Skip to main content
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America logoLink to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
. 2016 Aug 10;113(34):E4935–E4936. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1609700113

Reply to Inbar: Contextual sensitivity helps explain the reproducibility gap between social and cognitive psychology

Jay J Van Bavel a,1, Peter Mende-Siedlecki a, William J Brady a, Diego A Reinero a
PMCID: PMC5003241  PMID: 27512048

We find that contextual sensitivity correlates with the reproducibility of 100 psychology studies from the Reproducibility Project (1). This relationship remains after adjusting for several methodological factors believed to account for reproducibility (e.g., sample size, effect size) and does not differ across subdisciplines (social vs. cognitive psychology). We also report a strong relationship between subdiscipline and context (1): social psychology studies are more likely to examine contextually sensitive topics than cognitive psychology studies. We conclude that context might play a role in reproducibility across (rather than within) subdisciplines.

Inbar (2) notes that the reproducibility rate of social psychology (28%) is lower than cognitive psychology (53%). He also reports that the association between context and reproducibility becomes nonsignificant when adjusting for subdiscipline. On this basis, Inbar argues that the relationship between context and reproducibility is spurious. This claim is conceptually misguided and overlooks a more important issue: contextual sensitivity helps explain the reproducibility gap between social and cognitive psychology.

The authors of the Reproducibility Project: Psychology (RP:P) argued that the lower reproducibility rate of social psychology is a result of weaker statistical power and effect sizes (3). But subdiscipline predicts reproducibility (P = 0.044) even after adjusting for methodological factors (e.g., sample size, effect size; see model 2 in ref. 1). However, subdiscipline no longer predicts reproducibility once context is added to the model (P = 0.453; see https://osf.io/cgur9/ for details) (Table 1). Although he does not report it, Inbar’s (2) own model reveals that the association between subdiscipline and reproducibility becomes nonsignificant when adjusting for context. In sum, context appears to explain the reproducibility gap better than these methodological factors.

Table 1.

Regression coefficients predicting reproducibility and variance explained in two steps of a hierarchical regression

Variable b SE P R2
Step 1 0.60
 Effect size, original study 2.62 1.77 0.200
 Surprisingness of original study −0.67 0.37 0.072
 Power of replication 8.34 4.18 0.046
 Surprisingness of replication study −2.04 0.51 <0.001
 Replication similarity 0.18 0.30 0.552
n of original study <0.001 <0.001 0.899
Subdiscipline −1.45 0.72 0.044
Step 2 0.62
 Effect size, original study 1.78 1.83 0.330
 Surprisingness of original study −0.67 0.39 0.087
 Power of replication 10.86 4.68 0.020
 Surprisingness of replication study −2.26 0.59 <0.001
 Replication similarity 0.23 0.30 0.448
n of original study <0.001 <0.001 0.929
Subdiscipline −0.66 0.88 0.453
 Contextual sensitivity −0.60 0.40 0.132

R2 values refer to Nagelkerke R2. Bold text indicates the relationship between subdiscipline (social vs. cognitive psychology) and reproducibility adjusting for other variables. Subdiscipline predicts reproducibility over-and-above several methods' variables (step 1), but becomes nonsignificant once contextual sensitivity is added to the model (step 2).

The fact that context and subdiscipline become nonsignificant when entered simultaneously in a regression demonstrates multicollinearity and makes it inappropriate to categorize one as a confound (4). Before claiming a spurious relationship, one needs to rule out a mediating relationship. It seems more likely that subdiscipline affects reproducibility through contextual sensitivity, rather than the other way around. Contextual sensitivity cannot cause a change in subdiscipline. Thus, Inbar’s (2) implied conceptual model appears to be backward.

There are other problems with prioritizing subdiscipline. First, no coding scheme for subdiscipline was reported in the RP:P (3). It is therefore impossible to know if the RP:P coders used context as a heuristic for categorizing studies as social vs. cognitive. Second, the binary coding scheme imposes a false dichotomy between social and cognitive psychology that cannot account for fields like social cognition (5). These coding limitations might explain why several studies appear to have been miscoded in the RP:P [as we noted in our paper (1)].

Our analysis suggests that different rates of reproducibility between social and cognitive psychology partially stem from differences in contextual sensitivity. This will come as little surprise to social psychologists: The notion that human psychology is shaped by the social context has been the central premise of the field for nearly a century (6). We expect that same principle applies across the social sciences.

Footnotes

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  • 1.Van Bavel JJ, Mende-Siedlecki P, Brady WJ, Reinero DA. Contextual sensitivity in scientific reproducibility. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2016;113(23):6454–6459. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1521897113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Inbar Y. Association between contextual dependence and replicability in psychology may be spurious. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2016;113:E4933–E4934. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1608676113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Open Science Collaboration PSYCHOLOGY. Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science. 2015;349(6251):aac4716. doi: 10.1126/science.aac4716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Cohen J, Cohen P, West SG, Aiken LS. Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Routledge; New York: 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Fiske ST, Taylor SE. Social Cognition: From Brains to Culture. Sage; London: 2013. 2nd Ed. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Lewin K. 1936. Principles of Topological Psychology (McGraw-Hill, New York) trans Heider F and Heider G.

Articles from Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America are provided here courtesy of National Academy of Sciences

RESOURCES