Abstract
Background and objectives
Visuospatial working memory (WM) tasks performed concurrently or after an experimental trauma (traumatic film viewing) have been shown to reduce subsequent intrusive memories (concurrent or retroactive interference, respectively). This effect is thought to arise because, during the time window of memory consolidation, the film memory is labile and vulnerable to interference by the WM task. However, it is not known whether tasks before an experimental trauma (i.e. proactive interference) would also be effective. Therefore, we tested if a visuospatial WM task given before a traumatic film reduced intrusions. Findings are relevant to the development of preventative strategies to reduce intrusive memories of trauma for groups who are routinely exposed to trauma (e.g. emergency services personnel) and for whom tasks prior to trauma exposure might be beneficial.
Methods
Participants were randomly assigned to 1 of 2 conditions. In the Tetris condition (n = 28), participants engaged in the computer game for 11 min immediately before viewing a 12-min traumatic film, whereas those in the Control condition (n = 28) had no task during this period. Intrusive memory frequency was assessed using an intrusion diary over 1-week and an Intrusion Provocation Task at 1-week follow-up. Recognition memory for the film was also assessed at 1-week.
Results
Compared to the Control condition, participants in the Tetris condition did not report statistically significant difference in intrusive memories of the trauma film on either measure. There was also no statistically significant difference in recognition memory scores between conditions.
Limitations
The study used an experimental trauma paradigm and findings may not be generalizable to a clinical population.
Conclusions
Compared to control, playing Tetris before viewing a trauma film did not lead to a statistically significant reduction in the frequency of later intrusive memories of the film. It is unlikely that proactive interference, at least with this task, effectively influences intrusive memory development. WM tasks administered during or after trauma stimuli, rather than proactively, may be a better focus for intrusive memory amelioration.
Keywords: Intrusive memory, Trauma film paradigm, Working memory task interference, Tetris, Post-traumatic stress disorder, Memory consolidation, Trauma prevention
Highlights
-
•
When trauma is predictable (e.g. for emergency workers) preventative interventions delivered before trauma would be useful.
-
•
Could a visuospatial working memory (WM) task before analogue trauma reduce intrusions via proactive interference?
-
•
Participants played Tetris or had no task prior to viewing a traumatic film.
-
•
There was no statistically significant difference in intrusive memory frequency of the trauma film between both groups.
-
•
WM tasks delivered during/after trauma stimuli, rather than before, may be a better focus for intrusive memory amelioration.
1. Introduction
The presence of recurrent, involuntary and intrusive distressing memories of a traumatic event are common in the early aftermath of psychological trauma and are a hallmark of both Acute Stress Disorder (ASD) and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD; DSM-5, American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Such negative, intrusive memories are proposed to occur due to excessive perceptual (sensory) processing during a trauma (Brewin, 2014, Ehlers and Clark, 2000) resulting in sensory-based (predominately visual) images of the trauma that intrude into mind spontaneously.
In this special issue, we pay tribute to Van den Hout's pioneering work. Among so many other things, his work with colleagues provides an experimental psychopathology test of the above clinical theory. An experiment using the trauma film paradigm revealed that the style in which an individual processes a traumatic event influences later intrusive memories (Kindt, Van den Hout, Arntz, & Drost, 2008). Participants who were instructed to engage in conceptually-driven processing, relative to those who engaged in sensory-based, data-driven processing, reported fewer intrusive memories to the traumatic film (Experiment 1) and had a reduced tendency to suppress intrusive memories (Experiment 2). Results may also indicate that tasks which interfere with data-driven processing (e.g. sensory-perceptual, visuospatial-based tasks) may be beneficial in reducing the occurrence of intrusive memories (for a diagramatic summary see, Holmes & Bourne, 2008).
Understanding possible processes to prevent later trauma symptoms is an important task for experimental psychopathology research. Many individuals are routinely exposed to events that are stressful and traumatic, for example in the work place (e.g. emergency services, military personnel) or in hostile environments (e.g. war zones) and are at risk of developing ASD and PTSD (Breslau et al., 1998, Bryant et al., 2012, Kessler et al., 1995). In such instances where exposure to traumatic events is predictable, the development of pre-emptive strategies (e.g. methods that can be used before entering a traumatic situation) to reduce traumatic stress symptoms, such as intrusive memories, may be clinically useful. A recent line of our work has tested the delivery of cognitive tasks during (Bourne et al., 2010, Holmes et al., 2004) and after traumatic stimuli (Deeprose et al., 2012, Holmes et al., 2009, Holmes et al., 2010, James et al., 2015), but not before. Thus, it remains to be tested whether such cognitive tasks can interfere when delivered prior to the encoding of traumatic stimuli.
Memory impairment as a consequence of interference has been widely researched in the (non-clinical) memory literature, where interference is proposed to be a primary source of forgetting (e.g. Dewar et al., 2007, Muller and Pilzecker, 1900). Retroactive and proactive interference are two forms of interference (Wixted, 2004). Retroactive interference describes incidences where new information impedes the recall of previously learnt events (i.e. interference after the event). In contrast, proactive interference describes instances where prior information interferes with the recall of more recent events (i.e. interference before the event). Third, concurrent interference effects can occur when two tasks are undertaken simultaneously leading to impairment compared to if tasks were undertaken separately, as has been investigated using dual task paradigms (Silver et al., 2013). Critical to the current study hypothesis, memory interference theory suggests that tasks, regardless of their order presentation, can interfere with one another (Wixted, 2004). A memory interference perspective thus holds relevance to experimental psychopathology research on emotional memories, for example in considering the order and impact of cognitive tasks which may (or may not) interfere with such memories. We next discuss studies involving concurrent interference from Van den Hout and colleagues, then retroactive interference with cognitive tasks administered after trauma film viewing, and then note a gap in knowledge regarding proactive interference and trauma memory, with a fuller discussion of proactive interference from the non-clinical literature.
Marcel Van den Hout has pioneered research which suggests that certain working memory (WM) tasks undertaken concurrently with recalling memories of distressing events can reduce the vividness and/or emotionality of that memory. For example, Van den Hout, Muris, Salemink, and Kindt (2001) asked participants to recall a negative autobiographical memory whilst either engaging in a concurrent eye-movement task (tracking the experimenter's rapid hand movements), a concurrent rhythmic tapping task or no concurrent task. Results showed reduced emotionality and vividness for the memory in the eye-movement condition only. A further elegant clinical experiment with patients with PTSD compared three concurrent task manipulations: memory recall plus eye movements, memory recall plus alternate auditory tones, and memory recall in isolation. Findings indicated that memory recall plus eye movements reduced emotionality and vividness to a greater extent than memory recall plus tones and memory recall only. The authors argued that given the evidence, the use of tones as an alternative to eye movements within Eye Movement Desensitization and Reprocessing (EMDR) therapy was premature (Van den Hout et al., 2012). These studies exemplify Van den Hout's mastery and commitment in using experimental findings to inform clinical practice.
According to a WM account, simultaneously recalling an emotional memory and making eye movements compete for limited WM resources leaving less capacity for the memory resulting in it becoming less vivid and emotional (Gunter and Bodner, 2008, Van den Hout and Engelhard, 2012, Van den Hout et al., 2011). Van den Hout and Engelhard (2012) have reviewed experiments investigating the impact of various cognitive tasks on the recall of negative memories. As a variety of tasks, not just eye movements, was found to exert similar effects, the authors proposed that any task appropriately taxing WM should attenuate the vividness and emotionality of the memory (see also Gunter & Bodner, 2008). It is noted that an alternative account (cf. Baddeley & Andrade, 2000) has been proposed in earlier studies (e.g. Andrade et al., 1997, Van den Hout et al., 2001) hypothesizing the locus of the effect to be modality specific, e.g. via competing resources between visual WM task with visual aspects of the memory within the visuospatial sketchpad. This modality-specific proposal has been favoured to explain related findings from our lab in which engagement in a visuospatial WM task leads to a reduction in visual intrusive memory frequency after an experimental trauma (a traumatic film) as compared to verbal WM tasks (e.g. Bourne et al., 2010, Holmes et al., 2010), at least in some studies.
Results from different experimental paradigms may explain the discrepant views. While Van den Hout and colleagues research generally models voluntary retrieval of autobiographical memories with vividness/emotionality as outcome measures, our work above has modelled involuntary intrusive memories to trauma films and their frequency. Further research is needed to determine the precise WM mechanisms of emotional memory interference across paradigms and emotional memory types. Nevertheless, such paradigms are united in an interest in understanding cognitive mechanisms that might underlie the modulation of memory following trauma exposure. Furthermore, both rely on the notion of interference effects, and for both the temporal constrains of memory interference remain to be tested.
We have sought to test whether visuospatial WM tasks can reduce intrusive memories using the trauma film paradigm (e.g. Holmes et al., 2009, Holmes et al., 2010). In this laboratory-based paradigm, participants view short films containing scenes depicting traumatic events which involve actual/threatened death or serious injury, as an experimental analogue of viewing a traumatic event (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Subsequently, the occurrence of intrusive memories is recorded, over the course of 1-week in daily life, or on a laboratory intrusion provocation task. Visuospatial WM tasks (such as tapping a complex sequence on a keypad held out of sight) performed during viewing of traumatic film footage, i.e. concurrent memory interference, reduce the number of image-based intrusive memories of the film relative to non-visuospatial control conditions (Bourne et al., 2010, Holmes et al., 2004, Stuart et al., 2006). Further, a visuospatial WM task performed immediately after film viewing, i.e. retroactive interference, show the same pattern of results (Deeprose et al., 2012). A cognitive task procedure involving a brief reminder task for the film followed by playing the visuospatial-based computer game ‘Tetris’ (Pajitnov & Nelson, 2008) relative to a non-visuospatial-based computer game, some time after exposure to traumatic film footage also reduces intrusive memory frequency over the subsequent week (e.g. when played 30 min post-film, Holmes et al., 2009; and 4 h post-film, Holmes et al., 2010).
We have interpreted such findings as follows: after an event, memory undergoes a time-dependent process of stabilization, termed consolidation (McGaugh, 2000). During this period of consolidation, memory is fragile and vulnerable to interference (Nader et al., 2000, Walker et al., 2003). Visuospatial WM tasks presented concurrently or after experimental trauma, while memory is undergoing consolidation, are hypothesised to interfere concurrently or retroactively (Dewar et al., 2007, Wixted, 2004, Wixted, 2005) with visual representations of the trauma film, resulting in a reduction in subsequent intrusive memories to the film (e.g. Holmes et al., 2009).
Thus far, our research has focussed exclusively on the use of WM tasks presented either concurrent with or soon after experimental trauma (Deeprose et al., 2012, Holmes et al., 2009, Holmes et al., 2010). However, research suggests that memory interference can also occur proactively, as defined above (Wixted, 2004, Wixted, 2005). Proactive interference has been proposed to be the main cause of normal forgetting in everyday life (Underwood, 1957). Experimentally, proactive interference in memory has been observed using a range of paradigms, such as masking (Feredoes et al., 2006, Hartshorne, 2008), ‘AB–AC’ paired-associate cued recall (e.g. Henson, Shallice, Josephs, & Dolan, 2002), and Pavlovian learning paradigms (Bouton, 1993). Proactive interference effects have also been observed for high and low similarity items over the course of several days using word list discrimination tasks (for example; 7 days, Postman, 1962; 1 day, Underwood, 1957) and different tests of memory (recognition memory and recall memory tests completed consecutively over 2 days; Lustig & Hasher, 2002). The use of discrimination tasks have also been combined with dual task paradigms (where the simultaneous task is finger sequence tapping) demonstrating that dual tasks conditions led to an increase in proactive interference (Kane & Engle, 2000).
Proactive interference on emotional memory has also been investigated. For example, following repeated learning of emotional and neutral pictures and their specific spatial location on a computer screen, more recall mistakes were made for locations of emotional compared to neutral items, indicating that emotional items were more susceptible to proactive interference (Novak & Mather, 2009). Further, memory for novel associations between cues and contextual details was poorer when such cues had been previously associated with emotional as opposed to neutral items, again consistent with proactive interference effects (Mather & Knight, 2008). Using the trauma film paradigm to specifically study intrusive emotional memories, a relationship was found between two separate measures, one indexing individual differences in the ability to resist proactive interference and another measuring intrusion frequency following a trauma film (Verwoerd et al., 2011, Wessel et al., 2008). However, rather than studying separate tasks, the actual impact of a proactive WM task interference on intrusive emotional memory does not appear to have been examined.
We suggest that proactive interference effects on intrusive memory could be tested within the trauma film paradigm, using the type of cognitive task already known to interfere concurrently or retroactively with the film. Our previous studies have shown large effects on reducing intrusions when tasks were undertaken concurrently to (d = 0.80, Bourne et al., 2010) or 30 min after (d = 0.91; Holmes et al., 2009) trauma-film viewing. This suggests that the cognitive tasks exerted effects through concurrent or retroactive interference with the trauma-film memory. The cognitive tasks used were visuospatial – complex concealed pattern tapping or Tetris game play respectively. Memory interference theory suggests that interference from competing information (here the film and the task) may occur irrespective of the order of either event (Wixted, 2004). However, the impact of proactive interference (i.e. a task before trauma film viewing) remains to be tested.
The current study examined whether a visuospatial WM task (Tetris game play) played immediately prior to viewing experimental trauma (a film) would reduce subsequent intrusive memories of the film over one week, consistent with the idea that a WM task could proactively interfere with the consolidation of intrusive memories. Our power calculation for sample size assumed a large effect size for comparability to those studies in which visuospatial tasks were undertaken concurrently (Bourne et al., 2010) or soon after (Deeprose et al., 2012, Holmes et al., 2009, Holmes et al., 2010) experimental trauma. To recap, this question holds importance both theoretically and clinically. It allows us to examine whether theoretical predictions concerning proactive memory interference extend to intrusive emotional memory and using the trauma film paradigm (rather than just static emotional images). Given that there are many situations in which trauma are predictable, a preventative intervention which could be delivered prior to entering a traumatic situation would be useful.
We hypothesised that participants assigned to a cognitive task condition in which the computer game Tetris is played immediately before viewing traumatic film footage, relative to participants in a No-Task Control condition (participants do not play Tetris but instead sit quietly for the equivalent period of time prior to the film), would report:
-
i.
Fewer intrusive memories of the film over the subsequent week in daily life, as recorded in an intrusion diary.
-
ii.
Fewer intrusions in response to an Intrusion Provocation Task (IPT) administered at 1-week in the laboratory.
2. Materials and methods
2.1. Participants
Fifty six healthy volunteers (32 females) took part in the study and were remunerated for their participation. Participants were recruited from two university campuses and from the general public via advertisements in an online newspaper and in the community. Participants were eligible to take part if they; a) were between the ages of 18 and 65 years of age, b) reported no mental health problems, and c) were not familiar with the study or had not taken part in a study of a similar nature. Ethical approval was obtained from the University of Oxford Central University Research Ethics Committee [MSD/IDREC/C1/2011/102].
2.2. Study stimuli
2.2.1. Trauma film
The 12 min trauma film contained 11 scenes depicting actual or threatened death, and serious injury as an experimental analogue of viewing a traumatic event (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Each scene contained footage with different content e.g. a man drowning in the sea; a young girl hit by a car with blood dripping out of her ear. Film footage was presented on a 17 inch colour monitor. The film had been used in previous studies to induce image-based intrusions (Holmes et al., 2009; Exp 2 from Holmes et al., 2010, James et al., 2015).
2.3. Task
2.3.1. Tetris – a visuospatial WM task
The visuospatial-based computer game Tetris (Pajitnov & Nelson, 2008) involves manipulating a series of seven differently coloured and shaped 2-D geometric blocks using the arrow cursor keys on a standard keyboard. Blocks fall at a steady pace one at a time from the top of the playing screen in a random order. As they fall they can be moved from left to right and rotated 90°. The aim of the game is to place the blocks as they fall in such a way as to form continuous horizontal rows at the bottom of the screen. Participants were encouraged to use mental rotation when the blocks were falling. When a continuous row is made it is removed from the screen and the player is awarded points. Over the course of the game as more rows are completed the blocks descend faster. Participants played Tetris on a PC with no sound using the same monitor as film viewing.
2.4. Self-report questionnaires
2.4.1. Baseline measures
2.4.1.1. Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II)
Depressed mood was measured using the BDI-II (Beck, Brown, & Steer, 1996). The BDI-II has 21 self-report items each measured on a scale of 0–3. Scores range from 0 to 63, where higher scores indicate greater levels of depression. The BDI-II shows high internal validity, α = 0.81 (Beck, Steer, & Garbin, 1988).
2.4.1.2. State Trait Anxiety Inventory – Trait (STAI-T)
Trait anxiety was measured using the STAI-T (Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg, & Jacobs, 1983). The item has 20 items and contains several anxiety-absent items (e.g. I am content) that are reverse scored. Scores range from a minimum of 20 to a maximum of 80 with higher scores representing greater levels of trait anxiety. The STAI-T demonstrates good internal validity; α = 0.90 (Speilberger, Reheiser, Owen, & Sydeman, 2004).
2.4.1.3. Spontaneous Use of Imagery Scale (SUIS)
An individual's tendency to engage in spontaneous mental imagery in their day-to-day lives was measured using the SUIS (Reisberg, Pearson, & Kosslyn, 2003). Participants rate on a scale of 1 (never) to 5 (always) 12 items including; ‘When I hear a radio announcer or DJ I've never actually seen, I usually find myself picturing what they might look like’. Scores range from a minimum of 12 indicating ‘no use’ to 60 ‘high use’. The SUIS has been shown to have excellent internal consistency (α = 0.98; Reisberg et al., 2003).
2.4.1.4. Eysenck Personality Questionnaire – Neuroticism scale (EPQ-N)
The EPQ-N (Eysenck, Eysenck, & Barrett, 1985) contains 12 items. Each item requires a ‘yes’ or ‘no’ response. Positive scores are summed and range from zero indicating low neuroticism to 12 indicating high neuroticism. The EPQ-N has good internal validity; α = 0.84 (Eysenck et al., 1985).
2.4.1.5. Traumatic Experience Questionnaire (TEQ)
(Adapted from Foa, Ehlers, Clark, Tolin, & Orsillo, 1999). Participants reported their prior trauma history using a 12-item checklist adapted from criterion A of the Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale (Foa, 1995). Participants indicated whether they had experienced or witnessed each of a series of traumatic events. ‘Yes’ scores were summed and could range from 0 (no traumatic event) to 12 (each and every type of traumatic event experienced or witnessed).
2.4.2. Film-related mood and distress ratings
2.4.2.1. Pre- and post-film mood
Participants rated their levels of sadness, hopelessness depression, fear, horror, and anxiousness on six visual analogue scales (VAS) given both pre- and post-film. The VAS instructed participants to rate how they felt ‘right at this very moment’ and scales were anchored at one end with ‘not at all’ and the other ‘extremely’. A composite mood score was calculated by summing the six scales (James et al., 2015).
2.4.2.2. Film distress
Participants rated their distress in relation to the film after viewing had ended. An 11-point scale was used anchored from 0 (not at all) to 10 (extremely).
2.5. Manipulation checks
2.5.1. Film attention
Participants were asked to rate ‘how much attention did you pay to the film you just watched’ on an 11-point scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 10 (extremely).
2.5.2. Demand rating
All participants responded to the question, ‘How much do you predict that playing the game Tetris before viewing a distressing film (rather than watching it normally) would increase or decrease intrusive images of the film of the type you recorded in your diary?’ using a single VAS ranging from −10 (extreme decrease) to 10 (extreme increase) (based upon Holmes et al., 2004, Stuart et al., 2006) administered at the end of the second session on Day 7.
2.6. Intrusive memory measures for the trauma film
2.6.1. Intrusion diary
Participants were asked to keep a pen and paper, tabular diary of any image-based intrusive memories of the trauma film they experienced in their daily lives in the 7 days after the film viewing, based upon that used in previous studies (Holmes et al., 2004, Holmes et al., 2009, Holmes et al., 2010, James et al., 2015). Intrusive memories were described as ‘mental images’ (e.g. “in the form of pictures in your mind's eye”) and were defined as being spontaneous memories of the film that occur without deliberate recall. Participants noted down in the diary every time they had a spontaneous image of the film come to mind and were asked to briefly note the content of each intrusive memories (in order for it to be later matched to the film in content). Written and verbal instructions were provided with the diary.
2.6.1.1. Diary accuracy
A single VAS requested participants to ‘indicate how accurate you think your diary is’ on a scale from 1 (not at all accurate) to 10 (extremely accurate).
2.6.2. Intrusion Provocation Task (IPT)
In the laboratory-based IPT (adapted from James et al., 2015, Lang et al., 2009, Malik et al., 2014), participants viewed 11 blurred (using GIMP (2010) software, Gaussian Blur 2.0) static visual images – one from each scene of the trauma film. The images were presented for 2 s each on a 17 inch colour monitor using PowerPoint slideshow. Immediately afterwards participants had a 2 min break during which they were asked to sit with their eyes closed and press a specific key on a computer keyboard whenever they had an intrusive memory of the film.
2.7. Recognition memory
A verbal recognition memory test comprised 32 written statements regarding the film (as used in Holmes et al., 2009; Exp 2 Holmes et al., 2010). There were approximately three statements per film clip Participants were asked to evaluate each statement as true or false based on their memory of the film. Example statements include; ‘the bones in the girl's legs are seen to fall back into place’ (True/False) or ‘four small circles are made on the surface of the eye’ (True/False).
2.8. Data analysis
All data were examined for potential univariate outliers. No scores were more than 3 standard deviations away from the mean, and therefore no data were classified as outliers (Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996). Independent samples t-tests were used to compare means between conditions for baseline measures (BDI-II, STAI-T, SUIS, EPQ-N and TEQ), film distress ratings, manipulation checks (film attention and demand ratings), intrusive memory measures for the trauma film (intrusion diary frequency of intrusions, diary accuracy and IPT intrusion frequency) and recognition memory scores; repeated measures ANOVAs were used to compare mood from pre-to post-film. Nominal data were analysed using the chi-square test. All statistical tests were two-tailed and used an alpha level of 0.05.
2.8.1. Sample size estimation
Based on effect sizes of d = 0.91 found by Holmes et al. (2009; in which participants performed the Tetris task 30 min post-film viewing) and d = 0.80 found by Bourne et al. (2010; in which participants completed a visuospatial tapping task during trauma film viewing), the current study assumed a large but more conservative effect size of 0.8. A minimum sample size of N = 26 per condition is required to ensure an 80% power to detect this difference at the 5% significance level.
2.9. Procedure
Participants completed 2 sessions, 7 days apart, and were tested individually. The experimenter was present for all procedures except film viewing. See Fig. 1 for a procedural diagram.
Session 1 (Day 1). Upon arrival at the laboratory participants gave written informed consent and then completed pen and paper baseline measures. Following this, all participants in the Tetris condition completed a 1 min practice session of the computer game Tetris with experimenter guidance, followed immediately by a further 10 min play with no guidance. Participants in the Tetris condition were instructed to play using their dominant hand. Participants in the No-Task control condition sat quietly for an equivalent time period (11 min). Participants then watched the trauma film in a darkened room with the experimenter absent. Visual Analogue Scale (VAS) mood evaluations were taken before and immediately after film viewing. Attention paid to the film and distress ratings were also given after film viewing. Participants were then provided with detailed instructions (both verbally and written) on how to keep the intrusion diary over the course of the subsequent 7 days and were asked to return to the laboratory in 7 days.
Session 2 (Day 7). One week later participants returned to the laboratory with their intrusion diaries. Participants completed a verbal recognition memory test for the film and underwent the Intrusion Provocation Task. Finally participants completed demand and diary accuracy ratings, and were debriefed at the end of the experiment.
3. Results
3.1. Baselines measures
There were no significant differences between conditions for baseline measures (Table 1).
Table 1.
Measure | Tetris (N = 28) | No-task control (N = 28) | Analysis | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n | % | n | % | χ2 | df | p | |
Female | 13 | 46.4 | 19 | 67.9 | 2.63 | 1 | 0.11 |
M | SD | M | SD | t(54) | p | ||
Age (years) | 20.46 | 2.03 | 20.71 | 1.54 | 0.52 | 0.61 | |
BDI-II | 7.14 | 7.07 | 7.07 | 5.63 | 0.04 | 0.97 | |
STAI-T | 39.64 | 11.39 | 36.32 | 9.66 | 1.18 | 0.25 | |
SUIS | 35.86 | 7.28 | 38.79 | 7.69 | 1.46 | 0.15 | |
EPQ-N | 4.86 | 3.27 | 4.64 | 2.16 | 0.29 | 0.77 | |
TEQ | 0.89 | 0.83 | 1.07 | 1.15 | 0.67 | 0.51 |
Note. BDI-II = Beck Depression Inventory – II (Beck et al., 1996); EPQ-N = Eysenck Personality Questionnaire – Neuroticism Scale (Eysenck et al., 1985); STAI = State Trait Anxiety Inventory – Trait (Spielberger et al., 1983); SUIS = Spontaneous Use of Imagery Scale (Reisberg et al., 2003); TEQ = Traumatic Experiences Questionnaire (adapted from Foa et al., 1999).
3.2. Film related mood and distress ratings
3.2.1. Mood pre-to post-film
Repeated measures ANOVA confirmed a main effect of time, indicating participants' mood significantly deteriorated following the film. There was no main effect of group, or a group × time interaction. There were no significant differences between conditions for ratings of film distress (Table 2).
Table 2.
Measure | Tetris (N = 28) | No-task control (N = 28) | ANOVA | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | Time | Group | Group*Time | ||
Pre film mood VAS | 8.93 | 10.00 | 7.55 | 4.52 | F(1,54) = 68.16§ | F(1,54) = .08 | F(1,54) = 0.24 | |
Post film mood VAS | 21.11 | 11.66 | 21.25 | 12.82 | ||||
t(54) | p | |||||||
Film distress | 7.04 | 1.95 | 7.25 | 2.01 | 0.40 | 0.69 |
Note. VAS (Visual Analogue Scale) mood composite sum of Sadness, Hopelessness, Depressed, Fear, Horror and Anxiousnes. §P ≤ .001.
3.3. Intrusive memory and recognition memory in relation to the trauma film
There was no statistically significant difference in the number of intrusive memories reported in the diary in the week following trauma film viewing, or the IPT given at 1-week between conditions. Further, participants were comparable for self-rated measures of diary accuracy. Scores on the verbal recognition memory test for the film did not differ significantly between conditions (Table 3).
Table 3.
Measure | Tetris (N = 28) |
No-task control (N = 28) |
Analysis |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | t(54) | p | d | |
Intrusive memory frequency in diary | 4.86 | 3.70 | 6.00 | 4.91 | 0.98 | 0.33 | 0.26 |
Intrusive memory frequency during IPT | 5.00 | 3.68 | 4.57 | 2.78 | 0.49 | 0.63 | 0.13 |
Verbal recognition memory test score | 19.89 | 3.61 | 19.75 | 3.00 | 0.16 | 0.87 | 0.04 |
Diary accuracy | 7.57 | 1.87 | 7.82 | 1.76 | 0.51 | 0.61 | 0.14 |
Note. IPT = Intrusion Provocation Task.
3.4. Manipulation checks
3.4.1. Attention to film and demand rating
Participants did not significantly differ between conditions on ratings of attention paid to the film or experimental demand (Table 4).
Table 4.
Measure | Tetris (N = 28) |
No-task control (N = 28) |
Analysis |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | t(54) | p | |
Attention to film | 9.11 | 1.03 | 9.32 | 0.77 | 0.88 | 0.38 |
Demand rating | 0.29 | 3.56 | 0.11 | 4.24 | 0.17 | 0.87 |
4. Conclusion and discussion
The current experiment reports null findings, with potential clinical and theoretical relevance. Results failed to support our initial hypothesis: i.e. compared to a control condition, participants who played the computer game Tetris immediately before viewing traumatic film material did not report statistically significantly fewer intrusive memories of the film in the subsequent week as measured by (i) the intrusion diary in daily life or (ii) the Intrusion Provocation Task (IPT) at the 1-week follow up in the laboratory. The estimates of effect size for each intrusion measure were small, as shown by the size of the Cohen's d statistic. Thus, current results using a proactive interference design stand in contrast to the large effect sizes found in concurrent/retroactive interference studies with a trauma film paradigm (Bourne et al., 2010, Holmes et al., 2009). While it is possible there is a small effect of Tetris on reducing diary intrusions when played before the trauma film, corresponding to our d value of 0.26, future studies would be required to test this a priori. To have sufficient power to detect such an effect a sample of N = 468 would be required. Nevertheless, if this was the case such a reduction might suggest clinical potential given that such an intervention would be relatively low intensity to deliver. Overall, conclusions should be drawn with caution and current results suggest that a from an experimental psychopathology perspective, interventions to reduce intrusive memories using a WM interference rationale may not be as effective if administered pre-trauma, compared to the same cognitive task administered during (Bourne et al., 2010) or after trauma (Deeprose et al., 2012, Holmes et al., 2009).
Participants in Tetris and No-Task Control conditions were matched at baseline for levels of depression, trait anxiety, general imagery use, neuroticism and prior traumatic history. Moreover, change in mood pre- and post-film, attention paid to the film, distress relating to the film, diary accuracy, and recognition memory scores of the film at 1-week follow-up were not significantly different between conditions, suggesting that these factors are also unlikely to account for the current null findings.
Results also raise questions about the extension of proactive interference predictions from the non-clinical memory literature to emotional intrusive memory. The memory interference literature assumes interference will occur regardless of the order of the presentation tasks (Wixted, 2004). Thus we had hypothesised that Tetris played before traumatic film viewing may interfere with the processing of traumatic film content. However, current results failed to support this in relation to a statistically significant difference in the frequency of intrusions between groups. Why might this be? One argument from the memory interference literature would be that current findings may be due to the lack of similarity between the computer game Tetris and the film footage thus yielding insufficient interference. Interference stimuli used in the masking literature (e.g. Hartshorne, 2008) share similar properties such as category relatedness (e.g. same material type) and spatial similarity (e.g. shared location). Early studies showed proactive interference across variety of paradigms, even with relatively low similarity and with temporal variation (Underwood, 1957). More recent evidence suggests similarity-based interference is more effective (e.g. Bunting, 2006), though some instances of proactive interference have also been observed in paradigms with low similarity. For example, Lustig and Hasher (2002) used non-identical memory tasks performed on consecutive days to show proactive interference effects on a subsequent working memory span task 24 h later. However, we note this broader issue remains a puzzle to explore further since the Tetris task appeared sufficient in similarity to be able to retroactively interfere with the film (e.g. Holmes et al., 2009, Holmes et al., 2010), yet not so proactively.
Further, there are features which, we propose, make Tetris a good candidate for interfering with intrusive memories of the film. Shared features include the visuospatial nature of the different coloured, moving 2D blocks that comprise the game Tetris (Green & Bavelier, 2003) in relation to the colour 2D moving film over time, delivery on the same computer screen and so forth. Furthermore, the same task has been shown sufficient to interfere with the same film when delivered at other time frames. Yet, while this possibility seems unlikely there are other features such as the dynamic nature of Tetris game play and the film footage, which differ from kind of static images used more typically in proactive interference research (e.g. Novak & Mather, 2009), which could also contribute to current findings.
The interference literature also suggests temporal contingencies may be a factor influencing proactive interference. Thus one account for our findings may be that the Tetris game and film viewing were relatively as far apart in time, e.g. several minutes, compared to typical proactive interference research methods (in which the inter stimuli timing is milliseconds).
Another factor might be the duration of task relative to the film duration. Potentially, increasing the duration of pre-film Tetris game play could enhance its ability to interfere with consolidation of the 12 min trauma film content by increasing the extent of visual memory resources dedicated to representations of game play. The current play duration of 11 min was equivalent to studies in which Tetris was played after viewing a traumatic film but could be varied. Interestingly the ‘Tetris effect’ (Stickgold, Malia, Maguire, Roddenbury, & O'Connor, 2000) is a term given to the phenomenon whereby visual representations of Tetris persist in mind following extended periods of game play (for example, 1 h of game play, twice a day). Further, it has been shown that Tetris-themed memory traces interacted with remote autobiographical memories that were not of Tetris (Stickgold et al., 2000). Increasing the duration of pre-trauma Tetris game play to a level at which participants report experiencing the ‘Tetris effect’ (e.g. 1–2 h) might increase the impact on intrusive memories, although one might advise caution against excessive computer game play.
Other possible explanations for current findings relate to the degree of competition between prior Tetris game play (when in the form of a memory representation rather than actual game play) and the emotional nature of the film. Incoming emotional perceptual content “hijacks” attention (LaBar and Cabeza, 2006, Vuilleumier, 2005). Thus, the emotional content of the traumatic film may have overridden the ability of Tetris, when played prior to trauma film viewing, to be retained in short term memory to a sufficient degree to interfere with trauma film content. Another difference with our previous studies in which playing Tetris after a trauma film reduced later intrusive memories (e.g. Holmes et al., 2010), is that visual representations of emotional traumatic film content purportedly underwent interference from actual game play, rather than a memory representation of the game. Perhaps this alongside the emotionality of the film could weaken interference effects.
The current study focussed on proactive interference effects on intrusive memory. In addition, there was no statistically significant difference between conditions on recognition memory (d = 0.04). Interestingly, a previous study employing concurrent interference has shown a discrepancy between intrusive versus voluntary memory (Experiment 2, Bourne et al., 2010). We note this pattern challenges mainstream memory theories (e.g. Tulving, 2002) which would predict that disrupting memory consolidation would impact both involuntary and voluntary memory which are from the same underlying memory trace. However, our current study showed that a WM task administered to interfere proactively with the film did not impact either involuntary intrusions or a recognition test, suggesting neither forms of memory are disrupted.
Finally, we note that our previous studies used trauma-film reminder cues (memory reactivation task) prior to Tetris game play (Holmes et al., 2009, Holmes et al., 2010, James et al., 2015). Clearly one cannot deliver a reminder of a film before the film has been viewed. However, it is possible that reminders of the Tetris task during film viewing may have aided proactive interference effects. Recent work has shown that a that a cognitive procedure involving both a memory reactivation ask and Tetris game play is critical for reducing intrusions within a memory reconsolidation time-frame (e.g. when memory for the trauma film has consolidated >24 h old, James et al., 2015). The role of reminder cues (either for task or film), when using WM task interference procedures, should be further investigated.
Overall, on the basis of current evidence, these results permit the conclusion that a WM task of sufficient efficacy when presented after an experimental trauma (as in prior experiments) is not similarly efficacious when presented before an experimental trauma (the current experiment) to reduce intrusions. Hence, a critical question for further research is to delineate the optimal time frames for applying competing cognitive task procedures within a WM interference framework.
4.1. Limitations
The current study used an experimental trauma manipulation (viewing traumatic film footage) as an analogue for psychological trauma and subsequent intrusive memory symptoms in a healthy population. A common critique levied at the trauma film paradigm concerns whether indirect trauma in the form of film footage is sufficient to induce a traumatic response akin to that of ‘real-life’ trauma exposure. Recent guidelines in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders state that exposure to trauma through electronic media, television and movies in the line of work (e.g. police officers) can be considered a ‘traumatic event’ (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). In addition, research investigating the impact of electronic media exposure on acute stress symptoms found that individuals who viewed extended media footage of the Boston Bombings (approximately 6 h), showed increased acute stress symptoms, compared to individuals who experienced actual, ‘real life’ exposure to the same event (Holman, Garfin, & Silver, 2014). Taken together film footage, as in the kind used in the current study, may be a useful analogue method to prospectively study traumatic reactions and subsequent symptoms in experimental settings. The trauma film paradigm is now widely used by other researchers to investigate how individuals process (analogue) trauma and subsequent PTSD-like symptoms, including Van den Hout, to whom this special issue is addressed (Kindt et al., 2008).
The current study did not contain a self-report rating of task compliance following Tetris game play and the no-task control condition, which should be added to future studies. The addition of manipulation measure to check whether memory representation of Tetris persisted beyond game play or not would allow us to tease apart whether Tetris did not provide interference, or whether interference just did not impact intrusion frequency, should be included in future studies. Further, it would be worthwhile to include baseline measures of WM capacity in future studies to investigate the impact of individual differences in WM on interference susceptibility and subsequent intrusion development.
4.2. Conclusion
Previous studies have shown that engaging in tasks that have a WM component, for example those involving eye movements, either during or after autobiographical memory recall (e.g. Engelhard et al., 2010, Van den Hout and Engelhard, 2012) reduces the vividness/emotionality of those emotional memories. Relatedly, a procedure involving playing the computer game Tetris after traumatic film viewing (e.g. Holmes et al., 2009) reduces the frequency of later intrusive memories of that film. While such studies tested possible concurrent or retroactive memory interference effects, the possibility of proactive memory interference (Wixted, 2004, Wixted, 2005) remained to be tested. The current study therefore changed the temporal order of events such that the WM task (here Tetris game play) occurred prior to the analogue trauma exposure (film viewing). The current lack of statistically significant differences in intrusive memories between conditions is of clinical and theoretical interest. Results suggest the same cognitive task which was successful in reducing intrusions when delivered after analogue trauma exposure is not as effective as when administered prior to exposure. This has relevance for the development of preventative interventions for those clinical groups in which trauma can be predicted to occur. Results flag temporal constraints on the delivery of memory interference tasks for intrusive memories of emotional events, suggesting further work is needed to bridge the non-clinical and clinical literature. These divergent results between studies and methods raise intriguing questions about boundary conditions of memory interference mechanisms in relation to intrusive emotional memory.
Ultimately, the effect of Tetris game play on intrusion frequency remains to be empirically tested within a clinical setting in real life, for example with frontline responders who are repeatedly exposed to traumatic events in the line of work. We note that a current study is underway (ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT02080351; 2015) testing the retrospective impact of Tetris am emergency department with people who have experienced a road traffic accident. The results of future clinical investigations should seek to assess not only the potential benefits of an intervention but also possible negative side effects (Jonsson, Alaie, Parling, & Arnbert, 2014). As a hypothetical example (given current results), if for emergency personnel, it were possible that proactive interference reduced intrusive memories, then perhaps proactive interference could also interfere with other processes that one would not want to interfere with such as the need by emergency workers to use visuospatial capacity to quickly make decisions in their work setting, and this should be investigated.
The current experiment is just one example of how our research in experimental psychopathology has been influenced by the elegant studies on trauma and WM led by Marcel Van den Hout. Combining his clinical and experimental expertise, Marcel has made an unrivalled contribution to our understanding of the role of eye movements in the effective, but little understood treatment for PTSD – EMDR. His legacy creates numerous broader sources of inspiration too, including the investigation of additional WM tasks to help ameliorate unwanted negative, emotional memories and the further development of experimental paradigms through which to continue to explore the mechanisms underlying changes in related psychopathologies after trauma.
Acknowledgements
ELJ was supported by the Colt Foundation; EAH and ELJ by the Medical Research Council (UK) [MC-A060-5PR50]; EAH by a Wellcome Trust Clinical Fellowship [WT088217] and National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Oxford Biomedical Research Centre Programme; ALZ by the Cambridge Commonwealth, European & International Trust. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NIHR. We would like to thank Dr Stephanie Burnett Heyes and Dr Laura Hoppitt for their helpful comments on drafts of the manuscript, and Peter Watson for statistical help. We are grateful to LyleBailie International, Kino International, the British Film Institute National Archives, and David Large at the Royal College of Surgeons in Edinburgh for use of film-clips. Funding to pay the Open Access publication charges for this article was provided by the United Kingdom Medical Research Council.
References
- American Psychiatric Association . 5th ed. American Psychiatric Publishing; Arlington, VA: 2013. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. [Google Scholar]
- Andrade J., Kavanagh D.J., Baddeley A.D. Eye-movements and visual imagery: a working memory approach to the treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1997;36(2):209–223. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1997.tb01408.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley A.D., Andrade J. Working memory and the vividness of imagery. Journal of Experimental Psychology General. 2000;129(1):126–145. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.129.1.126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck A.T., Brown G.K., Steer R.A. Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1996. Beck Depression Inventory-II manual. [Google Scholar]
- Beck A.T., Steer R.A., Garbin M.C. Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review. 1988;8(1):77–100. [Google Scholar]
- Bourne C., Frasquilho F., Roth A.D., Holmes E.A. Is it mere distraction? Peri-traumatic verbal tasks can increase analogue flashbacks but reduce voluntary memory performance. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 2010;41(3):316–324. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouton M.E. Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. Psychological Bulletin. 1993;114(1):80–99. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breslau N., Kessler R.C., Chilcoat H.D., Schultz L.R., Davis G.C., Andreski A. Trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder in the community. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1998;55(7):626–632. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.7.626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brewin C.R. Episodic memory, perceptual memory and their interaction: foundations for a theory of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Bulletin. 2014;140(1):69–97. doi: 10.1037/a0033722. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bryant R.A., Creamer M., O'Donnell M., Silove D., McFarlane A.C. The capacity of acute stress disorder to predict posttraumatic psychiatric disorders. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2012;46(2):168–173. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2011.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bunting M. Proactive interference and item similarity in working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition. 2006;32(2):183–196. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.2.183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deeprose C., Zhang S., Dejong H., Dalgleish T., Holmes E.A. Imagery in the aftermath of viewing a traumatic film: using cognitive tasks to modulate the development of involuntary memory. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 2012;43(2):758–764. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.10.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dewar M.T., Cowan N., Della Salla S. Forgetting due to retroactive interference: a fusion of early insights into everyday forgetting and recent research on anterograde amnesia. National Institute of Health. 2007;43(5):616–634. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70492-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ehlers A., Clark D.M. A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2000;38(4):319–345. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(99)00123-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engelhard I.M., Van Uijen S.L., Van den Hout M.A. The impact of taxing working memory on negative and positive memories. European Journal of Psychotraumatology. 2010;1(5623):1–8. doi: 10.3402/ejpt.v1i0.5623. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eysenck S.B.G., Eysenck H.J., Barrett P. A revised version of the psychoticism scale. Personality and Individual Differences. 1985;6(1):21–29. [Google Scholar]
- Feredoes E., Tononi G., Postle B.R. Direct evidence for a prefrontal contribution to the control of proactive interference in verbal working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2006;103(51):19530–19534. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0604509103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foa E.B. National Computer Systems; Minneapolis, MN: 1995. The posttraumatic diagnostic scale (PDS) manual. [Google Scholar]
- Foa E.B., Ehlers A., Clark D.M., Tolin D.F., Orsillo S.M. The Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI): development and validation. Psychological Assessment. 1999;11(3):303–314. [Google Scholar]
- Green S.C., Bavelier D. Action video game modifies visual selective attention [letter to the editor] Nature. 2003;423(6939):534–537. doi: 10.1038/nature01647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gunter R.W., Bodner G.E. How eye movements affect unpleasant memories: support for a working memory account. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2008;46(8):913–931. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartshorne J.K. Visual working memory capacity and proactive interference. PLoS One. 2008;3:e2716. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henson R.N.A., Shallice T., Josephs O., Dolan R.J. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of proactive interference during spoken cued recall. Neuroimage. 2002;17:543–558. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holman E.A., Garfin D.R., Silver R.C. Media's role in broadcasting acute stress following the Boston marathon bombings. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2014;111(1):93–98. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1316265110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holmes E.A., Bourne C. Inducing and modulating intrusive emotional memories: a review of the trauma film paradigm. Acta Psychologica. 2008;127(3):553–566. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2007.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holmes E.A., Brewin C.R., Hennessy R.G. Trauma films, information processing, and intrusive memory development. Journal of Experimental Psychology General. 2004;133(1):3–22. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holmes E.A., James E.L., Coode-Bate T., Deeprose C. Can playing the computer game “Tetris” reduce the build-up of flashbacks for trauma? A proposal from cognitive science. PLoS One. 2009;4(1):e4153. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holmes E.A., James E.L., Kilford E.J., Deeprose C. Key steps in developing a cognitive vaccine against traumatic flashbacks: visuospatial Tetris versus verbal pub quiz. PLoS One. 2010;5(11):e13706. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- James E.L., Bonsall M.B., Hoppitt L., Tunbridge E.M., Geddes J.R., Milton A.L. Computer game play reduces intrusive memories of experimental trauma via reconsolidation update mechanisms. Psychological Science. 2015;26(8):1201–2015. doi: 10.1177/0956797615583071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jonsson U., Alaie I., Parling T., Arnbert F.K. Reporting of harms in randomized controlled trials of psychological interventions for mental and behavioral disorders: a review of current practice. Contemporary Clinical Trials. 2014;38(1):1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.cct.2014.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kane M.J., Engle R.W. Working-memory capacity, proactive interference, and divided attention: limits on long-term memory retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition. 2000;26(2):336–358. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.26.2.336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler R.C., Sonnega A., Bromet E., Hughes M., Nelson C.B. Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1995;52(12):1048–1060. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950240066012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kindt M., Van den Hout M., Arntz A., Drost J. The influence of data-driven versus conceptually-driven processing on the development of PTSD-like symptoms. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 2008;39:546–557. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LaBar K.S., Cabeza R. Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2006;7(1):54–64. doi: 10.1038/nrn1825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lang T.J., Moulds M.L., Holmes E.A. Reducing depressive intrusions via a computerized cognitive bias modification of appraisals task: developing a cognitive vaccine. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2009;47(2):139–145. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lustig C., Hasher L. Working memory span: the effect of prior learning. American Journal of Psychology. 2002;115(2):89–101. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malik A., Goodwin G.M., Hoppitt L., Holmes E.A. Hypomanic experience in young adults confers vulnerability to intrusive imagery after experimental trauma: relevance for bipolar disorder. Clinical Psychological Science. 2014;2(6):675–684. doi: 10.1177/2167702614527433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mather M., Knight M. The emotional harbinger effect: poor context memory for cues that previously predicted something arousing. Emotion. 2008;8(6):850–860. doi: 10.1037/a0014087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGaugh J.L. Memory – a century of consolidation. Science. 2000;287(5451):248–251. doi: 10.1126/science.287.5451.248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muller G.E., Pilzecker A. Vol. 1. 1900. pp. 1–288. (Experimentelle Beitrage zur Lehre vom Gedachtniss Zeitschift fur Psychologie). [Google Scholar]
- Nader K., Schafe G.E., LeDoux J.E. The labile nature of consolidation theory. Nature. 2000;1(3):216–219. doi: 10.1038/35044580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- National Institute of Health, University of Oxford . 2015. A simple cognitive task to reduce the build-up of flashbacks after a road traffic accident (SCARTA)https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT02080351 ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT02080351. 2015, from. [Google Scholar]
- Novak D.L., Mather M. The tenacious nature of memory binding for arounsing negative items. Memory and Cognition. 2009;37(7):945–952. doi: 10.3758/MC.37.7.945. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pajitnov A., Nelson C. The Tetris Company; 2008. Tetris zone. [Google Scholar]
- Postman L. The temporal course of proactive inhibition for serial lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1962;63(4):361–369. doi: 10.1037/h0048063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reisberg D., Pearson D.G., Kosslyn S.M. Intuitions and introspections about imagery: the role of imagery experience in shaping an investigator's theoretical views. Applied Cognitive Psychology. 2003;17(2):147–160. [Google Scholar]
- Silver R.C., Holman E.A., Anderson J.P., Poulin M., McIntosh D.N., Gil-Rivas V. Mental- and physical-health effects of acute exposure to media images of the September 11, 2001, attacks and the Iraq war. Psychological Science. 2013;24(9):1623–1634. doi: 10.1177/0956797612460406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spielberger C.D., Gorsuch R.L., Lushene R., Vagg P.R., Jacobs G.A. Consulting Psychologists Press; Palo Alto, CA: 1983. Manual for state-trait anxiety inventory. [Google Scholar]
- Speilberger C.D., Reheiser E.C., Owen A.E., Sydeman S.J. Vol. 3. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; New Jersey: 2004. (Measuring the psychological vital signs of anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity in treatment planning and outcomes assessment). [Google Scholar]
- Stickgold R., Malia A., Maguire D., Roddenbury D., O'Connor M. Replaying the game: hypnagogic images in normals and amnesics. Science. 2000;290(5490):350–353. doi: 10.1126/science.290.5490.350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stuart A.D.P., Holmes E.A., Brewin C.R. The influence of a visuospatial grounding task on intrusive images of a traumatic film. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2006;44(4):611–619. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.04.004. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2005.04.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tabachnick B.G., Fidell L.S. 3rd ed. Harper Collins; New York: 1996. Using multivariate statistics. [Google Scholar]
- Tulving E. Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology. 2002;53:1–25. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Underwood B.J. Interference and forgetting. Psychological Review. 1957;64(1):49–60. doi: 10.1037/h0044616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van den Hout M.A., Engelhard I.M. How does EMDR work? Journal of Experimental Psychopathology. 2012;3(5):724–738. [Google Scholar]
- Van den Hout M.A., Engelhard I.M., Rijkeboer M.M., Koekebakker J., Hornsveld H., Toffolo M.B.J. EMDR: eye movements superior to beeps in taxing working memory and reducing vividness of recollections. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2011;49(2):92–98. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van den Hout M.A., Muris P., Salemink E., Kindt M. Autobiographical memories become less vivid and emotional after eye movements. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2001;40(pt 2):121–131. doi: 10.1348/014466501163571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van den Hout M.A., Rijkeboer M.M., Engelhard I.M., Klugkist I., Hornsveld H., Toffolo M.J.B. Tones inferior to eye movements in the EMDR treatment of PTSD. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2012;50(5):275–279. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2012.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verwoerd J., Wessel I., de Jong P., Nieuwenhuis M., Huntjens R. Pre-Stressor interference control and intrusive memories. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2011;35(2):161–170. doi: 10.1007/s10608-010-9335-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vuilleumier P. How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences. 2005;9(12):585–594. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walker M.P., Brakefield T., Hobson J.A., Stickgold R. Dissociable stages of human memory consolidation and reconsolidation. Nature. 2003;425(6958):616–620. doi: 10.1038/nature01930. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wessel I., Overwijk S., Verwoerd J., de Vrieze N. Pre-stressor cognitive control is related to intrusive cognition of a stressful film. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2008;46(4):496–513. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.01.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wixted J.T. The psychology and neuroscience of forgetting. Annual Review of Psychology. 2004;55:235–269. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wixted J.T. A theory about why we forget what we once knew. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2005;14(1):6–9. [Google Scholar]