).
After consenting to the study, participants were warned of the potentially graphic nature of the video they would watch (in studies 1, 3, 4, and the follow-up experiments) and reminded that they could terminate the study at any time by closing their browser. After completing the dependent measures, participants indicated their age, ethnicity, and sex. Finally, we included attention filters that allowed for self-exclusion, asked if participants experienced any technical difficulties, and (in studies 3 and 4) asked if they were eligible to serve on a jury in the United States. Across these four studies, 271 (10.2%) unique participants who completed the survey were excluded for having seen the video in the study before, failing the attention filters, reporting technical difficulties, or not being jury-eligible (SI Materials and Methods). Of those participants who were not excluded, one participant in study 2 failed to answer all measures of intentionality, and one participant in study 3 and in study 4 failed to answer the willful, deliberate, and premeditated measure; these participants were not included in the analysis of the measures they failed to complete, which accounts for the different degrees of freedom reported in the results.
Statistical Results.
Levene's test indicated unequal variances on (i) the perceived time and continuous intent measures in study 1, so degrees of freedom were adjusted down from 487; (ii) the perceived time measure and intentionality composite in study 2, so degrees of freedom were adjusted down from 395; and (iii) all three dependent measures in study 4, so degrees of freedom were adjusted down from 902.
Study 1.
Procedure.
Participants watched surveillance footage from an incident outside a convenience store. They read that, after seeing the clip once to get a feel for it, they would see it again and answer a few questions about the events that unfolded and the shooter’s intent.
We presented the slow motion version of the video at a speed 2.25 times slower than the regular speed version of the video. This rate is similar to the rate at which the video in the John Lewis trial was slowed. As noted in the court’s denial of appeal, the defense counsel estimated that the crucial 2 s of the tape (i.e., from the time the defendant recognized the officer at the door to the time he fired) took about 4 to 6 s in the slow motion videotape (footnote 3 in ref. 1) (Movies S1 and S2). (Warning: The video contains graphic imagery that some viewers may find disturbing.)
After their first viewing, participants read instructions that directed their attention to specific parts of the scene and received factual information about the outcome of the incident (that the victim died as a result of the gunshot). This information was included to mimic closely the judgment context of a criminal trial, in which jurors would have known that the victim died.
Participants then watched the video two more times at the speed determined by their condition, and then responded to the following questions (each on a separate page; response options appear in parentheses):
-
i)
Did the person with the gun shoot with the intention to kill the victim? (Yes or No)
-
ii)
In determining whether the person is guilty of homicide, it is important to determine if an action was performed with the willful, deliberate, and premeditated intent to kill. To what extent would you say the person’s actions during this incident satisfy those three criteria? (Responses were given on a slider scale with end points labeled “Not at all” to “Completely,” and were coded on a 0–100 scale.)
-
iii)
How much time did it feel like the person with the gun had to assess the situation before he fired? (Responses were given on a slider scale with end points labeled “Almost no time at all” to “Quite a lot of time,” and were coded on a 0–100 scale.)
Jury bootstrap analysis.
We generated separate randomly selected “juries” from each of the two experimental conditions. For each jury, 12 participants from the condition were selected at random, without replacement. This process was repeated 1,000 times, replacing all participants between each jury selection. We then used these 12 participants’ responses to determine how the jury would have voted.
This method is nearly equivalent in the limit (as the number of juries approaches infinity) to using the probability mass function for a binomial distribution. Using this method, the probability of a “yes” vote is 0.773 in the regular speed condition, meaning that in 1,000 juries, ≈46 juries will have 12 of 12 members vote “yes.” The probability of a “yes” vote is 0.862 in the slow motion condition, meaning that in 1,000 juries, ≈168 juries will have 12 of 12 members vote “yes.”
The differences between the bootstrap analysis and the calculation are due to a combination of random sampling and sampling the juries without replacement. The binomial distribution considers each draw to be independent (and has a fixed probability), whereas when sampling the juries from the data, the selection of a jury member who will vote “yes” decreases the probability that a subsequently selected juror will also vote “yes.”
Study 2.
Pilot.
We conducted a pilot test using this paradigm with just regular speed and slow motion conditions. Results were comparable to the main study. A full description of the method is provided in SI Materials and Methods, and a summary of results is provided in Table S3.
Procedure.
Participants viewed a video of a helmet-to-helmet collision from an NFL game. The clip featured NFL linebacker, James Harrison, tackling a player from the opposing team in such a way that their helmets were the first points of contact. Whereas it is clear that the action of tackling is intentional, it is unclear whether Harrison intended to strike the other player’s helmet with his own, which was the focus of our questioning. Helmet-to-helmet contact has been a topic of concern in the NFL. This concern has led to rule changes that restrict such contact, made such tackles subject to review by league officials following the game, and imposed monetary fines on players who are judged to have engaged in intentional helmet-to-helmet contact.
We used the video clip from the original live broadcast footage of the game, but we did not include sound or any graphics from the broadcast superimposed on the screen. The clip was edited in a way to direct participants’ attention to the focal action: The periphery of the screen was subtly dimmed partway through the clip, giving the impression of a brighter circle around the focal action. We presented the slow motion version of the video at a speed two times slower than the regular speed version of the video, a rate that is roughly equivalent to typical slow motion replays in football broadcasts. For the paused version, we inserted a pause approximately halfway between the beginning of the video and the time when Harrison’s helmet hit the other player’s. The paused image remained on the screen for 3 s, such that the total length of the paused clip was equivalent to the total length of the slow motion clip.
Participants watched the video once, “just to get a feel for it,” and then were instructed to focus particularly on the helmet-to-helmet contact. After two additional viewings, participants responded to the following five questions, presented in randomized order:
-
i)
To what extent was the player trying to hit the other player in the helmet?
-
ii)
To what extent did the player plan to hit the other player in the helmet?
-
iii)
To what extent did the player have the ability to avoid hitting the other player in the helmet?
-
iv)
To what extent was this action intentional?
-
v)
To what extent was the player’s action willful, deliberate, and premeditated?
All responses were made on separate slider scales labeled “Not at all” to “Very much,” and were coded on a 0–100 scale. The five measures of intentionality were highly correlated (Cronbach’s α = 0.95). The complete correlation matrix is provided in Table S1.
Following the intentionality questions, participants answered the following question: How much time did it feel like the player had to assess the situation before making contact with the other player? (Responses were given on a slider scale with end points labeled “Almost no time at all” to “Quite a lot of time,” and were coded on a 0–100 scale.)
Study 3.
Participants were randomly assigned to one of four conditions in a 2 (video speed: regular vs. slow motion) × 2 (time salient: yes vs. no) between-participants design. All imagined they were members of a jury in a case where the defendant shot and killed a store clerk during an armed robbery. They were told that the prosecution and defense both agreed that the defendant killed the victim while committing a felony (in this case, a robbery) and acting with malice, but disagreed on whether the killing constituted first-degree murder or second-degree murder. They were provided with legal definitions of how to make that distinction (full details are provided in SI Materials and Methods), and then watched the video used in study 1 at the speed determined by their experimental condition.
After their first viewing, participants read instructions that reiterated their charge as jurors. In addition to the instructions given to all participants, those participants in the time salient conditions read the following: “As you will see from the digital time stamp displayed on the video, approximately three seconds elapsed from the time that the defendant let go of the store clerk’s arm until the time he fired. The sequence of the defendant’s actions in that time is relevant to the issue of whether the defendant possessed the intent to kill.” After their second viewing, participants were reminded of their task, with time salience participants receiving an additional instruction to keep in mind that the crucial portion of the defendant’s actions encompassed approximately 3 s.
After their third viewing, all participants responded to the following question: After letting go of the store clerk’s arm, how many seconds did the person with the gun actually have (in real time) to assess the situation before he fired? (Responses were given on a slider scale labeled “Time in Seconds,” ranging from 0.0 to 10.0, and were measured to one decimal place.)
Participants then answered (on separate pages) the amount of time it felt like the person had to assess the situation before he fired and the extent to which his actions were performed with the willful, deliberate, and premeditated intent to kill (from study 1).
The final measure directly assessed the relevant legal question of whether the action should be considered first-degree or second-degree murder. The question was modeled after Commonwealth of Pennsylvania jury instructions given in the Lewis case and ended with the following: “… In other words, first-degree murder is an intentional killing that is a willful, deliberate, and premeditated act. What is your verdict?” The responses available to participants were “first-degree murder” and “second-degree murder.”
Participants completed demographic questions similar to those questions in previous studies, and then indicated whether they were currently eligible to serve on a jury in the United States.
Study 4.
The methods used were nearly identical to study 3, but time salience was not manipulated (all participants were in the standard, time nonsalient condition). We added a both speeds condition to compare with the regular speed and slow motion conditions. Because participants in the regular speed and slow motion conditions of studies 1 and 3 each saw the video three times, we had participants in the both speeds condition first watch the regular speed version and then watch the slow motion version twice (to equate the total number of viewings across the conditions). Participants then completed the same dependent measures as in study 3, with the exception of the clock-time measure. In between these measures and the demographic questions, we assessed whether participants were clear on the facts of the case by having them read five separate statements and indicate whether the defense and prosecution agreed or disagreed on each issue based on the information they read in the case.
Follow-Up Studies.
Procedure.
We ran two replication studies that were similar to study 1 (with additional instructions to participants in the both speeds condition to avoid any confusion about seeing the video at different speeds) and one replication study that was identical to study 4 (omitting the slow motion-only condition). A full description of the method is provided in SI Materials and Methods, and a summary of results is provided in Table S4.
Meta-analyses.
We analyzed the dependent measures from study 4 and the three follow-up studies together using meta-analytic techniques (12). A full description of the method is provided in SI Materials and Methods, and a detailed summary of results is provided in Tables S5 and S6.
Mediation analyses.
We used the PROCESS procedure (13) to test mediation models with video condition as the independent variable, the continuous or dichotomous measure of intent as the dependent variable, and perceived time as the mediator. Across the four studies, we find robust evidence of mediation via perceived time on the continuous measure (mean effect size = 2.59, SE = 0.509, Z = 5.08, P < 0.0001) and on the dichotomous measure (mean effect size = 0.241, SE = 0.041, Z = 5.85, P < 0.0001). A full description of the method is provided in SI Materials and Methods, and a detailed summary of results is provided in Tables S7–S9.
Additional Analyses.
We used a variation on the jury bootstrapping simulation described in study 1 to illustrate the practical impact of slow motion video on judgments of intent across all of our studies (except study 2). A full description of the method and results is provided in SI Materials and Methods, and a detailed summary of results is provided in Tables S5, S6, S10, and S11.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank Jessica Bregant, Nicholas Epley, Reid Hastie, Emir Kamenica, Jonathan Masur, Joseph Noel, Joshua Plotkin, Adam Waytz, and Timothy Wilson for providing insightful feedback on this work; Morgan Britt, Michael Kardas, and David Levari for their assistance; and the Office of the Court Reporter (First Judicial District of Pennsylvania) for providing excerpts of the transcripts from the Lewis trial.
Footnotes
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.
Data deposition: The data files and referenced video clips are available at the Open Science Framework repository, https://osf.io/uzs3a/.
This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1603865113/-/DCSupplemental.
References
- 1.LEAGLE 2013 Com v. Jordan, 65 A.3d 318 (2013). Denial of appeal. Available at www.leagle.com/decision/In%20PACO%2020130424856/COM.%20v.%20JORDAN. Accessed July 8, 2016.
- 2.Passarella G. 2011 Pa. Justices Mull Showing Jury Slow-Motion Video of Murder. The Legal Intelligencer. Available at www.thelegalintelligencer.com/id=1202514497888&slreturn=1. Accessed July 8, 2016.
- 3.Block RA, Hancock PA, Zakay D. How cognitive load affects duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2010;134(3):330–343. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Tversky A, Kahneman D. Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science. 1974;185(4157):1124–1131. doi: 10.1126/science.185.4157.1124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Van Boven L, Loewenstein G. Social projection of transient drive states. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2003;29(9):1159–1168. doi: 10.1177/0146167203254597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Gilbert DT. Thinking lightly about others: Automatic components of the social inference process. In: Uleman JS, Bargh JH, editors. Unintended Thought. Guilford Press; New York: 1989. pp. 189–211. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Epley N, Keysar B, Van Boven L, Gilovich T. Perspective taking as egocentric anchoring and adjustment. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2004;87(3):327–339. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.3.327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Malle BF, Knobe J. The folk concept of intentionality. J Exp Soc Psychol. 1997;33(2):101–121. [Google Scholar]
- 9. Pennsylvania Bar Institute (2005) Pennsylvania Suggested Standard Criminal Jury Instructions (Pennsylvania Bar Institute, Mechanicsburg, PA), 2nd Ed, Vol. I-II, PBI No. 3470.
- 10.LEAGLE 1993 Com. v. Hindi, 631 A.2d 1341. Available at www.leagle.com/decision/1993598429PaSuper169_1585/COM.%20v.%20HINDI. Accessed July 8, 2016.
- 11.Morewedge CK, Preston J, Wegner DM. Timescale bias in the attribution of mind. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2007;93(1):1–11. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Lipsey MW, Wilson DB. Practical Meta-Analysis. Vol 49 Sage Publications; Thousand Oaks, CA: 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Hayes AF. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. Guilford; New York: 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Ames DL, Fiske ST. Perceived intent motivates people to magnify observed harms. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2015;112(12):3599–3605. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1501592112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Gray K, Wegner DM. The sting of intentional pain. Psychol Sci. 2008;19(12):1260–1262. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02208.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.