Abstract
This study examines the interplay between individual and social-developmental factors in the development of positive functioning, substance use problems, and mental health problems. This interplay is nested within positive and negative developmental cascades that span childhood, adolescence, the transition to adulthood, and adulthood. Data are drawn from the Seattle Social Development Project, a gender-balanced, ethnically diverse community sample of 808 participants interviewed 12 times from ages 10 to 33. Path modeling showed short- and long-term cascading effects of positive social environments, family history of depression, and substance using social environments throughout development. Positive family social environments set a template for future partner social environment interaction and had positive influences on proximal individual functioning, both in the next developmental period and long term. Family history of depression adversely affected mental health functioning throughout adulthood. Family substance use began a cascade of substance-specific social environments across development, which was the pathway through which increasing severity of substance use problems flowed. The model also indicated that adolescent, but not adult, individual functioning influenced selection into positive social environments, and significant cross-domain effects were found in which substance using social environments affected subsequent mental health.
Keywords: developmental cascades, substance abuse, mental health, positive functioning, positive social environments, substance use social environment
Developmental models predicting substance abuse and mental health have documented both negative and positive cascades. A developmental cascade is the longitudinal pathway examining how problems in individual functioning and environmental influences spill over into other domains of functioning and into subsequent developmental periods (Bornstein, Hahn, & Haynes, 2010; Lansford, Malone, Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, 2010; Masten & Cicchetti, 2010). Although research has established that social environments are critical to the development of both maladaptive and adaptive functioning, these social environmental influences are sometimes absent from cascade models of development. Furthermore, although it is well known that mental health and substance abuse outcomes are correlated, and may stem from similar etiological roots, their intertwined unfolding across the life course is rarely examined. Moreover, little is known about the development of positive functioning and its relationship to mental health and substance abuse. The present study builds on the social development model and the development cascades framework to model the interplay between individual and social factors in the development of positive functioning, mental health problems, and substance abuse in four developmental periods: childhood, adolescence, the transition to adulthood, and adulthood.
Theoretical Frameworks: Social Development Model and Developmental Cascades
Two frameworks provide the theoretical basis for this study: the social development model and the developmental cascades framework. The social development model outlines specific social environmental mechanisms within each developmental period that drive prosocial and antisocial functioning. The developmental cascades framework provides guidance in conceptualizing multiply determined adaptive and maladaptive processes of development over time (Masten, 2006). Putting these two theories together allows us to test a more holistic model of the interplay of individual functioning and social environmental influences in a life course informed, developmental model.
The social development model (SDM) organizes established risk and protective factors for prosocial and antisocial behavior into a developmental theory (Catalano, Kosterman, Hawkins, Newcomb, & Abbott, 1996). Building on social control theory (Hirschi, 1969), social learning theory (Bandura, 1977), and differential association theory (Sutherland & Cressey, 1970), the SDM provides an organizing framework for studying family, peer, and school and work influences in childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. The SDM articulates the mechanisms of socialization and identifies parallel but separate causal paths for prosocial and antisocial processes. On each path, four socialization processes establish a social bond between an individual and a socialization unit: (a) perception of opportunities for pro- or antisocial behavior; (b) involvement with pro- or antisocial groups; (c) social, emotional, and cognitive skills which enhance involvements and make recognition or reward more likely; and (d) the perception of rewards for interactions with pro- or antisocial groups. Each path is affected by position in the social structure (e.g., race, gender, and socioeconomic status) and by individual difference characteristics (e.g., internalizing or behavioral disinhibition). Following rewarding involvement, youth develop a bond to prosocial or antisocial others and adopt their beliefs, which in turn affects engaging in prosocial or antisocial (health-risk) behaviors. The processes of socialization outlined by the SDM serve to define the nature of the social environment in each developmental period in our model. The SDM has been tested in multiple datasets at different stages of development and was found to predict substance use and misuse, depression, and other problem behaviors as well as positive functioning in adulthood (Brown et al., 2005; Fleming, Catalano, Oxford, & Harachi, 2002; Huang, Kosterman, Catalano, Hawkins, & Abbott, 2001; Kosterman et al., 2014; Sullivan & Hirschfield, 2011).
The developmental cascades framework also guides the present study by theorizing the progression of adaptive and maladaptive development across the life course (Masten & Cicchetti, 2010). Stemming from the field of developmental psychopathology, developmental cascades refer to the within- and across-domain transactional or interactive processes of development from childhood to adulthood (Masten & Cicchetti, 2010). In this framework, outcomes are conceptualized as a result of interactions between individual functioning and environmental influences (Sameroff & MacKenzie, 2003). Another important characteristic of this approach is the focus on reciprocal spillover effects across domains of functioning, such as substance use and mental health. Mental health and substance use problems are often comorbid, and the social environments and individual behaviors within these domains likely influence each other. The bidirectional, cross-domain interactions between the individual and the social environment are important to analyze in order to understand the complexity of adaptive and maladaptive development (Masten, 2006; Masten & Cicchetti, 2010). By conceptualizing development as a series of cascades, we can learn why some pathways and not others result in lasting difficulties into adulthood.
The developmental cascades approach and the social development model naturally complement each other. Whereas the developmental cascades theory proposes broad principles to organize the interplay between the individual and environmental factors, the social development model offers a specific sequence of social influence by which behavior is shaped. In fact, the social development model is inherently a developmental cascade model where the bonds, beliefs, and behaviors that are a result of earlier developmental processes (opportunities, involvement, skills, and rewards) in one developmental period go on to determine the opportunities available in the next developmental period (Elder, 1998; Kosterman et al., 2014). However, no previous study has specifically modeled the way social development models flow from childhood to young adulthood and beyond. Additionally, the SDM does not specifically theorize bidirectional or long-term influences of individual and social environmental functioning, or the role of mental health in the pro- and antisocial pathways. It is plausible that the social development model structure used to predict antisocial behavior can also predict mental health outcomes. On the other hand, few cascade models have incorporated aspects of the social environment [drug using peers (Haller, Handley, Chassin, & Bountress, 2010; Lynne-Landsman, Bradshaw, & Ialongo, 2010), peer rejection (Lansford et al., 2010), social competence (Bornstein et al., 2010; Burt, Obradović, Long, & Masten, 2008), and family adversity (Herrenkohl et al., 2010)], and still fewer cascade models were found extending cascades of social environments to adulthood. Questions arise from the union of these theories: How do positive social environments in childhood influence later social environments? Is there a cascade of early substance use environment or family history of depression that affects substance-specific and mental health-specific outcomes in adolescence and beyond? To what extent are these cascades intertwined?
As youth progress through development, the most salient social environment changes over time. Starting with the family in childhood, youth become involved with additional ecological domains as they enter school, meet friends, and connect with their community (Parker, Rubin, Erath, Wojslawowicz, & Buskirk, 2006; Rubin & Bukowski, 2011). During the middle school period, peers take on increasing importance and social influence (Rubin & Bukowski, 2011). In late adolescence and increasingly in the transition to adulthood, newly formed romantic relationships become additional socializing agents, providing new opportunities, rewards, and norms (Furman, Simon, Shaffer, & Bouchey, 2002). The social development model is developmentally oriented, such that opportunities, rewards, and norms all change depending on the age of the child and the domain of influence (Catalano & Hawkins, 1996). For example, opportunities for substance use involvement with peers are most frequent and most rewarding during adolescence.
In the current work we distinguish social environmental influences that are general in nature from those that are specifically linked to substance use or mental health (Bailey, Hill, Meacham, Young, & Hawkins, 2011; Epstein, Hill, Bailey, & Hawkins, 2013; Hill, Hawkins, Catalano, Abbott, & Guo, 2005; Lee et al., 2012). This separation of general and specific allows models to separate influences that may share common risk factors versus risks that are predictive of mental health or substance use problems alone (Bailey et al., 2011). General factors include the overall functioning of the social environment such as family management and family conflict, and are linked to a variety of subsequent problem behaviors and mental health outcomes (Reinherz, Giaconia, Carmola Hauf, Wasserman, & Paradis, 2000). On the other hand, specific factors are conceptualized as those environmental factors specifically related to the outcome under study such as family substance use environment or family history of mental health problems. For instance, early exposure to mental health problems in the family domain has been linked to later mental health problems in offspring (Mars et al., 2012; Weissman et al., 2006).
To sum, the current study examines general positive social environments and specific mental health and substance abuse domains. Building on the social development model and the developmental cascades framework, our model seeks to (a) concatenate multiple social developmental models from childhood, adolescence, and adulthood; (b) integrate general positive social environments, mental health, and substance use domains; (c) model the interactions between social environment and individual functioning across development from childhood to adulthood; and (d) model potential long-reaching effects of early social environments on later social environments and outcomes.
Positive Developmental Cascade
Positive social environments, such as well-functioning families and nondelinquent peers, play a critical role in healthy development (Catalano, Hawkins, Berglund, Pollard, & Arthur, 2002). Each social environment contributes to development as individuals age, and has the potential to have cascading influences over time. Because of their primacy in the developmental order, families have the greatest potential to influence further behavior as well as create a structure for future social interactions with peers and, later on, with romantic partners (Catalano et al., 2002; Donnellan, Larsen-Rife, & Conger, 2005; Rhule-Louie & McMahon, 2007). Positive family environments have been shown to predict positive youth development and youth community contributions (Lewin-Bizan, Bowers, & Lerner, 2010), positive social support in early and middle adulthood (Graves, Wang, Mead, Johnson, & Klag, 1998), reduced delinquency (Yoshikawa, 1994), and better physical health in midlife (Repetti, Taylor, & Seeman, 2002; Russek & Schwartz, 1997). In the positive social environment cascade, the current study explores long-term effects of positive families, and the effect of positive families on peer and romantic partner social environments and individual functioning.
Peer influences increase in importance as youth transition into middle school where they encounter more and more diverse peer networks (Catalano & Hawkins, 1996; Parker et al., 2006; Rubin & Bukowski, 2011). As children spend more time away from their parents, peers become a powerful socializing force, one that can be either prosocial or antisocial. Peer relationships with prosocial peers can serve as powerful protective factors and have been linked to a lower likelihood of externalizing behavior (Criss, Pettit, Bates, Dodge, & Lapp, 2002). Positive peers have also been shown to mitigate the effects of negative family experiences and victimization (Schwartz, McFadyen-Ketchum, Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, 1999). On the other hand, having antisocial or substance-using peers is strongly linked to increased risk for substance use and poor mental health (Hawkins, Catalano, & Miller, 1992; Stone, Becker, Huber, & Catalano, 2012).
In young adulthood, as individuals begin forming lasting unions with romantic partners they bring the skills and practices learned in their family of origin to their partner environments (Bachman et al., 2002). Some have argued that peer relationships serve as important templates for later romantic relationships, and indeed most romantic partners are selected from the peer group (Connolly, Furman, & Konarski, 2000; Furman et al., 2002). Forming a supportive and positive bond with a romantic partner can have cascading effects that increase positive functioning and reduce the likelihood of mental illness and substance use disorders (Horwitz, White, & Howell-White, 1996; Leonard & Rothbard, 1999; Rhule-Louie & McMahon, 2007).
Mental Health Cascade
Life course trajectories to mental health problems have many sources, some of which stem from early social experiences within families, family members that experience mental health problems, and individual difference characteristics. Each of these risk factors has been shown to contribute to the development of mental health problems across development (Mason et al., 2004; McCauley, Pavlidis, & Kendall, 2001; Reinherz et al., 2000; Reinherz, Paradis, Giaconia, Stashwick, & Fitzmaurice, 2003; Repetti et al., 2002; Weissman et al., 2006). The mental health cascade examined in the current study includes family history of depression, internalizing tendencies in adolescence, and major depressive episode and generalized anxiety disorder symptoms and diagnosis.
Just as the general family environment has been shown to have long-lasting effects on functioning, early exposure to mental illness of family members has been linked to children’s mental health problems in adolescence and young adulthood (McCauley et al., 2001). Family history of depression has been studied extensively and shown to be a strong predictor of mental health problems in childhood and adulthood, including internalizing and depression (Leve, Kim, & Pears, 2005; Mars et al., 2012; McCauley et al., 2001; Reinherz et al., 2003; Weissman et al., 2006).
One mechanism for transmitting mental health problems from parents to children is through child internalizing. Internalizing has been conceptualized as a “core disturbance in intropunitive emotions and mood” (Zahn-Waxler, Klimes-Dougan, & Slattery, 2000, p. 443), suggesting an increased tendency toward anxiety and depression. That internalizing is a risk factor for later mood and anxiety symptoms is well established (Reinherz et al., 2000), but studies of the cascading influences of internalizing have shown mixed results. Many researchers have found that internalizing impacts important developmental outcomes at various developmental periods. For example, Masten (2005) found that internalizing in adolescence was protective for developing later externalizing problems in adulthood, while Bornstein (2010) found that internalizing in childhood predicted externalizing problems at age 14. Further research is needed to understand the developmental progression of internalizing tendencies as well as potential cross-domain cascading effects on other developmental outcomes.
Mental health problems in childhood and adolescence are often precursors to later life mood and anxiety problems. Etiological pathways predicting anxiety and depression have been found to be distinct (Roza, Hofstra, van der Ende, & Verhulst, 2003), as well as interrelated across time (Beesdo, Pine, Lieb, & Wittchen, 2010; Grant et al., 2009). Symptoms of depression in adolescence have been linked to depression diagnosis in adulthood (Birmaher et al., 1996; Lewinsohn, Rohde, Klein, & Seeley, 1999; McCauley et al., 2001; Pine, Cohen, Cohen, & Brook, 1999). Similarly, continuity of anxiety from adolescence to adulthood has also been found in several epidemiological studies (Pine, Cohen, Gurley, Brook, & Ma, 1998). In turn, anxiety and depression disorders in adulthood have been shown to impair psychosocial functioning, including reduced physical activity (Goodwin, 2003) and quality of life (Schmitz, Kruse, & Kugler, 2004). Less is known about the longitudinal progression of anxiety and depressive disorders in relation to social environmental influences and substances use.
Substance Use and Abuse Cascade
The etiology of substance abuse includes social environmental influences from family, peers, and romantic partners (Bailey et al., 2011; Haller et al., 2010; Lynne-Landsman et al., 2010), as well as individual difference characteristics such as behavioral disinhibition (Englund & Siebenbruner, 2012; Iacono, Malone, & McGue, 2008; Merline, Jager, & Schulenberg, 2008; Piehler, Véronneau, & Dishion, 2012; Stone et al., 2012). Early substance use and behavioral disinhibition are both indicators of individual functioning that have been found to influence trajectories of substance abuse (Franken, Muris, & Georgieva, 2006). The present study examines substance use–specific family, peer, and partner social environments as well as individual factors of behavioral disinhibition, early substance use, and continued patterns of abuse and/or dependence.
As mentioned in the earlier section on positive cascades, poor family functioning, such as increased conflict and low bonding, have been related to youth substance use outcomes. Another important mechanism involves families that expose their children to substance use or involve youth in their own substance using behaviors. These substance-specific attributes of families have been shown to significantly increase the likelihood that youth will use substances as adolescents and have substance use problems as adults (Bailey et al., 2011; Stone et al., 2012). Parental alcohol abuse is also a well-established risk factor for alcohol use disorder, which is likely related to both shared genetic risk as well as modeling of alcohol-related behaviors (McGue, Iacono, & Krueger, 2006; Merline et al., 2008). Individuals raised in families with histories of substance abuse have significant increased risk to develop substance use disorders later in life (Stone et al., 2012) and are more likely to start using substances early and use persistently (Chassin, Flora, & King, 2004). Children of parents who smoke are also more likely to smoke, and children of parents who use marijuana are more likely to use marijuana (Bailey, Hill, Oesterle, & Hawkins, 2006).
While family substance using environments are a well-supported risk factor for substance use, less is known about how family substance use environment will influence subsequent substance using social environments. Growing up in a substance using environment has been shown to be a risk factor for selecting into substance using partner social environments (Epstein et al., 2013; Harter, 2000). In another example, Haller and colleagues (2010) found that parental alcoholism’s cascading effect on adult alcohol dependence was mediated by affiliation with substance using peers. These studies point to the importance of understanding the cascading effects families, peers, and partners have on the development of substance abuse problems at each stage in development.
Family substance use problems can also affect child substance use in indirect ways, such as increasing behavioral disinhibition tendencies which are, in turn, linked to problem behavior (Handley et al., 2011). Behavioral disinhibition is theorized to be an enduring individual difference characteristic; it has been associated with an increased likelihood of early onset substance use (McGue, Iacono, Legrand, Malone, & Elkins, 2001; Tarter et al., 2003) and substance abuse in young adulthood (Bijttebier, Beck, Claes, & Vandereycken, 2009; Franken & Muris, 2006; Pardo, Aguilar, Molinuevo, & Torrubia, 2007). Studies have also indicated that behavioral disinhibition is precursor to association with deviant peers, and may in fact be a mechanism driving youth to seek out substance using peers (Kirisci, Mezzich, Reynolds, Tarter, & Aytaclar, 2009; Yanovitzky, 2005).
In adolescence, peer substance use has been shown to be a robust predictor of concurrent substance abuse as most substance use takes place in the peer social environment (Haller et al., 2010; Hawkins et al., 1992; Oxford, Oxford, Harachi, Catalano, & Abbott, 2001; Piehler et al., 2012). Adolescents choose their peers, and they may do so based on shared activities, including substance use; peers may also influence each other to try new things (Dishion & Owen, 2002; A. Knecht, Snijders, Baerveldt, Steglich, & Raub, 2010; A. B. Knecht, Burk, Weesie, & Steglich, 2011; Poulin, Kiesner, Pedersen, & Dishion, 2011). Researchers have found that associating with antisocial peers predicts substance use initiation (Guo, Hill, Hawkins, Catalano, & Abbott, 2002) and persistent substance use into adulthood (Haller et al., 2010; Lee et al., 2012). In addition, since the peer environment is often the source of potential romantic partners and serves as an important social environment for learning relationship skills and patterns (Connolly et al., 2000; Furman et al., 2002; Rhule-Louie & McMahon, 2007), peer substance using social environments can influence youth to develop romantic relationships with substance using partners.
Romantic partners have a high concordance of substance use behavior (Rhule-Louie & McMahon, 2007). This may be due to a selection of romantic partners based on one’s own substance use behavior, or through the social environmental influences of opportunities for substance use, perceived rewards for involvement, and bonding to the substance using partner (Fleming, White, & Catalano, 2010; Leonard & Rothbard, 1999). For example, Fleming and colleagues (2010) found that romantic partnerships were protective against substance use when one was bonded to a non-substance using partner.
Interplay Between Social Environments and Individual Functioning
In addition to the effects of the social environment on individual functioning, individual functioning can have reciprocal effects on social environments. These person-environment correlations have often been discussed in the context of gene-environment correlation (rGE), citing evocative or reactive (different individuals evoke different environmental responses), selective (different individuals select different environments), and passive (parents who create a child’s environment also share much of that child’s genotype) mechanisms to account for these effects (Hicks et al., 2013; Plomin, DeFries, & Loehlin, 1977). This relationship is not simple, however, because ultimately the pathway from genes to environments must involve behavior, which is itself determined by both genes and environments (Jaffee & Price, 2007; Knafo & Jaffee, 2013), and much of the work on measured gene-environment interplay over the last decade has been difficult to replicate (Colhoun, McKeigue, & Smith, 2003; Duncan & Keller, 2011; Duncan, Pollastri, & Smoller, 2014). Whatever the source of these individual differences, the possibility that individuals can influence their social environment through their behavior is at the core of the interplay of individual functioning and social environments. This interplay influences the developmental processes that lead to adaptive or maladaptive outcomes (Sameroff & MacKenzie, 2003).
The connecting link between individual functioning and social environment is especially evident in the peer domain during adolescence, where individual differences may influence the selection of friends with similar behaviors (Parker et al., 2006). For example, several studies have shown that individuals with early manifestations of psychopathology, including tendencies toward internalizing, select less positive peers (Bornstein et al., 2010; Burt et al., 2008; Obradović, Burt, & Masten, 2009), and higher behavioral disinhibition has been linked to selecting into drug-using peer networks (Kirisci et al., 2009; Yanovitzky, 2005). Others have found that adolescents who experienced depression in their youth are more likely to have social problems and more difficulties with peer relationships (Lewinsohn, Rohde, Seeley, Klein, & Gotlib, 2003).
Similarly, individuals tend to select romantic partners with similar patterns of behavior, and those mentally healthy individuals are more likely to be partnered (Knight, 2011; Rhule-Louie & McMahon, 2007). Thus, we might expect that one’s mental health and substance use problems would impact the quality of an individual’s romantic partner relationship by influencing selection of a less positive partner environment and difficulties in partnership due to mental health and substance use issues. Correlational studies have established associations between depression and romantic partner relationship strain (Teo, Choi, & Valenstein, 2013), but no longitudinal studies were found linking depression to later romantic partner relationship quality. Further investigation of the relationship between early indicators of psychopathology and the selection of positive peer and partner environments, and their reciprocal influences over time is needed.
Interplay of Substance Abuse and Mental Health Cascades
The common co-occurrence of substance abuse and mental health problems is well documented in cross–sectional studies (Brady & Sinha, 2005; Compton, Thomas, Stinson, & Grant, 2007) and has been estimated to range from 32% to 54% (Brady & Sinha, 2005). Some evidence suggests that substance use may drive mental health problems as well as the reverse. However, few studies have approached the comorbidity between substance use and mental health developmentally; consequently, we know little about the interplay between these disorders across developmental periods.
The pattern of effects in the literature testing the first hypothesis—that substance use predicts mental health problems—is mixed. Substance use (and other externalizing behaviors) in youth have been linked with adult depression in some studies (Capaldi & Stoolmiller, 1999; Wolff & Ollendick, 2006; Zoccolillo, 1992), whereas others found no association (Wiesner & Windle, 2006). In a review of 48 studies linking psychological problems and illicit drug use in young adults, Macleod et al. (2004) found inconclusive evidence of this hypothesis and noted that associations between substance use and psychological problems were inconsistent across studies.
The reverse hypothesis that mental health problems may drive substance use is often referred to as the theory of self-medication, which suggests that individuals with mental health problems attempt to self-treat their symptoms by using substances (Khantzian, 1997; Markou, Kosten, & Koob, 1998). For example, researchers have demonstrated that early internalizing has been shown to predict later alcohol and other drug use, as well as alcohol dependence (Englund & Siebenbruner, 2012; Merline et al., 2008; Tomlinson & Brown, 2012). Some studies of adults have also found that depression or other mental health disorders often precede substance abuse (Burke Jr, Burke, & Rae, 1994; Mason, Hitchings, & Spoth, 2008). However, others have questioned the continued use of the self-medication hypothesis given its modest supporting evidence, especially the weakness of arguments establishing that mental health causes subsequent substance use (Lembke, 2012). In addition to the lack of consensus about mental health’s influence on substance use, differences in the developmental timing of these effects are also missing from the literature.
Overall, the high concordance of mental health and substance use problems suggests that these issues should be examined together. Moreover, the interplay between substance use and mental health is likely to differ at different points in development. Finally, the concurrence is likely to be driven by shared risk and protective factors over time and may have common consequences. Thus, a developmentally sensitive cascade approach that models the interplay of social environmental and individual risk is needed.
The Present Study
In the present study, three developmental cascades were examined: positive environment and functioning, mental-health related environment and functioning, and substance use environment and functioning. Three features were examined within each cascade: (a) continuity of social environments and individual functioning across developmental periods; (b) the effect of social environments on proximal individual functioning; and (c) long-term effects of early social environmental influences on later social environments and outcomes within the same domain. In addition, we examined the reciprocal influence of individual factors and the social environment within a cascade, as well as the interplay between developmental cascades. We propose two broad research questions. Research Question 1(a-c) examined the developmental cascades of positive social environments, mental health, and substance use. Research Question 2 (a-b) focused on the interplay between individuals and their social environment and the interplay of mental health and substance abuse cascades across development.
Research Question 1a -- What are the cascading effects of positive social environments across development?
We hypothesized that early exposure to positive family environment begins a developmental cascade influencing (a) the selection of positive peers in adolescence; (b) the selection of positive romantic partners in young adulthood; and (c) improved proximal individual functioning, including in the areas of internalizing, behavioral disinhibition, and early substance use in adolescence; and (d) improved mental health and substance use outcomes in adulthood.
Research Question 1b – What are the cascading effects of family history of depression on mental health across development?
Similar to Research Question 1a, we hypothesized that early exposure to mental health problems in the family would begin a cascade of mental health problems. We hypothesized that family mental health histories impact youth mental health by (a) increasing internalizing in adolescence, (b) increasing the likelihood of developing mood and anxiety disorder symptoms in young adulthood, and (c) increasing the occurrence of mental health diagnoses in adulthood.
Research Question 1c – What are the cascading influences of early substance using environments and substance use across development?
Similar to Research Questions 1a and 1b, we hypothesized that the effects of family substance use environments cascade to (a) proximal individual functioning outcomes, including early substance use and behavioral disinhibition; (b) later substance using environments, including selection of substance using peers and romantic partners; and (c) substance use problems in increasing severity across development.
Research Question 2a – How do individual differences and behaviors influence the selection of positive social environments?
We hypothesized that individual functioning also plays an important role in the development of positive social environment. We predicted that behavioral disinhibition, substance use, and mental health problems all negatively affect an individual’s ability to engage with positive social environments. Specifically, we hypothesized that a tendency toward internalizing, behavioral disinhibition, and early substance use all influence the selection of fewer prosocial peers in adolescence. Similarly, we hypothesized that mental health and substance use problems in young adulthood negatively impact romantic partner social environment in early adulthood.
Research Question 2b – How do mental health and substance abuse problems interact to influence each other across development?
We hypothesized that substance using environments and one’s own substance use will have an impact on the development of mental health problems across the life course. We also investigated the self-medication hypothesis by examining whether internalizing, mood, and anxiety disorder symptoms increase the likelihood of developing substance use problems at later time points.
Methods
Sample
This study used longitudinal data from the Seattle Social Development Project (SSDP), a community-based sample of 808 participants enrolled in a longitudinal study of the development of prosocial and antisocial behaviors. The sample was originally recruited in 1985 from 18 elementary schools serving primarily higher risk neighborhoods; however, due to mandatory bussing at that time, some participants were included from a range of other neighborhoods. Participants are ethnically diverse, with 47% identifying as Caucasian, 26% African American, 22% Asian American, and 5% Native American. The sample is also gender balanced (49% female), and 52% of the participants met criteria for the National School Lunch/School Breakfast program during at least 1 year between fifth and seventh grade. Participants were interviewed at ages 10, 11, 12, 13, 14, 15, 16, and 18, and their parents completed questionnaires when participants were age 10–16. Follow-up interviews were conducted with participants at ages 21, 24, 27, 30, and 33. SSDP has maintained retention rates above 90% in adulthood, with 92% of the still living sample interviewed at age 33 (23 participants were deceased by age 33). A subset of the SSDP sample received a preventative intervention in elementary school, consisting of individual, parent, and teacher components (see Hawkins, Catalano, Kosterman, Abbott, & Hill, 1999 for a full description of the intervention).
Measures
Measures of the environment that are operationalized as scales that include items representing an underlying construct (e.g., general family environment), are reported with Cronbach’s alpha reliabilities. Measures that are dichotomous (e.g., early substance use) or index-based (e.g., major depressive disorder symptoms, family history of depression) are not expected to have internal consistency, as they are not representative of an underlying construct. Thus, no reliabilities are reported for dichotomous or index variables. Details of each variable included in the analysis are enumerated below.
Family domain
The positive family environment scale included youth reports at ages 10 and 11 of family involvement, family bonding, family conflict (reverse coded), and family management. Items within each construct were averaged and standardized to create a scale score at each age, which was then combined for a single measure of family functioning at ages 10–12. The average reliability across ages 10–12 was α = 0.83. Examples of items included “the rules in my family are clear” and “do you share your thoughts and feelings with your father?” The family substance use environment scale included parent reports of parents and siblings using alcohol, tobacco, or marijuana; their attitudes towards these substances; and parents’ involvement of youth in their use of these substances (e.g., lighting a cigarette for parent). Items were measured prospectively, when youth were between 10 and 12 years old. Scales were constructed by averaging and standardizing items. The average reliability of drug-specific environment across ages 10–12 was α = 0.71. Examples of items include “Has your child ever brought cigarettes for a family member” and “Has your child ever brought, opened or poured a drink containing alcohol for a family member?” Family history of depression index was measured retrospectively by the participants at age 24. Participants reported whether their biological mother, father, siblings, or other family members had a history of depression. The number of family members with depression was summed for an overall score.
Peer domain
The positive peer environment scale in high school (ages 15–18) included measures of prosocial peer involvement in school, positive support from peers, peer efforts at success in school, and opportunities for antisocial involvement (reverse coded). These measures were collected prospectively at ages 15, 16, and 18, and had an average reliability across ages of α = 0.67. Positive peer support and involvement measures included items such as “Does your best friend try to do well in school,” “Does your best friend let you know when you’ve done something well?”, “Has your best friend ever asked or expected you to do things that could get you in trouble with your parents, the school or the police?” The positive peer and antisocial opportunities (reverse coded) scales were averaged and standardized to create a scale of the general positive functioning of an individual’s peer environment. The peer substance use environment scale included measures of peer drinking and marijuana use. Peer marijuana use was measured prospectively at ages 15, 16, and 18, and included items about how many of the respondents’ close friends used marijuana. Peer alcohol use was the number of friends who had tried alcohol (beer, wine, or liquor) without their parents’ knowledge, gotten drunk, or used alcohol with friends. We created a measure of the proportion of respondents’ friends who used alcohol or marijuana. If a respondent had more friends who used these substances, they would have a higher proportion of substance using friends.
Partner domain
Positive partner environment is a scale of bonding to romantic partners, positive involvement with partner, and a lack of conflict with one’s partner. Romantic partnerships were self-defined and included heterosexual and homosexual couples and married and non-married couples. Measures were collected prospectively at ages 24 and 27, and include items such as “Do you enjoy spending time with your partner,” “My partner includes me in important decisions,” and “How often do you and your partner quarrel?” The average reliability of these items across ages 24–27 was α = 0.89. Items were averaged and then standardized to create a scale score. Participants were given a scale score if they were partnered at either wave of data collection, age 24 or 27. There were 620 respondents who had a relationship at least one time point, 154 who did not, with 34 missing values. Those who were not partnered during either wave were treated as missing. Participants who were not partnered were included in the analysis because romantic partner relationships are just one mechanism in the model, and including un-partnered individuals allows their responses to inform the remainder of the model. Partner substance use environment. Participants were asked to report on their partner’s substance use and whether they smoked, drank heavily, or used marijuana or other drugs in the past year. A dichotomous variable was created, coded as 1 if the participant’s partner used any substances or drank heavily in the past year, and as 0 if they did not.
Individual functioning
An internalizing scale was created using teacher-reported items from the Child Behavior Checklist at ages 13–14. Using Achenbach’s (2001) system of translating the CBCL items to DSM-like categories, items that were similar to affective problems and anxiety problems were used to create a scale of internalizing behavior. Examples of items used in the affective problems subscale include “Cries a lot,” “Feels worthless or inferior,” “apathetic or unmotivated,” and “unhappy, sad or depressed.” Items from the anxiety subscale include “Nervous, high-strung or tense,” “too fearful or anxious,” and “worrying.” Item response options include “not true,” “sometimes true,” and “often or very true.” These subscales were averaged and standardized to create a scale score of internalizing, with an average reliability across ages 13–14 of α = 0.77. Teacher reports of internalizing have been found to have significant convergent validity (Kosterman et al., 2010). A behavioral disinhibition scale was based on the measure of behavioral disinhibition from Hill et al. (2010). This scale was created from items asked prospectively of participants at age 14. Examples of items include: “How many times have you done the following things: Done what feels good, regardless of the consequences? Gone to a wild, uninhibited party? Do risky things even if they are a little frightening.” Items were averaged and standardized to create a scale of behavioral disinhibition at age 14 with reliability of α = 0.78. Early substance use was created as a dichotomous variable of whether the participant reported using any marijuana, cigarettes, or alcohol at ages 13 or 14. Generalized anxiety disorder symptoms was the count of the number of symptoms of generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) that a participant experienced in the past year on the age 21 survey using the Diagnostic Interview Schedule (DIS) (Robins, Helzer, Croughan, Williams, & Spitzer, 1981). The DIS was based on the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual III-R (DSM-III-R) (American Psychiatric Association, 1987) diagnostic criteria. The DIS has frequently been used in studies of mental illness in adults from the general population, and been found to be both reliable and valid (Mason et al., 2004; Newman et al., 1996; Reinherz et al., 2000). Generalized anxiety disorder diagnosis was created by determining whether the participant met the diagnostic criteria for GAD diagnosis in the past year as determined by the DIS at ages 30 or 33. A participant was given a 1 if they met criteria for diagnosis at either age 30 or 33; 0 meaning that they did not meet criteria at either age. Symptom count was used as the measure at age 21 to capture subclinical symptoms, while diagnosis was used at ages 30–33 to assess an increase in severity. Major depressive episode symptoms was the count of symptoms of a major depressive episode (MDE) experienced by the participant at age 21. This count was also collected using the DIS based on DSM III-R criteria. Similar to GAD diagnosis, major depressive episode diagnosis at ages 30 or 33 was determined by whether the participant met criteria for a major depression episode in the past year at either the data collection at age 30 or 33. A dichotomous variable was created with 1 meaning that the participant had met criteria for an MDE episode at least once at age 30 or 33, and 0 meaning they didn’t meet criteria at either age. A global measure of positive functioning was created and based on Kosterman et al.’s (2014) index of positive adult functioning. Using the constructs of constructive engagement, civic engagement, and physical exercise, an index of positive functioning was created at ages 30–33. Constructive engagement was defined as the total number of hours spent in work, school, or homework; civic engagement refers to the hours per month spent in community groups and volunteering; and physical exercise was measured by self-reported minutes per week of at least moderate intensity exercise. These scales were then averaged and standardized and the mean standardized score across ages 30–33 formed the final continuous measure of positive adult functioning. Because we do not expect high correlation between the disparate scales of positive functioning, we constructed this measure as an index and not a scale, and therefore do not report reliability.
Control variables
We included a number of control variables in our model. Gender was self-reported, with males coded as 1 and females as 0. Race/ethnicity was a dummy variable of the primary ethnicity reported by respondents. European Americans were the referent group. Childhood poverty was included as a control, and was coded as a dichotomous variable of whether a participant’s family met criteria for free lunch program at school in either fifth, sixth, or seventh grade. Intervention condition. To test for differences by intervention group, a dummy variable was created with 1 indicating that a respondent was part of the full intervention condition; a 0 meant that they were not. A parallel process was used for the control group, a 1 indicated that the respondent was part of the control group and received no intervention and a 0 meant that they were not in the control group.
Results
Analyses
Path analysis was employed to estimate all models using Mplus version 7.11 ((Muthén & Muthén, 2008). Eight variables (family positive environment, family substance use environment, family history of depression, internalizing, behavioral disinhibition, peer substance use environment, partner general environment, positive functioning in adulthood) were treated as continuous whereas the remainder were treated as dichotomous or ordered categorical. The weighted least squares mean and variance-adjusted (WLSMV) estimator was used to account for non-normal distribution of dependent variables. Missingness was accommodated with full-information maximum likelihood (FIML) estimation. Correlations among exogenous variables as well as correlations among variables measured at the same time point were estimated in the model. Table 1 contains intercorrelations among variables in the model. Note at the level of zero-order correlations, the primary hypotheses of the study are generally supported. All variables in the model were adjusted for gender, race/ethnicity, and childhood SES. Due to their smaller sample size (n = 43), Native Americans were not included in this analysis, bringing our sample size of analysis to 765.
Table 1.
Variable | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | 12 | 13 | 14 | 15 | 16 | 17 | 18 | 19 | 20 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Family sub. use env. | -- | |||||||||||||||||||
2. Positive family env. | −0.06 | -- | ||||||||||||||||||
3. Family hx. depr. | 0.06 | 0.01 | -- | |||||||||||||||||
4. BD | 0.13 | −0.11 | 0.07 | -- | ||||||||||||||||
5. Internalizing | 0.09 | −0.09 | 0.11 | 0.22 | -- | |||||||||||||||
6. Early sub. use | 0.11 | −0.09 | 0.08 | 0.35 | 0.19 | -- | ||||||||||||||
7. Peer sub. use env. | 0.18 | −0.09 | 0.17 | 0.43 | 0.21 | 0.39 | -- | |||||||||||||
8. Positive peer env. | −0.06 | 0.09 | −0.10 | −0.34 | −0.19 | −0.21 | −0.52 | -- | ||||||||||||
9. Sub. ab./dep. sx. | 0.15 | −0.10 | 0.07 | 0.21 | 0.01 | 0.12 | 0.30 | −0.25 | -- | |||||||||||
10. MDE sx | 0.06 | −0.12 | 0.19 | 0.11 | 0.07 | 0.10 | 0.20 | −0.14 | 0.22 | -- | ||||||||||
11. GAD sx. | 0.01 | −0.06 | 0.26 | 0.08 | 0.03 | 0.05 | 0.18 | −0.14 | 0.10 | 0.48 | -- | |||||||||
12. Partner sub. use | 0.14 | −0.02 | 0.10 | 0.13 | 0.06 | 0.11 | 0.24 | −0.09 | 0.16 | 0.10 | 0.10 | -- | ||||||||
13. Positive partner env. | −0.08 | 0.18 | −0.10 | −0.06 | −0.10 | −0.01 | −0.17 | 0.20 | −0.13 | −0.16 | −0.13 | −0.14 | -- | |||||||
14. Positive func. | 0.06 | 0.13 | 0.02 | −0.01 | −0.10 | 0.00 | −0.04 | 0.06 | 0.00 | −0.09 | 0.00 | −0.10 | 0.14 | -- | ||||||
15. Sub. ab./dep. dx. | 0.08 | −0.09 | 0.16 | 0.20 | 0.06 | 0.13 | 0.26 | −0.22 | 0.20 | 0.11 | 0.08 | 0.26 | −0.15 | −0.17 | -- | |||||
16. MDE dx. | −0.01 | −0.05 | 0.26 | 0.06 | 0.06 | 0.02 | 0.16 | −0.10 | 0.06 | 0.23 | 0.20 | 0.16 | −0.23 | −0.19 | 0.20 | -- | ||||
17. GAD dx. | 0.04 | −0.01 | 0.15 | 0.07 | 0.08 | 0.09 | 0.15 | −0.06 | 0.03 | 0.22 | 0.21 | 0.12 | −0.18 | −0.10 | 0.20 | 0.37 | -- | |||
18. Male | 0.01 | −0.08 | −0.18 | 0.17 | 0.07 | 0.03 | −0.02 | −0.23 | 0.24 | −0.09 | −0.12 | −0.02 | −0.04 | 0.05 | 0.11 | −0.13 | −0.10 | -- | ||
19. Poverty | −0.05 | −0.05 | −0.07 | −0.03 | 0.10 | 0.03 | −0.03 | −0.03 | −0.06 | 0.12 | 0.06 | −0.02 | −0.05 | −0.15 | 0.01 | 0.03 | 0.05 | −0.05 | -- | |
20. Black | −0.01 | 0.07 | −0.09 | 0.11 | 0.09 | 0.03 | 0.14 | −0.11 | 0.00 | 0.06 | 0.10 | 0.04 | −0.14 | 0.00 | 0.14 | 0.09 | 0.05 | −0.01 | 0.29 | -- |
21. Asian | −0.22 | −0.08 | −0.25 | −0.25 | −0.19 | −0.18 | −0.35 | 0.13 | −0.11 | −0.03 | −0.05 | −0.15 | 0.10 | −0.09 | −0.13 | −0.11 | −0.01 | 0.03 | 0.18 | −0.33 |
Note: Bolded coefficients significant at p<.05. Ethnicity reference group is White. Sub.= substance; env.=environment; hx= history of; depr.=depression; BD=behavioral disinhibition; ab./dep.= abuse or dependence; sx=symptoms; dx=diagnosis; MDE=Major Depressive Episode; GAD=Generalized Anxiety Disorder; func.=functioning.
Table 2 presents the hypothesized model estimates from the path analyses. The hypothesized estimates reflect those parameter estimates for associations specifically hypothesized above. The final results of the path analyses are presented in Figure 1. Our discussion of the literature and research questions reflect both specific pathways as well as an interplay between pathways, thus we modeled all hypothesized pathways in a single omnibus model. Specific paths hypothesized are discussed in detail below. Overall model fit was assessed using chi-square, comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) and root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) statistics. Fit of the model was good, with χ2 (61) = 107.149 (p<.0002), RMSEA = .031, CFI = 0.979, and TLI = 0.929.
Table 2.
Standardized Estimate |
|||
---|---|---|---|
Predictor | Outcome | Hypothesized | Saturated |
Research Question 1a | |||
Positive family environment | Early substance use | −0.15* | −0.12* |
Internalizing | −0.09* | −0.09* | |
Behavioral disinhibition | −0.13* | −0.11* | |
Positive peer environment | 0.05 | 0.03* | |
Positive partner environment | 0.17* | 0.16* | |
Positive functioning | 0.10* | 0.10* | |
Peer substance use | - | −0.05 | |
MDE symptoms | - | −0.11* | |
GAD symptoms | - | −0.06 | |
Substance ab/dep. symptoms | - | −0.07 | |
Partner substance use | - | 0.02 | |
MDE diagnosis | - | −0.04 | |
GAD diagnosis | - | 0.08 | |
Substance ab./dep. diagnosis | - | −0.09 | |
Positive peer environment | MDE symptoms | −0.09 | −0.07 |
GAD symptoms | −0.09 | −0.09 | |
Substance ab./dep. symptoms | −0.08 | −0.06 | |
Positive partner environment | 0.13* | 0.14* | |
Partner substance use environment | - | 0.07 | |
Positive functioning | - | 0.05 | |
MDE diagnosis | - | −0.03 | |
GAD diagnosis | - | 0.09 | |
Substance ab./dep. diagnosis | - | −0.05 | |
Positive partner environment | Positive functioning | 0.12* | 0.11* |
MDE diagnosis | −0.23* | −0.20* | |
GAD diagnosis | −0.20* | −0.22* | |
Substance ab./dep. diagnosis | −0.09 | −0.05 | |
Research Question 1b | |||
Family history of depression | Internalizing | 0.06 | 0.08* |
MDE symptoms | 0.26* | 0.21* | |
GAD Symptoms | 0.33* | 0.26* | |
MDE diagnosis | 0.27* | 0.20* | |
GAD diagnosis | 0.13 | 0.12 | |
Early substance use | - | 0.04 | |
Behavioral disinhibition | - | 0.05 | |
Peer substance use environment | - | 0.08* | |
Positive peer environment | - | −0.11* | |
Substance ab./dep. symptoms | - | 0.07 | |
Positive partner environment | - | −0.06 | |
Partner substance use environment | - | 0.03 | |
Positive functioning | - | 0.03 | |
Substance ab./dep. diagnosis | - | 0.17* | |
Internalizing | MDE symptoms | 0.04 | 0.00 |
GAD symptoms | −0.01 | −0.02 | |
Peer substance use environment | - | 0.04 | |
Partner substance use environment | - | 0.00 | |
Positive partner environment | - | −0.05 | |
Positive functioning | - | −0.09* | |
MDE diagnosis | - | 0.01 | |
GAD diagnosis | - | 0.05 | |
Substance ab./dep. diagnosis | - | −0.07 | |
MDE Symptoms | Positive functioning | −0.08 | −0.07 |
MDE diagnosis | 0.23* | 0.19* | |
GAD diagnosis | - | 0.24* | |
Partner substance use environment | - | 0.03 | |
GAD Symptoms | Positive functioning | 0.09 | 0.10 |
GAD diagnosis | 0.31* | 0.12 | |
MDE diagnosis | - | 0.05 | |
Partner substance use environment | - | 0.06 | |
Research Question 1c | |||
Family substance use environment | Early substance use | 0.06 | 0.07 |
Behavioral disinhibition | 0.06 | 0.07* | |
Peer substance use environment | 0.07* | 0.07* | |
Substance ab./dep. symptoms | 0.09* | 0.09* | |
Partner substance use environment | 0.07 | 0.10 | |
Substance ab./dep. diagnosis | −0.04 | −0.04 | |
Positive peer environment | - | 0.00 | |
MDE symptoms | - | 0.05 | |
GAD symptoms | - | 0.00 | |
Positive partner environment | - | −0.04 | |
Positive functioning | - | 0.08* | |
GAD diagnosis | - | 0.02 | |
MDE diagnosis | - | −0.11 | |
Early substance use | Peer substance use environment | 0.31* | 0.30* |
Substance ab./dep. symptoms | 0.04 | 0.00 | |
Partner substance use environment | - | 0.03 | |
Positive partner environment | - | 0.10 | |
Positive functioning | - | 0.05 | |
MDE symptoms | - | −0.09 | |
GAD symptoms | - | 0.13 | |
Substance ab./dep. diagnosis | - | 0.06 | |
Behavioral disinhibition | Peer substance use environment | 0.28* | 0.24* |
MDE symptoms | 0.04 | ||
GAD symptoms | - | 0.01 | |
Substance ab./dep. symptoms | - | 0.05 | |
Partner substance use environment | - | 0.03 | |
Positive partner environment | - | 0.05 | |
Positive functioning | - | 0.00 | |
MDE diagnosis | - | 0.02 | |
GAD diagnosis | - | 0.01 | |
Substance ab./dep. diagnosis | - | 0.01 | |
Peer substance use environment | Substance ab./dep. symptoms | 0.27* | 0.26* |
Partner substance use environment | 0.27* | 0.21* | |
Positive partner environment | - | −0.05 | |
Positive functioning | - | −0.01 | |
MDE Diagnosis | - | 0.05 | |
GAD Diagnosis | - | 0.13 | |
Substance ab./dep. diagnosis | - | 0.10 | |
Substance ab./dep. symptoms | Partner substance use environment | 0.13* | 0.13* |
Substance ab./dep. diagnosis | 0.12 | 0.09 | |
Positive functioning | - | 0.02 | |
Partner substance use | Substance ab./dep. diagnosis | 0.50* | 0.48* |
Positive functioning | −0.12* | −0.14* | |
Research Question 2a | |||
Internalizing | Positive peer environment | −0.08* | −0.07* |
Behavioral disinhibition | Positive peer environment | −0.21* | −0.21* |
Early substance use | Positive peer environment | −0.10* | −0.13* |
GAD Symptoms | Positive partner environment | −0.05 | −0.04 |
MDE Symptoms | Positive partner environment | −0.10 | −0.07 |
Substance ab./dep. symptoms | Positive partner environment | −0.04 | −0.05 |
Research Question 2b | |||
Family substance use environment | Internalizing | 0.04 | 0.04 |
Internalizing | Substance ab./dep. symptoms | −0.06 | −0.09* |
Early substance use | GAD symptoms | 0.02 | −0.01 |
MDE symptoms | 0.03 | 0.01 | |
Peer substance use | GAD symptoms | 0.19* | 0.13* |
MDE symptoms | 0.19* | 0.12* | |
GAD symptoms | Substance ab./dep. diagnosis | 0.10 | −0.06 |
MDE symptoms | Substance ab./dep. diagnosis | 0.06 | 0.06 |
Substance ab./dep. symptoms | GAD diagnosis | 0.07 | −0.08 |
MDE diagnosis | −0.03 | −0.04 | |
Partner substance. use environment | GAD diagnosis | 0.23* | 0.16 |
MDE diagnosis | 0.23* | 0.22* |
Note: Coefficients significant at p<.05. Ab./dep.= abuse or dependence; MDE=Major Depressive Episode; GAD=Generalized Anxiety Disorder.
Table 2 also presents estimates of the saturated path analysis. There are a number of pathways for which we did not have a specific hypothesis to warrant their inclusion. For example, we did not hypothesize relationships between family history of depression and positive environments, substance abuse, or substance abuse environments, nor did we hypothesize the cross-domain effects of substance using environments on positive social environments. However, to test whether any of these pathways were significant, we also examined the saturated model. Differences between the saturated and hypothesized model may suggest areas for further research and exploration in the future. Saturated model estimates are reported in the final column of Table 2 and the results are explored below.
Etiological analyses on samples where an intervention has been conducted must take into account potential validity threats caused by changes created by the intervention. Previous analyses have found differences in levels and prevalence of risk and protective factors and outcomes (Hawkins, Kosterman, Catalano, Hill, & Abbott, 2008), but we have found no evidence of differences in etiological processes between conditions. That is, there is little reason to believe that the relationships between the variables examined here are different among participants in the intervention and control groups. However, to test for potential effects of the intervention on structural pathways of the model, we tested a multiple-group model with the intervention and control groups and tested for differences using a chi square difference test (DIFFTEST in Mplus). We constrained all structural paths and tested against the fully unconstrained model. No significant differences were found, indicating that the model fit equally well for treatment and control groups (p = 0.41). Level differences between intervention and control conditions were present on various outcomes previously reported, but our analysis of differences in structural pathways indicated that etiological processes did not differ between intervention groups.
Research Question 1a – What are the cascading influences of positive social environments across development from age 10 to 33?
We expected to see effects of family general environment on (a) functioning in the most proximal developmental period at ages 13–14, (b) future positive environments, and (c) positive functioning at ages 30–33. We also hypothesized that families would set up a template for developing future positive environments and have a positive effect on positive peer environment at ages 15–18 and on positive partner environment at ages 24–27. Finally, we expected a long-term effect of positive general family on positive functioning at ages 30–33.
Consistent with our prediction, there was a series of cascading effects of positive social environments across development from age 10 to ages 30–33 (see Figure 1). Family positive environment at ages 10–12 had a protective effect on early adolescent functioning (ages 13–14) by decreasing internalizing symptoms, reducing behavioral disinhibition, and decreasing the likelihood of early substance use. Family positive environment also had long-reaching effects to promote positive romantic partner environments at ages 24–27 and positive functioning at ages 30–33. Positive family environment did not have a unique effect on positive peer environment at ages 15–18 as expected.
We hypothesized that positive peer environments at ages 15–18 would also have a cascade of protective effects on individual functioning at age 21, (symptoms of drug abuse or dependence, generalized anxiety disorder, or major depressive episode) and on the selection of positive partners at ages 24–27. As predicted, positive peer environments at ages 15–18 was associated with selection of more positive partner environments at ages 24–27. However, contrary to prediction, positive peer environments at ages 15–18 did not have a significant unique effect on age 21 functioning after accounting for other variables in the model. Marginal associations were found between positive peer environment and MDE and drug abuse or dependence symptoms, though no association was found for GAD symptoms.
The cascade of positive partner environment at ages 24–27 was hypothesized to have protective effects on individual functioning (positive functioning, GAD, MDE, and drug abuse or dependence diagnosis) at ages 30–33. Consistent with prediction, partner general environment had a protective effect on mental health by reducing the likelihood of meeting criteria for generalized anxiety disorder and major depressive episode. Partner positive environment also predicted positive functioning. However, positive partner environments did not have a unique significant effect on symptoms of drug abuse or dependence once other variables were included.
Research Question 1b – What are the cascading effects of family history of depression on mental health across development from ages 10 to 33?
The cascading effects of family history of depression and individual mental health problems were hypothesized to have an effect on mental health functioning in the next developmental period. Mental health problems were expected to increase in severity, progressing from family history of depression, to childhood internalizing at ages 13–14, to symptoms of anxiety and depression at age 21, and finally to meeting criteria for diagnosis of anxiety or depression at ages 30–33. Mental health problems at age 21 were predicted to decrease positive functioning at ages 30–33.
We found partial confirmation for the mental health cascade hypothesis that began with family history of depression. Family history of depression did not affect internalizing at ages 13–14. However, family history of depression had a direct effect on depression and anxiety symptoms at age 21 and on MDE (but not GAD) diagnosis at ages 30–33 above and beyond the impact on earlier mental health symptoms. We also found continuity in mental health problems over time, but only in adulthood. Internalizing symptoms at ages 13–14 did not predict mental health symptoms at age 21. However, GAD symptoms at age 21 predicted GAD diagnosis at ages 30–33, and MDE symptoms at age 21 predicted MDE diagnosis at ages 30–33. Finally, contrary to prediction, mental health symptoms at age 21 did not reduce positive functioning at ages 30–33.
Research Question 1c – What are the cascading influences of drug using environments and drug use across development?
Similar to having long-term effects of positive family environment in childhood, family substance use environments were hypothesized to have cascading effects both on individual functioning and on future substance use environments. We predicted that family substance using environments would affect the proximal problems of behavioral disinhibition and early substance use at ages 13–14 and begin a cascade of involvement with substance using peer and partner environments. We expected to find long-term impacts of family substance use environments on drug abuse and dependence symptoms at age 21 and drug abuse and dependence diagnosis at ages 30–33. We expected that peer substance using environment at ages 15–18 would have cascading effects on drug abuse and dependence symptoms at age 21 and on partner substance use environment. Next, we predicted that partner substance use environment at ages 24–27 would play a role in substance abuse or dependence diagnosis and positive functioning at ages 30–33. Finally, we expected continuity in substance use across development, with early substance use predicting substance abuse or dependence symptoms at age 21, which would in turn predict substance abuse or dependence diagnosis at ages 30–33.
Contrary to expectations, family substance use environment at ages 10–12 did not predict behavioral disinhibition or substance use at ages 13–14. We did find strong continuity from family substance use environment at ages 10–12 to peer substance use environment at ages 15–18. There was also a long-term effect of family substance use environment on symptoms of drug abuse and dependence at age 21 but not at ages 30–33. We did not find a unique long-term influence of family substance use environment on partner substance use environment at ages 24–27 once other variables were in the model.
The hypothesized cascading effects of peer substance use environment on substance use and on choosing a substance using romantic partner were confirmed by our analysis. Peer substance use environment at ages 15–18 significantly increased substance abuse and dependence symptoms at age 21 as well as increased the likelihood of selecting a partner who uses substances at ages 24 or 27. In turn, romantic partner substance using environments had strong proximal effects on both drug abuse and dependence diagnosis at ages 30–33 and on decreased positive functioning. With a standardized effect size of 0.5, the influence of partner drug use on individual drug abuse or dependence is the strongest in the model. Partner substance use environment also decreased positive functioning at ages 30–33 as expected.
Counter to our hypotheses, we did not find continuity of substance abuse between time points. Early substance abuse at ages 13–14 did not uniquely predict symptoms of abuse or dependence at age 21, which in turn did not uniquely predict drug abuse or dependence diagnosis at ages 30–33 once other variables were in the model.
Research Question 2a – How does individual functioning influence the selection of positive social environments?
For this hypothesis, we expected that individual functioning would impact the selection of positive social environments in subsequent periods. At ages 13–14 we predicted that internalizing, behavioral disinhibition, and substance use would influence individuals to select into a less positive peer environment at ages 15–18. Similarly, we expected that having symptoms of GAD, MDE, or substance abuse or dependence at age 21 would influence the proclivity to select into a positive romantic partner environment at ages 24–27.
We found support for this hypothesis; behavioral disinhibition, internalizing, and substance use at ages 13–14 all significantly predicted less positive peer environment at ages 15–18. Behavioral disinhibition and early substance use at ages 13–14 also predicted peer substance using environment as hypothesized. However, this pattern of results was not repeated in the transition from age 21 to ages 24–27. Generalized anxiety and substance abuse or dependence symptoms at age 21 did not impact partner general environment. The path between MDE symptoms and positive partner environments was marginal.
Hypothesis 2b – How do mental health and substance abuse problems interact to influence each other across development?
We hypothesized that substance use and substance-using environments would affect mental health functioning in subsequent time periods. Specifically, we hypothesized that (a) family substance use environment would predict internalizing tendencies at ages 13–14; (b) peer substance use environment at ages 15–18 would increase mental health problems (symptoms of GAD and MDE) at age 21; (c) partner substance using environment would predict both MDE and GAD diagnoses at ages 30–33; and d) individual substance use at ages 13–14 and at age 21 would predict mental health problems at age 21 and ages 30–33, respectively. As suggested by the self-medication hypothesis, we expected reciprocal effects of mental health problems on substance use. Internalizing at ages 13–14 was predicted to increase the likelihood of symptoms of substance abuse or dependence at age 21, and mental health at age 21 was expected to increase drug dependence at ages 30–33.
Contrary to prediction, family substance using environment did not predict mental health (internalizing) at ages 13–14. However, consistent with the hypothesis, both peer substance use environment and partner substance use environment predicted subsequent mental health symptoms (GAD and MDE) at age 21, and mental health diagnosis (GAD and MDE) at ages 30–33. Individual substance use at ages 13–14 did not predict later mental health functioning at age 21 as we expected.
We found no support for the self-medication hypothesis. Internalizing at ages 13–14 did not predict symptoms of substance abuse or dependence at age 21, and mental health symptoms (MDE and GAD) at age 21 did not predict drug abuse or dependence diagnosis at ages 30–33.
Saturated Model Results
We also examined associations that we did not hypothesize a priori by testing a saturated path model. We found that childhood positive family environment, family drug environments, and family history of depression each had cascading effects on future environments and functioning beyond those we originally theorized. Positive family environments had an additional protective effect, reducing the likelihood of MDE symptoms at age 21, over and above the other predictors. Family drug environments predicted a decrease in positive functioning at ages 30–33. Finally, family history of depression significantly increased the likelihood of selecting into a peer substance use environment, positive peer environment, and substance abuse or dependence diagnosis at ages 30–33 in the saturated model.
Other differences between the saturated and hypothesized models are important to note. When the path between MDE symptoms at age 21 and GAD diagnosis at ages 30–33 was included in the model, the continuity of GAD symptoms and partner drug environment on GAD diagnosis was no longer significant. Also of interest, the effect of internalizing at ages 13–14 significantly decreases positive functioning at ages 30–33, and decreases the likelihood of having symptoms of substance abuse or dependence at age 21. In the saturated model, family drug environments did impact behavioral disinhibition, which was a path we hypothesized that was not significant in the model shown in Figure 1.
Discussion
We proposed and tested two broad research questions that examined developmental cascades and the impact of the interplay of environmental influences and individual functioning on positive functioning, mental health, and substance abuse outcomes. Overall, the proposed model was largely supported by the results. These findings highlight important lessons learned from combining the social development model with a developmental cascades framework. First, there were short- and long-term cascading effects of positive social environments throughout development, with positive influences on proximal individual functioning and long-reaching effects of early positive family environment. We found cascades of family history of depression on mental health functioning throughout adulthood. Family substance use also began a cascade on substance-specific social environments across development, which was the pathway through which increasing severity of substance use problems flowed. Second, including social environmental influences in a developmental cascades model allowed interactions between individual functioning and the social environment to be examined over time. We found that social environmental influences were pivotal in the propagation of problems with individual functioning, especially for substance use. Third, patterns of results modeling the interplay between individual functioning and social environments and mental health and substance abuse highlight the interactive nature of these factors across development. Furthermore, we found evidence that adolescent but not adult individual functioning influenced selection into positive social environments, and substance using social environments had significant cross-domain detrimental effects on mental health.
Importance of Early Family Experiences
Families play a critical role in beginning a trajectory towards positive and pathological development, and the effects of family social environments cascade across development. Results in the current study show that positive family functioning, family history of depression, and family substance use environments echo forward through time in multiple developmental domains. This work is one of few to incorporate family influences in a cascade framework (Herrenkohl et al., 2010). Fewer still have examined positive family environment alongside family psychopathology. Incorporating family influences in cascade models demonstrates how this important social environment shapes early trajectories through specific parenting behaviors and family psychopathology, all of which offer opportunities to intervene to interrupt processes of risk accumulation or support processes of protection.
Positive families help shape the nature of future social environments by influencing the selection of later positive social environments (Connolly et al., 2000; Furman et al., 2002). Previously these links have been studied separately, or in a limited developmental span. The results of the present model suggest that families in childhood impact partner positive social environment and positive functioning in adulthood, above and beyond the presence of parental psychopathology, parent substance use, and all of the other factors in the model. The saturated model further revealed that positive families might also provide protection against symptoms of depression in young adulthood.
The results also provide strong support for the effect of family history of depression on the development of major depressive episode symptoms and generalized anxiety symptoms in early adulthood, as well as on depression diagnosis in adulthood. These findings add to the conversation about intergenerational transmission of depression (McCauley et al., 2001), as strong effects remained after including the effects of positive families, individual functioning, and substance using social environments. Not only are the effects of family history of depression far reaching and cross-domain, they are also some of the strongest effect sizes in the model. In fact, the saturated model showed that family history of depression had additional unanticipated effects on peer positive environments and peer substance using environment, as well as drug abuse or dependence diagnosis in adulthood.
One area where the model hypotheses were not supported was the role of internalizing in the mental health cascade. Internalizing was hypothesized to be influenced by family history of depression and to be the beginning of a developmental path toward mental health problems. However, internalizing was not predicted by family history of depression (except in the saturated model), nor did it act as a precursor to later mental health problems as expected. It may be that teacher-rated measures of internalizing lack sensitivity or that internalizing at ages 13–14 is reflective of a passing state in our sample and is not a robust risk factor for later mental health problems. What is clear is that links between early internalizing and later mental health require further investigation in future analyses.
Another hypothesis that was not supported was our expectation that peer positive social environment operates similarly to the family and has cascading effects on functioning in the proximal developmental period. We also predicted that selecting into a positive peer environment would be predicted by the positive family environment, as families were theorized to provide a model of social relationships. However, we did not find a relationship between positive family environment and positive peer environment. We suspect that the influence of positive families may flow indirectly through the influence on individual functioning. It also may be that the relationships with peers are not as deep as with family and romantic partners, thus the idea of thinking of families as templates for peer relationships is inaccurate.
As with positive families and family history of depression, early family substance use environment began a cascade of influence across development. Although previous research has found that family substance use is a risk factor for adolescent and adult substance use problems (Hawkins et al., 1992; Stone et al., 2012), our results show that family substance use had a greater effect on adolescent peer substance use environment than adolescent substance use. It also set in motion continuity in substance use environment, with substance-using families driving the selection of substance-using peers, which in turn predicted choosing substance-using romantic partners. The strength of these associations also increased at each step in the cascade. Finally, family substance use environment had a long-reaching effect on substance use problems in young adulthood. By the time individuals select partners who use substances, these cascades have set in motion a pattern of substance use and substance using social environments.
The domain-specific cascades begun by positive families, family history of depression, and substance using families offer opportunities to intervene to interrupt a process of risk accumulation or support a processes of protection. The aspects of family functioning explored by this study could serve as targets of prevention interventions. The fact that families are important for development is not a new concept, but the cascading influence of specific aspects of family environments on mental health and substance use outcomes through to adulthood provides additional clarity on the nature of families’ influence, as well as the long-lasting influence of families through time.
Uniting the Social Environment with Developmental Cascades
Blending the social development model with the developmental cascades approach adds to the knowledge base about the interacting and cascading influences of functioning and social environmental influences on substance abuse and dependence, mental health, and positive functioning outcomes. The present study demonstrated important lessons learned by uniting these theories—that social environments have an important influence on the development and trajectories of psychopathology, and that these influences vary by developmental period and have important cross-domain effects.
The importance of social environmental influences on individual functioning is particularly evident in the substance use cascade. Prior substance use did not directly predict future substance use problems but operated through substance using social environments. Thus, substance using social environments are the primary driver for the continuation of substance use into adulthood. Because many cascade models do not incorporate the role of the social environment, individual substance use can be attributed to other individual-level factors, when it may be the social environment that is a crucial force maintaining substance use.
The reciprocal influences of substance using social environments and individual substance use are evident in the model, as both selection and influence factors are in effect for peer and partner substance use environments. Using substances early predicted selecting peers who use substances. Similarly, partner substance use environment predicted substance abuse or dependence with some of the strongest effect sizes in our model. The strength of this association calls for additional research and intervention attention focused on partner social environments as critical in the etiological pathway to substance abuse and dependence diagnoses in adulthood, as well as a social domain to consider for intervention. This is critically important as many have found that partnering has a large impact on desistence of drug use during young adulthood (Bachman et al., 2002). Our data indicate that partnering with a substance using person will actually increase the likelihood of abuse and dependence diagnosis, suggesting the importance of considering the substance use of the partner, not just partnering per se.
These findings motivate continued research uniting the social development model and the development cascades approach, particularly in etiological work informing preventive interventions. Individuals who use substances are influenced to continue to use by the social environments to which they are exposed. The strong influence of the social environment points to the importance of substance using social environments as points for intervention throughout the lifecourse. Further, preventing early use may impact selecting peers who use substances, and preventing association with substance using peers has the potential to decrease future problem substance use.
The Interplay Between Cascades of Individual Functioning and Social Environments
Previous studies have attempted to understand the complex relationships between individuals and their environment and between mental health and substance abuse problems. Yet few have examined either of these phenomena in a developmental cascade framework extending to adulthood. Taking a developmental cascades approach is advantageous to modeling interplay between these domains of functioning, as it calls attention to cross-domain effects and the potentiation of risk factors through time. One important finding resulting from this approach is the importance of the influence of individual functioning on selection into social environments, particularly in the adolescent period where individual functioning was shown to predict the selection into positive and substance using peer networks. This finding is consistent with estimates of gene-environment correlation from twin studies reporting heritability estimates of 15%–35% for environment measures relevant to psychiatry/psychology (Kendler & Baker, 2007), and highlights the importance of this developmental period as a pivotal time point for intervention. Poor functioning may impact the ability of adolescents to form positive peer relationships. Because we do not see the same effects of functioning on the social environment later in development, middle adolescence may be a critical point for intervening in the lives of those with poor individual functioning.
Another important cross-domain finding concerned the complex interplay of mental health and substance abuse problems across development. This analysis provides evidence that substance using environments affect mental health, above and beyond the additional influences of substance use behavior, positive social environmental factors, or family history of depression. However, support for the self-medication hypothesis was inconsistent in the results. If self-medication were a factor, we would expect that the presence of mental health problems would predict substance use problems in subsequent time periods, but the hypothesized model did not support these relationships.
Substance using environments, but not substance use itself, had significant impacts on mental health both in the transition to adulthood and in adulthood. This may be because individuals select peers or romantic partners that have similar problems. Substance abuse and mental health problems are significantly correlated within each time point, lending support for this interpretation. Being surrounded by others who use substances could be distressing and drive mental health problems. Finally, selecting into substance using social environments may have the same underlying risk factors (social and genetic) that drive mental health problems. Interrupting these mechanisms may be useful to mental health prevention strategies. Peer and romantic partner social environments are the two areas that individuals can select into (versus family, who are generally not chosen) and are thus potentially malleable through intervention. This pattern of results provides further support for the importance of incorporating substance using social environments into both substance use research as well as research regarding the development of mood and anxiety disorders. By incorporating additional potential sources of mental health problems into the model, the effect of substance using environments on mental health can be further isolated. This finding helps to explain why prevention interventions focused on positive youth development and decreased substance abuse have had positive effects on the development of mental health problems, despite being designed to impact substance use outcomes directly (Hawkins, Kosterman, Catalano, Hill, & Abbott, 2008; Mason et al., 2007).
Implications and Future Directions
Integrating the social development model and developmental cascades in this model has produced novel understanding of the interplay of positive and pathological development. The results confirm that mental health and substance abuse problems are multiply determined, interact over development, and that social environments play a critical role in the propagation of problems with mental health and substance use. The present study also had constraints that should be addressed in future studies. The retrospective measure of family history of depression may have resulted in biased reports based on the respondents’ current mental health state. Future work should examine the effect of family history of depression that is prospective and self-reported by parents, or taken directly from medical records. In addition, having teachers assess the internal state of a students based on their external behaviors may have affected the strength of our findings about the influence of internalizing on subsequent mental health. Parent or self-report of internalizing may be a better measure of children’s internal states. Finally, future work should replicate these findings in samples in other geographic and ethnic compositions.
The present study has implications for theoretical advances in the field developmental psychopathology. First, developmental cascades models have tended to focus on cascades of behavior (e.g., cascading externalizing and cascading social competence). Some of our strongest findings were the cascading influence between social environments over time as well as between social environments and behavioral functioning, which suggests a broader focus of developmental cascades as the interplay between social environment and behavioral functioning. Second, our findings strongly suggest that these social-behavioral developmental cascades consider multiple domains of the social environment since salient proximal environments change across developmental periods. Third, our results suggest that social-developmental cascades models consider the distinct influences of general environmental functioning as well as problem-specific (e.g., substance using) social environments.
Finally, our findings provide implications for the development of interventions that seek to promote positive adult functioning and prevent mental health and substance abuse problems. Research has shown that social development interventions in childhood can have long-term, cascading impacts on developmental processes (e.g., Hawkins et al., 2008; Olds, Sadler, & Kitzman, 2007; Patterson, Forgatch, & DeGarmo, 2010). Our findings support the importance of intervening in these early social environments in shaping these developmental cascades; however, our results also point to the importance of continued intervention in adolescence and adulthood. Uniting the developmental cascades framework with the social development model has extended our understanding of developmental processes leading to adaptive and maladaptive functioning, as well as articulated specific, actionable targets for intervention in childhood, adolescence, and adulthood.
Acknowledgments
Funding for this study was provided by grants from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (5R01DA003721-01-08, 1R01DA09679-01-14 and R01DA024411-01-06). The content of this paper is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the funding agency. The authors gratefully acknowledge SSDP study participants for their continued contribution to the longitudinal study. They also acknowledge the SDRG Survey Research Division for their hard work maintaining high panel retention, and the SDRG editorial and administrative staff for their editorial and project support.
References
- Achenbach TM, Dumenci L, Rescorla LA. Ratings of relations between DSM-IV diagnostic categories and items of the CBCL/6-18, TRF, and YSR. University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families; Burlington: 2001. [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 3rd ed American Psychiatric Association; Washington, DC: 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Bachman JG, O'Malley PM, Schulenberg JE, Johnston LD, Bryant AL, Merline AC. The decline of substance use in young adulthood: Changes in social activities, roles, and beliefs. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Mahwah, NJ: 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Bailey JA, Hill KG, Meacham MC, Young SE, Hawkins JD. Strategies for characterizing complex phenotypes and environments: General and specific family environmental predictors of young adult tobacco dependence, alcohol use disorder, and co-occurring problems. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2011;118:444–451. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.05.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bailey JA, Hill KG, Oesterle S, Hawkins JD. Linking substance use and problem behavior across three generations. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2006;34:273–292. doi: 10.1007/s10802-006-9033-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A. Social learning theory. Prentice Hall; Englewood Cliffs, NJ: 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Beesdo K, Pine DS, Lieb R, Wittchen H. Incidence and risk patterns of anxiety and depressive disorders and categorization of generalized anxiety disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2010;67:47–57. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bijttebier P, Beck I, Claes L, Vandereycken W. Gray's Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory as a framework for research on personality–psychopathology associations. Clinical Psychology Review. 2009;29:421–430. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birmaher B, Ryan ND, Williamson DE, Brent DA, Kaufman J, Dahl RE, Nelson B. Childhood and adolescent depression: A review of the past 10 years. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1996;35:1427–1439. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199611000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bornstein MH, Hahn C-S, Haynes OM. Social competence, externalizing, and internalizing behavioral adjustment from early childhood through early adolescence: Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology. 2010;22:717–735. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brady KT, Sinha R. Co-occurring mental and substance use disorders: The neurobiological effects of chronic stress. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;162:1483–1493. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.8.1483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown EC, Catalano RF, Fleming CB, Haggerty KP, Abbott RD, Cortes RC, Park J. Mediator effects in the social development model: An examination of constituent theories. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health. 2005;15:221–235. doi: 10.1002/cbm.27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burke JD, Jr, Burke KC, Rae DS. Increased rates of drug abuse and dependence after onset of mood or anxiety disorders in adolescence. Hospital and Community Psychiatry. 1994;45:451–455. doi: 10.1176/ps.45.5.451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burt KB, Obradović J, Long JD, Masten AS. The interplay of social competence and psychopathology over 20 years: Testing transactional and cascade models. Child Development. 2008;79:359–374. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01130.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Capaldi DM, Stoolmiller M. Co-occurrence of conduct problems and depressive symptoms in early adolescent boys: III. Prediction to young-adult adjustment. Development and Psychopathology. 1999;11:59–84. doi: 10.1017/s0954579499001959. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Catalano RF, Hawkins JD. The social development model: A theory of antisocial behavior. In: Hawkins JD, editor. Delinquency and crime: Current theories. Cambridge University Press; New York: 1996. pp. 149–197. [Google Scholar]
- Catalano RF, Hawkins JD, Berglund ML, Pollard JA, Arthur MW. Prevention science and positive youth development: Competitive or cooperative frameworks? Journal of Adolescent Health. 2002;31:230–239. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(02)00496-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Catalano RF, Kosterman R, Hawkins JD, Newcomb MD, Abbott RD. Modeling the etiology of adolescent substance use: A test of the social development model. Journal of Drug Issues. 1996;26:429–455. doi: 10.1177/002204269602600207. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chassin L, Flora DB, King KM. Trajectories of alcohol and drug use and dependence from adolescence to adulthood: The effects of familial alcoholism and personality. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2004;113:483–498. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.4.483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colhoun HM, McKeigue PM, Smith GD. Problems of reporting genetic associations with complex outcomes. The Lancet. 2003;361:865–872. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(03)12715-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Compton WM, Thomas YF, Stinson FS, Grant BF. Prevalence, correlates, disability, and comorbidity of DSM-IV drug abuse and dependence in the United States: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2007;64:566–576. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.5.566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Connolly J, Furman W, Konarski R. The role of peers in the emergence of heterosexual romantic relationships in adolescence. Child Development. 2000;71:1395–1408. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Criss MM, Pettit GS, Bates JE, Dodge KA, Lapp AL. Family adversity, positive peer relationships, and children's externalizing behavior: A longitudinal perspective on risk and resilience. Child Development. 2002;73:1220–1237. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00468. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dishion TJ, Owen LD. A longitudinal analysis of friendships and substance use: Bidirectional influence from adolescence to adulthood. Developmental Psychology. 2002;38:480–491. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.38.4.480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donnellan MB, Larsen-Rife D, Conger RD. Personality, family history, and competence in early adult romantic relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2005;88:562–576. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.3.562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duncan LE, Keller MC. A Critical Review of the First 10 Years of Candidate Gene-by-Environment Interaction Research in Psychiatry. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2011;168:1041–1049. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11020191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duncan LE, Pollastri AR, Smoller JW. Mind the Gap Why Many Geneticists and Psychological Scientists Have Discrepant Views About Gene-Environment Interaction (GXE) Research. American Psychologist. 2014;69:249–268. doi: 10.1037/a0036320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elder GH., Jr. The life course as developmental theory. Child Development. 1998;69:1–12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Englund MM, Siebenbruner J. Developmental pathways linking externalizing symptoms, internalizing symptoms, and academic competence to adolescent substance use. Journal of Adolescence. 2012;35:1123–1140. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Epstein M, Hill KG, Bailey JA, Hawkins JD. The effect of general and drug-specific family environments on comorbid and drug-specific problem behavior: A longitudinal examination. Developmental Psychology. 2013;49:1151–1164. doi: 10.1037/a0029309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fleming CB, Catalano RF, Oxford ML, Harachi TW. A test of generalizability of the social development model across gender and income groups with longitudinal data from the elementary school developmental period. Journal of Quantitative Criminology. 2002;18:423–439. [Google Scholar]
- Fleming CB, White HR, Catalano RF. Romantic relationships and substance use in early adulthood: An examination of the influences of relationship type, partner substance use, and relationship quality. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2010;51:153–167. doi: 10.1177/0022146510368930. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franken IHA, Muris P. BIS/BAS personality characteristics and college students’ substance use. Personality and Individual Differences. 2006;40:1497–1503. [Google Scholar]
- Franken IHA, Muris P, Georgieva I. Gray's model of personality and addiction. Addictive Behaviors. 2006;31:399–403. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.05.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Furman W, Simon VA, Shaffer L, Bouchey HA. Adolescents' working models and styles for relationships with parents, friends, and romantic partners. Child Development. 2002;73:241–255. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodwin RD. Association between physical activity and mental disorders among adults in the United States. Preventive Medicine. 2003;36:698–703. doi: 10.1016/s0091-7435(03)00042-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant BF, Goldstein RB, Chou SP, Huang B, Stinson FS, Dawson DA, Pickering RP. Sociodemographic and psychopathologic predictors of first incidence of DSM-IV substance use, mood and anxiety disorders: Results from the Wave 2 National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Molecular Psychiatry. 2009;14:1051–1066. doi: 10.1038/mp.2008.41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graves PL, Wang N-Y, Mead LA, Johnson JV, Klag MJ. Youthful precursors of midlife social support. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;74:1329–1336. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.5.1329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guo J, Hill KG, Hawkins JD, Catalano RF, Abbott RD. A developmental analysis of sociodemographic, family, and peer effects on adolescent illicit drug initiation. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2002;41:838–845. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200207000-00017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haller M, Handley E, Chassin L, Bountress K. Developmental cascades: Linking adolescent substance use, affiliation with substance use promoting peers, and academic achievement to adult substance use disorders. Development and Psychopathology. 2010;22:899–916. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000532. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Handley ED, Chassin L, Haller MM, Bountress KE, Dandreaux D, Beltran I. Do executive and reactive disinhibition mediate the effects of familial substance use disorders on adolescent externalizing outcomes? Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2011;120:528–542. doi: 10.1037/a0024162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harter SL. Psychosocial adjustment of adult children of alcoholics: A review of the recent empirical literature. Clinical Psychology Review. 2000;20:311–337. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(98)00084-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hawkins JD, Catalano RF, Kosterman R, Abbott R, Hill KG. Preventing adolescent health-risk behaviors by strengthening protection during childhood. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine. 1999;153:226–234. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.153.3.226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hawkins JD, Catalano RF, Miller JY. Risk and protective factors for alcohol and other drug problems in adolescence and early adulthood: Implications for substance abuse prevention. Psychological Bulletin. 1992;112:64–105. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hawkins JD, Kosterman R, Catalano RF, Hill KG, Abbott RD. Effects of social development intervention in childhood 15 years later. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine. 2008;162:1133–1141. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.162.12.1133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herrenkohl TI, Kosterman R, Mason WA, Hawkins JD, McCarty CA, McCauley E. Effects of childhood conduct problems and family adversity on health, health behaviors, and service use in early adulthood: Tests of developmental pathways involving adolescent risk taking and depression. Development and Psychopathology. 2010;22:655–665. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hicks BM, Johnson W, Durbin CE, Blonigen DM, Iacono WG, McGue M. Gene-environment correlation in the development of adolescent substance abuse: Selection effects of child personality and mediation via contextual risk factors. Development and Psychopathology. 2013;25:119–132. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412000946. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hill KG, Hawkins JD, Bailey JA, Catalano RF, Abbott RD, Shapiro V. Person-environment interaction in the prediction of alcohol abuse and alcohol dependence in adulthood. Drug & Alcohol Dependence. 2010;110:62–69. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hill KG, Hawkins JD, Catalano RF, Abbott RD, Guo J. Family influences on the risk of daily smoking initiation. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2005;37:202–210. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2004.08.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirschi T. Causes of delinquency. University of California Press; Berkeley, CA: 1969. [Google Scholar]
- Horwitz AV, White HR, Howell-White S. Becoming married and mental health: A longitudinal study of a cohort of young adults. Journal of Marriage and Family. 1996;58:895–907. [Google Scholar]
- Huang B, Kosterman R, Catalano RF, Hawkins JD, Abbott RD. Modeling mediation in the etiology of violent behavior in adolescence: A test of the social development model. Criminology. 2001;39:75–107. [Google Scholar]
- Iacono WG, Malone SM, McGue M. Behavioral disinhibition and the development of early-onset addiction: Common and specific influences. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2008;4:325–348. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.4.022007.141157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaffee SR, Price TS. Gene-environment correlations: A review of the evidence and implications for prevention of mental illness. Molecular Psychiatry. 2007;12:432–442. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001950. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kendler KS, Baker JH. Genetic influences on measures of the environment: A systematic review. Psychological Medicine. 2007;37:615–626. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706009524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khantzian EJ. The self-medication hypothesis of substance use disorders: A reconsideration and recent applications. Harvard Review of Psychiatry. 1997;4:231–244. doi: 10.3109/10673229709030550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirisci L, Mezzich AC, Reynolds M, Tarter RE, Aytaclar S. Prospective study of the association between neurobehavior disinhibition and peer environment on illegal drug use in boys and girls. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse. 2009;35:145–150. doi: 10.1080/00952990902825405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knafo A, Jaffee SR. Gene-environment correlation in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 2013;25:1–6. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412000855. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knecht A, Snijders TAB, Baerveldt C, Steglich CEG, Raub W. Friendship and delinquency: Selection and influence processes in early adolescence. Social Development. 2010;19:494–514. [Google Scholar]
- Knecht AB, Burk WJ, Weesie J, Steglich C. Friendship and alcohol use in early adolescence: A multilevel social network approach. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2011;21:475–487. [Google Scholar]
- Knight KE. Assortative mating and partner influence on antisocial behavior across the life course. Journal of Family Theory & Review. 2011;3:198–219. [Google Scholar]
- Kosterman R, Hawkins JD, Mason WA, Herrenkohl T, Lengua L, McCauley E. Assessment of behavior problems in childhood and adolescence as predictors of early adult depression. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2010;32:118–127. doi: 10.1007/s10862-009-9138-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kosterman R, Hill KG, Lee JO, Meacham MC, Abbott RD, Catalano RF, Hawkins JD. Young adult social development as a mediator of alcohol use disorder symptoms from age 21 to 30. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2014;28:348–358. doi: 10.1037/a0034970. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lansford JE, Malone PS, Dodge KA, Pettit GS, Bates JE. Developmental cascades of peer rejection, social information processing biases, and aggression during middle childhood. Development and Psychopathology. 2010;22:593–602. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee JO, Hill KG, Guttmannova K, Bailey JA, Hartigan LA, Hawkins JD, Catalano RF. The effects of general and alcohol-specific peer factors in adolescence on trajectories of alcohol abuse disorder symptoms from 21 to 33 years. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2012;121:213–219. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.08.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lembke A. Time to abandon the self-medication hypothesis in patients with psychiatric disorders. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse. 2012;38:524–529. doi: 10.3109/00952990.2012.694532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leonard KE, Rothbard JC. Alcohol and the marriage effect. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1999;13:139–146. doi: 10.15288/jsas.1999.s13.139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leve L, Kim H, Pears K. Childhood temperament and family environment as predictors of internalizing and externalizing trajectories from ages 5 to 17. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2005;33:505–520. doi: 10.1007/s10802-005-6734-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewin-Bizan S, Bowers EP, Lerner RM. One good thing leads to another: Cascades of positive youth development among American adolescents. Development and Psychopathology. 2010;22:759–770. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewinsohn PM, Rohde P, Klein DN, Seeley JR. Natural course of adolescent major depressive disorder: I. Continuity into young adulthood. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1999;38:56–63. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199901000-00020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewinsohn PM, Rohde P, Seeley JR, Klein DN, Gotlib IH. Psychosocial functioning of young adults who have experienced and recovered from major depressive disorder during adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2003;112:353–363. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.112.3.353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynne-Landsman SD, Bradshaw CP, Ialongo NS. Testing a developmental cascade model of adolescent substance use trajectories and young adult adjustment. Development and Psychopathology. 2010;22:933–948. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000556. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macleod J, Oakes R, Copello A, Crome I, Egger M, Hickman M, Smith GD. Psychological and social sequelae of cannabis and other illicit drug use by young people: A systematic review of longitudinal, general population studies. The Lancet. 2004;363:1579–1588. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(04)16200-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markou A, Kosten TR, Koob GF. Neurobiological similarities in depression and drug dependence: A self-medication hypothesis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1998;18:135–174. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(97)00113-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mars B, Collishaw S, Smith D, Thapar A, Potter R, Sellers R, Thapar A. Offspring of parents with recurrent depression: Which features of parent depression index risk for offspring psychopathology? Journal of Affective Disorders. 2012;136:44–53. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mason WA, Hitchings JE, Spoth RL. The interaction of conduct problems and depressed mood in relation to adolescent substance involvement and peer substance use. Drug & Alcohol Dependence. 2008;96:233–248. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.03.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mason WA, Kosterman R, Hawkins JD, Haggerty KP, Spoth RL, Redmond C. Influence of a family-focused substance use preventive intervention on growth in adolescent depressive symptoms. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2007;17:541–564. [Google Scholar]
- Mason WA, Kosterman R, Hawkins JD, Herrenkohl TI, Lengua LJ, McCauley E. Predicting depression, social phobia, and violence in early adulthood from childhood behavior problems. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2004;43:307–315. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200403000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masten AS. Developmental psychopathology: Pathways to the future. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2006;30:47–54. [Google Scholar]
- Masten AS, Cicchetti D. Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology. 2010;22:491–495. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masten AS, Roisman GI, Long JD, Burt KB, Obradović J, Riley JR, Tellegen A. Developmental cascades: Linking academic achievement and externalizing and internalizing symptoms over 20 years. Developmental Psychology. 2005;41:733–746. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.5.733. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCauley E, Pavlidis K, Kendall K. Developmental precursors of depression: The child and the social environment. The Depressed Child and Adolescent. 2001;2:46–78. [Google Scholar]
- McGue M, Iacono WG, Krueger R. The association of early adolescent problem behavior and adult psychopathology: A multivariate behavioral genetic perspective. Behavior Genetics. 2006;36:591–602. doi: 10.1007/s10519-006-9061-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGue M, Iacono WG, Legrand LN, Malone S, Elkins I. Origins and consequences of age at first drink. I. Associations with substance-use disorders, disinhibitory behavior and psychopathology, and P3 amplitude. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2001;25:1156–1165. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merline A, Jager J, Schulenberg JE. Adolescent risk factors for adult alcohol use and abuse: Stability and change of predictive value across early and middle adulthood. Addiction. 2008;103:84–99. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02178.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus user's guide. 4th ed Muthén & Muthén; Los Angeles: 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Newman DL, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Magdol L, Silva PA, Stanton WR. Psychiatric disorder in a birth cohort of young adults: Prevalence, comorbidity, clinical significance, and new case incidence from ages 11 to 21. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1996;64:552–562. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Obradović J, Burt KB, Masten AS. Testing a dual cascade model linking competence and symptoms over 20 years from childhood to adulthood. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2009;39:90–102. doi: 10.1080/15374410903401120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olds DL, Sadler L, Kitzman H. Programs for parents of infants and toddlers: Recent evidence from randomized trials. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;48:355–391. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01702.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oxford M, Oxford ML, Harachi TW, Catalano RF, Abbott RD. Preadolescent predictors of substance initiation: A test of both the direct and mediated effect of family social control factors on deviant peer associations and substance initiation. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse. 2001;27:599–616. doi: 10.1081/ada-100107658. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pardo Y, Aguilar R, Molinuevo B, Torrubia R. Alcohol use as a behavioural sign of disinhibition: Evidence from J.A. Gray's model of personality. Addictive Behaviors. 2007;32:2398–2403. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parker JG, Rubin K, Erath SA, Wojslawowicz JC, Buskirk A. Peer relationships, child development, and adjustment: A developmental psychopathology perspective. In: Chicchetti D, Cohen DJ, editors. Developmental psychopathology, theory and method. John Wiley & Sons; 2006. pp. 419–493. [Google Scholar]
- Patterson GR, Forgatch MS, DeGarmo DS. Cascading effects following intervention. Development and psychopathology. 2010;22:949–970. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piehler T, Véronneau M-H, Dishion T. Substance use progression from adolescence to early adulthood: Effortful control in the context of friendship influence and early-onset use. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2012;40:1045–1058. doi: 10.1007/s10802-012-9626-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pine DS, Cohen E, Cohen P, Brook J. Adolescent depressive symptoms as predictors of adult depression: Moodiness or mood disorder? American Journal of Psychiatry. 1999;156:133–135. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.1.133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pine DS, Cohen P, Gurley D, Brook J, Ma Y. The risk for early-adulthood anxiety and depressive disorders in adolescents with anxiety and depressive disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1998;55:56–64. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.1.56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plomin R, DeFries JC, Loehlin JC. Genotype-environment interaction and correlation in the analysis of human behavior. Psychological Bulletin. 1977;84:309–322. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poulin F, Kiesner J, Pedersen S, Dishion TJ. A short-term longitudinal analysis of friendship selection on early adolescent substance use. Journal of Adolescence. 2011;34:249–256. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reinherz HZ, Giaconia RM, Carmola Hauf AM, Wasserman MS, Paradis AD. General and specific childhood risk factors for depression and drug disorders by early adulthood. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2000;39:223–231. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200002000-00023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reinherz HZ, Paradis AD, Giaconia RM, Stashwick CK, Fitzmaurice G. Childhood and adolescent predictors of major depression in the transition to adulthood. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:2141–2147. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.12.2141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Repetti RL, Taylor SE, Seeman TE. Risky families: Family social environments and the mental and physical health of offspring. Psychological Bulletin. 2002;128:330–366. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rhule-Louie D, McMahon R. Problem behavior and romantic relationships: Assortative mating, behavior contagion, and desistance. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2007;10:53–100. doi: 10.1007/s10567-006-0016-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robins LN, Helzer JE, Croughan J, Williams JBW, Spitzer RL. NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule: Version III (May 1981) National Institute of Mental Health; Rockville, MD: 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Roza SJ, Hofstra MB, van der Ende J, Verhulst FC. Stable prediction of mood and anxiety disorders based on behavioral and emotional problems in childhood: A 14-year follow-up during childhood, adolescence, and young adulthood. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:2116–2121. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.12.2116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubin KH, Bukowski WM. Handbook of peer interactions, relationships, and groups. Guilford Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Russek LG, Schwartz GE. Feelings of parental caring predict health status in midlife: A 35-year follow-up of the Harvard Mastery of Stress Study. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 1997;20:1–13. doi: 10.1023/a:1025525428213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sameroff AJ, MacKenzie MJ. Research strategies for capturing transactional models of development: The limits of the possible. Development and Psychopathology. 2003;15:613–640. doi: 10.1017/s0954579403000312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmitz N, Kruse J, Kugler J. The association between physical exercises and health-related quality of life in subjects with mental disorders: Results from a cross-sectional survey. Preventive Medicine. 2004;39:1200–1207. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2004.04.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwartz D, McFadyen-Ketchum S, Dodge KA, Pettit GS, Bates JE. Early behavior problems as a predictor of later peer group victimization: Moderators and mediators in the pathways of social risk. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1999;27:191–201. doi: 10.1023/a:1021948206165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stone AL, Becker LG, Huber AM, Catalano RF. Review of risk and protective factors of substance use and problem use in emerging adulthood. Addictive behaviors. 2012;37:747–775. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.02.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sullivan CJ, Hirschfield P. Problem behavior in the middle school years: An assessment of the social development model. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency. 2011;48:566–593. [Google Scholar]
- Sutherland EH, Cressey DR. Criminology. Lippincott; New York: 1970. [Google Scholar]
- Tarter RE, Kirisci L, Mezzich A, Cornelius JR, Pajer K, Vanyukov M, Clark D. Neurobehavioral disinhibition in childhood predicts early age at onset of substance use disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:1078–1085. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teo AR, Choi H, Valenstein M. Social relationships and depression: Ten-year follow-up from a nationally representative study. PLoS One. 2013;8:e62396. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0062396. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomlinson KL, Brown SA. Self-medication or social learning? A comparison of models to predict early adolescent drinking. Addictive Behaviors. 2012;37:179–186. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.09.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weissman MM, Wickramaratne P, Nomura Y, Warner V, Pilowsky D, Verdeli H. Offspring of depressed parents: 20 years later. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;163:1001–1008. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.6.1001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiesner M, Windle M. Young adult substance use and depression as a consequence of delinquency trajectories during middle adolescence. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2006;16:239–264. [Google Scholar]
- Wolff JC, Ollendick TH. The comorbidity of conduct problems and depression in childhood and adolescence. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2006;9:201–220. doi: 10.1007/s10567-006-0011-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yanovitzky I. Sensation seeking and adolescent drug use: The mediating role of association with deviant peers and pro-drug discussions. Health Communication. 2005;17:67–89. doi: 10.1207/s15327027hc1701_5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yoshikawa H. Prevention as cumulative protection: Effects of early family support and education on chronic delinquency and its risks. Psychological Bulletin. 1994;115:28–54. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zahn-Waxler C, Klimes-Dougan B, Slattery MJ. Internalizing problems of childhood and adolescence: Prospects, pitfalls, and progress in understanding the development of anxiety and depression. Development and Psychopathology. 2000;12:443–466. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zoccolillo M. Co-occurrence of conduct disorder and its adult outcomes with depressive and anxiety disorders: A review. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1992;31:547–556. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199205000-00024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]