Skip to main content
Royal College of Psychiatrists Open Access logoLink to Royal College of Psychiatrists Open Access
. 2016 Sep;209(3):236–243. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.115.168229

Genome-wide association study of response to cognitive–behavioural therapy in children with anxiety disorders

Jonathan R I Coleman 1, Kathryn J Lester 1, Robert Keers 1, Susanna Roberts 1, Charles Curtis 1, Kristian Arendt 1, Susan Bögels 1, Peter Cooper 1, Cathy Creswell 1, Tim Dalgleish 1, Catharina A Hartman 1, Einar R Heiervang 1, Katrin Hötzel 1, Jennifer L Hudson 1, Tina In-Albon 1, Kristen Lavallee 1, Heidi J Lyneham 1, Carla E Marin 1, Richard Meiser-Stedman 1, Talia Morris 1, Maaike H Nauta 1, Ronald M Rapee 1, Silvia Schneider 1, Sophie C Schneider 1, Wendy K Silverman 1, Mikael Thastum 1, Kerstin Thirlwall 1, Polly Waite 1, Gro Janne Wergeland 1, Gerome Breen 1,*, Thalia C Eley 1,*,
PMCID: PMC5007453  PMID: 26989097

Abstract

Background

Anxiety disorders are common, and cognitive–behavioural therapy (CBT) is a first-line treatment. Candidate gene studies have suggested a genetic basis to treatment response, but findings have been inconsistent.

Aims

To perform the first genome-wide association study (GWAS) of psychological treatment response in children with anxiety disorders (n = 980).

Method

Presence and severity of anxiety was assessed using semi-structured interview at baseline, on completion of treatment (post-treatment), and 3 to 12 months after treatment completion (follow-up). DNA was genotyped using the Illumina Human Core Exome-12v1.0 array. Linear mixed models were used to test associations between genetic variants and response (change in symptom severity) immediately post-treatment and at 6-month follow-up.

Results

No variants passed a genome-wide significance threshold (P = 5 × 10−8) in either analysis. Four variants met criteria for suggestive significance (P<5 × 10−6) in association with response post-treatment, and three variants in the 6-month follow-up analysis.

Conclusions

This is the first genome-wide therapygenetic study. It suggests no common variants of very high effect underlie response to CBT. Future investigations should maximise power to detect single-variant and polygenic effects by using larger, more homogeneous cohorts.


Anxiety disorders are the most common psychiatric disorders, with a lifetime prevalence of ~30%.1 They are a major cause of global disability, and impose considerable economic burdens on society.2,3 They commonly have their onset in childhood or adolescence and have been linked to the occurrence of later disorders, including depression and conduct disorder.1,4 Adults with anxiety disorders show rates of childhood anxiety diagnoses significantly above baseline.5 Given this potential gateway effect, and the distress caused by these disorders, there is a need to optimise and understand treatment effectiveness in childhood.

Cognitive–behavioural therapy (CBT) is a first-line treatment for anxiety disorders in the UK, with 59% remission reported immediately post-treatment.6,7 Despite this high reported efficacy, variability exists in patient response that may be influenced in part by genetic variants. Multiple studies examining the genetics of differential response to psychological therapies (therapygenetics8) have been undertaken, and variants in seven genes (5HTT/SLC6A4, TPH2, MAOA, COMT, NGF, BDNF and GRIK4) have been implicated at least once in studies of CBT for anxiety disorders.9 However, findings have proven difficult to replicate,10 and the direction of effects found inconsistent. These problems may result from the low power of small cohort sizes, resulting in a high rate of false positives, and a narrow focus on a few genes that may have limited relevance to the phenotype.

Genome-wide association studies (GWAS) provide a hypothesis-neutral alternative, agnostic to prior assumptions of relevance and with the potential to discover novel findings at a single variant level. By analysing thousands of variants across the genome, GWAS yield more information than the candidate gene approach, allowing for the acknowledgement and control of confounds such as ancestry and the quality of genotyping. Genome-wide information can also be used to investigate associations between phenotypic change and different levels of the genetic architecture, including the effect of all variants in a given gene, and the effect of all genotyped variants across the genome. However, the explicit requirement for multiple testing correction in GWAS imposes a need for large sample sizes.

Although GWAS have not been used to study response to CBT, they have shown early promise in studying anxiety disorders. Genetic influences on the development of anxiety disorders may indicate processes underlying treatment response, and provide interesting genetic candidates.11 A detailed review of the genetics of anxiety disorders is available elsewhere.12 In brief, one variant, rs7309727 (TMEM132D), was associated with panic disorder in a cohort of European ancestry (P = 1.1 × 10−8, odds ratio (OR) = 1.45 (95% CI 1.20–1.72).13 A variant in the TMEM16B gene was reported at genome-wide significance in a Japanese cohort with panic disorder, but was not significant in replication analyses.14 Two GWAS of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) have identified variants at genome-wide significance in the TLL1 gene (rs6812849, P = 3.13 × 10−9, OR not reported)15 and PRTFDC1 (rs6482463, P = 2.04 × 10−9, OR = 1.47 (95% CI 1.35–1.59)).16 However, these results require replication in larger studies; for example, variants in the RORA gene previously implicated in a GWAS of PTSD failed to attain significance in a larger replication effort.17 No significant findings from the anxiety literature to date had previously been considered in candidate gene studies.12

To our knowledge, this is the first GWAS to examine response to psychological therapy in any disorder, and the first to examine treatment response of any kind in anxiety disorders. Participants were drawn from the Genes for Treatment (GxT) study, an international, multisite investigation of clinical, demographic and genetic predictors of response to CBT for anxiety in childhood and adolescence.10,18 Two analyses of association between single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) and response to CBT were conducted, investigating change in symptom severity between baseline and immediately post-treatment (post-treatment), and between baseline and 6 months after treatment cessation (follow-up).

Method

Study design and sample

A detailed description of the participants and the treatment programmes from which they were drawn is provided elsewhere (online supplemental material).18 In brief, participants provided DNA for the GxT study between 2005 and 2013, at 11 sites across the USA, Australia and Western Europe. Children and adolescents (5–17 years old, 94% aged 5–13) were included if they met DSM-IV criteria19 for a primary anxiety disorder diagnosis, with further psychiatric diagnoses made as appropriate. Exclusion criteria were significant physical or intellectual impairment, and the presence of psychotic symptoms. All participants completed a full course of individual-based CBT (with or without parental involvement), group-based CBT or guided self-help either as part of a trial or as treatment as usual within a clinical research department. All treatments were manualised and treatment protocols across all sites were comparable for core elements of CBT including teaching of coping skills, cognitive restructuring, and exposure.

Assessments were made using the Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-IV, Parent and Child Versions (ADIS-IV-C/P),20 except at Bochum (Germany) and Basel (Switzerland) where the German equivalent, Kinder-DIPS,21 was used. All participants were assessed prior to and immediately after treatment, with further assessments made at 3-, 6- or 12-month follow-up where possible. Output from the ADIS (or equivalent) was converted into Clinical Severity Ratings (CSR) on a scale of 0–8. A diagnosis was made when the child met the diagnostic criteria and received a CSR of 4 or more, usually based on a composite of parent and child report. Diagnoses were made from the ADIS for multiple anxiety disorders, and primary status allocated to the most severe, defined as the highest CSR, with ties resolved by clinical judgement (online Table DS1(b) and (c)).

To minimise differential assessment across sites, raters at Reading (UK), Oxford (UK) and Aarhus (Denmark) all received training in evaluation from the Sydney (Australia) site, and clinicians at Aarhus received additional training in the ADIS from W.K.S., principal investigator of the Florida (USA) site. As such, standardised assessments were made for at least 85% of the analysed sample (for further details see the online supplement).

Definition of the treatment response phenotype

As in previous analyses of the GxT sample, outcome was assessed across two periods: baseline to post-treatment and baseline to follow-up. Although dichotomised treatment outcomes are often used in clinical decision making in treatment response, a continuous measure of change in severity provides substantially more power for analyses.22

Response post-treatment was therefore defined as percentage change in CSR score between baseline and immediately following treatment. Percentage change, rather than absolute change, was used as it has been shown to better reflect clinical ratings of improvement by its successful use in pharmacogenetics GWAS.23 For follow-up analyses, a range of time points were available; assessments taken at the 6-month time point were used, as these were the most complete (n = 483). Missing data at this time point was imputed using the best linear unbiased estimates from linear mixture models fitted to the GxT data as part of analyses predicting response from clinical variables alone.18 The mixture models included the linear and quadratic effects of time as well as gender, age, primary diagnosis, treatment type and the random effects of individual and trial (for a full explanation, see Hudson et al18). This allowed us to compute response at follow-up as the percentage improvement in CSR score from baseline to 6 months after the end of treatment. Analyses were performed on residual scores generated from a linear regression of the percentage change measure adjusted for baseline severity, age, gender, treatment type, diagnosis and trial.

Both sets of residual scores were created as output variables from our previous study, which found a number of significant non-genetic influences on treatment outcome (online supplement).18

DNA extraction and genotyping

DNA was collected and extracted using standard protocols, from buccal swabs24 and saliva kits (OG-500 / PrepitL2P, DNAgenotek, Kanata, Canada). Sample preparation (including concentration and quantification) prior to genotyping is described in the online supplement. Genotyping was performed on Illumina HumanCoreExome-12v1.0 microarrays (Illumina, San Diego, California, USA), using a standard protocol.25 Samples were genotyped in two batches, and randomized by site on each microarray.

Quality control

SNPs were mapped to build version 37/hg19 of the human genome. Initial genotype calls were made with GenCall software (GenomeStudio, Illumina, San Diego, California, USA), reprocessed to remove poorly performing samples, re-clustered, and manually recalled where appropriate. Further recalling, targeted at improving the identification of rare variants (such as the exonic content of the microarray) was performed using ZCall.26 Following recalling, the data were transferred to a multinode computing cluster, and quality control was performed following previously published protocols (online supplement).

Quality controlled data were imputed to the December 2013 release of the 1000 Genomes Project reference (for autosomes; March 2012 release for the X chromosome27), using the posterior-sampling method in IMPUTE2 with concurrent phasing.28 SNPs imputed with an info metric >0.8 and a minor allele frequency (MAF) >1% were considered best-guess genotypes, and converted back to PLINK binary format using GTOOL (Freeman and Marchini, available at www.well.ox.ac.uk/~cfreeman/software/gwas/gtool.html). SNPs with a genotype probability of <0.9 were set as missing, and those present in <98% of the sample were excluded from the analysis.

Statistical analysis

Two analyses were performed, examining adjusted percentage change in CSR score from baseline to post-treatment, and from baseline to 6-month follow-up, as described above. Principal component analysis (PCA) of the genotype data was performed to attempt to control for population stratification. However, this yielded components that were not sensitive to differences in outcome. This was likely due to the quantitative nature of the phenotype, the fact that multiple covariates were controlled for in constructing the phenotype, and because participants were drawn from a variety of sites across the globe (online supplement). Accordingly, PCA was deemed unsuitable for controlling for population stratification, prompting the adoption of mixed linear modelling for the association analyses (MLMA). MLMA uses genome-wide genotype data to derive a genomic relationship matrix (GRM), which is used to control for genetic similarity between participants as a random effect.29

MLMA association analysis was performed in GCTA, using the mlma-loco option for autosomes and the mlma option for the X chromosome (online supplement).30 For each SNP in the study, percentage change in CSR was regressed on the number of copies of the reference allele of the SNP (0, 1 or 2), weighted by its additive effect. A random effect of genetic similarity (from the GRM) was included as a covariate, as were fixed effects of sample concentration at genotyping, sample type (buccal swab or saliva), and ultrafiltration status (whether the sample was filtered in preparation for genotyping; online supplement). Using the assumptions of this approach, power for the GWAS was estimated using the Genetic Power Calculator.31 The sample of 980 participants has 80% power to detect a variant explaining ~4% of variance and 1% power to detect variants explaining 1%.

Results from the association analysis were clumped according to P-value using PLINK.32,33 Each clump is represented by a sentinel SNP (that with the lowest P-value in the clump), and contains all SNPs in linkage disequilibrium with the sentinel (R2>0.25, within 250kb of the sentinel). One imputed sentinel SNP in the 6-month follow-up analysis was on the borderline of genome-wide significance (rs72850669, P = 7.54 × 10−8), and was re-genotyped post hoc (LGC Genomics, Teddington, UK). This showed the genotype calling of rs72850669 was unreliable (data not shown), and it was removed from the analyses.

To assess the ability of the GWAS to replicate previous findings, the association of SNPs implicated in CBT response in previous candidate gene studies was examined.9 Exploratory secondary analyses were performed to assess the combined effects of SNPs on response (details can be found in the online supplement). The proportion of variance in CSR change across time accounted for by all the SNPs in the study was assessed with univariate genomic-relatedness-matrix restricted maximum likelihood (GREML), performed in GCTA using the GRM derived for the GWAS. Polygenic risk score profiling was used to investigate the ability of external data-sets to predict CBT response, using risk profiles from publicly available GWAS of major depressive disorder34 and schizophrenia,35 as well as from a meta-analysis of response to antidepressants.36 To test the ability of the GxT data to predict response to CBT, five analyses were performed. Participants with generalised anxiety disorder, separation anxiety, social phobia and specific phobia, and those from the Reading (UK) site, were separately removed from the dataset and risk profiles derived from the remaining participants. Each profile was then used to predict outcome in the relevant set of removed participants.

Ethics

All trials and collection of samples were approved by site-specific human ethics and biosafety committees. Parents provided informed consent, children provided assent. The storage and analysis of DNA was approved by the King's College London Psychiatry, Nursing and Midwifery Research Ethics Sub-Committee.

Results

Sample and SNP exclusions are shown in online Fig. DS1. Phenotype and high-quality genotype data were available for 939 participants in the analysis post-treatment, with an additional 41 participants available for analysis at 6-month follow-up (n = 980). Baseline demographic information for these 980 participants is described in online Table DS1(a). The position of the samples on principal component axes derived from the HapMap reference populations suggests 92% of the sample are of White Western European ancestry.37 A total of 260824 common SNPs passed quality control, which rose to 3017604 SNPs when imputed genotypes were added.

No SNPs were found at formal genome-wide significance for either analysis (all P>5 × 10−8). In the post-treatment analysis, four independent clumps passed threshold for suggestive significance (P<5 × 10−6; Table 1 and Fig. 1). Quantile–quantile plots show no departure from the chi-squared distribution of P-values expected under the null hypothesis, suggesting there is no underlying inflation of association statistics by uncontrolled confounds (lambda median = 0.972, Fig. 2). Three independent clumps were suggestive of significance in the 6-month follow-up analysis (Table 2 and Fig. 3), with no evidence of inflation (lambda = 1.02, Fig. 4). All clumps with P<1 × 10−4 are displayed in online Table DS2.

Table 1.

Independent clumps associated with cognitive–behavioural therapy response at post-treatment with P<5 × 10−6

Sentinel SNP CHR Clump BP Sentinel
SNP P
Sentinel
SNP MAF
Sentinel SNP
information
Genes +/−100kb
rs10881475 1 108113663–108203647 2.45 × 10−6 0.187 0.993 NTNG1, VAV3

rs11834041 12 128232721–128239057 3.50 × 10−6 0.135 Genotyped

rs12464559 2 152498699–152679462 4.09 × 10−6 0.0410 0.941 NEB, ARL5A, CACNB4

rs881301 8 38322346–38332318 4.46 × 10−6 0.403 Genotyped WHSC1L1, LETM2, FGFR1, C8orf86

SNP, single nucleotide polymorphism; CHR, chromosome; BP, base pair; MAF, minor allele frequency.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Manhattan plot of genetic associations with cognitive–behavioural therapy response baseline to post-treatment.

X-axis shows the top million most associated single nucleotide polymorphisms, arranged by position on the chromosome. Lines show conventional thresholds for genome-wide significance (P = 5 × 10−8) and suggestive significance (P = 5 × 10−6).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Quantile–quantile plot of P-values (pruned for linkage disequilibrium) from genetic associations with cognitive–behavioural therapy response post-treatment.

X-axis shows spread of P-values expected under the null chi-squared distribution. Y-axis shows observed data. Grey region shows rough 95% confidence intervals around each point on the line x = y. Lambda median is a measure of inflation of the observed distribution of associations compared with expected null distribution. Lambda ⩽1 implies no inflation.

Table 2.

Independent clumps associated with cognitive–behavioural therapy response at 6-month follow-up with P<5 × 10−6

Sentinel SNP CHR Clump BP Sentinel
SNP P
Sentinel
SNP MAF
Sentinel SNP
information
Genes +/−100kb
rs72711240 4 135657189–135695807 4.49 × 10−7 0.0269 0.903

rs9875578 3 13707416–13810670 1.43 × 10−6 0.424 0.994 FBLN2, WNT7A

rs6813264 4 146509970–146631854 4.68 × 10−6 0.410 Genotyped SMAD1, MMAA, C4orf51, ZNF827

SNP, single nucleotide polymorphism; CHR, chromosome; BP, base pair; MAF, minor allele frequency.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Manhattan plot of genetic associations with cognitive–behavioural therapy response baseline to 6 months after treatment.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Quantile–quantile plot of P-values from genetic associations with cognitive–behavioural therapy response baseline to 6-month follow-up, including lambda median.

A secondary analysis with increased power was performed restricted to nine SNPs previously associated with response to CBT in candidate gene studies (five other SNPs have been previously implicated in CBT response, but did not pass quality control). Assuming a significance threshold of 0.005455 (0.05/9), none of the nine previously associated SNPs was significant (Table 3 and online supplement). The sample had 80% power to detect an SNP accounting for 1.4% of variance at this significance threshold, suggesting any effect of these SNPs in this data-set is smaller than this.

Table 3.

Genome-wide association study P-values of single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) previously associated with cognitive–behavioural therapy response.12,a

Gene SNP P (post-treatment) P (follow-up)
SLC6A4 rs25531 Imputed with info <0.8 Imputed with info <0.8

HTR2A rs6311 0.4717 0.9692
rs6313 0.5451 0.8109
rs6314 Imputed with info < 0.8 Imputed with info <0.8
rs7997012 Completeness after imputation <0.98 Completeness after imputation <0.98

TPH2 rs4570625 Completeness after imputation <0.98 Completeness after imputation <0.98

COMT rs4680 0.7699 0.5956

NGF rs6330 0.5093 0.4559

BDNF rs6265 (val158met) 0.3408 0.9078
rs7934165 0.5231 0.9880
rs1519480 0.8211 0.5013
rs11030104 0.3158 0.9675

GRIN2B rs1019385 Imputed with info <0.8 Imputed with info <0.8

GRIK4 rs1954787 0.1315 0.1914
a.

No P-value is significant after multiple testing correction.

Exploratory secondary analyses (GREML, gene-wide analyses and polygenic risk score profiling) were performed. No significant estimate of SNP heritability could be obtained from GREML, and the effect of adding principal components was minimal. In the post-treatment analysis, all estimates were non-significant. In the 6-month follow-up data the highest estimate was 0.0797 (95% CI −0.194 to 0.35) without principal components. The power of univariate GREML in the sample was estimated for a range of true heritabilities.38 Power ranged from 9 to 46% assuming true heritability between 0.2 and 0.6. To achieve 80% power within this range of heritabilities will require 1450–4450 samples (for heritabilities between 0.6 and 0.2).

Polygenic risk score profiling failed to generate predictions that were consistently significant, either for external GWAS or in the internal predictions of response.

Discussion

Main findings

We report the first genome-wide association study of psychological therapy. Although no region reached genome-wide significance, the single SNP and polygenic results are consistent with the wider literature of treatment genetics in psychiatry, given the sample size studied. Genome-wide significant variants detected in GWAS of psychiatric phenotypes have shown small effect sizes (with the exception of late-onset dementia), requiring thousands of participants to discover. The pattern of results in psychiatric genomics to date suggests that a critical number of participants (varying by disorder) are required before robust findings begin to be made. In studies of schizophrenia, this critical number was ~9000 cases.39 Our results, although preliminary, suggest response to CBT could be a complex phenotype at the early point of this trajectory, although the critical sample size is not yet clear.

The purpose of this study was to identify genetic variants capable of predicting change in symptom severity during treatment. No common, high-effect SNPs were identified, suggesting that it is very unlikely a single variant could be used as a predictor. This also places an upper bound on expected effect sizes in studies of CBT response. This is relevant considering that neither GWAS replicated previous findings from the literature. This does not appear to be due to insufficient statistical power. For example, the COMT val158met polymorphism (rs6265) was reported to account for 8% of variance in CBT response in adults with panic disorder, well above the 4% of variance explained for which this GWAS was powered.40 Failure to replicate previous findings from the candidate gene literature has proved common in psychiatric genetics, whereas GWAS is proving more reliable.35,41 The failure to replicate any published variants suggests previous assumptions of gene relevance may be erroneous, resulting from underpowered candidate gene studies that overestimated the likely effect sizes of studied variants, and that reported variants are likely to be false positives, or to have effect sizes inflated due to winner's curse.42 Proximity to a gene does not imply an effect on gene expression, so the failure to replicate the effects of candidate SNPs does not exclude a role for candidate genes, as the SNPs assessed may not capture true functional variation.

Not all candidate variants are SNPs, and one limitation of GWAS is the difficulty of assessing structural variants not captured by the probes on microarrays. For example, we cannot comment on the previously reported role of the MAOA-u variable number tandem repeat in CBT response.43 Nor could we assess the effect of the 5HTTLPR variant of SLC6A4, previously associated with remission from anxiety disorders at follow-up; however, we directly genotyped this variant in this cohort, and were unable to replicate our earlier finding.8,10

Although small when compared with high-profile studies such as the PGC studies in schizophrenia and depression,34,35 our sample is similar in size to studies in the depression pharmacogenetic literature.23,44 The first of these used a multistage design (n = 1532) and identified several associations at nominal significance, but none remained significant after correction for multiple testing.44 The second (n = 706) found one genome-wide significant locus (for response to nortryptiline treatment) and six loci at suggestive significance across four subanalyses.23 More recent meta-analyses were unable to find genome-wide significant variants.36 However, a significant GREML estimate of SNP-chip heritability of 42% (95% CI 6%–78%) was identified, suggesting useful information about treatment response can be obtained at the whole-genome level.45 Future studies in psychological therapy-genetics should aim to build a cohort of sufficient size to estimate SNP-chip heritability and bivariate genetic correlations, enabling further comparison with pharmacogenetic studies. Such a cohort could act as a target data-set for polygenic risk scoring, exploring the predictive value of variants associated with potentially relevant phenotypes assessed in other GWAS.

Limitations

There are parallels between the antidepressant GWAS literature and this study, including the necessity of combining many studies to obtain sufficient participants for analysis. Herein, we examined a naturalistic clinical cohort, drawn from CBT trials or from treatment as usual. As all participants received CBT, there was no placebo group for comparison. Therefore, the results may reflect natural regression to the mean, rather than an effect of treatment. Theoretically, a parallel GWAS of change in severity could be performed on wait-list controls to identify associations with regression to the mean. Results from the GWAS of CBT response could be weighted by the likelihood that any given association resulted from regression to the mean. However, this would require deliberate non-treatment of thousands of wait-list controls over a period of at least 7 months for the purpose of comparison only. As CBT is effective in this age group, with significant improvement seen in treated groups relative to wait-list controls, non-treatment would raise serious ethical concerns.7 The aim of therapygenetics is to discover predictors of differential response to treatment. These predictors need not capture a treatment effect per se; they may describe processes separate to treatment that nonetheless lead to better (or worse) response. Nevertheless, in the absence of a control group, this study specifically examines the association between genetic variation and change in CSR across the period of CBT treatment and follow-up, not the biological mechanism of response to CBT.

The naturalistic nature of the cohort creates heterogeneity, including differences in the details of the treatment given, the target disorder of the treatment, and several participant characteristics. The effectiveness of CBT is influenced by a variety of environmental factors. Some of these can be considered within the design, such as treatment type, diagnosis and severity. Others are less easily accounted for, including therapeutic alliance and other social influences, which may only be partly controlled for by the inclusion of trial as a covariate.18,46 This reduces the statistical power of analyses, but should not be viewed as an argument against therapygenetics. The ability to offer personalised advice to patients about treatment could avoid considerable amounts of unnecessary distress and expense. Obtaining a set of genes able to assist in clinical prediction will require a cohort that is powerful enough to detect true variants while remaining clinically representative. Thus, a degree of heterogeneity is unavoidable in studying response to CBT, and similar difficulties in pharmacogenetic GWAS suggest this limitation applies to treatment response genomics more generally.

Combining data from trials at multiple sites necessitated compromises in study design. Participants were included if they completed treatment, but drop-out from treatment is common and likely to be related to poorer response. As such, future studies should aim to include severity data for non-completing participants. This would require appropriate modelling of the treatment period, and the proportion of the treatment process completed, before participation ceased. Similarly, combining measurements from different sites and from participants with varying diagnoses required the use of a general, widely applicable outcome measure. The ADIS fit these requirements well, but relies on clinical judgement derived from parent and child report. It may be less sensitive to the effects of CBT than a self-report measure, and be more vulnerable to site-specific biases. However, a suitable diagnosis-general self-report scale was unavailable, and standardising outcomes to combine multiple diagnosis-specific scales is likely to lead to a generalised and difficult-to-interpret result.

Future directions

This study represents the first GWAS of psychological therapy. Although no genome-wide significant findings emerged, the spread of significance in the associations captured is similar to other early general psychiatric and pharmacogenetic GWAS. The best approach in the immediate future is to increase the sample size available through combining existing cohorts in mega- and meta-analyses. Such a cohort would allow replication of the findings presented in this paper to be attempted, which currently is not possible due to the lack of an independent cohort of suitable size. However, individual variants are likely to have small effect sizes, so future studies should utilise higher-order approaches such as polygenic risk scoring and GREML to leverage the predictive effects of the whole genome. This would also provide an estimate of heritability, which is difficult to obtain through traditional family-based approaches. If the heritability of CBT response were around 30% (similar to that of anxiety disorders), a high-powered polygenic risk score could capture 10–15% of variance, which could be clinically useful when combined with known environmental risk factors.47 However, creating such a score will require a sample size of at least 10 000, which would involve considerable effort to obtain.

Alternative approaches may also yield interesting findings. Response to CBT is a behavioural change following exposure to a positive environment, so epigenetic studies investigating how these exposures influence gene expression via DNA methylation will be informative.48 Similarly, it will be useful to examine changes in gene transcript expression across treatment and in the longer term. Used in parallel to these approaches, studying specific genetic variants remains a potential method of predicting response to CBT (and understanding its biological basis) and genome-wide investigations represent the most promising avenue in which to focus the gathering momentum of therapygenetics.49

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank: Irma Knuistingh Neven, Marianne Bjerregaard Madsen, Lisbeth Jørgensen, Carmen Adornetto, Kristine Fonnes Griffin, Krister Fjermestad, Elisa Kulewski, Jonas Großekathöfer, Nora Dirks, Evelyn Kmelnitski, Sabrina Heuser, Adrian Boyle, Clare Dixon, Harma Moorlag, Nienke Boersma, Sanne Hogendoorn, Sue Cruddace, Marie Weber, Zoe Hughes, Kiri Clarke, Francoise Hentges, Liz White, Nynke Wagenaar, Luci Motoca, Yesenia Rodriguez, Klaudia Perreira, Erin Hedemann, Cristina Del Busto, Gillian O'Neill, Lucy Lindley, Rachel Banham, Nora Delvendahl, Hjalti Jonsson, Judith Blatter-Meunier, Chantal Herren, Odd E. Havik, Karen Krause, Anna McKinnon, Patrick Smith and Yasmin Rey; and all participants in the study.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

R.M.R., J.L.H. and H.J.L. are co-authors of the Cool Kids program but receive no direct payments. C. Creswell is co-author of books in the ’Overcoming’ series and receives royalties. W.K.S. is author of the Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for Children and receives royalties. G.B. is a consultant in pre-clinical genetics for Eli Lilly.

Funding

Combined study supported by UK Medical Research Council grant G0901874/1 to T.C.E. Individual trials support by Australian Research Council grant DP0878609 to J.L.H., R.M.R. and T.C.E.; Australian NHMRC grants to R.M.R., J.L.H. and H.J.L. (1027556), H.J.L, J.L.H and R.M.R (488505), and J.L.H and R.M.R. (382008); TrygFonden grant (7-10-1391) to M.T.; Edith og Godtfred Kirk Christiansens Fond grant (21-5675) to M.T.; Swiss National Science Foundation grant (105314-116517) to S.S., Western Norway Regional Health Authority grant to E.R.H. (911366); UK Medical Research Council Clinical Fellowship (G0802821) to R.M-S.; NIMH R01 (MH079943) to W.K.S.; UK NIHR grants to C. Creswell and P.C. (PB-PG-0110-21190) and P.C. and C. Creswell (PB-PG-0107-12042); UK Medical Research Council Grants to P.C. and C. Creswell (09-800-17), K.T., P.C. and C. Creswell (G0802326), P.W., C. Creswell and P.C. (G1002011), and C. Creswell (G0601874). Grant 09/800/17 was managed by National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) on behalf of the MRC-NIHR partnership. This study presents independent research part-funded by the NIHR Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King's College London. The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health.

References

  • 1. Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Merikangas KR, Walters EE. Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005; 62: 593–602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Fineberg NA, Haddad PM, Carpenter L, Gannon B, Sharpe R, Young AH, et al. The size, burden and cost of disorders of the brain in the UK. J Psychopharm 2013; 27: 761–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Baxter AJ, Vos T, Scott KM, Ferrari AJ, Whiteford HA. The global burden of anxiety disorders in 2010. Psychol Med 2014; 44: 2363–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Bittner A, Egger HL, Erkanli A, Costello EJ, Foley DL, Angold A. What do childhood anxiety disorders predict? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2007; 48: 1174–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Gregory A, Caspi A, Moffitt T, Koenen K, Eley T, Poulton R. Juvenile mental health histories of adults with anxiety disorders. Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164: 301–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. National Collaborating Centre for Mental H Common Mental Health Disorders: Identification and Pathways to Care. British Psychological Society & Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2011. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. James AC, James G, Cowdrey FA, Soler A, Choke A. Cognitive behavioural therapy for anxiety disorders in children and adolescents. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2013; 6: CD004690. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Eley TC, Hudson JL, Creswell C, Tropeano M, Lester KJ, Cooper P, et al. Therapygenetics: the 5HTTLPR and response to psychological therapy. Mol Psychiatry 2012; 17: 236–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Lester KJ, Eley TC. Therapygenetics: using genetic markers to predict response to psychological treatment for mood and anxiety disorders. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord 2013; 3: 1–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Lester KJ, Roberts S, Keers R, Coleman JRI, Breen G, Wong CCY, et al. Non-replication of the association between 5HTTLPR and response to psychological therapy for child anxiety disorders. Br J Psychiatry 2016, 208: 182–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Uher R. The implications of gene-environment interactions in depression: will cause inform cure? Mol Psychiatry 2008; 13: 1070–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Shimada-Sugimoto M, Otowa T, Hettema JM. Genetics of anxiety disorders: genetic epidemiological and molecular studies in humans. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2015; 69: 388–401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Erhardt A, Akula N, Schumacher J, Czamara D, Karbalai N, Muller-Myhsok B, et al. Replication and meta-analysis of TMEM132D gene variants in panic disorder. Transl Psychiatry 2012; 2: e156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Otowa T, Kawamura Y, Nishida N, Sugaya N, Koike A, Yoshida E, et al. Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies for panic disorder in the Japanese population. Transl Psychiatry 2012; 2: e186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Xie P, Kranzler HR, Yang C, Zhao H, Farrer LA, Gelernter J. Genome-wide association study identifies new susceptibility loci for posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2013; 74: 656–63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Nievergelt CM, Maihofer AX, Mustapic M, Yurgil KA, Schork NJ, Miller MW, et al. Genomic predictors of combat stress vulnerability and resilience in U.S. Marines: a genome-wide association study across multiple ancestries implicates PRTFDC1 as a potential PTSD gene. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2015; 51: 459–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Guffanti G, Ashley-Koch AE, Roberts AL, Garrett ME, Solovieff N, Ratanatharathorn A, et al. No association between RORA polymorphisms and PTSD in two independent samples. Mol Psychiatry 2014; 19: 1056–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Hudson JL, Keers R, Roberts S, Coleman JRI, Breen G, Arendt K, et al. Clinical predictors of response to cognitive-behavioral therapy in pediatric anxiety disorders: the Genes for Treatment (GxT) Study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2015; 54: 454–63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn) (DSM-IV). APA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Silverman WK, Albano AM. Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule (ADIS-IV) Child and Parent Interview Schedules: 5 of Each. Graywind Publications, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Schneider S, Unnewehr S, Margraf J. Diagnostisches interview psychischer Störungen im Kindes-und Jugendalter (Kinder-DIPS) [Diagnostic Interview for Mental Disorders in Childhood and Adolescence]. Springer, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Streiner DL. Breaking up is hard to do: the heartbreak of dichotomizing continuous data. Can J Psychiatry 2002; 47: 262–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Uher R, Perroud N, Ng MY, Hauser J, Henigsberg N, Maier W, et al. Genome-wide pharmacogenetics of antidepressant response in the GENDEP project. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167: 555–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Freeman B, Smith N, Curtis C, Huckett L, Mill J, Craig I. DNA from buccal swabs recruited by mail: evaluation of storage effects on long-term stability and suitability for multiplex polymerase chain reaction genotyping. Behav Genet 2003; 33: 67–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Gunderson KL, Steemers FJ, Ren H, Ng P, Zhou L, Tsan C, et al. Whole-genome genotyping. In Methods Enzymol (eds Alan K, Brian O.): 359–76. Academic Press, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Goldstein JI, Crenshaw A, Carey J, Grant GB, Maguire J, Fromer M, et al. zCall: a rare variant caller for array-based genotyping: genetics and population analysis. Bioinformatics 2012; 28: 2543–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. The 1000 Genomes Project Consortium An integrated map of genetic variation from 1,092 human genomes. Nature 2012; 491: 56–65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Howie BN, Donnelly P, Marchini J. A flexible and accurate genotype imputation method for the next generation of genome-wide association studies. PLoS Genet 2009; 5: e1000529. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Yang J, Zaitlen NA, Goddard ME, Visscher PM, Price AL. Advantages and pitfalls in the application of mixed-model association methods. Nat Genet 2014; 46: 100–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Yang J, Lee SH, Goddard ME, Visscher PM. GCTA: a tool for genome-wide complex trait analysis. Am J Hum Genet 2011; 88: 76–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Purcell S, Cherny SS, Sham PC. Genetic Power Calculator: design of linkage and association genetic mapping studies of complex traits. Bioinformatics 2003; 19: 149–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Purcell S, Neale B, Todd-Brown K, Thomas L, Ferreira MA, Bender D, et al. PLINK: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. Am J Hum Genet 2007; 81: 559–75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Chang C, Chow C, Tellier L, Vattikuti S, Purcell S, Lee J. Second-generation PLINK: rising to the challenge of larger and richer datasets. GigaScience 2015; 4: 7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Ripke S, Lewis CM, Lin D-Y, Wray N. A mega-analysis of genome-wide association studies for major depressive disorder. Mol Psychiatry 2012; 18: 497–511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature 2014; 511: 421–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Tansey KE, Guipponi M, Perroud N, Bondolfi G, Domenici E, Evans D, et al. Genetic predictors of response to serotonergic and noradrenergic antidepressants in major depressive disorder: a genome-wide analysis of individual-level data and a meta-analysis. PLoS Med 2012; 9: e1001326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Consortium IH Integrating common and rare genetic variation in diverse human populations. Nature 2010; 467: 52–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Visscher PM, Hemani G, Vinkhuyzen AAE, Chen G-B, Lee SH, Wray NR, et al. Statistical power to detect genetic (Co)variance of complex traits using SNP data in unrelated samples. PLoS Genet 2014; 10: e1004269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Hyman S. Mental health: depression needs large human-genetics studies. Nature 2014; 515: 189–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Lonsdorf TB, Ruck C, Bergstrom J, Andersson G, Ohman A, Lindefors N, et al. The COMTval158met polymorphism is associated with symptom relief during exposure-based cognitive-behavioral treatment in panic disorder. BMC Psychiatry 2010; 10: 9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Bosker F, Hartman C, Nolte I, Prins B, Terpstra P, Posthuma D, et al. Poor replication of candidate genes for major depressive disorder using genome-wide association data. Mol Psychiatry 2010; 16: 516–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Ioannidis JPA. Genetic associations: false or true? Trends Mol Med 2003; 9: 135–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Reif A, Richter J, Straube B, Höfler M, Lueken U, Gloster A, et al. MAOA and mechanisms of panic disorder revisited: from bench to molecular psychotherapy. Mol Psychiatry 2014; 19: 122–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Ising M, Lucae S, Binder EB, Bettecken T, Uhr M, Ripke S, et al. A genomewide association study points to multiple loci that predict antidepressant drug treatment outcome in depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2009; 66: 966–75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Tansey KE, Guipponi M, Hu X, Domenici E, Lewis G, Malafosse A, et al. Contribution of common genetic variants to antidepressant response. Biol Psychiatry 2013; 73: 679–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Hudson JL, Kendall PC, Chu BC, Gosch E, Martin E, Taylor A, et al. Child involvement, alliance, and therapist flexibility: process variables in cognitive-behavioural therapy for anxiety disorders in childhood. Behav Res Ther 2014; 52: 1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Dudbridge F. Power and predictive accuracy of polygenic risk scores. PLoS Genet 2013; 9: e1003348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Roberts S, Lester KJ, Hudson JL, Rapee RM, Creswell C, Cooper PJ, et al. Serotonin tranporter methylation and response to cognitive behaviour therapy in children with anxiety disorders. Transl Psychiatry 2014; 4: e444. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Eley TC. The future of therapygenetics: where will studies predicting psychological treatment response from genomic markers lead? Depress Anxiety 2014; 31: 617–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The British Journal of Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Royal College of Psychiatrists

RESOURCES