Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2016 Aug 30;5:e17089. doi: 10.7554/eLife.17089

Grid-cell representations in mental simulation

Jacob LS Bellmund 1,2,*, Lorena Deuker 1,3, Tobias Navarro Schröder 1,2, Christian F Doeller 1,2,*
Editor: Timothy EJ Behrens4
PMCID: PMC5005038  PMID: 27572056

Abstract

Anticipating the future is a key motif of the brain, possibly supported by mental simulation of upcoming events. Rodent single-cell recordings suggest the ability of spatially tuned cells to represent subsequent locations. Grid-like representations have been observed in the human entorhinal cortex during virtual and imagined navigation. However, hitherto it remains unknown if grid-like representations contribute to mental simulation in the absence of imagined movement. Participants imagined directions between building locations in a large-scale virtual-reality city while undergoing fMRI without re-exposure to the environment. Using multi-voxel pattern analysis, we provide evidence for representations of absolute imagined direction at a resolution of 30° in the parahippocampal gyrus, consistent with the head-direction system. Furthermore, we capitalize on the six-fold rotational symmetry of grid-cell firing to demonstrate a 60° periodic pattern-similarity structure in the entorhinal cortex. Our findings imply a role of the entorhinal grid-system in mental simulation and future thinking beyond spatial navigation.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.17089.001

Research Organism: Human

eLife digest

Recordings of brain activity in moving rats have found neurons that fire when the rat is at specific locations. These neurons are known as grid cells because their activity produces a grid-like pattern. A separate group of neurons, called head direction cells, represents the rat’s facing direction. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies that have tracked brain activity in humans as they navigate virtual environments have found similar grid-like and direction-related responses. A recent study showed grid-like responses even if the people being studied just imagined moving around an arena while lying still. Theoretical work suggests that spatially tuned cells might generally be important for our ability to imagine and simulate future events. However, it is not clear whether these location- and direction-responsive cells are active when people do not visualize themselves moving.

Bellmund et al. used fMRI to track brain activity in volunteers as they imagined different views in a virtual reality city. Before the fMRI experiment, the volunteers completed extensive training where they learned the layout of the city and the names of its buildings. Then, during the fMRI experiment, the volunteers had to imagine themselves standing in front of certain buildings and facing different directions. Crucially, they did not imagine themselves moving between these buildings.

By using representational similarity analysis, which compares patterns of brain activity, Bellmund et al. could distinguish between the directions the volunteers were imagining. Activity patterns in the parahippocampal gyrus (a brain region known to be important for navigation) were more similar when participants were imagining similar directions.

The fMRI results also show grid-like responses in a brain area called entorhinal cortex, which is known to contain grid cells. While participants were imagining, this region exhibited activity patterns with a six-fold symmetry, as Bellmund et al. predicted from the characteristic firing patterns of grid cells.

The findings presented by Bellmund et al. provide evidence that suggests that grid cells are involved in planning how to navigate, and so support previous theoretical assumptions. The computations of these cells might contribute to other kinds of thinking too, such as remembering the past or imagining future events.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.17089.002

Introduction

Anticipation of the future is a central adaptive function of the brain and enables adequate decision-making and planning. Simulating or imagining future events and scenarios relies on a network of brain regions known to be involved in episodic memory, navigation and prediction (Buckner, 2010; Byrne et al., 2007; Hassabis and Maguire, 2007; Hasselmo, 2009; Schacter et al., 2012). For instance, before leaving your favorite cafe, you may picture the scenery in front of the cafe in your mind’s eye to determine whether to take a left or a right turn to get home. To accomplish this you have to recall both the location of the cafe as well as the direction you are facing when leaving the building.

Electrophysiological recordings in freely moving rodents have demonstrated that positional information during navigation is represented by place cells in the hippocampus (O’Keefe and Dostrovsky, 1971) and grid cells in entorhinal cortex (Hafting et al., 2005). Place cells typically exhibit one firing field (O’Keefe and Dostrovsky, 1971), while grid cells are characterized by multiple firing fields arranged in a regular hexagonal pattern tessellating the entire environment (Hafting et al., 2005). Complementarily, directional information is carried by head direction cells, which increase their firing rate as a function of the animal’s directional heading irrespective of its location (Taube et al., 1990; Taube, 2007). Intracranial recordings in patients exploring virtual-reality (VR) environments demonstrated the existence of place and grid cells in the human hippocampus and entorhinal cortex, respectively (Ekstrom et al., 2003; Jacobs et al., 2010, 2013). A 60° directional periodicity of BOLD-signal modulations in the entorhinal cortex during virtual navigation indicates that grid-like entorhinal signals can also be detected with fMRI (Doeller et al., 2010; Kunz et al., 2015; Horner et al., 2016).

Notably, place cell activity can also represent locations other than the one currently occupied by the animal as illustrated by activation sequences corresponding to upcoming trajectories during rest periods (Dragoi and Tonegawa, 2011). Intriguingly, these ‘preplay’ sequences preferentially represent paths leading up to motivationally relevant locations (Ólafsdóttir et al., 2015). These observations support the notion that prospective coding of hippocampal place cells relates to the well-established role of the human hippocampus in mental simulation and imagination (Buckner, 2010; Byrne et al., 2007; Hassabis and Maguire, 2007; Hasselmo, 2009; Schacter et al., 2012). Akin to firing rate increases of neurons in the human medial temporal lobe specific to the content of imagination (Kreiman et al., 2000), firing patterns of spatially tuned cells might be reinstated to imagine the view from a certain location during mental simulation (Bird et al., 2012; Byrne et al., 2007; Hasselmo, 2009). Prospective coding properties of grid cells (De Almeida et al., 2012; Kropff et al., 2015) and recent evidence for spatial coherence of grid with place cell activity during replay (Ólafsdóttir et al., 2016) further suggest a similar involvement of the entorhinal grid system in future anticipation and prediction. This is in line with the observation of grid-like representations during imagined movement through an environment (Horner et al., 2016). However, hitherto it remains unknown if grid-like representations support mental simulation independent of imagined movement, which could suggest a more general role of grid cell computations in navigational planning, future anticipation and cognition.

Results

We combined fMRI with multi-voxel pattern analysis and VR to investigate whether the entorhinal grid system contributes to the imagination of directions from stationary viewpoints (Figure 1a,b). After extensive navigation training (see Materials and methods and Figure 1—figure supplement 1), participants were asked to imagine directions between pairs of buildings in ‘Donderstown’, a large-scale realistic VR city (http://www.doellerlab.com/donderstown/). In a carefully counterbalanced design, we probed the fine-grained representations of twelve equally spaced directions (see Materials and methods and Figure 1—figure supplement 2). Imagined directions had to be indicated (Figure 1b) and participants successfully performed this task (mean error 33.68° ± 19.09° SD; Figure 1c). Behavioral performance in the direction-imagination task was highly correlated with navigation success (r = 0.85, p<0.001) and the accuracy of direction estimates (r = 0.94, p<0.001) during training and performance in a post-scan map test (r = 0.95, p<0.001), indicating successful translation of the acquired representation of Donderstown to the imagination task (Figure 1—figure supplement 3).

Figure 1. Direction-imagination task.

(a) Twelve evenly spaced directions were sampled using 18 buildings distributed regularly across Donderstown. We sampled each direction (indicated by black arrows) from different start locations (yellow circles), which dissociated the directions from visual features of imagined views (Figure 1—figure supplement 2), and employed a counterbalancing regime ensuring equal sampling of directions and start locations throughout the experiment (see Materials and methods). Buildings marked with a green circle served as target locations only. Importantly, the regular arrangement of building locations did not correspond to the street layout and was not revealed to participants, who experienced Donderstown only from a first-person perspective (see also Figure 1—figure supplement 1d). (b) Trials began with a cue indicating start (top building name) and target (bottom building name) location and thereby defining the relevant direction (black arrow). During an imagination period the screen was black and participants were instructed to imagine the view they would encounter when standing in front of the start building facing the direction of the target building. An auditory signal terminated the imagination period and participants indicated the imagined direction (red arrow) in a sparse VR environment, followed by a confidence judgment. Performance was measured as the absolute angular difference between the correct and the indicated direction (red arc). Note that only the bottom row of images was presented to participants, top row for illustration only. (c) Circular histogram of average absolute angular difference between correct and indicated directions across participants (mean error 33.68° ± 19.09° SD).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.17089.003

Figure 1—source data 1. Average absolute angular errors.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.17089.004

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Overview of behavioral training.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(a) To familiarize themselves with the controls of the computer game and the layout of the city, participants explored Donderstown for 10 min and searched for a set of landmarks irrelevant for the direction imagination task. This exploration phase was omitted in the second training session. The black circle and arrow on the map in the top panel indicate the participants’ position and orientation when first encountering Donderstown. (b) Subsequently, participants learned the names of 18 task-relevant buildings (top) to criterion. Knowledge of the building names was assessed in test blocks during which participants had to select the building belonging to the presented name from a display of three buildings by pressing one of three buttons (bottom). (c) For the remainder of the session, participants were trained on the building locations in Donderstown. Bottom row shows the trial structure as presented to the participants, top row for illustration only. Participants were instructed to navigate to the building whose name was presented on the screen. Once the building was located, participants encoded the position and were then asked to estimate the direction to the following target building. Performance was measured as the number of buildings located during the training session and the absolute angular difference between the estimated direction and the correct direction as defined by the current location and the new target building. (d) Overview of Donderstown highlighting the task-relevant buildings, which largely differed in features salient from the first-person perspective such as size, shape and rotation with respect to the hexagonal building layout (red arrows), which makes an influence of the regular arrangement of their entrances on participants’ cognitive representation of the city unlikely.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Sampling of directions in the imagination task.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

(af) From each of the six start locations (yellow circles) ten directions were sampled. Directions (black arrows) were defined based on the angle of the vector connecting the start and the target locations (green circles). Screenshots show view from Donderstown corresponding to direction indicated by dashed arrow. Note that start locations could also be goal locations. The building combinations used in the direction imagination task were carefully counterbalanced so that in a task block of 24 trials each direction was sampled twice, each start building served as a start location four times and each building combination did not occur more than twice throughout the experiment (see Materials and methods). Trials sampling directions using buildings located on the same street (purple in b and c) were subject to an additional control analysis (see Materials and methods).

Figure 1—figure supplement 3. Accuracy of direction judgments during imagination task is related to behavioral performance during training and a post-scan map test.

Figure 1—figure supplement 3.

(a) Across subjects, the accuracy of direction judgments in the direction imagination task (π minus the mean angular difference between the correct and indicated directions in radians) correlated significantly with navigation success indexed by the number of buildings found during training. (b) Accuracy of direction judgments during the training sessions also correlated highly with performance during the imagination task. (c) Additionally, performance during the imagination task was correlated with z-scored accuracy in the post-scan map test. All correlations remained significant when controlling for variability in the time spent navigating the VR city during training using partial correlations (partial correlations coefficients r>0.84, all p<0.001). Correlations were also significant when calculated between the accuracy of direction judgments and training performance measures separately for the two training sessions (all correlations coefficients r>0.80, all p<0.001).

The contribution of spatial representations to imagination was assessed using representational similarity analysis (Kriegeskorte and Kievit, 2013), which compares activation patterns across voxels to estimate neural similarity. In line with the suggested role for grid cell computations in vector navigation (Bush et al., 2015), we expected the grid-cell system to be involved in determining the vector comprising the direction and distance from the start to the target building in our task. Our approach focused on the direction to the target to track putative grid-cell representations during imagination by systematically comparing neural similarity of imagined directions with varying angular differences (see Materials and methods). We predicted that an involvement of the grid system in mental simulation should be reflected in a 60° periodic pattern-similarity structure in the entorhinal cortex, consistent with the hexagonal firing properties of grid cells (Hafting et al., 2005) and the hexadirectional fMRI signal in entorhinal cortex observed during virtual navigation (Doeller et al., 2010; Horner et al., 2016; Kunz et al., 2015). It is important to note that we did not rely on the building layout as an absolute reference frame in our analyses, but rather assessed pattern similarity based on the relative angle between the directions sampled in a trial pair, see below.

In a first step, we ascertained that absolute directional representations are detectable with our novel imagination task. We expected increased neural similarity during imagination of similar directions, consistent with previous findings of absolute directional coding during navigation in parahippocampal cortex (Doeller et al., 2010) and two recent studies reporting directional representations during imagination in a local reference frame in the retrosplenial complex (Marchette et al., 2014) and coarse representations of directions to a goal in the entorhinal/subicular region (Chadwick et al., 2015). However, it remains unclear whether global spatial representations are involved in human imagination in the absence of visual input.

Here, we compared pattern similarity during imagination of directions in pairs of trials sampling similar directions (angular difference ≤ 30°) to pairs of trials sampling dissimilar directions (Figure 2a) in brain regions representing facing direction (Baumann and Mattingley, 2010; Chadwick et al., 2015; Marchette et al., 2014; Vass and Epstein, 2016, 2013). We observed the predicted one-fold symmetric pattern-similarity structure in a cluster of voxels in the left posterior parahippocampal gyrus (T23 = 4.82, p = 0.024, FWE-corrected for multiple comparisons using small volume correction; Figure 2b,c; see Materials and methods). Increased pattern similarity for similar directions was not due to trial comparisons with identical building combinations (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Further, this effect was not driven by the specific locations used to sample directions or the distances between these locations in Donderstown (Figure 2—figure supplement 2, see Materials and methods).

Figure 2. Absolute directional coding in posterior parahippocampal gyrus.

(a) Analysis logic of the one-fold directional analysis for three example trials. High pattern similarity was predicted for pairs of trials sampling similar directions with a maximum angular difference of 30° (red) compared to trials sampling directions 60° or more apart (blue). Note that for illustration purposes the predicted similarity matrix is shown for comparisons across conditions, not single trials. (b) Searchlight results show a significant cluster of voxels in the posterior parahippocampal gyrus (peak voxel MNI coordinates: 34 -34 -10; T23 = 4.82, p = 0.024 corrected for multiple comparisons using small-volume correction) with higher pattern similarity for trials sampling similar directions compared to trials sampling dissimilar directions. Results are shown on the structural MNI template. For display purposes, the statistical map is thresholded at p<0.001 uncorrected. (c) Sagittal and coronal view of the mask used to correct for multiple comparisons (see Materials and methods) displayed on the MNI template brain.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.17089.008

Figure 2—source data 1. Searchlight results for absolute directional coding analysis.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.17089.009

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Increased pattern similarity for similar directions after excluding trial pairs sampling a direction with the same combination of buildings.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

To control for the possibility of increased pattern similarity in the parahippocampal gyrus for similar direction pairs being due to the imagination of identical scenes, we excluded trial pairs from the analysis in which the same combination of start and target building was used. This revealed a significant cluster of voxels (peak voxel MNI coordinates: 34 -36 -10, T23 = 4.54, p = 0.042 corrected for multiple comparisons using small volume correction) very similar to the one observed in the main analysis (see Figure 2b). For display purposes, the statistical map is thresholded at p<0.001 uncorrected.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Absolute directional coding during imagination is independent of locations and distances in Donderstown.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

(a) Pattern similarity difference between trial pairs sampling similar and dissimilar directions in the peak voxel of the main absolute directional coding analysis (Figure 2) after exclusion of comparisons with trials using the same start (bar I) and the same target location (bar II). Both T23 > 3.00, both p<0.007. (b) We considered the distances between start and target locations in a trial pair by controlling for three distance measures, which differed between pairs of trials sampling similar and dissimilar directions. The distance measures are illustrated based on two example trials. For each trial pair, we calculated (I) the mean length of the vectors connecting start and target location, (II) the difference in length of the two vectors and (III) the mean length of the vectors connecting all four relevant locations of a pair. In separate GLMs we used the distance measures as predictors of pairwise pattern similarity. (c) Mean pattern similarity difference in peak voxel of cluster from main absolute directional coding analysis (Figure 2) between trial pairs sampling similar directions and pairs sampling dissimilar directions computed on the residuals of the GLMs. With this approach we controlled for pattern similarity due to (I) the average distance from start to target location in a trial pair, (II) the difference in distance from start to target location in a trial pair and (III) the average distance between all four buildings in a trial pair (see Materials and methods). All T23 > 3.60, all p<0.001. Bars in a and c show mean pattern similarity difference with error bars reflecting S.E.M., dashed line shows mean pattern similarity difference in peak voxel from main analysis.

Having verified that we can detect directional representations in our novel imagination paradigm, we tested, in a next step, whether activation patterns during imagination follow a six-fold rotational symmetry, akin to the six-fold symmetric firing pattern of grid cells (Hafting et al., 2005) and the six-fold modulation of entorhinal fMRI signals during virtual (Doeller et al., 2010; Kunz et al., 2015) and imagined (Horner et al., 2016) navigation in humans. The rationale underlying our analysis is that activation patterns during directional imagination should exhibit the highest neural similarity for directions that are (multiples of) 60° apart from each other (see Figure 3a–d and Figure 3—figure supplement 1 for details of analysis logic). Because grid cells are most abundant in the medial entorhinal cortex in rodents (Hafting et al., 2005), we predicted the effect to be present in posterior medial entorhinal cortex (pmEC), the likely homologue region of the rodent medial entorhinal cortex in the human brain (Navarro Schröder et al., 2015) (Figure 3e).

Figure 3. Grid-like representations during imagination.

(a) Six-fold symmetric firing fields of a hypothetical grid cell (dark blue dotted circles) superimposed on an aerial view of Donderstown. Black arrows indicate the twelve sampled directions; light and dark shading highlights directions (multiples of) 60° apart. For illustration purposes, the grid orientation is aligned to the sampled directions; see Figure 3—figure supplement 1 for a different example. (b) The firing rate of the hypothetical response of the grid-cell system as a function of direction, showing a 60° modulation. Shading displays sampling of directions and red and blue markers indicate the two conditions. Note that the oscillatory firing pattern is sampled at the same phase in the 0° modulo 60° condition, but at different phases in the 30° modulo 60° condition. (c) Based on this, we expected a 60° modulation of fMRI pattern similarity values when comparing trial pairs based on the angular difference of their sampled directions. Red and blue shading illustrates the two conditions. (d) Specifically, we predicted higher pattern similarity for trial pairs with a remainder of 0° (0° modulo 60° condition, red) compared to trial pairs with a remainder of 30° (30° modulo 60° condition, blue), when dividing the angular difference of the pair’s sampling directions by 60°. Note that for illustration purposes the predicted similarity matrix is shown for comparisons across conditions, not single trials. (e) ROI mask for posterior medial entorhinal cortex (pmEC) from previous report (Navarro Schröder et al., 2015). (f) Pattern similarity difference (mean and S.E.M.) between the two conditions. The left pmEC exhibited a significant 60° modulation of pattern similarity. No significant differences in pattern similarity were observed in the right pmEC (T23 = 0.57, p = 0.58).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.17089.012

Figure 3—source data 1. Pattern similarity difference between 0° modulo 60° and 30° modulo 60° condition in left and right posterior medial entorhinal cortex.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.17089.013

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Rationale of 60° modulation analysis.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

(a) Six-fold symmetric firing fields of a hypothetical grid cell (dark blue dotted circles) superimposed on a top-down view of Donderstown. Black arrows indicate the twelve sampled directions. Light and dark shading indicates directions (multiples of) 60° apart. (b) The firing rate of the hypothetical grid cell as a function of sampling direction exhibits a 60° modulation. Shading shows sampled directions with red and blue markers illustrating the two conditions. Note that the oscillatory firing pattern is sampled at the same phase in the 0° modulo 60° condition, but at different phases in the 30° modulo 60° condition. (c) Based on this, we expected increased pattern similarity when comparing trial pairs from the 0° modulo 60° condition to trial pairs from the 30° modulo 60° condition. The difference between the conditions is smaller than in Figure 3c due to the different sampling of directions with respect to the grid orientation.

Figure 3—figure supplement 2. Pattern similarity difference between 0° modulo 60° condition and 30° modulo 60° condition in pmEC and alEC.

Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

(a) ROI mask for anterior lateral entorhinal cortex (alEC) based on our previous report (Navarro Schröder et al., 2015). (b) Pattern similarity did not differ between the 0° modulo 60° and the 30° modulo 60° condition in alEC (T23 = 0.04, p = 0.97 and T23 = − 0.08, p = 0.94 for left and right alEC, respectively). (c) Colored markers show pattern similarity difference for each participant in pmEC and alEC. Boxplots indicate 25th and 75th percentile with the middle line representing median pattern similarity difference across participants. Whiskers extend to most extreme data points not considered outliers. Data points defined as outliers (values more than 1.5 times the interquartile range above the 75th percentile or more than 1.5 times the interquartile range below the 25th percentile) are represented by square markers.

Figure 3—figure supplement 3. Signal quality in the entorhinal cortex.

Figure 3—figure supplement 3.

(a) To assess signal quality in pmEC and alEC, we computed the temporal signal-to-noise ratio (tSNR, see Materials and methods). A repeated-measures ANOVA revealed neither a main effect of region (F1,23 = 0.60, p = 0.448) or hemisphere (F1,23 = 0.00, p = 0.953) nor an interaction between the factors region and hemisphere (F1,23 = 0.97, p = 0.336). Bars represent mean and S.E.M. (b) Left slice shows the mean functional scan averaged across participants. The mean functional images from the four fMRI runs were averaged for each participant before averaging the resulting mean images across participants. Right slice shows the corresponding section of the MNI template. Note that for some participants the edge of the superior parietal lobe was outside the field of view.

Figure 3—figure supplement 4. 60° periodicity of pattern similarity is consistent across angular differences only in left posterior medial entorhinal cortex.

Figure 3—figure supplement 4.

(a) Pattern similarity was analyzed based on the angular differences of the directions sampled in a trial pair. High pattern similarity was predicted for pairs in the 0° modulo 60° condition (red) in contrast to pairs in the 30° modulo 60° condition (blue). Filled pattern and color indicates angular differences and corresponds to bars in (be), which visualize average pattern similarity for all possible angular differences for exploratory purposes in the entorhinal ROIs. Note the consistent 60° periodicity of the pattern similarity profile across angular differences in left pmEC. A statistical test was performed on the within-subject difference between the two conditions and was significant in left pmEC only (T23 = 2.37, p = 0.027, for all other ROIs, p>0.5). Error bars for each angular difference would not reflect the statistical test performed and are therefore omitted.

Figure 3—figure supplement 5. Pattern similarity structure across pair-wise comparisons of trials for entorhinal ROIs.

Figure 3—figure supplement 5.

(ad) Matrices show the pair-wise correlations between voxel patterns in the subregions of the entorhinal cortex (a: left pmEC; b: right pmEC; c: left alEC; d: right alEC) across all possible trial comparisons averaged across participants. Arrows signal sampled directions in a given pair of trials. Colorbar indicates size of the correlations in panels (ad).

Figure 3—figure supplement 6. 60° modulation of pattern similarity during imagination is not driven by specifics of task design.

Figure 3—figure supplement 6.

(a) Pattern similarity difference between the 0° modulo 60° and the 30° modulo 60° condition remained significant in left pmEC after controlling for specifics of the design. Bars show the mean pattern similarity difference after excluding trial pairs with (I) the same start location (T23 = 2.39, p = 0.025), (II) the same target location (T23 = 2.57, p = 0.017), (III) the same combination of start and target location (T23 = 2.45, p = 0.022), (IV) pairs from the same run (T23 = 2.08, p = 0.049) and (V) pairs with target locations in the inner ring of buildings (T23 = 5.29, p<0.001; see Materials and methods). This excludes potential influences of imagining the same start or same target location, the same combination of start and target location and temporal auto-correlations on the effect. Error bars indicate S.E.M., dashed line shows mean pattern similarity difference in left pmEC from main analysis (Figure 3b).

Figure 3—figure supplement 7. 60° modulation of pattern similarity during imagination after controlling for distance measures.

Figure 3—figure supplement 7.

We controlled for the distances between start and target locations in a trial pair using three distance measures, which differed between the 0° modulo 60° and the 30° modulo 60° condition (see Materials and methods). The distance measures are illustrated in Figure 2—figure supplement 2b. In separate GLMs we used the distance measures as predictors of pairwise pattern similarity and computed the mean pattern similarity difference between the 0° modulo 60° and the 30° modulo 60° condition on the residuals of these GLMs. With this approach we controlled for pattern similarity due to (I) the average distance from start to target location in a trial pair, (II) the difference in distance from start to target location in a trial pair and (III) the average distance between all four buildings in a trial pair (see Materials and methods). All T23 > 2.36, all p<0.03, error bars indicate S.E.M., dashed line shows mean pattern similarity difference in left pmEC from main analysis (Figure 3b). The effect also remained significant when using binary (high vs. low) distance predictors (all T23 > 2.44, all p<0.03).

Figure 3—figure supplement 8. Behavioral performance for trial pairs in the 0° modulo 60° and the 30° modulo 60° condition.

Figure 3—figure supplement 8.

Error values were multiplied for the two trials of each pair and averaged for the two conditions. Boxplots indicate 25th and 75th percentile with the middle line representing median combined error across participants. Whiskers extend to most extreme data points not considered outliers. Data points connected by lines show combined errors for all subjects in the two conditions (no difference between conditions, T23 = 1.24, p = 0.227). Data points defined as outliers (values more than 1.5 times the interquartile range above the 75th percentile or more than 1.5 times the interquartile range below the 25th percentile) are represented by square markers. Note that the participant shown here as an outlier performed above chance and that there were no outliers in our main pattern similarity analysis (see Figure 3—figure supplement 2).

Figure 3—figure supplement 9. No evidence for representations of cardinal directions or 90° modulation of pattern similarity in the entorhinal cortex.

Figure 3—figure supplement 9.

(a) Structure of pattern similarity values used to test for coding of cardinal directions in the entorhinal cortex. If entorhinal cortex activity would be sensitive to cardinal directions, high pattern similarity would be expected for pairs of trials sampling cardinal directions in comparison with trials sampling other directions. Note that for illustration purposes the tested similarity matrix is shown for comparisons across conditions, not single trials. (b) Pattern similarity did not differ between pairs of trials sampling cardinal directions and pairs of trials sampling other directions in pmEC (differences scores: left: T23 = -0.136, p = 0.893; right: T23 = −0.449, p = 0.658) or alEC (differences scores: left: T23 = 0.266, p = 0.793; right: T23 = 0.530, p = 0.601). (c) To corroborate the specificity of the 60° modulation of pattern similarity in pmEC, we examined a possible, yet biologically implausible four-fold symmetry in entorhinal pattern similarity values. We tested for increased pattern similarity for pairs of trials sampling directions 90° or multiples thereof apart (0° modulo 90° against 30° or 60° modulo 90°), using the same analysis logic as for the main analysis (0° modulo 60° against 30° modulo 60°) but now with a 90° periodicity. (d) Pattern similarity values did not differ between these conditions in pmEC (differences scores: left: T23 = −0.48, p = 0.637; right: T23 = −1.81, p = 0.084) or alEC (differences scores: left: T23 = −0.83, p = 0.413; right: T23 = 0.50, p = 0.618). Bars in (b and d) represent mean and S.E.M.

Figure 3—figure supplement 10. Searchlight analysis for 60° modulation of pattern similarity during imagination.

Figure 3—figure supplement 10.

For each search sphere, the difference in pattern similarity for trial pairs in the 0° modulo 60° condition and the 30° modulo 60° condition was calculated. (a) One entorhinal cluster exhibited increased pattern similarity for the 0° modulo 60° condition (peak MNI coordinates: −18 − 20 −22; T23 = 4.04, p = 0.046, FWE-corrected for multiple comparisons in bilateral pmEC and alEC using small volume correction) and is shown together with the masks of pmEC and alEC, outlined in dark and light green, respectively. This result confirms our finding from the ROI analysis. Statistical map is thresholded at p < 0.005 uncorrected and masked to show entorhinal cortex only. (b) Further exploratory whole-brain searchlight analysis showed greater pattern similarity for the 0° modulo 60° condition in the lingual gyrus (MNI coordinates: 8 −62 6, T23 = 6.11), precuneus (−20 −60 4, T23 = 5.35), cuneal cortex (10 −74 26, T23 = 5.26), lateral occipital cortex (−42 −66 6, T23 = 5.06), occipital pole (−32 −90 10, T23 = 4.93), supplementary motor cortex (0 −14 56, T23 = 4.54), central opercular cortex (−50 −16 12, T23 = 4.46), occipital fusiform gyrus (38 −74 −18, T23 = 4.36), angular gyrus (54 −56 26, T23 = 4.34), superior parietal lobule (30 −52 58, T23 = 4.12) and supramarginal gyrus (−64 −50 22, T23 = 4.09). Slices show the statistical image at 1 mm resolution at a significance threshold of p<0.001. Labels were obtained from the Harvard-Oxford Structural Cortical Structural Atlas available in FSL. For each region peak voxel MNI coordinates and statistics are reported.

We observed pattern similarity increases with a 60° periodicity in the left pmEC (T23 = 2.37, p = 0.027; one-tailed test, Bonferroni corrected for test in both hemispheres; Cohen’s d = 0.48; Figure 3f and Figure 3—figure supplement 2c). The effect was further confirmed using permutation-based significance testing (pseudo T23= 2.89, p = 0.008; see Materials and methods). A control analysis showed that the effect was not present in the anterior lateral entorhinal cortex (p>0.9; Figure 3—figure supplement 2; see Figure 3—figure supplement 3 for information on signal quality in the entorhinal cortex), the human homologue of lateral EC, which does not contain grid cells (Hafting et al., 2005). The 60° periodicity in left pmEC was consistent across all angular differences (Figure 3—figure supplement 4 and Figure 3—figure supplement 5) and the effect was not driven by the specifics of our design and the VR town used. Specifically, the effect remained significant after excluding combinations of trial pairs (Figure 3—figure supplement 6) with the same start (T23 = 2.39, p = 0.025) or target location (T23 = 2.57, p = 0.017), the same combination of start and target location (T23 = 2.45, p = 0.022) and comparisons from the same task block (T23 = 2.08, p = 0.049). Further control analyses demonstrated that the effect was independent of the mean distance between start and target locations in a trial pair (T23 = 2.37, p = 0.027; Figure 3—figure supplement 7), the difference of this distance within a pair (T23 = 4.32, p<0.001) and the mean distance between all four buildings in a given trial pair (T23 = 2.37, p = 0.027). Behavioral performance did not differ between the conditions (T23 = 1.24, p = 0.227, Figure 3—figure supplement 8).

Furthermore, the effect was specific to a 60° modulation of pattern similarity values and there was no evidence for coding of cardinal directions in the entorhinal cortex (Figure 3—figure supplement 9; see also Materials and methods). Results of a whole-brain searchlight analysis confirmed the 60° periodicity of pattern similarity increases in pmEC observed in the ROI analysis (T23 = 4.04, p = 0.046, FWE-corrected for multiple comparisons using small volume correction; Figure 3—figure supplement 10). A similar pattern similarity structure was observed in regions in parietal and visual cortices (Figure 3—figure supplement 10), which might reflect reactivation of egocentric and visual representations associated with imagined directions, possibly modulated by entorhinal representations in line with a model of mental imagery (Bird et al., 2012; Byrne et al., 2007). Future research will need to investigate these putative interactions in more detail.

Discussion

In sum, we report two important findings: Firstly, pattern similarity values in the parahippocampal gyrus exhibited a one-fold symmetry congruent with fine-grained representations of imagined facing direction, reflecting the role of this brain region - which has been implicated in spatial processing in the absence of visual input (Wolbers et al., 2011) - in representing the directional aspect of the imagined views. An alternative explanation of this effect through visual similarity of the imagined views appears unlikely due to the complex nature of the task in which each direction was sampled from multiple locations in our large-scale environment (Figure 1—figure supplement 2) and buildings served as cues for a wider range of sampling directions. Therefore, our finding provides the first evidence for fine-grained coding of absolute direction at an unprecedented angular resolution of 30°, consistent with the characteristics of the head direction system in rodents (Taube et al., 1990; Taube, 2007), and constitutes a three-fold increase in resolution of the directional representations observed in humans compared to previous studies (Marchette et al., 2014; Chadwick et al., 2015; Baumann and Mattingley, 2010; Vass and Epstein, 2013, 2016; Shine et al., 2016). Secondly, the structure of pattern similarity in entorhinal cortex was characterized by a six-fold rotational symmetry akin to the firing properties of grid cells (Hafting et al., 2005). Our findings provide evidence for an involvement of grid-like representations in mental simulation in the absence of imagined movement.

Crucially, participants imagined directions from stationary viewpoints in a realistic, large-scale virtual city and were not re-exposed to the virtual town during the imagination task. Therefore, our findings provide novel evidence, complementary to a recent report (Horner et al., 2016) showing evidence for grid-like entorhinal processing during imagined movement through a simple virtual arena. Furthermore, in contrast, we investigated spatial processing in a large-scale, urban environment (Stokes et al., 2015) and, moreover focused on multi-voxel patterns. In particular, we demonstrate that this novel analysis approach, which does not rely on the estimation of the orientation of the hexadirectional signal in entorhinal cortex in an independent data set (Doeller et al., 2010; Kunz et al., 2015; Horner et al., 2016; Constantinescu et al., 2016), is sensitive to grid-like entorhinal signals by capitalizing on the six-fold symmetry of grid cell firing patterns. Contrary to the previously employed approach relying on the estimation of the orientation of the hexadirectional signal for each participant (Doeller et al., 2010; Kunz et al., 2015; Horner et al., 2016; Constantinescu et al., 2016), the individual grid orientation is not approximated using the multivariate analysis. Yet, the grid orientation might influence the strength of the grid-like entorhinal signal observed in a given participant because the sampled directions might be more or less aligned with this individual’s grid orientation (Figure 3a–c and Figure 3—figure supplement 1 for illustration). This needs to be taken into consideration when aiming to relate grid-like signals to behavior. However, only the multivariate approach enabled us to investigate the six-fold rotational symmetry in our large-scale environment, in which a continuous sampling of directions as required for the estimation of the orientation of the hexadirectional signal would not have been feasible. This parsimonious approach might prove valuable for future studies investigating the role of grid-like signals in human cognition, in particular in studies with children (Bullens et al., 2010) or older participants (Schuck et al., 2015) and in clinical settings (Hartley et al., 2007; Maguire et al., 2001), where time for data acquisition is typically limited and could for instance help to further understand the putative link between the entorhinal grid system and Alzheimer’s disease (Kunz et al., 2015).

On a theoretical level, our findings are consistent with accounts of imagination positing medial-temporal-lobe involvement in the reactivation and recombination of prior experiences (Buckner, 2010; Byrne et al., 2007; Hassabis et al., 2007; Hassabis and Maguire, 2007; Hasselmo, 2009; Schacter et al., 2012). The hippocampal formation and grid cells in particular have been implicated in path integration (Hafting et al., 2005; Wolbers et al., 2007), for which computing a homing vector based on translations from a given starting point is central (Vickerstaff and Cheung, 2010). Notably, the grid system is well-suited to also perform the inverse operation of calculating relative vectors between known positions in the service of navigational planning (Bush et al., 2015). Hence, it is plausible that the grid-cell system contributes to the calculation of vectors between start and target location during imagination (Bird et al., 2012; Bush et al., 2015; Hasselmo, 2009; Horner et al., 2016), while the head direction system (Taube, 2007; Taube et al., 1990) processes the absolute direction between the two locations (Bird et al., 2012; Byrne et al., 2007; Hasselmo, 2009) in our task. Our findings suggest an involvement of the entorhinal grid system in calculating vectors to target locations during navigational planning, in line with a theoretical account of vector navigation (Bush et al., 2015).

Functional neuroimaging can measure the firing pattern of specific cell types only indirectly (Logothetis, 2008). However, intracranial recordings in patients exploring virtual-reality environments demonstrated the existence of place (Ekstrom et al., 2003; Jacobs et al., 2010) and grid (Jacobs et al., 2013) cells in the human hippocampus and entorhinal cortex, respectively. Importantly, our results are in line with single-cell recordings in rodents that suggest a possible contribution of spatially tuned cells to future anticipation via place cell preplay of upcoming trajectories (Dragoi and Tonegawa, 2011) and preferential preplay of firing sequences of paths leading to motivationally relevant locations (Ólafsdóttir et al., 2015). Prospective coding properties of grid cells (De Almeida et al., 2012; Kropff et al., 2015) and recent evidence for grid cell replay (Ólafsdóttir et al., 2016) further suggest a similar involvement of the entorhinal grid system in future anticipation and prediction. By translating these ideas to human imagination, during which content-specific firing rate increases of neurons in the human medial temporal lobe have been observed (Kreiman et al., 2000), it is conceivable that spatially tuned cells provide the machinery for the flexible recombination of spatial and mnemonic details necessary for the construction of mental simulations (Bird et al., 2012; Brown et al., 2016; Buckner, 2010; Byrne et al., 2007; Eichenbaum and Cohen, 2014; Hassabis et al., 2007; Hassabis and Maguire, 2007; Hasselmo, 2009; Schacter et al., 2012) and the representation of conceptual knowledge (Constantinescu et al., 2016).

In concert with the recent report of grid-like processing in the entorhinal cortex during imagined navigation (Horner et al., 2016) our findings provide a substantial advancement for the field. Importantly, grid-like entorhinal signals during imagined navigation were observed with a similar orientation as during actual navigation through the VR environment (Horner et al., 2016). This finding strengthens our interpretation of the six-fold symmetric pattern similarity structure in the entorhinal cortex during imagination of directions from stationary viewpoints observed in this study as reflecting computations of the entorhinal grid system operating similarly in our realistic large-scale VR city as during navigation in smaller and simpler environments typically used in electrophysiological recording studies in rodents (Hafting et al., 2005) or fMRI experiments in humans (Doeller et al., 2010; Horner et al., 2016; Kunz et al., 2015). Importantly, the interpretation of our results as a global grid signal coding for space beyond boundaries and obstacles is in line with the report of a global grid pattern emerging with experience in rodents exploring an environment divided into two connected compartments (Carpenter et al., 2015).

In conclusion, we show involvement of both absolute directional parahippocampal and grid-like entorhinal signals in imagination, which provides important evidence for these representations in the absence of sensory input or imagined movement. This might suggest a more fundamental role of spatial computations in the grid-cell system during mental simulation and possibly other forms of prospective coding and future thinking in the service of goal-directed decision-making (Bird et al., 2012; Buckner, 2010; Byrne et al., 2007).

Materials and methods

Participants

32 male participants were recruited via the online recruitment system of Radboud University Nijmegen. The study was approved by the local ethics committee (CMO Arnhem-Nijmegen, the Netherlands) and participants gave their written informed consent prior to the experiment. All participants had normal or corrected to normal vision and were compensated for their participation. Eight participants were excluded from the analysis because of motion sickness during the VR navigation training (n = 2), technical problems with the MRI scanner (n = 1) or chance-level performance during the direction imagination task (n = 5; median absolute angular error not significantly smaller than 90° as determined by Wilcoxon signed-rank test). Thus, 24 participants (age range 18–29 years, mean age 24.52 years, standard deviation 2.91 years) entered the analysis.

Procedure

The experiment was conducted on two days and consisted of an extensive behavioral training in our virtual-reality city ‘Donderstown’ (http://www.doellerlab.com/donderstown/) and a direction imagination task in the MRI scanner. Additionally, participants’ ability to locate the task-relevant buildings on a map was assessed. On the first day, participants learned the names and locations of 18 buildings in Donderstown in a two-hour training session in the virtual city. Before the fMRI session on the following day, participants were trained for an additional hour.

VR city

Donderstown provides a large-scale, realistic urban environment comprising a complex layout of streets, squares and parks. Inspired by the street-map of a medievally founded German town, the layout of Donderstown resembles the irregular outline of a typical European city with curved roads. The streets of the city were not named. Participants were instructed to use the centrally positioned radio tower for orientation along with the mountain range surrounding the city. The skybox was rendered at infinity and the sun as the major cue of directional lighting was placed at the zenith above the center of the city. Thus, shadows could not serve as directional cues during any part of the experiment. 3D models of buildings for the city were created using 3ds Max (version 2014, www.autodesk.com/products/3ds-max/). The city was built and presented using the Unreal Development Kit (version 2013–07, http://udn.epicgames.com/Three/WebHome.html).

Crucially, the task-relevant buildings were placed at specific locations in Donderstown allowing us to sample twelve evenly spaced directions in the imagination task (Figure 1a) and to dissociate the start location from the sampled direction. Specifically, the entrance doors of these 18 buildings were approximately located at the vertices of equilateral triangles, which were arranged in a hexagon. Note that participants never saw a top-down view of the city, but only experienced the city from a first-person view and that the layout of streets and landmarks did not correspond to the regular pattern of buildings relevant for the direction imagination task. Therefore it is unlikely that participants were aware of the regular layout of task-relevant buildings (see Figure 1—figure supplement 1d).

Behavioral training

Familiarization phase

On the first day of training, participants were familiarized with Donderstown and the controls of the computer game by freely navigating the city for ten minutes in search for a set of landmarks spread across the city (see Figure 1—figure supplement 1a). Participants first encountered Donderstown at a position close to the city’s center facing the radio tower located in the center of the city. This position and orientation is indicated in Figure 1—figure supplement 1a and also served as the start location during the delivery task described below. For the familiarization phase, seven landmarks (e.g. a statue) distributed across the city were selected and participants were instructed to look for these landmarks while exploring the city. Donderstown was presented on a computer screen at a resolution of 1024 × 768 pixels. Participants experienced Donderstown exclusively from a first-person perspective and navigated using the mouse and keyboard to control the player movements.

Association task

Afterwards, participants learned the names of the 18 task-relevant buildings, with names randomly assigned to the buildings for each participant. We used names of unknown US-based companies consisting of two or three words. Participants were asked to remember the building-name associations during learning blocks, in which the image of a building was presented together with its name using Presentation (version 16.4, www.neurobs.com/presentation; Figure 1—figure supplement 1b). Learning blocks were followed by blocks of test trials during which participants had to select the building corresponding to a presented name from a set of three building images within 4 s. After each test block, participants received feedback about their performance in that block (percentage of correct answers). After reaching a learning criterion of three error-free test blocks (mean 7.00 ± 3.27 SD blocks to criterion), participants were trained in Donderstown for the rest of the 2-h training session as described below.

Delivery task

In order to learn the building locations in the city, participants completed a delivery task (average length of navigation training 80.32 min ± 8.00 SD min), in which the goal was to find the target building whose name was presented on the screen (Figure 1—figure supplement 1c). To familiarize participants with the building locations, participants were guided to the buildings by following pylons until having visited each building three times. After this initial phase, participants searched for the buildings themselves, which were targeted in random order. If a participant could not find a building during the first 36 trials of this phase, a help button made a widely visible red arrow appear in the sky above the target building. We determined navigation performance by the number of buildings found after the end of the guided navigation phase. Upon finding a building, the player position was fixed for 7 s to allow time for the encoding of the building’s location. Navigation was restricted to rotations to look around during this time period.

Direction training

To emphasize the relevance of the directional relationships between buildings, participants were prompted to estimate the direction to the next target building after having found a building (Figure 1—figure supplement 1c). The name of the new target building was presented and participants were instructed to rotate until facing the new target. Performance on this task was measured as the absolute angular difference between the correct and the indicated direction. No explicit feedback on the direction was given, but participants were instructed to verify their direction estimates by searching for the target building in the indicated direction.

During the second training session, which took part approximately 24 hr after the first, participants completed a shorter version (1 hr) of the training procedure described above. First, participants were again trained on the building names using the association task described above. The learning criterion was reduced to one error-free test block (mean 1.23 ± 0.47 SD blocks to criterion). Again, participants spent the remainder of the training session performing the delivery task (average length of navigation training 40.44 min ± 5.09 SD min). The setup for the navigation training on the second day was identical to the previous day with the only difference that participants were guided to each building by pylons only once. Again, we measured navigational success as the number of buildings found after the guided navigation period as well as the accuracy of the direction judgments obtained.

To test the relationship of performance during training to behavioral performance in the MRI direction imagination task, the number of buildings found was aggregated across both training sessions and the accuracy of direction judgments was averaged across all estimates. The close relationship between training performance and direction estimates in the imagination task (Figure 1—figure supplement 3) was not due to the minor differences in the length of the navigation training between participants. Following the second training session, participants were instructed about the direction imagination task for the subsequent fMRI session and performed three practice trials in order to ensure all aspects of the task were understood correctly. All training sessions were conducted in a behavioral laboratory.

fMRI direction imagination task

While undergoing fMRI, participants performed a novel direction imagination task. The task consisted of four blocks of 24 trials. In each trial, participants were cued with the names of two buildings and were instructed to close their eyes and imagine themselves standing in front of the start building facing the direction of the target building. To successfully imagine views from variable locations defined by the relative positions of buildings without re-exposure to Donderstown participants had to make use of their allocentric representation of the city. The angle of the vector between start and target building defined the correct direction in each trial with respect to the coordinates of Donderstown and defined the trial conditions for the representational similarity analysis (RSA) (Kriegeskorte and Kievit, 2013). The two building names were presented simultaneously for two seconds.

The layout of the buildings relevant for this task enabled the controlled sampling of twelve directions with an angular difference of 30° between adjacent directions (Figure 1a). Using the inner six buildings as start buildings for the direction imagination task allowed for the dissociation of start location and sampled direction. From each of the six start buildings ten of the twelve directions were sampled (Figure 1—figure supplement 2). Since the 60 unique building combinations oversampled the directions aligned with the main axes of the hexagon of buildings, we randomly excluded two trials sampling each of these directions with the constraint to exclude two building combinations per start location. The remaining 48 unique building combinations sampled each direction four times and were used for two task blocks of 24 trials. For the two blocks, trials were randomly drawn from the available combinations of start and target building with the constraints that all directions were sampled twice and each start building served as a start location four times in a block. The order of trials was randomized within blocks. For the second set of two blocks we followed the same randomization procedure.

Participants were instructed to imagine the direction between the two buildings with their eyes closed for 10 s during which the screen was black. Then they were prompted by a tone to open their eyes and indicate the imagined direction. For this behavioral response, a sparse VR environment was used (Figure 1b). The city was replaced by a grass plain with the only remaining cues for orientation being the radio tower, which had been located in the center of Donderstown during navigation and the mountain range surrounding the city. One of the mountains on the south-east side of the city had a summit cross on it, which had also been visible during training. In each trial, the start building was also presented on the grass plain at the position corresponding to the building’s location in Donderstown. The participant was spawned at the imagined location in front of the start building with the task to indicate the previously imagined direction to the target building by rotating until a red cross presented centrally on the screen was pointing in this direction. In order to facilitate the estimation of directions to targets behind the start building, the building was turned transparent after 2 s in every trial. The accuracy of the behavioral response was measured as the angular distance between the correct direction defined by the start and target building and the response direction (Figure 1b and c). The magnitude of errors observed in the behavioral responses was consistent with performance levels reported in previous studies using similar tasks (Schinazi et al., 2013; Zhang et al., 2014).

Each trial ended with a confidence rating on a four-point scale and was followed by an inter-trial-interval (randomized length of 1.8, 3.6 or 5.4 s). Three trials within a block served as catch trials in which a snapshot from the VR city was presented after the imagination period, but before the onset of the response screen. This snapshot was taken from the imagined location in front of the start building and participants had to indicate within 2 s after the end of the imagination phase whether it was facing the direction of the target building or a direction different by 90°. In 50% of the catch trials, the snapshot showed the correct view. We introduced these trials in order to emphasize the relevance of vivid imagination of the relevant views. Participants responded significantly above chance for the catch trials in which they responded in time (T23 = 2.70, p = 0.013).

Map test

After completion of the direction imagination task, participants’ memory for the building locations was tested. In each trial, the name of a building was presented together with a map showing the outline of the city’s streets. The objective of the task was to move a cursor to the location where the building was thought to be positioned in the city. In order to facilitate orientation on the map, the landmarks participants searched during the familiarization phase were represented by symbols on the map. Participants were instructed about the identity of these symbols. Every building was to be located once. Participants’ accuracy was defined as the Euclidean distance between the correct building location on the map and the location indicated by the participant.

MRI data acquisition

Functional images were acquired at a 3 T Siemens Trio MRI system (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) using a 3D EPI sequence with an isotropic voxel size of 2 mm and a TR of 1800 ms (TE = 25 ms, 64 slices, distance factor 50%, flip angle 15°, field of view 224 × 224 × 128 mm). Four runs corresponding to the four task blocks were collected. The duration of each run depended on the time required by the participant to complete the task block. On average, a run lasted 11.76 min (± 1.63 SD min). T1-weighted structural images were acquired with an MPRAGE sequence (TR = 2300 ms, TE = 3.03 ms, flip angle 8°, in-plane resolution = 256 × 256 mm, voxel size 1 mm isotropic).

MRI data analysis

Preprocessing

Preprocessing of the functional images of each of the four runs was carried out using the FSL toolbox (version 5.0.4, http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/). The images were high-pass filtered (cut-off: 100 s) and motion-corrected. For the RSA each run was linearly registered to the participant’s structural scan, which served as a common reference space for functional scans from all four blocks and was downsampled to an isotropic voxel size of 2 mm to correspond to the voxel size of the functional scans. A gray matter segmentation was carried out to obtain a gray matter mask for use in the analysis described below.

Representational similarity analysis: absolute directional coding

RSA (Kriegeskorte and Kievit, 2013) was implemented using custom Matlab (version R2014a, www.mathworks.com/matlab/) scripts. For the preprocessed time series of every voxel, a general linear model (GLM) was calculated with the motion parameters obtained during preprocessing as predictors. The residuals of these GLMs (i.e. what could not be explained by the motion parameters) were then used for representational similarity analysis.

For this analysis, three volumes corresponding to the peak of the BOLD response of the second half of the imagination time window were averaged for each trial. Specifically, the analysis time window consisted of the volume during which the 10 s imagination period ended and the two following volumes. This time period was chosen due to the complex nature of the task, which required the participant to first retrieve the respective buildings and their locations before being able to imagine the direction between them. To rule out a potential influence of brain activity corresponding to the onset of the response screen on the analyzed volumes we conducted a whole-brain searchlight analysis (with the parameters described in detail below), which compared trial pairs with identical visual input at the onset of the response screen to pairs where this visual input differed. Since the initial response screen always showed the start building, trial pairs with the same start location would exhibit increased pattern similarity in brain areas processing visual information if the volumes analyzed contained brain activity corresponding to visual processing. No clusters showed this effect at a significance threshold of p<0.001 uncorrected and a cluster extent threshold of 10 voxels.

We performed the searchlight analyses using search spheres with a diameter of 7 voxels (1.4 mm). This approach iteratively calculates pattern similarity values for search spheres centered on the current voxel. All within brain voxels could be the center of a sphere, but within each sphere, the analysis was restricted to gray matter voxels by using the mask obtained from the structural image segmentation during preprocessing. Only search spheres including more than 30 voxels were analyzed. Pattern similarity was calculated as the Pearson correlation coefficient across all voxels within a sphere, separately for all possible pairs of trials. To test the hypothesis of increased representational similarity for similar directions, we averaged Fisher z-transformed correlation coefficients for two different conditions: Pairs of trials probing the same direction or directions with an angular difference of 30° were contrasted against pairs of trials sampling directions 60° or more apart (Figure 2a). We chose to investigate representations of absolute directions with a resolution of 30° based on the firing properties of head direction cells (Taube, 2007), which might underlie an absolute directional code in our task, and to achieve sufficient statistical power. The difference in mean pattern similarity between these conditions was calculated for each search sphere on the single subject level.

The resulting difference images were normalized (non-linear registration) to the MNI template (2 mm resolution) using FSL and smoothed using a Gaussian kernel of 4 mm full width at half maximum. On the group level, significance was assessed by performing a one-sample T-test in SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) on the difference images obtained at the subject level (cluster extent threshold 10 voxels). The resulting statistical map is displayed at 0.5 mm resolution on the MNI template in Figure 2b. Small volume correction was performed based on our a priori hypothesis about the brain regions involved in the representation of facing direction in humans as reported in previous studies (Baumann and Mattingley, 2010; Chadwick et al., 2015; Doeller et al., 2010; Marchette et al., 2014; Vass and Epstein, 2013). Therefore, a mask (Figure 2c) containing the retrosplenial complex, parahippocampal gyrus, entorhinal cortex and the subiculum was constructed from thresholded masks of these regions. For the subiculum and the parahippocampal gyrus masks were selected from the Jülich Histological atlas (Eickhoff et al., 2007) and the Harvard-Oxford Cortical Structural atlas (Desikan et al., 2006) distributed with FSL, respectively. The retrosplenial complex was defined based on peak voxel coordinates from a previous study in which participants imagined directions in a local reference frame (Marchette et al., 2014). We created a spherical mask with a radius of 6 mm around the peak voxel in the right retrosplenial complex (MNI coordinates: 14 -58 11) and added a second sphere with the same radius in the same location in the left hemisphere. Masks for the posterior medial and anterior lateral parts of the entorhinal cortex were based on a previous study (Navarro Schröder et al., 2015). The mask used for small volume correction is displayed at 1 mm resolution in Figure 2c.

Absolute directional coding: Control analyses

Excluding trial pairs with the same start and target location

The parahippocampus is known to be involved in the processing of visual scenes (Epstein, 2008). Therefore, increased pattern similarity for similar directions could potentially be driven by trial pairs in which participants were cued to imagine the identical spatial scene. We performed a searchlight analysis as described above and excluded trial pairs from the analysis, which sampled a direction with the identical combination of buildings. On the group level, we observed an effect in a very similar location in the left posterior parahippocampal gyrus (Figure 2—figure supplement 1).

Exclusion of trial pairs with the same start location

To control for the possibility that increased pattern similarity was due to pairs of trials sampling directions from the same start location, we excluded these comparisons from the analysis. Specifically, we extracted the matrix containing all pair-wise correlation coefficients from the sphere centered on the peak voxel of the cluster observed in our main analysis (Figure 2). To this end we registered the peak voxel coordinate to each subject’s structural image (2 mm resolution), which again served as the common space for the analysis of the functional images, and computed the pair-wise correlations for all possible pairs of trials in the sphere centered on this voxel (same parameters as for main searchlight analysis). After excluding all comparisons of trial pairs sampling directions from the same start location, we computed the difference in mean pattern similarity between pairs sampling similar (angular differences ≤ 30°) and dissimilar (angular differences ≥ 60°) directions as in our main analysis. We still observed increased pattern similarity for pairs of trials sampling similar compared to dissimilar directions (T23 = 3.01, p = 0.006; Figure 2—figure supplement 2a, bar I).

Exclusion of trial pairs with the same target location

Following the same approach, we also excluded the possibility that increased pattern similarity for similar directions was driven by more frequent imagination of the same target buildings. As shown in Figure 1 — supplement 2, directions were sampled from the inner ring of buildings in Donderstown and mostly targeted locations towards the outside of the city. Therefore, target buildings could not be sampled from all directions. After excluding comparisons of trials targeting the same location in a second analysis, pattern similarity remained greater for comparisons of similar directions (T23 = 4.07, p<0.001; Figure 2—figure supplement 2a, bar II).

Rationale for controlling distance-related effects

In every trial, the direction to be imagined was defined as the angle of the vector between the start and target building. In this set of control analyses we additionally considered the Euclidean distance between the start and the target location. We adopted three different approaches to combine the distance information for a given trial pair and to obtain a distance matrix with the distance for each comparison (Figure 2—figure supplement 2b). To test whether distances differed between conditions we computed the same contrast as in our main analysis: For each subject, we calculated the difference between the mean distance of trial pairs sampling similar directions and the mean for pairs sampling dissimilar directions and tested these differences against 0 using a one-sample t-test. The results are described below. As described above, we obtained the pair-wise similarity matrix from the sphere centered on the peak voxel of the cluster from the main analysis (Figure 2). In each analysis, we then computed a GLM with pairwise distances as a continuous predictor and the pairwise Fisher z-transformed correlation coefficients as the criterion for each participant. The residuals of these GLMs (i.e. pattern similarity that could not be explained by the distance predictor) were then used to compute the pattern similarity differences between trial pairs sampling directions with a maximum angular difference of 30° and pairs sampling directions with an angular difference of 60° or more. After all analyses, pattern similarity remained significantly greater for trial pairs sampling similar directions compared to trial pairs sampling dissimilar directions. The details of these analyses are described in the following.

Controlling for mean distance of trial pairs

First, we averaged the Euclidean distances between start and target building in a trial pair. The mean Euclidean distance was greater for pairs sampling directions with a maximum angular difference of 30° (T23 = 8.30, p<0.001). By accounting for the mean length of the two vectors, we excluded a potential influence of imagined distance to the target location. Pattern similarity remained increased for trial pairs sampling similar directions (T23 = 3.90, p<0.001, Figure 2—figure supplement 2c, bar I).

Controlling for different distances within trial pairs

Second, we considered a modulation of pattern similarity based on the difference in distance between start and target location of the trials defining a pair. Trial pairs with angular differences of maximally 30° had more different distances (T23 = 58.23, p<0.001). After controlling for the difference in length of the two trial vectors in a pair, pattern similarity was still increased for pairs of trials sampling similar directions (T23 = 3.61, p<0.001, Figure 2—figure supplement 2c, bar II).

Controlling for the ‘neighborhood distance’

Lastly, we quantified a ‘neighborhood distance', which we defined as the mean of the Euclidean distances of the six vectors connecting all four buildings in a trial pair. This distance was low when both start and both target buildings were in the same part of Donderstown. These distances were smaller for pairs sampling similar directions (T23 = −77.51, p<0.001). After controlling for the fact that buildings used to sample similar directions were located more closely together in Donderstown, pattern similarity remained increased for pairs of trials sampling similar directions (T23 = 3.97, p<0.001, Figure 2—figure supplement 2c, bar III). This rules out the possibility that sampling in similar parts of Donderstown is responsible for increased pattern similarity for trial pairs sampling similar directions.

Visual similarity of imagined views

Based on the well-established role of the parahippocampal cortex in scene processing, increased pattern similarity for similar directions in this region could potentially be driven by increased visual similarity of the scenes imagined by the participants. Even though this appears unlikely due to the sampling of each direction from multiple locations (Figure 1—figure supplement 2) we compared the visual similarity of the scenes to be imagined based on a set of visual features (Milivojevic et al., 2015). For each trial, the view from the start position facing the direction of the target building was captured at a resolution of 1024 × 768 pixels. Note that participants did not see these images during the task, but were instructed to imagine the views. Five statistics were used to quantify visual features of each image: By computing a two-dimensional discrete Fast-Fourier-Transform we obtained magnitude and phase metrics of the images. Next, the red, green and blue values for each image were converted to CIELAB color space. This color space provides information about the luminance and two color opponent dimensions (red-green and yellow-blue) corresponding to the cone responses of the human retina. Each of the five image statistics was vectorized and Pearson correlations were computed to quantify visual similarity across pairs of images.

We compared visual similarity for pairs of trials sampling similar directions (treating angular differences of 30° or less as similar akin to the absolute directional coding analysis) from different start positions. For each participant, we performed a two-sample t-test on the Fisher z-transformed correlation coefficients of each image dimension. While we did not observe significant differences for the magnitude information or the color dimensions (all Bonferroni-adjusted p>0.2), phase and luminance information of imagined scenes were significantly less similar for comparisons of trials sampling similar directions compared to pairs sampling dissimilar directions in a subset of participants. Specifically, Fisher z-transformed correlations were significantly lower (corrected for 24 comparisons) for image pairs from similar direction trials for twelve participants when considering the phase information (minimum significant T3838 = −3.25; maximum significant T3838 = −4.42) and for six participants when considering luminance information (minimum significant T3838 = −3.11; maximum significant T3838 = −4.70). Based on these results it seems highly unlikely that differences in visual similarity of the scenes to be imagined drive the observed increased representational similarity of trial pairs sampling similar directions in the parahippocampal gyrus.

Representational similarity analysis: six-fold symmetry

In rodents, grid cells are typically found in the medial entorhinal cortex (Hafting et al., 2005). Recently, two reports suggested the posterior medial part of the human entorhinal cortex as the human homologue of the rodent medial entorhinal cortex based on local and global connectivity patterns (Maass et al., 2015; Navarro Schröder et al., 2015). Therefore, we expected an influence of grid-like representations on imagination only in the posterior medial entorhinal cortex (pmEC). The anterior lateral part of the entorhinal cortex (alEC) served as a control region. ROI masks for the left and right pmEC and alEC from our previous report (Navarro Schröder et al., 2015) were first registered to the MNI template at a resolution of 2 mm using FSL (1 mm resolution for display in Figure 3e and Figure 3—figure supplement 2a). For each participant, the masks were then warped to the anatomical scan with a resolution of 2 mm, which again served as the reference space for the different runs.

The analysis was performed on the same preprocessed and motion-corrected volumes as the one-fold symmetry analysis. Again, RSA was performed on the residuals of a GLM with the motion parameters as predictors. For each ROI, we calculated pattern similarity for all pairs of trials and defined two conditions to compare Fisher z-transformed Pearson correlations coefficients. We hypothesized that a possible influence of grid-cell representations on imagination should be reflected in a 60° periodicity of pmEC activity patterns (Figure 3a,b and Figure 3—figure supplement 1a,b) analogously to the 60° modulation of the amplitude of the BOLD signal observed during navigation (Doeller et al., 2010). Extending this previous method, we pursued a more parsimonious and more powerful multivariate approach, which does not rely on the univariate estimation of a putative grid orientation on an independent dataset (which is only feasible with exhaustive sampling of directions during navigation [Doeller et al., 2010]). Here, pattern similarity in pmEC was expected to be modulated by the angular difference of the directions sampled in a pair of trials (Figure 3c and Figure 3—figure supplement 1c). To test this hypothesis two conditions were defined based on the modulus 60° of the angular difference between the sampled directions in a trial pair (Figure 3d). This remainder of modulus 60° could either be 0° or 30° due to the regular sampling of directions in our paradigm. We calculated the difference in mean pattern similarity between the 0° modulo 60° and the 30° modulo 60° condition for each subject. On the group level, we tested for increased pattern similarity in the 0° modulo 60° condition by comparing these pattern similarity difference values against 0 using a one-tailed one-sample T-test. Reported p-values are Bonferroni corrected for separate comparisons in left and right pmEC. To visualize whether the significant difference observed between the conditions in pmEC reflected a consistent 60° modulation of pattern similarity across angular differences, we plotted pattern similarity as a function of angular difference between trials pairs (Figure 3—figure supplement 4).

Permutation-based significance testing

To further assess the increased pattern similarity for trials from the 0° modulo 60° and the 30° modulo 60° condition in left pmEC, we additionally performed a permutation-based non-parametric significance test. We computed the probability of the observed pattern similarity difference between the conditions under a permutation-based null distribution for each participant. Null distributions were obtained by shuffling the trial labels and analyzing the resulting correlation matrix for each of 10,000 permutations. The resulting p-value was converted to a z-statistic (allowing both positive and negative values). On the group level, we used a one-sample t-test (non-parametric with 10,000 permutations) to test these z-statistics against 0.

Grid-like entorhinal signals during imagination: Control analyses

As can be seen in a Figure 1a, the buildings relevant for the direction imagination task were distributed in a hexagonal pattern across Donderstown. A series of control analyses described in detail below suggests that the 60° modulation of pattern similarity in pmEC was not imposed by the layout of Donderstown and the specifics of the task design (Figure 3—figure supplement 6 and Figure 3—figure supplement 7).

Exclusion of trial pairs with the same start location

The number of comparisons of trials with the same start location was different between the 0° modulo 60° and the 30° modulo 60° condition. Thus, the 60° modulation of pattern similarity could have potentially been driven by the number of trial pairs in which the same start building had to be imagined. To account for this possibility, we excluded all pairs consisting of trials with the same start location in a first control analyses (Figure 3—figure supplement 6, bar I).

Exclusion of trial pairs with the same target location

With respect to the target locations, a similar aspect was considered. Due to the sampling of directions from the start locations near the center of Donderstown, buildings could only serve as targets for trials sampling a limited range of directions. Since trial pairs with the same target location were more likely to belong to the 0° modulo 60° condition, we excluded comparisons of trials with the same target location to rule out that the 60° modulation of pattern similarity in pmEC was driven by more frequent imagination of the same target building (Figure 3—figure supplement 6, bar II).

Exclusion of trial pairs with the same combination of start and target location

A third control analysis excluded pairs of trials with the same start and target building combination to ensure that imagination of identical scenes did not drive the effect (Figure 3—figure supplement 6, bar III).

Exclusion of trial pairs from the same MRI run

To rule out a potential influence of temporal autocorrelation, we restricted the analysis to pairs of trials from different task blocks. The effect also remained significant when excluding all comparisons of trials from the same block (Figure 3—figure supplement 6, bar IV).

Exclusion of trial pairs with target locations in inner ring of buildings

In some trials, directions were sampled using target buildings located on the inner ring of buildings (see Figure 1—figure supplement 2). Average angular error was lower for trials targeting a building on the inner compared to the outer ring (T23 = −5.29, p<0.001). Pairs in which both trials sampled directions using target locations on the inner ring made up 12.23% of comparisons in the 0° modulo 60° condition and did not occur in the 30° modulo 60° condition. When excluding these comparisons from the analysis, pattern similarity remained increased for the 0° modulo 60° condition compared to the 30° modulo 60° condition (Figure 3—figure supplement 6, bar V).

Rationale for controlling distance-related effects

In a second set of control analyses we additionally considered the Euclidean distance between the start and the target location as described above for the absolute directional coding effect. Again we adopted three different approaches to combine the distance information for a given trial pair (Figure 2—figure supplement 2a) and computed a pairwise distance matrix for each measure. We assessed whether a distance measure differed between the 0° modulo 60° and the 30° modulo 60° condition by testing the difference between the mean distance values of the two conditions against 0 using a one-sample t-test, analogous to our approach in the pattern similarity analysis. The results of these tests are reported in the following paragraphs. In each control analysis, we then computed a GLM with a continuous distance predictor and pairwise Fisher z-transformed correlation coefficients as the criterion. The residuals of these GLMs (i.e. pattern similarity that could not be explained by the distance predictor) were then used to compute the pattern similarity differences between the 0° modulo 60° and the 30° modulo 60° condition (Figure 3—figure supplement 7). After all analyses, pattern similarity remained significantly greater for trial pairs in the 0° modulo 60° condition in the left pmEC. The effect also remained significant when using binary (high vs. low) distance predictors (all T23 > 2.44, all p<0.03).

Controlling for mean distance of trial pairs

First, we averaged the lengths of the two vectors. The mean Euclidean distance was higher for pairs in the 0° modulo 60° condition (T23 = 5.25, p<0.001). By accounting for the mean length of the two vectors, we exclude a potential influence of imagined distance to the target location on pattern similarity (Figure 3—figure supplement 7, bar I).

Controlling for different distances within trial pairs

A second possibility could be a modulation of pattern similarity based on the difference in Euclidean distance of the trials defining a pair. Trial pairs from the 0° modulo 60° condition had more similar distances (T23 = −57.02, p<0.001). Potentially, pattern similarity could be increased for trials of similar length. Therefore, we used the difference in length of the two trial vectors in a pair as a predictor (Figure 3—figure supplement 7, bar II).

Controlling for the ‘neighborhood distance’

Lastly, we quantified the distance of the six vectors connecting all four buildings in a trial pair. This resulted in a ‘neighborhood distance', which was low when both start and both target buildings were in the same area of Donderstown. These distances were larger for pairs in the 0° modulo 60° condition (T23  = 54.20, p<0.001). We attempted to control for effects of imagining directions in specific parts of Donderstown with this analysis (Figure 3—figure supplement 7, bar III).

Representations of cardinal directions

To examine whether potential representations of the cardinal directions were detectable in the pattern similarity structure exhibited by the pmEC during the direction imagination task, we compared pattern similarity values of pairs where both trials sampled a cardinal direction against pairs where this was not the case (Figure 3—figure supplement 9a). The cardinal directions were defined based on the orientation of Donderstown as displayed for example in Figure 1a. Pattern similarity was not increased for cardinal direction pairs in either pmEC (left: T23 = −0.136, p = 0.893; right: T23 = −0.449, p = 0.658, Figure 3—figure supplement 9b) or alEC (left: T23 = 0.266, p = 0.793; right: T23 = 0.530, p = 0.601).

To ascertain that trial pairs sampling directions using buildings located on the same street running along the North-South axis of Donderstown (based on its orientation for example in Figure 1a), which could possibly bias participants’ representations of the city due to a number of streets roughly aligned with it, were not driving our effects, we excluded all comparisons of pairs in which both trials sampled directions along this street from the analysis (see Figure 1 —figure supplement 2). Pattern similarity was significantly greater for pairs of trials sampling similar directions compared to pairs with larger angular differences (T23 = 4.00, p<0.001) in the peak voxel of the parahippocampal cluster from the main absolute directional coding analysis (Figure 2), indicating that absolute directional coding was independent of pairs of trials sampling directions along the same street running along the North-South axis. Similarly, when examining the 60° modulation of pattern similarity values in left pmEC after excluding all trial pairs sampling along this street, the difference between the conditions remained significant (T23 = 2.40, p = 0.025).

A number of considerations further make an over-representation of the North-South axis due to directions sampled on a street running along this axis unlikely. Participants’ performance did not differ between trials sampling along the North-South axis compared to other directions. We did not observe differences in accuracy (mean ± standard deviation (SD) for average absolute angular errors of trials sampling North-South 35.9° ± 24.5° vs. 33.2° ± 18.5° for other directions; means not significantly different: T23 = 1.06, p = 0.300) or consistency (mean ± SD of individual standard deviations for absolute angular errors for North-South trials 31.2° ± 19.5° vs. 30.1° ± 13.5° for other directions; standard deviations not significantly different: T23 = 0.54, p = 0.596) of participants' direction estimates. Furthermore, participants were recruited from the student body of a Dutch university and were therefore most familiar with irregular European street layouts (unlike the rectangular grid network common in the US), in which the use of cardinal directions is less common because most streets are curved and do not follow the cardinal directions. Importantly, even if participants preferentially represented directions along the North-South axis, which is unlikely based on the reasons laid out above, the population response of entorhinal grid cells would still remain the most likely explanation for the observed 60° modulation of pattern similarity values in pmEC, which could not be explained by the presence of one cardinal axis.

Four-fold symmetry

To further corroborate the specificity of the 60° modulation observed in pmEC, we conducted a control analysis testing for a 90° modulation of pattern similarity values. A four-fold similarity pattern would be inconsistent with grid-cell like representations (Doeller et al., 2010; Kunz et al., 2015). We compared pattern similarity as function of angular difference for trial pairs with a 0° modulo 90° against trials pairs with 30° or 60° modulo 90° (Figure 3—figure supplement 9c). There was no significant 90° modulation of pattern similarity in either pmEC (left: T23 = −0.48, p = 0.637; right: T23 = −1.81, p = 0.084, Figure 3—figure supplement 9d) or alEC (left: T23 = −0.83, p = 0.413; right: T23 = 0.50, p = 0.618).

Estimation of signal-to-noise ratio in the entorhinal cortex

In order to compare signal quality in the entorhinal ROIs we calculated the temporal signal-to-noise ratio (tSNR). We quantified tSNR as the mean signal within a region divided by the standard deviation of this signal over time. A two-way repeated measures ANOVA with the factors region (pmEC vs. alEC) and hemisphere (left vs. right) revealed no significant differences in tSNR (no main effect of region (F1,23 = 0.60, p = 0.448), no main effect of hemisphere (F1,23 = 0.00, p = 0.953) and no interaction (F1,23 = 0.97, p = 0.336); Figure 3—figure supplement 3).

Whole-brain searchlight analyses

Our main analysis and subsequent control analyses described above focused on the head direction network and pmEC, respectively, based on our specific a priori hypotheses and previous studies (Baumann and Mattingley, 2010; Chadwick et al., 2015; Doeller et al., 2010; Marchette et al., 2014; Vass and Epstein, 2016, 2013). In particular, our pmEC ROI was motivated anatomically by the presence of grid cells in the rodent medial entorhinal cortex (Hafting et al., 2005) and by the functional identification of its human homologue region in our previous work (Navarro Schröder et al., 2015). Due to our complex novel imagination paradigm, set in a realistic large-scale VR city, we opted to further explore our data. We performed exploratory whole-brain searchlight analyses with the same parameters as reported above to test whether additional brain regions might display absolute directional coding and a 60° modulation in their pattern similarity structure, respectively. No brain regions other than the parahippocampal gyrus were observed for the absolute directional effect at a significance threshold of p<0.001 uncorrected and a voxel extent threshold of ten voxels. Results for the six-fold symmetry whole-brain searchlight analysis are shown in Figure 3—figure supplement 10. We did not perform searchlight analyses within pmEC or alEC due to the small size of these entorhinal subregions.

Exploration of behavioral data

We investigated whether a 60° modulation of absolute angular error values was also present in the behavioral data. We multiplied angular error values to combine performance for all pairs of trials and compared the resulting values as a function of angular difference of the directions sampled in a pair. Analogous to the 60° modulation analysis, we calculated the difference between combined error values in the 0° modulo 60° and the 30° modulo 60° condition for each participant on the first level and tested for a difference on the second level using a one-sample t-test. The combined error values were not different between the conditions (T23 = 1.24, p = 0.227, Figure 3—figure supplement 8).

We further explored the behavioral data obtained during the direction imagination task. To assess whether the distance between the start and target building of a trial was predictive of the angular error of that trial, we calculated Pearson correlations between the absolute angular error and the distance from start to target building for each participant. This relationship did not reach statistical significance in any of the participants (mean r = −0.08 ± 0.09 standard deviation, range −0.24–0.10; all Bonferroni-adjusted p>0.454).

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the European Research Council (ERC-StG RECONTEXT 261177) and the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO-Vidi 452-12-009). The authors would like to thank S Collin and B Milivojevic for comments on the manuscript and A Backus for helpful discussions.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • European Research Council ERC-StG RECONTEXT 261177 to Tobias Navarro Schröder, Christian F Doeller.

  • Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek NWO-Vidi 452-12-009 to Jacob LS Bellmund, Lorena Deuker, Christian F Doeller.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Author contributions

JLSB, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

LD, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data.

TNS, Conception and design, Acquisition of data.

CFD, Conception and design, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

Ethics

Human subjects: The study was approved by the local ethics committee (CMO Arnhem-Nijmegen, The Netherlands) and participants gave their written informed consent prior to the experiment.

References

  1. Baumann O, Mattingley JB. Medial parietal cortex encodes perceived heading direction in humans. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:12897–12901. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3077-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bird CM, Bisby JA, Burgess N. The hippocampus and spatial constraints on mental imagery. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2012;6:e17089. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Brown TI, Carr VA, LaRocque KF, Favila SE, Gordon AM, Bowles B, Bailenson JN, Wagner AD. Prospective representation of navigational goals in the human hippocampus. Science. 2016;352:1323–1326. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf0784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Buckner RL. The role of the hippocampus in prediction and imagination. Annual Review of Psychology. 2010;61:27–48. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bullens J, Nardini M, Doeller CF, Braddick O, Postma A, Burgess N. The role of landmarks and boundaries in the development of spatial memory. Developmental Science. 2010;13:170–180. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00870.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bush D, Barry C, Manson D, Burgess N. Using grid cells for navigation. Neuron. 2015;87:507–520. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.07.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Byrne P, Becker S, Burgess N. Remembering the past and imagining the future: a neural model of spatial memory and imagery. Psychological Review. 2007;114:340–375. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Carpenter F, Manson D, Jeffery K, Burgess N, Barry C. Grid cells form a global representation of connected environments. Current Biology. 2015;25:1176–1182. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.02.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Chadwick MJ, Jolly AE, Amos DP, Hassabis D, Spiers HJ. A goal direction signal in the human entorhinal/subicular region. Current Biology. 2015;25:87–92. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Constantinescu AO, O'Reilly JX, Behrens TE. Organizing conceptual knowledge in humans with a gridlike code. Science. 2016;352:1464–1468. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf0941. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. De Almeida L, Idiart M, Villavicencio A, Lisman J. Alternating predictive and short-term memory modes of entorhinal grid cells. Hippocampus. 2012;22:1647–1651. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Desikan RS, Ségonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, Blacker D, Buckner RL, Dale AM, Maguire RP, Hyman BT, Albert MS, Killiany RJ. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage. 2006;31:968–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Doeller CF, Barry C, Burgess N. Evidence for grid cells in a human memory network. Nature. 2010;463:657–661. doi: 10.1038/nature08704. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dragoi G, Tonegawa S. Preplay of future place cell sequences by hippocampal cellular assemblies. Nature. 2011;469:397–401. doi: 10.1038/nature09633. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Eichenbaum H, Cohen NJ. Can we reconcile the declarative memory and spatial navigation views on hippocampal function? Neuron. 2014;83:764–770. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.07.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Eickhoff SB, Paus T, Caspers S, Grosbras MH, Evans AC, Zilles K, Amunts K. Assignment of functional activations to probabilistic cytoarchitectonic areas revisited. NeuroImage. 2007;36:511–521. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Ekstrom AD, Kahana MJ, Caplan JB, Fields TA, Isham EA, Newman EL, Fried I. Cellular networks underlying human spatial navigation. Nature. 2003;425:184–188. doi: 10.1038/nature01964. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Epstein RA. Parahippocampal and retrosplenial contributions to human spatial navigation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2008;12:388–396. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.07.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hafting T, Fyhn M, Molden S, Moser MB, Moser EI. Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature. 2005;436:801–806. doi: 10.1038/nature03721. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Hartley T, Bird CM, Chan D, Cipolotti L, Husain M, Vargha-Khadem F, Burgess N. The hippocampus is required for short-term topographical memory in humans. Hippocampus. 2007;17:34–48. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20240. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hassabis D, Kumaran D, Vann SD, Maguire EA. Patients with hippocampal amnesia cannot imagine new experiences. PNAS. 2007;104:1726–1731. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0610561104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hassabis D, Maguire EA. Deconstructing episodic memory with construction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2007;11:299–306. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hasselmo ME. A model of episodic memory: mental time travel along encoded trajectories using grid cells. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2009;92:559–573. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2009.07.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Horner AJ, Bisby JA, Zotow E, Bush D, Burgess N. Grid-like processing of imagined navigation. Current Biology. 2016;26:842–847. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.01.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Jacobs J, Kahana MJ, Ekstrom AD, Mollison MV, Fried I. A sense of direction in human entorhinal cortex. PNAS. 2010;107:6487–6492. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0911213107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Jacobs J, Weidemann CT, Miller JF, Solway A, Burke JF, Wei XX, Suthana N, Sperling MR, Sharan AD, Fried I, Kahana MJ. Direct recordings of grid-like neuronal activity in human spatial navigation. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:1188–1190. doi: 10.1038/nn.3466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kreiman G, Koch C, Fried I. Imagery neurons in the human brain. Nature. 2000;408:357–361. doi: 10.1038/35042575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kriegeskorte N, Kievit RA. Representational geometry: integrating cognition, computation, and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2013;17:401–412. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kropff E, Carmichael JE, Moser MB, Moser EI. Speed cells in the medial entorhinal cortex. Nature. 2015;523:419–424. doi: 10.1038/nature14622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kunz L, Schröder TN, Lee H, Montag C, Lachmann B, Sariyska R, Reuter M, Stirnberg R, Stöcker T, Messing-Floeter PC, Fell J, Doeller CF, Axmacher N. Reduced grid-cell-like representations in adults at genetic risk for Alzheimer's disease. Science. 2015;350:430–433. doi: 10.1126/science.aac8128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Logothetis NK. What we can do and what we cannot do with fMRI. Nature. 2008;453:869–878. doi: 10.1038/nature06976. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Maass A, Berron D, Libby LA, Ranganath C, Düzel E. Functional subregions of the human entorhinal cortex. eLife. 2015;4:e17089. doi: 10.7554/eLife.06426. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Maguire EA, Vargha-Khadem F, Mishkin M. The effects of bilateral hippocampal damage on fMRI regional activations and interactions during memory retrieval. Brain. 2001;124:1156–1170. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.6.1156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Marchette SA, Vass LK, Ryan J, Epstein RA. Anchoring the neural compass: coding of local spatial reference frames in human medial parietal lobe. Nature Neuroscience. 2014;17:1598–1606. doi: 10.1038/nn.3834. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Milivojevic B, Vicente-Grabovetsky A, Doeller CF. Insight reconfigures hippocampal-prefrontal memories. Current Biology. 2015;25:821–830. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.01.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Navarro Schröder T, Haak KV, Zaragoza Jimenez NI, Beckmann CF, Doeller CF. Functional topography of the human entorhinal cortex. eLife. 2015;4:e17089. doi: 10.7554/eLife.06738. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. O'Keefe J, Dostrovsky J. The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Research. 1971;34:171–175. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(71)90358-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Ólafsdóttir HF, Barry C, Saleem AB, Hassabis D, Spiers HJ. Hippocampal place cells construct reward related sequences through unexplored space. eLife. 2015;4:e17089. doi: 10.7554/eLife.06063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Ólafsdóttir HF, Carpenter F, Barry C. Coordinated grid and place cell replay during rest. Nature Neuroscience. 2016;19:792–794. doi: 10.1038/nn.4291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Schacter DL, Addis DR, Hassabis D, Martin VC, Spreng RN, Szpunar KK. The future of memory: remembering, imagining, and the brain. Neuron. 2012;76:677–694. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Schinazi VR, Nardi D, Newcombe NS, Shipley TF, Epstein RA. Hippocampal size predicts rapid learning of a cognitive map in humans. Hippocampus. 2013;23:515–528. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Schuck NW, Doeller CF, Polk TA, Lindenberger U, Li SC. Human aging alters the neural computation and representation of space. NeuroImage. 2015;117:141–150. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Shine JP, Valdés-Herrera JP, Hegarty M, Wolbers T. The human retrosplenial cortex and thalamus code head direction in a global reference frame. Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:6371–6381. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1268-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Stokes J, Kyle C, Ekstrom AD. Complementary roles of human hippocampal subfields in differentiation and integration of spatial context. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2015;27:546–559. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00736. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Taube JS, Muller RU, Ranck JB. Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. I. Description and quantitative analysis. Journal of Neuroscience. 1990;10:420–435. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-02-00420.1990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Taube JS. The head direction signal: origins and sensory-motor integration. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2007;30:181–207. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112854. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Vass LK, Epstein RA. Abstract representations of location and facing direction in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:6133–6142. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3873-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Vass LK, Epstein RA. Common neural representations for visually guided reorientation and spatial imagery. Cerebral Cortex. 2016:1–15. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Vickerstaff RJ, Cheung A. Which coordinate system for modelling path integration? Journal of Theoretical Biology. 2010;263:242–261. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2009.11.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Wolbers T, Klatzky RL, Loomis JM, Wutte MG, Giudice NA. Modality-independent coding of spatial layout in the human brain. Current Biology. 2011;21:984–989. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.04.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Wolbers T, Wiener JM, Mallot HA, Büchel C. Differential recruitment of the hippocampus, medial prefrontal cortex, and the human motion complex during path integration in humans. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:9408–9416. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2146-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Zhang H, Zherdeva K, Ekstrom AD. Different "routes" to a cognitive map: dissociable forms of spatial knowledge derived from route and cartographic map learning. Memory & Cognition. 2014;42:1106–1117. doi: 10.3758/s13421-014-0418-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
eLife. 2016 Aug 30;5:e17089. doi: 10.7554/eLife.17089.027

Decision letter

Editor: Timothy EJ Behrens1

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "Grid-cell representations in mental simulation" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been favorably evaluated by three reviewers, one of whom, Timothy EJ Behrens (Reviewer #3) also served as Reviewing and Senior Editor. Joshua Jacobs (Reviewer #2) has agreed to reveal his identity.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

Bellmund and colleagues' study investigated the presence of a grid-like signal (a fMRI proxy to rodent grid cells) in the human entorhinal cortex during mental simulations. Namely, the authors aimed to determine whether one could detect a grid representation in the fMRI BOLD signal while study participants carried out a spatial task requiring them to imagine the direction one would need to face to navigate from one place in a virtual environment to another. This is an important area of research as grid cells, in the rodent brain, are thought to underlie navigation and support general mnemonic processes. Moreover, recent theoretical accounts suggest they may be in important for planning future navigation (Bush, Barry et al. (2015)). As such, finding grid representations during navigational simulations would support claims and provide more general insight into how the brain prepares for future behaviour. The study is a theoretically important extension of existing work in this area, suggesting that grid cells are important for general types of cognition, which do not align to current physical movement. The experiment is well designed and introduces new methodology into the field. The inclusion of the many control analyses and visualisations of the constituent parts of the data are helpful towards supporting the reported findings.

Essential revisions:

1) As the authors are aware, there is overlap between this study and a concurrently run study recently published in Current Biology (Horner et al), which reports a similar signal during imagined physical movement. The authors acknowledge this in the paper. The reviewers clearly support publication of the current study. First, we believe it contains substantial novelties over the Horner study (explained shortly), and second we appreciate the replication of such an interesting signal in two concurrently run studies. All reviewers, however, were unsure about the authors attempt to disambiguate the two studies by claiming that imagining directions to goals (rather than imagining physical movement) took the study out of the domain of spatial navigation. We agree that it is a different phenomenon. We think that the current study widens the scope of grid-like activity to the domain of navigational planning of the sort that is predicted theoretically in (Bush et al. Neuron 2015). Because the imagined directions could not be directly enacted (because many of them passed through buildings), we also think that the current result implies a global grid-like signal of the sort investigated in (Carpenter et al. Current biology.), but for imagined navigation. We also think that the novel finding of head-direction specific activity in the current manuscript is a potentially important one. We think that the authors could focus on these strengths of the current manuscript, and do not need to rely on the claims about non-spatial processes that are more difficult to justify in a completely spatial experiment.

Technical questions:

The authors show a head direction-like signal in the parahippocampal gyrus during mental simulations. That is, using a pattern similarity approach they analyse voxels in the parahippocampal gyrus during different imagined directions and find similar (<30°) imagined directions show similar patterns of activity in the parahippocampal gyrus. Although the authors perform controls to rule out visual similarity confounds, I was not convinced they have shown this effect was independent of location in the virtual environment. That is, did comparing trial pairs where a subject imagined the same direction but in two different parts of the environment show the same pattern similarity as comparing trials where the imagined direction happened in the same part (or similar part) of the virtual environment? To convincingly show their result reflects a head direction signal they need to control for location in the environment. They could perform this control analysis by correlating pattern similarity values against distance between the places for a given trial pair. Alternatively, they could input both direction and location into the same GLM, to see if there is an independent effect of direction once direction has been controlled for.

The authors show a grid-like representation during imagined directions, such that pattern similarity was higher when comparing trials 60° apart than 30° apart. The authors perform various control analyses to confirm the robustness of this result. The analysis ruling out the effect of cardinal directions is not reported in the main text. In this analysis they exclude trials that sample the north-south axis of the environment, doing this makes their main effect non-significant. They then use a different exclusion criterion, which leaves the main effect significant. First of all, the difference in exclusion criteria is not clear. It would be useful if the authors clarified this point. Moreover, given how much text is dedicated to this result in the method it should be included in the main text. Permitting the method that maintains the significance of their main effect is reasonable and fair, they could just report that result. Finally, it would be useful to clarify why so many trials seem to sample the north-south axis, as the author suggest lack of statistical power is what makes their main effect non-significant when you exclude these trials.

The new technique is interesting in that it removes the requirement to estimate the grid angle, but as far as we can see, it does not remove it completely because the sensitivity of the technique depends on the subject-by-subject grid angle (if the grid angle is 15° or 45° there is no sensitivity at all, but if the grid angle is 0 or 30 the sensitivity is maximal). This is due to the minimal sampling of directions. Essentially, then, the technique is similar to fitting one of sin(6θ) or cos(6θ) and ignoring the other one and taking the absolute values of the betas. Obviously there is a sensitivity gain from pooling over voxels, which may regain much of the sensitivity loss. This is obviously ok, if you find a significant effect, but these issues are not well presented in the manuscript. A naive reader might easily think that there is some magic whereby there is no longer any requirement to know the grid angle. This should be clarified.

Related to the comment above, the technique presumably makes it harder to examine individual differences as the sensitivity of the technique is so dependent on the grid angle, which will differ between subjects. Is this one potential reason why the authors do not report brain-behaviour correlations? Again, the authors should discuss.

In Figure 3—figure supplement 7, the authors write "We considered the distances between start and target locations in a trial pair by controlling for three distance measures, which differed between the 0 modulo 60 and the 30 modulo 60 condition". In what way? Were the 0 modulo 60 distances longer than 30 modulo 60? It is important to document this even if you can still find the hexagonal results after working with the residuals.

More broadly, the angle selection procedure is a very clever way to increase the sensitivity of the experiment, but it raises concerns that there might be differences on average between the 30° differences and the 60° differences. Is there a way of alleviating these concerns by graphing the differences according to various metrics such as how often they include the inner vs outer ring? Such as average behavioural performance included in the two different types of trial?

There is also a concern with pattern similarity techniques of autocorrelation. We have thought about it but the various combinations are so mind-bendingly complex that it is hard to know whether this will be true. Are there some trials that are more often included in the 60° pairs than the 30° pairs? If there are more or less of these than their complements (trials that occur mostly in the 30° pairs), is it possible that they can cause some statistical autocorrelation in the question of interest. We think this is unlikely, but would be interested to hear your thoughts. We are aware that this is a weakly specified criticism. If no analyses come to mind, some comment to the reviewers would suffice.

eLife. 2016 Aug 30;5:e17089. doi: 10.7554/eLife.17089.028

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) As the authors are aware, there is overlap between this study and a concurrently run study recently published in Current Biology (Horner et al), which reports a similar signal during imagined physical movement. The authors acknowledge this in the paper. The reviewers clearly support publication of the current study. First, we believe it contains substantial novelties over the Horner study (explained shortly), and second we appreciate the replication of such an interesting signal in two concurrently run studies. All reviewers, however, were unsure about the authors attempt to disambiguate the two studies by claiming that imagining directions to goals (rather than imagining physical movement) took the study out of the domain of spatial navigation. We agree that it is a different phenomenon. We think that the current study widens the scope of grid-like activity to the domain of navigational planning of the sort that is predicted theoretically in (Bush et al. Neuron 2015). Because the imagined directions could not be directly enacted (because many of them passed through buildings), we also think that the current result implies a global grid-like signal of the sort investigated in (Carpenter et al. Current biology.), but for imagined navigation. We also think that the novel finding of head-direction specific activity in the current manuscript is a potentially important one. We think that the authors could focus on these strengths of the current manuscript, and do not need to rely on the claims about non-spatial processes that are more difficult to justify in a completely spatial experiment.

We appreciate the careful consideration of our findings with respect to the existing literature. We agree that the imagination of directions in our VR city is a task inherently spatial in nature. Therefore, we follow the suggestions by the reviewers to emphasize that we observed grid-like entorhinal signals in the absence of virtual or imagined movement. Further, we discuss our findings in more detail with respect to the proposed functions of grid-cell computations in vector navigation (Bush et al., Neuron, 2015) and the report of a global grid signal in a multi-compartment environment (Carpenter et al., Current Biology, 2015). Indeed, we believe that our results can be interpreted as a global grid-like signal independent of obstacles in the environment.

The revised sections of the manuscript now read as follows:

Abstract:

“However, hitherto it remains unknown if grid-like representations contribute to mental simulation in the absence of imagined movement.”

Introduction:

“However, hitherto it remains unknown if grid-like representations support mental simulation independent of imagined movement, which could suggest a more general role of grid cell computations in navigational planning, future anticipation and cognition.”

Discussion:

“Our findings provide evidence for an involvement of grid-like representations in mental simulation in the absence of imagined movement.”

Discussion:

“The hippocampal formation and grid cells in particular have been implicated in path integration (Hafting et al., 2005; Wolbers et al., 2007), a central aspect of which is computing a homing vector based on translations from a given starting point (Vickerstaff and Cheung, 2010). […] Our findings suggest an involvement of the entorhinal grid system in calculating vectors to target locations during navigational planning, in line with a theoretical account of vector navigation (Bush et al., 2015).”

Discussion:

“Importantly, the interpretation of our results as a global grid signal coding for space beyond boundaries and obstacles is in line with the report of a global grid pattern emerging with experience in rodents exploring an environment divided into two connected compartments (Carpenter et al., 2015).”

Discussion:

“In conclusion, we show involvement of both absolute directional parahippocampal and grid- like entorhinal signals in imagination, which provides important evidence for these representations in the absence of sensory input or imagined movement.”

Technical questions:

The authors show a head direction-like signal in the parahippocampal gyrus during mental simulations. That is, using a pattern similarity approach they analyse voxels in the parahippocampal gyrus during different imagined directions and find similar (<30°) imagined directions show similar patterns of activity in the parahippocampal gyrus. Although the authors perform controls to rule out visual similarity confounds, I was not convinced they have shown this effect was independent of location in the virtual environment. That is, did comparing trial pairs where a subject imagined the same direction but in two different parts of the environment show the same pattern similarity as comparing trials where the imagined direction happened in the same part (or similar part) of the virtual environment? To convincingly show their result reflects a head direction signal they need to control for location in the environment. They could perform this control analysis by correlating pattern similarity values against distance between the places for a given trial pair. Alternatively, they could input both direction and location into the same GLM, to see if there is an independent effect of direction once direction has been controlled for.

We understand the concern raised by the reviewers that coding for locations or parts of the virtual city might play a role for the absolute directional signal observed in the parahippocampal gyrus (PHG). Following the reviewers’ advice, we conducted two sets of control analyses to demonstrate that the observed head direction-like signal is independent of location in Donderstown and cannot be explained by the distances between the buildings used to sample directions. To demonstrate that absolute directional coding in the parahippocampal gyrus was not driven by building locations we excluded (I) comparisons of trials sampling directions from the same start location and (II) comparisons of trials using the same target building. Specifically, we obtained the pair-wise similarity matrix from the peak voxel of the absolute directional coding effect of our main analysis (Figure 2B) and calculated the pattern similarity difference between pairs of trials sampling similar (angular differences ≤ 30°) and dissimilar (angular differences ≥ 60°) directions after excluding the respective comparisons. Pattern similarity remained increased for similar directions after excluding comparisons sampling from the same start location (T23 = 3.01, p = 0.006) as well as after excluding comparisons targeting the same location (T23 = 4.07, p < 0.001). These results, which are shown in the new Figure 2—figure supplement 2A, rule out the possibility that coding for locations in Donderstown underlies the observed absolute directional coding effect.

Additionally, we demonstrate in a second set of control analyses that the effect is independent of the distance between start and target location. For consistency, we turned towards the approach employed when ruling out potential distance effects in the grid-like signal observed in entorhinal cortex. This two-step procedure is mathematically similar to the multiple regression approach suggested by the reviewers in that variance explained by the distance predictor is removed before computing our effect. Specifically, we set out from the locations of the buildings entering each pair- wise comparison and quantified three distance measures for each trial pair: (I) the mean length of the vectors connecting the respective start and target buildings, (II) the difference in length between these vectors and (III) the average distance of the six vectors marking all possible connections between the four buildings of a trial pair. Calculating the same contrast as for the pattern similarity analysis (computing the difference between the mean distances for trial pairs with angular differences ≤ 30° and for pairs with larger angular differences and then testing these difference scores against 0) revealed that both the mean distance (T23 = 8.30, p < 0.001) and the difference in distance (T23 = 58.23, p < 0.001) was greater for trial pairs sampling similar directions, while the ‘neighborhood distance’ was smaller (T23 = -77.51, p < 0.001) for these comparisons. Crucially, absolute directional coding in the PHG was independent of all distance measures: For each subject, we used the different distance scores as regressors in three separate GLMs to predict pattern similarity values of all trial pairs in the peak voxel of the main absolute directional coding effect (Figure 2) and computed the difference in pattern similarity between pairs sampling similar versus dissimilar directions on the residuals of these GLMs. We still observed increased pattern similarity for trial pairs sampling similar directions compared to pairs sampling dissimilar directions after controlling for the mean trial distance (T23 = 3.90, p < 0.001), the difference in distance within a trial pair (T23 = 3.61, p < 0.001) and the average distance between all buildings of a pair (T23 = 3.97, p < 0.001). These results are shown in new Figure 2—figure supplement 2C.

Taken together, the results of these control analyses support the interpretation of our effect being specific to an absolute directional code independent of locations and distances in Donderstown. The sections of the manuscript describing these analyses are shown below.

Results:

“Further, this effect was not driven by the specific locations used to sample directions or the distances between these locations in Donderstown (Figure 2—figure supplement 2, see Materials and methods).”

Materials and methods:

“Exclusion of trial pairs with the same start location. To control for the possibility that increased pattern similarity was due to pairs of trials sampling directions from the same start location, we excluded these comparisons from the analysis. […] This rules out the possibility that sampling in similar parts of Donderstown is responsible for increased pattern similarity for trial pairs sampling similar directions.”

The authors show a grid-like representation during imagined directions, such that pattern similarity was higher when comparing trials 60° apart than 30° apart. The authors perform various control analyses to confirm the robustness of this result. The analysis ruling out the effect of cardinal directions is not reported in the main text. In this analysis they exclude trials that sample the north-south axis of the environment, doing this makes their main effect non-significant. They then use a different exclusion criterion, which leaves the main effect significant. First of all, the difference in exclusion criteria is not clear. It would be useful if the authors clarified this point. Moreover, given how much text is dedicated to this result in the method it should be included in the main text. Permitting the method that maintains the significance of their main effect is reasonable and fair, they could just report that result. Finally, it would be useful to clarify why so many trials seem to sample the north-south axis, as the author suggest lack of statistical power is what makes their main effect non-significant when you exclude these trials.

We would like to thank the reviewers for giving us the opportunity to provide clarification on these issues. We apologize for having overlooked to refer to the analyses ruling out an effect of representations of cardinal directions in the entorhinal cortex. We have added a reference to the revised manuscript (Results, last paragraph).

The analyses in question go back to comments by a reviewer during peer review of this manuscript at a different journal. The reviewer voiced the concern that a number of roughly parallel streets running from North to South might have dominated participants’ representations of the city. Based on this we were asked to exclude all comparisons of trials sampling North or South (all arrows in Author response image 1). First and foremost, we would like to emphasize that all twelve directions were sampled an equal number of times. Further, if any axis of the environment would dominate participants’ representations of the city, then this should be visible in increased/decreased pattern similarity for trial comparisons with an angular difference of 0° and/ or 180°. However, a repeated measures ANOVA did not reveal pattern similarity differences between trial pairs with different angular distances (0°, 60°, 120° and 180°) in the 0° modulo 60° condition (F3,69 = 1.28, p = 0.289).

Author response image 1. Trials sampling North or South are indicated by arrows.

Author response image 1.

Originally, we were asked to exclude comparisons of all shown trials, leading to a reduction in statistical power. To rule out an effect of sampling along the North-South axis using buildings located on the same street we excluded comparisons indicated in purple from the analyses.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.17089.024

Additionally, behavioral performance in trials sampling the North-South axis did not differ from performance when sampling other directions (see text from manuscript below for details). Importantly, the population response of entorhinal grid cells would still remain the most likely explanation for the observed 60° modulation of pattern similarity values even if one axis was used as reference by our participants. Therefore, we believe that the non-significant result when contrasting the 0° modulo 60° condition with the 30° modulo 60° condition after excluding all comparisons along the North-South axis reflects reduced statistical power in our analysis due to the exclusion of a substantial number of comparisons. Since we believe, for the reasons laid out above, that the analysis does not add substantial information to the report, we have removed it from the revised manuscript as suggested by the reviewers.

We addressed the concern of the aforementioned reviewer that a set of streets running from North to South might drive the effect in an additional analysis. Here, we excluded comparisons of pairs sampling the North or South direction using buildings actually located on the same North-South running street (purple arrows in the figure above). We have highlighted these trials in Figure 1—figure supplement 2 of the revised manuscript to present this information more clearly to the reader. Notably, only a small subset of trials used buildings actually located on the streets in question. After excluding these trial pairs from the analysis, we observed greater pattern similarity in the peak voxel of the cluster from our main analysis (Figure 2) for trial comparisons sampling similar compared to dissimilar directions (T23 = 4.00, p < 0.001). Similarly, pattern similarity in left pmEC remained increased for the 0° modulo 60° compared to the 30° modulo 60° condition (T23 = 2.40, p = 0.025).

The adapted figure and the revised sections of the manuscript are shown below.

Results:

“Furthermore, the effect was specific to a 60° modulation of pattern similarity values and there was no evidence for coding of cardinal directions in the entorhinal cortex (Figure 3—figure supplement 9; see also Materials and methods).

Materials and methods:

“To ascertain that trial pairs sampling directions using buildings located on the same street running along the North-South axis of Donderstown (based on its orientation for example in Figure 1A), which could possibly bias participants’ representations of the city due to a number of streets roughly aligned with it, were not driving our effects, we excluded all comparisons of pairs in which both trials sampled directions along this street from the analysis (see Figure 1—figure supplement 2). […] Importantly, even if participants preferentially represented directions along the North-South axis, which is unlikely based on the reasons laid out above, the population response of entorhinal grid cells would still remain the most likely explanation for the observed 60° modulation of pattern similarity values in pmEC, which could not be explained by the presence of one cardinal axis.”

The new technique is interesting in that it removes the requirement to estimate the grid angle, but as far as we can see, it does not remove it completely because the sensitivity of the technique depends on the subject-by-subject grid angle (if the grid angle is 15° or 45° there is no sensitivity at all, but if the grid angle is 0 or 30 the sensitivity is maximal). This is due to the minimal sampling of directions. Essentially, then, the technique is similar to fitting one of sin(6θ) or cos(6θ) and ignoring the other one and taking the absolute values of the betas. Obviously there is a sensitivity gain from pooling over voxels, which may regain much of the sensitivity loss. This is obviously ok, if you find a significant effect, but these issues are not well presented in the manuscript. A naive reader might easily think that there is some magic whereby there is no longer any requirement to know the grid angle. This should be clarified.

We would like to thank the reviewers for raising this important issue concerning our multivariate approach to investigate grid-like signals using fMRI. Contrary to the approach established by Doeller et al. (Nature, 2010) and subsequently used by Kunz et al. (Science, 2015), Horner et al. (Current Biology, 2016) and Constantinescu et al. (Science, 2016) the approach we employ does not rely on estimation of the grid orientation, but it capitalizes exclusively on the six-fold-symmetric firing patterns of grid cells. In the original paper, grid orientations were estimated per voxel and then averaged across the voxels in the entorhinal cortex. Grid orientations were significantly clustered in most participants (Doeller et al., Nature, 2010, Suppl. Figure 9). These findings are in line with recent reports of clustering of grid orientations across cells in simple environments (Krupic et al., Nature, 2015; Stensola et al., Nature, 2015). Taking the assumption of a shared grid orientation across voxels to the extreme, it is indeed conceivable that there might be no sensitivity to the 60° modulation of pattern similarity values targeted by our multivariate approach if the grid orientation is 15° or 45° with the sampling of only twelve directions.

As already alluded to by the reviewers, it can be assumed that even in the case of an average grid orientation sub-optimally suited for the multivariate approach, there would be important information carried by the variance in preferred orientations across voxels, which the representational similarity analysis approach could pick up. This stands in contrast to the voxel- wise, univariate analysis employed by Doeller et al. (Nature, 2010). Differences in grid orientation across voxels could potentially also reflect variation in the grid orientation across grid cell modules reported by Stensola et al. (Nature, 2012, Figure 2C, D). Variation in the orientation of grid modules is also present in the two more recent studies reporting an alignment of grid orientations to environmental boundaries (Krupic et al., Nature, 2015, Figure 2B; Stensola et al., Nature, 2015, Extended Data Figure 4A, B). Differences in orientation between grid modules could result in differences in putative grid orientation across voxels in the entorhinal cortex, which might normalize sensitivity differences.

However, even when considering the possibility of orientation-differences across grid modules resulting in different preferred orientations for each voxel in the entorhinal cortex, it seems plausible to expect an overall clustering of voxel orientations at different angles for different participants in the entorhinal cortex in a complex large-scale environment such as Donderstown. This was also observed by Doeller et al. (Nature, 2010), where participants navigated an environment with multiple prominent landmarks for orientation. Potentially, this might still result in different sensitivity of our analysis across participants due to the sampling of twelve directions. While the sampling of twelve directions constitutes the minimum number of directions necessary for our analysis, a higher number of directions would have been infeasible to assess in a controlled fashion in our direction imagination task. Since the more fine-grained sampling of directions needed to estimate individual grid orientations could not be realized with our design only the multivariate approach allowed for the investigation of grid-like entorhinal signals.

We have added the discussion of this issue to the revised manuscript. Further we have modified Figure 3—figure supplement 1C for illustration. The changed sections of the manuscript can be found below.

Discussion:

“In particular, we demonstrate that this novel analysis approach, which does not rely on the estimation of the orientation of the hexadirectional signal in entorhinal cortex in an independent data set (Doeller et al., 2010; Horner et al., 2016; Kunz et al., 2015; Constantinescu et al., 2016), is sensitive to grid-like entorhinal signals by capitalizing on the six-fold symmetry of grid cell firing patterns. […] However, only the multivariate approach enabled us to investigate the six-fold rotational symmetry in our large-scale environment, in which a continuous sampling of directions as required for the estimation of the orientation of the hexadirectional signal would not have been feasible.”

Related to the comment above, the technique presumably makes it harder to examine individual differences as the sensitivity of the technique is so dependent on the grid angle, which will differ between subjects. Is this one potential reason why the authors do not report brain-behaviour correlations? Again, the authors should discuss.

We would like to thank the reviewers for this insightful comment. We agree with the reviewers that the potentially different sensitivity of the multivariate approach to investigate grid-like entorhinal signals discussed has to be kept in mind when considering correlations with behavior. Such a correlation is less likely to be observed, when variance in the measured strength of the neural signal across participants can be due to sensitivity differences of the analysis employed. However, as mentioned above, a continuous sampling of directions as required for the estimation of putative grid orientations was infeasible with our direction imagination paradigm.

We have added the caveat about relating grid-like signals obtained using the multivariate approach to behavior to the Discussion.

Discussion:

“This needs to be taken into consideration when aiming to relate grid-like signals to behavior.”

In Figure 3—figure supplement 7, the authors write "We considered the distances between start and target locations in a trial pair by controlling for three distance measures, which differed between the 0 modulo 60 and the 30 modulo 60 condition". In what way? Were the 0 modulo 60 distances longer than 30 modulo 60? It is important to document this even if you can still find the hexagonal results after working with the residuals.

We agree with the reviewers that it is important to document more clearly the differences between the conditions. The nature of the differences between the conditions was quantified separately for each distance measure. To test for differences between the conditions, we obtained a distance matrix quantifying the distance measure for each pairwise comparison of two trials, thus resembling the pairwise correlation matrix in our representational similarity analysis (RSA). Analogous to the RSA approach we computed the difference between the mean distance in the 0° modulo 60° condition and the mean distance in the 30° modulo 60° condition for each participant and tested the resulting differences against 0 using a one-sample t-test. The results of these tests, which were already reported in the manuscript, revealed that the mean trial distance (average length of the vectors from start to target building in a trial pair) was larger for the 0° modulo 60° condition (T23 = 5.25, p < 0.001). Further, the difference in distance between start and target building was smaller for trial pairs in the 0° modulo 60° condition (T23 = -57.02, p < 0.001). Lastly, the ‘neighborhood distance’ (average length of the six vectors connecting the four buildings in a trial pair), which we used as a measure of imagining directions in a certain area of Donderstown, was larger in the 0° modulo 60° condition (T23 = 54.20, p < 0.001). However, in light of the described differences between the 0° modulo 60° and the 30° modulo 60° condition, we would like to emphasize again that the increased pattern similarity in the 0° modulo 60° condition was not due to these differences. This effect was present after statistically accounting for variance explained by the distance measures.

We have clarified how we tested for distance differences between the conditions in the revised manuscript and documented the nature of these differences. The relevant sections of the manuscript read as follows:

Materials and methods:

“In a second set of control analyses we additionally considered the Euclidean distance between the start and the target location as described above for the absolute directional coding effect. […] The results of these tests are reported in the following paragraphs. In each control analysis, we then computed a GLM with a continuous distance predictor and pairwise Fisher z-transformed correlation coefficients as the criterion.”

Materials and methods:

“The mean Euclidean distance was higher for pairs in the 0° modulo 60° condition (T23 = 5.25, p < 0.001).”

Materials and methods:

“Trial pairs from the 0° modulo 60° condition had more similar distances (T23 = -57.02, p < 0.001).”

Materials and methods:

“This resulted in a ‘neighborhood’ distance, which was low when both start and both target buildings were in the same area of Donderstown. These distances were larger for pairs in the 0° modulo 60° condition (T23 = 54.20, p < 0.001).”

More broadly, the angle selection procedure is a very clever way to increase the sensitivity of the experiment, but it raises concerns that there might be differences on average between the 30° differences and the 60° differences. Is there a way of alleviating these concerns by graphing the differences according to various metrics such as how often they include the inner vs outer ring? Such as average behavioural performance included in the two different types of trial?

We agree with the reviewers that it is of great importance to ensure that our effects are not driven by differences between trial pairs in the 0° modulo 60° and the 30° modulo 60° condition introduced by the specific sampling of directions in our design. As suggested by the reviewers we investigated participants’ behavioral performance in more detail. Specifically, we combined angular errors for all pairwise comparisons by multiplying the two error values of each pair (e.g. a pair with pointing errors of 30° and 50° would have a value of 1500 in this analysis) and averaged the combined error values for the two conditions. We tested for a difference in behavioral performance using a one-sample t-test on the difference between the conditions. Behavioral performance did not differ between the 0° modulo 60° and the 30° modulo 60° condition (T23 = 1.24, p = 0.227). We have highlighted this analysis in the manuscript and added Figure 3—figure supplement 8 to illustrate equal behavioral performance in the two conditions.

Further, we followed the reviewers’ suggestion to focus on trials, where directions were sampled using a target location in the inner ring of buildings. Pairwise comparisons in which both trials sampled a direction using an inner ring target location made up 12.23% of comparisons in the 0° modulo 60° condition and did not occur in the 30° modulo 60° condition (see Author response image 2). Since we observed lower pointing errors for trials with targets in the inner ring compared to the outer ring (one-sample t-test on the difference between errors, T23 = -5.29, p < 0.001), we conducted an additional control analysis to exclude the possibility that the increase in pattern similarity in the 0° modulo 60° condition was due to comparisons of trials with targets in the inner ring. In this control analysis, we excluded pairwise comparisons in which both trials used a target building located in the inner ring before computing the pattern similarity difference between the 0° modulo 60° condition and the 30° modulo 60° condition in each subject’s left pmEC ROI. Pattern similarity remained increased for the 0° modulo 60° condition compared to the 30° modulo 60° condition (T23 = 5.29, p < 0.001). This rules out that our effect is due to an over-representation of trials targeting buildings in the inner ring.

Author response image 2. Percentage of pairwise comparisons in the two conditions as a function of the number of trials in a pair targeting a building located on the inner ring.

Author response image 2.

Trial pairs in which both trials targeted a building in the inner ring made up 12.23% of comparisons in the 0° modulo 60° condition and did not exist in the 30° modulo 60° condition.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.17089.025

We have added this control analysis to the manuscript and show its results in Figure 3—figure supplement 6. The revised sections of the manuscript now read as follows.

Results:

“Behavioral performance did not differ between the conditions (T23 = 1.24, p = 0.227, Figure 3 —figure supplement 8).”

Materials and methods:

“We investigated whether a 60° modulation of absolute angular error values was also present in the behavioral data. […] The combined error values were not different between the conditions (T23 = 1.24, p = 0.227, Figure 3—figure supplement 8).”

Materials and methods:

Exclusion of trial pairs with target locations in inner ring of buildings. In some trials, directions were sampled using target buildings located on the inner ring of buildings (see Figure 1—figure supplement 2). […] When excluding these comparisons from the analysis, pattern similarity remained increased for the 0° modulo 60° condition compared to the 30° modulo 60° condition (Figure 3—figure supplement 6,bar V).”

There is also a concern with pattern similarity techniques of autocorrelation. We have thought about it but the various combinations are so mind-bendingly complex that it is hard to know whether this will be true. Are there some trials that are more often included in the 60° pairs than the 30° pairs? If there are more or less of these than their complements (trials that occur mostly in the 30° pairs), is it possible that they can cause some statistical autocorrelation in the question of interest. We think this is unlikely, but would be interested to hear your thoughts. We are aware that this is a weakly specified criticism. If no analyses come to mind, some comment to the reviewers would suffice.

We understand the concern raised by the reviewers as representational similarity analysis is very sensitive to temporal autocorrelation. However, we do not see how autocorrelation could drive the reported effect. Since the analysis is conducted based on the relative angular difference between the homogenously sampled directions, each trial is included equally often in both conditions.

Therefore, the only possibility for autocorrelation to contribute to the effect would be if trials in one condition were presented closer together in time. However, we do not believe this is the case for two reasons: One of the control analyses reported in the manuscript showed that pattern similarity was increased in the 0° modulo 60° condition when only comparing voxel patterns of trials recorded in different scanning runs (T23 = 2.08, p = 0.049; Figure 3—figure supplement 6, bar IV). We believe that excluding comparisons of trials from the same run makes a contribution of temporal autocorrelation to the effect highly unlikely as the scanner was stopped in between runs and runs were separated by breaks typically lasting a few minutes to allow participants to rest. These breaks were terminated by the participant indicating he was ready to continue with the next task block.

Further, we followed up on the comment by the reviewers by assessing the time between trials constituting the pairs in both conditions. We quantified the time between the onsets of the imagination periods of the two trials of each pair for all pairwise comparisons within a run. There was no apparent difference between the 0° modulo 60° condition and the 30° modulo 60° condition.

Author response image 3. Mean proportion of pairwise comparisons in the 0° modulo 60° condition and the 30° modulo 60° condition across participants for different time bins.

Author response image 3.

The onset asynchrony was defined as the absolute difference in seconds between the onsets of the imagination periods of the trials in a pair. Error bars reflect SEM.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.17089.026

Taken together, we believe that these data speak against the possibility that our effect might be influenced by autocorrelation.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 1—source data 1. Average absolute angular errors.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.17089.004

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.17089.004
    Figure 2—source data 1. Searchlight results for absolute directional coding analysis.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.17089.009

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.17089.009
    Figure 3—source data 1. Pattern similarity difference between 0° modulo 60° and 30° modulo 60° condition in left and right posterior medial entorhinal cortex.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.17089.013

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.17089.013

    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES