Abstract
Recent research has demonstrated that affective states elicited by viewing pictures varying in valence and arousal are identifiable from whole brain activation patterns observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Identification of affective states from more naturalistic stimuli has clinical relevance, but the feasibility of identifying these states on an individual trial basis from fMRI data elicited by dynamic multimodal stimuli is unclear. The goal of this study was to determine whether affective states can be similarly identified when participants view dynamic naturalistic audiovisual stimuli. Eleven participants viewed 5s audiovisual clips in a passive viewing task in the scanner. Valence and arousal for individual trials were identified both within and across participants based on distributed patterns of activity in areas selectively responsive to audiovisual naturalistic stimuli while controlling for lower level features of the stimuli. In addition, the brain regions identified by searchlight analyses to represent valence and arousal were consistent with previously identified regions associated with emotion processing. These findings extend previous results on the distributed representation of affect to multimodal dynamic stimuli.
1.0 Introduction
The two major components of core affect posited to underlie more complex emotions are valence, varying from negative to positive, and arousal, varying from low to high [1, 2]. Researchers have identified physiological correlates of valence and arousal across several different types of affect eliciting stimuli [3–5]. Similarly, neuroimaging studies have also demonstrated a number of correspondences between different neural activation patterns and levels of valence and arousal for typical [6–8] and clinical [9] populations. These results support the idea that the neural representations of valence and arousal should be identifiable on a trial-by-trial basis for a variety of different types of stimuli.
There has been growing interest in investigating the utility of multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA) approaches applied to fMRI data in clinical populations. MVPA is ideal in this regard because it is designed to analyze the data at the level of individual, which is critical in clinical applications. Much of the clinical work to date has examined functional responses to static affective stimuli, such as pictures of faces [10–12] or the images found in the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) [13]. There is a recent growing interest in affective processing of dynamic multimodal stimuli (see recent special issue, [14]) At the same time, it has been argued that naturalistic viewing conditions that are dynamic and multimodal, as well as low in task demands, may be critical in examining emotion perception in clinical populations [15]. For instance, a selective deficit to dynamic social stimuli, but not static stimuli, has been identified in individuals with autism spectrum disorders [16]. Impaired multisensory integration of emotions has been shown in schizophrenia [17] [18]. Moreover, context, that is more readily available in dynamic multimodal stimuli, has been shown to play an important role in emotion perception [19]. Therefore, it may be most relevant to examine affective responses to naturalistic multimodal stimuli that are closer to what is encountered in more realistic and natural settings, increasing the ecological validity of unimodal experimental designs [20, 21]. A first step in this direction is to establish that valence and arousal elicited by dynamic naturalistic stimuli can be reliably identified from fMRI data in a typical sample.
Previous fMRI studies have used MVPA to investigate the representation of affect, successfully decoding affective states from patterns of brain activity located in specific regions of interest as well as from patterns of whole brain activity [22]. However, most of those studies used static, visual stimuli and only a few studies investigated other modalities or dynamic stimuli [23], such as sounds [24, 25], smells [26], or autobiographic recall [27]. Thus it is unclear whether the affective dimensions of valence and arousal are identifiable from fMRI data using dynamic multimodal stimuli.
Behavioral studies have shown that multimodal presentation of congruent face and voice expressions facilitated emotion perception [28] compared to unimodal presentation. Context-rich multimodal stimuli may enhance affect recognition for patients with traumatic brain injury [29] and autism [30], but the reverse pattern was also reported for schizophrenic patients [31], possibly due to the failure of information integration across the two modalities. Brain activation evoked by natural dynamic stimuli has been shown to be highly reliable [32]. Behavioral studies of facial expressions have shown that dynamic stimuli are more easily recognized than static stimuli in both healthy populations [33, 34] and in clinical populations [35]. Neuroimaging studies have also found more extended activation patterns for dynamic facial expressions [36]. Many of the emotion studies comparing static versus dynamic conditions have utilized facial expressions, with few employing other types of stimuli [37]. Thus the representation of affective states in the brain induced by naturalistic dynamic stimuli requires further investigation.
Audiovisual clips may be a particularly relevant stimulus format to explore in linking experiences to more real world situations. In a constantly changing, dynamic environment, perceptual and affective components from multiple modalities tend to co-occur so that, for instance, one both sees and hears a laughing child on a playground. Audiovisual clips of affectively charged everyday occurrences present a dynamic and continuous audio-visual unfolding of events over time, more typical of naturalistic experience. These types of stimuli have a long history in emotion research [38, 39]. For example, film stimuli have been shown to produce differential psychophysiological response patterns to valence [40, 41]. Visual and auditory modalities that are stimulated together are more naturalistic in everyday life and may result in response enhancement and an advantage of redundancy [42]. Unlike static stimuli, audiovisual stimuli preserve natural timing relations and resolve ambiguities present in each separate modality [43].
One of the challenges of using naturalistic stimuli in research is the possible confounding of lower level features. For example, valence has been shown to be positively correlated with brightness [44] and level of arousal has been shown to be related to loudness and motion [45, 46]. Moreover, it has been demonstrated that affective states can be identified solely from lower level features. For example, using musical pieces, Coutinho and Cangelosi [47] identified arousal from lower level features such as loudness, tempo, pitch level, and sharpness, and they identified valence from features such as tempo and pitch level. In our experiment, we manipulate the affective states elicited by short videos along the dimensions of valence and arousal, producing four groupings of stimuli. We exercise some control of the semantic content of these audiovisual clips by distributing topics (human, animal, and inanimate) across the affective categories. To keep the stimuli naturalistic, we did not directly control the perceptual features, with an exception of sound intensity. Instead, we attempted to have a broad range of values of lower level features within each affective category and then control for these effects statistically and through the use of functional localizers. Thus, we sought to identify affective states under more naturalistic viewing conditions from neural patterns of activity measured with fMRI, after controlling for semantic and lower level features. By controlling for lower level features, we enhance the interpretation of our classification results as reflecting valence and arousal.
In summary, the goal of this study was to identify affective content of naturalistic audiovisual stimuli in individuals from fMRI data on a single trial basis, thus extending previous work that identified valence and/or arousal derived from static visual stimuli using MVPA methods based on fMRI data [7, 48]. In addition, we examine the neural representation of arousal and valence components of core affect in terms of areas that lead to identification of these core dimensions of affect. In this auxiliary analyses, we employ a decoding based searchlight analysis and compare the regions identified with those implicated in previous research as being important in emotion processing.
2.0 Method
2.1 Participants
This research was approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of South Carolina. All volunteers gave written informed consent in accordance with the Institutional Review Board at the University of South Carolina. Eleven (five female, two left handed) volunteer adults (mean age 23.91 years, SD = 4.02) from the University of South Carolina community participated in the fMRI experiment. A separate group of volunteer adults (n = 49) from the same community participated in a preliminary behavioral experiment for stimulus validation. All participants in the fMRI experiment reported normal hearing, normal or corrected to normal vision, and no history of neurological diseases.
2.2 Materials
Participants viewed affect-eliciting audiovisual clips that varied on levels of valence and arousal. Naturalistic audiovisual stimuli were selected ad hoc from a larger in-house behaviorally-validated stimulus set, with the goal to maximize differences in valence for positive versus negative sets and maximize differences in arousal for low versus high arousal sets, while attempting to match levels on the shared dimensions across sets so that valence and arousal values are orthogonal. Eight stimuli were selected for each affective category corresponding to each of the four quadrants of the affective space: high arousal-negative valence (HN), low arousal-negative valence (LN), low arousal-positive valence (LP), and high arousal-positive valence (HP). The stimuli were balanced on semantic content across the affective categories. Each affective category included four clips with humans, two clips with animals, and two clips with inanimate content. The stimuli did not contain speech or written language. Some audio components contained prosodic information. All stimuli were selected to reflect a single topic with homogeneous affective content for the duration of the clip (e.g., children laughing and running around a playground).
The 32 selected audiovisual stimuli were validated in a behavioral experiment on a separate group of participants (n = 49). Affective categories of stimuli were shown to differ in a non-overlapping way on the dimensions of valence and arousal (Table 1 and S1 Table). Participants were asked to rate their emotional states along one of six dimensions after each video. The dimensions reflected the degree to which the participant reported feeling excited, positive, calm, anxious, negative, or sad. A correlation matrix of the 32 stimuli across the six ratings was constructed for each individual and the combined data was analyzed with INDSCAL [49]. The dimensions of valence and arousal naturally emerged from the ratings of the 6 dimensions, even though the two dimensions were not rated directly (Fig 1). This method was used to minimize bias in demand characteristics in responding that could arise from direct ratings of valence and arousal [50]. In an Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) conducted on the dimensional values from the MDS solution, the positive videos were significantly higher than the negative videos in mean valence configuration values, F(1,28) = 439.21, p < .001, ηp2 = .969 (MPositive = .87 and MNegative = -.87) and the high arousal videos were significantly higher than the low arousal videos in mean arousal values, F(1,28) = 79.48, p < .001, ηp2 = .739 (MHigh = .37 and MLow = -.37).
Table 1. Description of audiovisual stimuli.
Description | Affective Category | F-test | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Negative Valence, High Arousal | Negative Valence, Low Arousal | Positive Valence, High Arousal | Positive Valence, Low Arousal | Valence | Arousal | |
Valence | -0.92 (0.24) | -0.83 (0.25) | 0.78 (0.16) | 0.96 (0.16) | F(1,28) = 583.87*** | ns. |
Arousal | 0.37 (0.11) | -0.24 (0.27) | 0.25 (0.18) | -0.38 (0.29) | ns. | F(1,28) = 62.17*** |
Hue | 0.35 (0.12) | 0.26 (0.11) | 0.41 (0.18) | 0.34 (0.24) | ns. | ns. |
Saturation | 0.25 (0.08) | 0.27 (0.16) | 0.33 (0.16) | 0.35 (0.18) | ns. | ns. |
Value (Brightness) | 0.47 (0.09) | 0.52 (0.07) | 0.60 (0.12) | 0.54 (0.14) | ns. | ns. |
Amplitude (dB) (left) | 10.00 (3.38) | 14.75 (3.26) | 13.78 (2.79) | 11.85 (2.86) | ns. | ns. |
Amplitude (dB) (right) | 10.44 (3.55) | 14.76 (3.26) | 13.73 (2.80) | 11.31 (2.60) | ns. | ns. |
Frequency (Hz) (left) | 379.22 (361.05) | 241.26 (137.93) | 465.58 (697.45) | 1161.76 (1441.87) | ns. | ns. |
Frequency (Hz) (right) | 454.71 (372.69) | 241.66 (137.70) | 491.21 (687.18) | 1170.00 (1435.29) | ns. | ns. |
Motion 1 (slow and drifting) | 122108.01 (54831.82) | 36616.89 (34589.29) | 91659.27 (44101.18) | 32263.66 (31748.91) | ns. | F(1,28) = 23.469*** |
Motion 2 | 53812.82 (22459.94) | 17790.62 (14413.37) | 43402.15 (20830.68) | 15407.53 (14798.81) | ns. | F(1,28) = 24.016*** |
Motion 3 | 38860.56 (15262.57) | 14337.7 (10537.67) | 30810.13 (14032) | 12256.57 (11724.12) | ns. | F(1,28) = 21.884*** |
Motion 4 | 24938.01 (9014.75) | 10388.2 (6386.2) | 18924.16 (8638.21) | 8335.32 (7803.32) | ns. | F(1,28) = 19.629*** |
Motion 5 | 15024.27 (4520.56) | 6986.16 (3533.85) | 12377.26 (5407.69) | 5579.42 (4900.99) | ns. | F(1,28) = 20.431*** |
Motion 6 | 7305.06 (2337.75) | 3863.75 (1697.84) | 6830.85 (3381.41) | 2720.06 (2723.28) | ns. | F(1,28) = 16.776*** |
Motion 7 (fast and transient) | 2597.47 (846.05) | 1308.79 (695.17) | 2668.86 (2197.91) | 989.22 (911.23) | ns. | F(1,28) = 10.275** |
Note:
** p < .01
*** p < .001.
Hue, saturation, and value (brightness) were measured on 0 to 1 HSV scale; motion features were measured by the number of pixels of differences between frames.
Auditory components of the stimuli were normalized to the same mean amplitude by setting the values for each sound file extracted from the original video stimuli to the overall mean computed across all sound files using MATLAB (R2010b, MathWorks, Inc.). To keep the audiovisual stimuli naturalistic, no attempt was made to equate the stimuli on any other lower level features. The differences in lower level visual (hue, saturation, brightness, and motion) and four acoustical (bilateral frequency and amplitude) features across the affective categories were examined. Hue, saturation, and brightness of visual features were computed for each video by averaging the means of each frame of the video using the MATLAB rgb2hsv function. No significant differences in mean hue, saturation, or brightness values were found across valence or arousal conditions, ps >.05 (Table 1). Estimation of total motion for each video stimulus was based on absolute differences of pixels between frames at several time rates from slower or drifting motions to fast transient motions, without respect to the direction of motion that causes differences. Seven motion parameters were estimated at several time differences from slow drifting motions (e.g., walking) to fast transient motions (e.g., running). ANOVAs conducted on the total motion parameter for the 32 stimuli indicated that positive and negative videos differed in the total motion, F(7, 22) = 2.95, p =. 024, ηp2 = .484, and high and low arousal videos differed in the total motion, F(7, 22) = 4.474, p = .003, ηp2 = .587. Negative videos had greater total motion than positive videos and high arousal videos had greater total motion than low arousal videos. Separate ANOVAs conducted on each constituent motion parameter failed to find any significant effects of valence (p > .05), although all seven indicated significant effects of arousal (ps < .001). Finally, frequency and amplitude for left and right channels were measured with Frequency Analysis and Amplitude Statistics functions of Adobe Audition CS6. The four experimental sets did not differ from each other in mean frequency or amplitude, ps > .05.
2.3 fMRI acquisition
MRI data were acquired on a Siemens Magnetom Trio 3.0T whole-body scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) at the McCausland Center for Brain Imaging at the University of South Carolina. The functional images were acquired using a single-shot echo-planar imaging pulse sequence (TR = 2200ms, TE = 35ms, 90° flip angle) with a 12-channel head coil. Thirty-six 3 mm thick oblique-axial slices were imaged in interleaved scanning order with no gap. The acquisition matrix was 64×64 with 3×3×3 mm voxels. Functional data was acquired using a slow event-related design in two scanning sessions (two runs for each session). High-resolution whole-brain anatomical images were acquired using a standard T1-weighted 3D MP-RAGE protocol (TR = 2250 ms, TE = 4.18 ms, FOV = 256 mm, flip angle = 9°, voxel size = 1×1×1 mm) to facilitate normalization of the functional data.
2.4 Functional Localizer
Areas responsive to naturalistic audiovisual presentation were identified in a separate localizer session for each participant. There were four experimental conditions: baseline, auditory (beep), dynamic visual (checkerboard), and a combined naturalistic audiovisual condition (audiovisual). Additionally, the functional localizer contained two other conditions that were not part of the present experiment. The conditions were presented in a block design with each block lasting 12s (Fig 2A). Baseline condition consisted of a black screen with a white fixation cross shown in the center of the screen and background noise. During an auditory condition, designed to localize primary auditory cortex, a binaural sine tone of 1,000 Hz pulsating at 6 Hz was presented [51]. During a dynamic visual condition, designed to localize primary visual areas, there were 12s of high-contrast flickering checkerboard reversals, 200ms per cycle [52]. During a naturalistic audiovisual condition, four 3s audiovisual clips sampled from the four affective quadrants were played back to back. By including a wide range of affective conditions the localizer is not sensitive to facets of the stimuli that discriminate between affective conditions. There were eight runs in the localizer scan and each run consisted of six conditions presented in a pseudo randomized order such that the same condition was not presented twice in a row for the two subsequent runs. There were a total of 48 blocks resulting in 273 acquired volumes. All stimuli were different from those presented in the main experiment. Audio-visual clips used for the localizer scan were not repeated within the localizer scan.
2.5 Experimental paradigm
Functional magnetic resonance imaging was used to measure brain activity while participants were presented with affect-eliciting audiovisual clips. All stimuli were presented using E-prime software (Psychology Software Tools, Sharpsburg, PA). Audiovisual stimuli were presented in 320 × 240 pixels resolution in 32-bit color at a rate of 25 frames per second onto a 640 × 480 resolution screen. Sound was delivered via Serene Sound Audio System (Resonance Technology Inc, Northridge, CA). Each clip was presented for 5s, followed by a white fixation cross shown on black background for 7s (Fig 2B). Participants were instructed to focus on the fixation cross in the center of the screen throughout the experiment. For each participant, 128 trials were presented in 4 blocks of the 32 unique exemplars. Within each block, the 32 exemplars were randomly presented with the restriction that no stimuli from the same affective condition were presented twice in a row and that each affective state was presented within each successive block of four trials.
2.6 fMRI data processing and analysis
Data processing and statistical analyses were performed in MATLAB environment using standard procedures in Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM 8, Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK). The data were corrected for motion and linear trend. Structural data were segmented into white and gray matter to facilitate the normalization. Functional and anatomical images were co-registered and spatially normalized into the standard Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space based on T1-derived normalization parameters.
Functional localizer: For each participant we have identified voxels that were more responsive to audiovisual condition compared to baseline (VA) (p < .05, FWE-corrected, cluster size > 5), but excluding those voxels that were more responsive to checkerboard condition compared to baseline (VP) (p < .05, FWE-corrected, cluster size > 5) and those voxels that were more responsive to beep condition compared to baseline (AP) (p < .05, FWE-corrected, cluster size > 5).
Main experiment: To improve signal-to-noise ratio, the time-series data for each voxel were fit using GLMdenoise [53], a technique successfully used in MVPA and other applications [54, 55]. GLMdenoise used the four affective categories to estimate regressors of no interest. Notably the procedure was blind to valence and arousal categories, and thus did not bias the results when comparing across combined categories (i.e., positive versus negative valence or high versus low arousal). Furthermore, to control for possible confounds arising from lower level features of the stimuli, five lower level feature components were regressed out as covariates of no interest together with six head motion parameter estimates. We used principal components to reduce the number of regressors, while keeping most of the variability in the data. The five lower level feature components were generated from three separate principal components analyses: two scores from three visual features which captured 81.91% of the variance for the visual features, one score from seven motion features (88.37%), and two scores from four auditory features (99.03%). The residuals from this analysis were used for all further analyses. The percent signal change (PSC) relative to the average activity in a voxel was computed for each voxel in every volume from the residuals. The mean PSC of two volumes, offset 4.4s from the stimulus onset (to account for the delay in hemodynamic response), was used as the input for further analyses [7]. Data for each condition were standardized across voxels to have zero mean and unit variance [56].
2.7 Multivariate Pattern Analyses
The MVPA methods employed in this work are similar to those that have been successfully used in our other exploration of affective space [7, 57]. Logistic regression classifiers [58] were trained to identify valence and arousal patterns of brain activity associated with affect-eliciting audiovisual stimuli. Two-way classifications were performed to identify valence (positive vs. negative) trials, as well as arousal (high vs. low) trials.
Within-participant classification was performed within functionally localized gray matter voxels that selectively responded to naturalistic audiovisual stimuli. In each of the four cross-validation folds, one presentation of the 32 exemplars was left out as test data when the classifiers were trained on the other three presentations. Prior to classification, trials were divided into training and test sets. The classifier was constructed from the training set and applied subsequently to the unused test set. Classification accuracies were computed based on the average classification accuracy across the four cross-validation folds. Furthermore, to test the generalizability of the affect representation across stimuli, we trained a valence classifier and an arousal classifier on 31 exemplars, and tested on the left out exemplar by decoding its valence or arousal. Each of the 32 exemplars was left out once for testing in a cross-validation fold. The average classification accuracy for the four presentations of the test exemplars was reported.
Cross-participant classification performance was evaluated with eleven-fold cross-validation, where data from one of the participants was left out for testing in each fold. The classifier was trained on data from all but one participant and used to make predictions for individual trial data from the left-out participant. Average classification accuracy across trials was computed as a measure of how well individual affective states can be identified based on data from other individuals. This procedure was repeated for all participants. Classifications were performed using all voxels as well as the union of functional localizer masks.
Statistical significance for the classification accuracies was evaluated by comparison to an empirically derived null distribution constructed by 1,000 non-informative permutations of labels in the training set. Classification accuracies with p-values smaller than .05 were considered significant.
2.8 Searchlight analyses
Searchlight analyses [59] were performed to localize regions that were sensitive to valence and arousal information. Searchlight analyses employ a sliding neighborhood with a predefined search radius to scan an entire volume. A union of areas sensitive to checkerboard and beep sounds (compared to baseline) were excluded from the subsequent searchlight analyses for each participant.
For each participant and each voxel, data from a 5×5×5 voxels neighborhood, centered at a given voxel, was extracted and used for the same MVPA procedure described above. The average classification accuracy was assigned to the center voxel. Two-way classifications for valence (positive vs. negative) and arousal (high vs. low) were performed across the gray matter voxels in the mask described above. Thus two classification accuracy maps were generated for each individual. Chance-level accuracy (.5) was subtracted from obtained classification accuracy maps [60] before they were submitted to a random-effects whole-brain group analyses.
Permutation tests were performed to find the empirically significant cluster sizes [61]. For each run, the same searchlight procedure was conducted as described above, but with a random permutation of condition labels for each individual. The individual accuracy maps generated using a random permutation of condition labels were submitted to a group analysis and the largest cluster size was recorded. This entire procedure was repeated 1,000 times each for valence and arousal classification (which are equivalent), yielding null distributions of cluster sizes.
Finally, additional confirmatory analyses were performed to verify that the identified clusters were sensitive to valence and arousal information [62]. The significant clusters found by searchlight analyses were based on group analysis, so that not all of the voxels within each searchlight cluster may be informative and represent affective states at the individual level. Thus an additional feature selection procedure was performed for each individual to exclude voxels that were not related to affective states. In the first confirmatory analysis, a within-individual classification analysis was performed for each cluster identified by the searchlight analysis. Prior to classification, a measure of stability was computed for all voxels within each searchlight by computing correlations across three folds in the training set. The top 80% of the most stable voxels from the training set were used for subsequent classification for each fold. Classification accuracies across the four cross-validation folds were averaged for each participant. Significance testing was conducted with a one-sample t-test to evaluate if the group mean accuracy was significantly above chance (.5). In the second confirmatory analysis, lower dimensional representation analyses were performed for each cluster identified by the searchlight analyses. These analyses used the top 80% of stable voxels. STATIS [63], a generalization of principal components analysis for multiple similarity matrices, was conducted to visualize the underlying structure of the exemplars within each searchlight cluster. This technique is based on the cross-product matrix, thus allowing the number of voxels in the analysis to vary across individuals [64]. Point-biserial correlations (rpb) between design values (1 for positive or high arousal, and -1 for negative or low arousal) and corresponding coordinates from the STATIS solutions were computed to evaluate if the solutions were indeed sensitive to valence information from valence clusters and arousal information from arousal clusters.
3.0 Results
3.1 Functional localizer
For each participant we have identified functional masks for voxels that were more responsive to audiovisual condition compared to baseline but excluding those voxels that were more responsive to checkerboard condition compared to baseline and those voxels that were more responsive to beep (VA∩(VP⋃AP)c). The number of voxels in each mask identified for each participant is shown in Table 2. For a concise summary, the functional localizer results are presented at a group level in S2 Table. Please note, individual masks (S1 Fig) were used for subsequent MVPA analyses; group localizer results are presented to facilitate comparison to other studies.
Table 2. Number of voxels for each of the masks reported by participant.
Participant | Gray matter | VA | VP | AP | VA∩(VP⋃AP)c | GM∩(VA∩(VP⋃AP)c) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | 12243 | 15554 | 7475 | 577 | 8864 | 2551 |
2 | 20279 | 11723 | 4006 | 682 | 8066 | 3086 |
3 | 24024 | 12255 | 15253 | 1286 | 4031 | 2040 |
4 | 12544 | 13294 | 6664 | 5193 | 6590 | 1261 |
5 | 16064 | 8926 | 3028 | 1008 | 5855 | 1977 |
6 | 15931 | 11947 | 10337 | 3298 | 3195 | 1031 |
7 | 17528 | 5221 | 1784 | 185 | 3449 | 1207 |
8 | 15766 | 8078 | 3372 | 538 | 5039 | 1351 |
9 | 21940 | 7558 | 1949 | 2048 | 5032 | 2142 |
10 | 8689 | 9935 | 3504 | 433 | 6810 | 1101 |
11 | 20825 | 13872 | 9461 | 1202 | 6674 | 2951 |
VA, voxels that were more responsive to audiovisual condition compared to baseline (p < .05, FWE-corrected, cluster size > 5); VP, voxels that were more responsive to checkerboard condition compared to baseline (p < .05, FWE-corrected, cluster size > 5); AP, voxels that were more responsive to beep condition compared to baseline (p < .05, FWE -corrected, cluster size > 5); VA∩(VP⋃AP)c: voxels that were more responsive to audiovisual condition compared to baseline (p < .05, FWE-corrected, cluster size > 5), but excluding those voxels that were more responsive to checkerboard condition compared to baseline (p < .05, FWE-corrected, cluster size > 5) and those voxels that were more responsive to beep condition compared to baseline (p < .05, FWE -corrected, cluster size > 5); GM∩(VA∩(VP⋃AP)c), the intersection between individual gray matter mask and VA∩(VP⋃AP)c. Participants are ordered by within-participant classification performance (see below).
3.2 Identification of valence
We examined whether the valence of the stimuli (positive or negative) could be identified for individual trials based on activity patterns elicited by viewing audiovisual clips, defined by the quadrants of the valence-arousal space. First, classifiers were trained for each participant to identify the valence category of the stimuli they were watching: positive or negative. Significant classification accuracies (p < .05) were found for ten of 11 participants (Fig 3A). Classification accuracies for eleven participants ranged from .59 to .80, with the mean of the accuracies (M = 0.66, SD = 0.06) significantly greater than chance, t(10) = 8.17, p < .001. Moreover, the valence states of audiovisual clips that were previously unseen by the classifier were identified with accuracy reliably above chance for nine out of 11 participants (cross-exemplar decoding, M = .74, SD = .13, range = (0.56, 0.94)), suggesting that it was the neural representation of valence, rather than individual stimuli-specific properties that drove the decoding accuracy.
To examine the similarity of valence representation across participants and to show the typicality of the decoding results we have used data from all but one participant to train a classification model and predict valence in the left out participant at the whole brain level of analysis. From all voxels, we were able to identify valence in nine of 11 participants with accuracies above chance, p < .05 (Fig 3A). Classification accuracies ranged from 0.48 to 0.69, with the mean of the accuracies (M = .61, SD = .06) significantly greater than chance, t(10) = 6.34, p < .001. To link these results more directly to the results for decoding within individuals, we restricted the analyses to the union of functional localizer masks. Under this restriction, we were able to identify valence in six out of 11 participants with accuracies above chance, p < .05. Classification accuracies ranged from .51 to .65, with the mean of the accuracies (M = .60, SD = .05) significantly greater than chance, t(10) = 7.05, p < .001.
Having established the typicality of the decoding results with cross-participant identification of valence, searchlight analyses were performed to spatially localize brain regions that were sensitive to valence. These revealed five clusters: the left medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), the right posterior part of the cingulate cortex (PCC), the left superior/middle temporal gyrus (STG/MTG), the thalamus, and the middle frontal gyrus (MFG) (p < .05, cluster size > 43).
Confirmatory MVPA and STATIS analyses were performed within each cluster to verify the information content of identified clusters. The results of MVPA showed four out of five valence clusters (with mean accuracies of .60, .57, .56, and .54) significantly discriminated valence information, ps < .05 (Fig 3B; Table 3; S2 Fig). The classification accuracy from the thalamus cluster did not reach significance (M = .53, p = .14). The lower dimensional representation of 32 exemplars from the four valence clusters confirmed that each of the clusters identified by searchlight was informative of valence. The point-biserial correlations between design values and component values corresponding to valence for the four regions were .41, .46, .47, and .55, ps < .05 (also see S3 Fig). In sum, four clusters sensitive to valence (PCC, MFG, STG/MTG, and mPFC) were identified by searchlight analyses (Fig 3B, Table 3).
Table 3. Searchlight results for valence and arousal.
MNI coordinates | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Anatomical region | Hemisphere | Cluster size | x | y | z | T | Z |
Valence | |||||||
PCC | R | 109 | 6 | -46 | 31 | 8.89 | 4.58 |
MFG | R | 87 | 30 | 11 | 52 | 7.57 | 4.28 |
STG/MTG | L | 46 | -63 | -58 | 10 | 8.23 | 4.44 |
mPFC | L | 44 | -15 | 56 | 1 | 9.25 | 4.66 |
Arousal | |||||||
PC | R | 56 | 9 | -49 | 52 | 6.67 | 4.03 |
OFC | R | 54 | 27 | 62 | -17 | 7.47 | 4.25 |
Note: p < .001, uncorrected, cluster size > 40 for valence and 50 for arousal. R, right; L, left; cluster size reported in voxels; T indicates peak t values; Z indicates peak z values; OFC: anterior part of orbitofrontal cortex; PC: precuneus; mPFC: medial prefrontal cortex; PCC: posterior part of the cingulate cortex; STG/MTG: superior/middle temporal gyrus; MFG: middle frontal gyrus.
3.3 Identification of arousal
We examined whether the arousal of the stimuli (low or high) could be identified for individual trials based on activity patterns elicited by viewing audiovisual clips, defined by the quadrants of the valence-arousal space. Classifiers were trained for each participant to identify the arousal category of the audiovisual stimuli: high or low. Significant classification accuracies (p < .05) were found for eight of 11 participants (Fig 4A). Classification accuracies based on functionally defined ROI for eleven participants ranged from 0.52 to 0.70, with the mean of the accuracies (M = .60, SD = .06) significantly greater than chance, t(10) = 6.00, p < .001. The arousal states elicited by audiovisual clips that were previously unseen by the classifier were identified with reliably above-chance accuracy for nine out of 11 participants (M = .66, SD = .09, range = (0.50, 0.81)), suggesting that it was the neural representation of arousal, rather than individual stimuli-specific properties that drove the decoding accuracy.
To examine the similarity of arousal representation across participants and show the typicality of the decoding results we used data from all but one participant to train a classification model and predict arousal, high or low, in the left out participant. From all voxels, we were able to identify arousal in eight of 11 participants with accuracies above chance. Classification accuracies ranged from .48 to .69, with the mean of the accuracies (M = .61, SD = .05) significantly greater than chance, t(10) = 6.37, p < .001. To link these results more directly to the results for decoding within individuals, we restricted the analyses to the union of functional localizer masks. Under this restriction, we were able to identify arousal in six out of 11 participants with accuracies above chance, p < .05. Classification accuracies ranged from .43 to .64, with the mean of the accuracies (M = .58, SD = .06) significantly greater than chance, t(10) = 4.29, p < .01.
Having established the typicality of the decoding results with cross-participant identification of arousal, searchlight analyses were performed to spatially localize brain regions that were sensitive to arousal. Searchlight analyses revealed two significant clusters: the right precuneus (PC) and the right orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) (p < .05, cluster size > 50). Confirmatory MVPA and STATIS analyses were performed within each cluster to verify the information content of identified clusters. MVPA within each of the identified arousal clusters confirmed that mean classification accuracies (M = .56 and M = .57) from each cluster were significantly higher than chance, ps < .01 (Fig 4B; Table 3; S2 Fig). The lower dimensional representation confirmed that each of the clusters identified by searchlight was sensitive to arousal (S3 Fig). The point-biserial correlations between design values and component values corresponding to arousal for the two regions were indicative of the predicted relationship (OFC: rpb = .33, p = .07, PC: rpb = .59, p < .001). In sum, two regions sensitive to arousal (OFC and PC) were identified by the searchlight analyses (Fig 4B, Table 3).
4.0 Discussion
This study investigated how valence and arousal elicited by naturalistic multimodal stimuli are represented in the brain. We were able to identify valence and arousal for individual trials based on activity patterns elicited by the audiovisual stimuli for most of the individuals. This result was achieved when controlling for lower level features of the stimuli by excluding brain regions associated with lower level perceptual processing and removing the effects of lower level stimulus parameters statistically. By doing so we bolster the inference that identification of affective states was indeed attributable to valence and arousal. The fact that the neural representation of affect was conveyed by brain areas selectively responsive to naturalistic dynamic multisensory information is consistent with the idea that valence and arousal are two fundamental properties that are readily accessed when processing dynamic multimodal stimuli.
In addition to classifying valence and arousal within individuals, we also found that the affective states of an individual could be reliably modeled by training the classifiers on data from other individuals. This cross-participant classification implies common areas of representation of affective states across individuals. As in the case of within-participant classification, the cross-participant classification was based on voxel activity that controlled for similarity of lower level perceptual features across stimuli, supporting the conclusion that these activation patterns reflect the core dimensions of affect and not just perceptual regularities within affective categories. We used searchlight techniques to visualize spatially localized regions containing valence or arousal information. The areas identified by searchlight as sensitive to valence (PCC, MFG, STG, MTG, mPFC) and arousal (PC and OFC) information were consistent with the areas previously implicated in emotion processing.
The regions found to be involved in valence in the current study were PCC, MFG, STG/MTG, and mPFC. It has been reported that the PCC is activated in emotional word processing [65–67], representation of associated emotions [60], and modality-general representation of complex emotional information [68]. The STG and MTG have been implicated in integration of multimodal affective information [28, 69–72], and more generally, in integration of multimodal (mostly visual and auditory) sensory information [73]. However, it is not clear whether valence, arousal or both are represented in this region, because most of the aforementioned studies utilized discrete emotions (e.g., happy, disgusted, or angry). While the current study provides support for valence related processing in these regions, more work is needed to determine if arousal is represented in these regions. The mPFC is often found to be involved in processing of emotion [74]. It has been implicated in the perception of affect in faces and scenes [75]. The mPFC has been shown to be related to consistent representation of discrete emotional states from face, voice, and body movement, suggesting modality-general processing of emotion within the mPFC [70]. The current study found the mPFC is sensitive to valence, suggesting that valence might be driving this distinction.
The regions found to be involved in arousal in the current study were PC and OFC. The OFC has been reported to be engaged in processing of affective word stimuli [76], odors [77] and tastes [78]. It has been identified as representing valence for both pictures and tastes, i.e., a modality-general representation of affect [78]. In our study, the OFC was identified to be sensitive to arousal information, which is another dimension of core affect (the cluster size within OFC for valence fell at the top 7.4% within the null distribution, which was not statistically significant). These findings, taken together, implicate the OFC in general affective processing.
In sum, the regions identified as sensitive to valence and arousal information in the current study are consistent with the regions identified in literature linked to emotion. Moreover, many of six regions have been linked to modality-general representations of emotion [68, 70, 78]. Our findings are consistent with the idea that the core affect dimensions of valence and arousal may underlie the processing of emotions in these regions for multimodal affective stimuli. The clusters that we have shown to be sensitive to valence and arousal in this study are consistent with the idea that these areas may contribute to the brain’s affective workspace [79].
Past research has demonstrated that modality-congruent sensory areas are also involved in affect processing. For example, patterns of activity in voice-sensitive cortices were found to represent categorical emotional vocal expressions [80], while activity patterns in fusiform face area represent facial expressions [81]. Similarly, Lang, Bradley [82] presented emotion-inducing pictures and found a greater functional activity for emotional pictures than for neutral pictures in primary visual regions, including occipital gyrus, fusiform gyrus, and lingual gyrus. Both valence and arousal have been demonstrated to be represented in the visual cortex [83]. Thus, successful classifications of valence and arousal are expected in the modality-congruent regions. We excluded sensory regions that were localized functionally to minimize the effect of the lower level features and thereby enhance interpretation of our classification in terms of affective features rather than sensory features.
In developing a stimulus set that independently manipulated valence and arousal, we chose to exclude the neutral valence condition. This is because neutral valence conditions are difficult to match on arousal levels with corresponding positive and negative valence conditions. Though two core affect dimensions, valence and arousal, are assumed to be independent, unsigned valence dimension (positive/negative vs. neutral) may relate to arousal. For example, affective stimuli data sets such as the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) and International Affective Digitized Sounds (IADS) databases [84, 85] show distributions of U-shaped pattern, indicating valenced stimuli tend to be more arousing compared to neutral stimuli. Thus, it is difficult to find stimuli that are neutral with moderate or high arousal level and it is more likely for neutral stimuli to be associated with less arousal level compared to valenced stimuli. For example, [86] found brain regions that activated as quadratic function of valence raising a possibility that these results might be confounded with arousal dimension. Consistent with this idea, Viinikainen, Kätsyri [87] reported extreme positive and negative valence stimuli were associated with high arousal. The relationship between valence and arousal is still open for the further investigation.
The results of our study have important implications for affective processing in clinical populations. First, we used a passive viewing paradigm to successfully decode valence and arousal. This type of task may be more amenable to clinical populations, as it has been argued that implicit tasks may reduce task demands. Explicit tasks, such as emotion categorization or matching of emotional stimuli, can be problematic in clinical populations that may suffer from executive dysfunction [15]. Second, the present study extends previous work by demonstrating that the affective dimensions of valence and arousal can be identified from dynamic audio-visual stimuli. Given the distinction between processing of affect from static and dynamic stimuli for some clinical populations noted earlier [16], these representational analyses may be helpful in better understanding affective disorders. Third, the cross-participant classification utilized in the current study provides a basis for future classification studies of clinical populations based on a match to specific profiles for affective representation [13].
Several design facets limit the generalizability of the findings. First, we tried to make the stimuli as homogeneous as possible for the duration of the clip with an assumption of constant affective response to a single stimulus, but the methods in the current study have not been demonstrated for more transient nature of affective states in truly naturalistic settings. Future research may examine applications to longer and more variable stimuli. Second, this study is based on categorical affective states rather than continuous ones due to the difficulty of collecting neutral stimuli with varying arousal levels. As discussed above, it is challenging to manipulate neutral condition with moderate and high arousal levels. Finally, our results are correlational in nature; although we have shown that BOLD data contain enough information to identify the affective category of the stimuli, no inference can be made on how the brain uses this information.
In conclusion, we were able to identify valence and arousal states in individuals for single trials of multimodal dynamic stimuli while controlling for lower level features statistically and with functional localizers. We were able to do this both within and across individuals. The spatially localized areas found to be sensitive to valence and arousal information were consistent with the literature on affective states. These findings extend previous results on affective representation [7, 13] to naturalistic dynamic stimuli and identify possible brain regions for encoding valence and arousal representations.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
We thank Laura B. Baucom and Matthew J. Facciani for help with fMRI data collection, Mary-Catherine Newell for help with stimuli development, and all participants for their time.
Data Availability
fMRI data are available via OpenfMRI database (ds000205). Behavioral data are summarized in S1 Table.
Funding Statement
This work was funded in part by the Social Sciences Grant Program, Office of the Provost, University of South Carolina, awarded to SVS and DHW. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Russell JA. Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychol Rev. 2003;110(1):145–72. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Russell JA, Barrett LF. Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: dissecting the elephant. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1999;76(5):805–19. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Bradley MM, Lang PJ. Affective reactions to acoustic stimuli. Psychophysiology. 2000;37:204–15. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Cacioppo JT, Berntson GG, Larsen JT, Poehlmann KM, Ito TA. The psychophysiology of emotion. In: Lewis R, Haviland-Jones JM, editors. The handbook of emotion. 2nd ed2000. p. 173–91.
- 5.Gomez P, Danuser B. Affective and physiological responses to environmental noises and music. Int J Psychophysiol. 2004;53(2):91–103. Epub 2004/06/24. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2004.02.002 S0167876004000273 [pii]. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Anders S, Eippert F, Weiskopf N, Veit R. The human amygdala is sensitive to the valence of pictures and sounds irrespective of arousal: an fMRI study. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2008;3(3):233–43. Epub 2008/11/19. nsn017 [pii] 10.1093/scan/nsn017 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Baucom LB, Wedell DH, Wang J, Blitzer DN, Shinkareva SV. Decoding the neural representation of affective states. NeuroImage. 2012;59(1):718–27. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Wilson-Mendenhall CD, Barrett LF, Barsalou LW. Neural evidence that human emotions share core affective properties. Psychol Sci. 2013;24(6):947–56. Epub 2013/04/23. 0956797612464242 [pii] 10.1177/0956797612464242 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Tseng A, Wang Z, Huo Y, Goh S, Russell JA, Peterson BS. Differences in neural activity when processing emotional arousal and valence in autism spectrum disorders. Human brain mapping. 2016;37(2):443–61. 10.1002/hbm.23041 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Mourão-Miranda J, Almeida JR, Hassel S, de Oliveira L, Versace A, Marquand AF, et al. Pattern recognition analyses of brain activation elicited by happy and neutral faces in unipolar and bipolar depression. Bipolar Disord. 2012;14(4):451–60. Epub 2012/05/29. 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2012.01019.x . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Mourão-Miranda J, Oliveira L, Ladouceur CD, Marquand A, Brammer M, Birmaher B, et al. Pattern recognition and functional neuroimaging Help to discriminate healthy adolescents at risk for mood disorders from low risk adolescents. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(2):e29482 10.1371/journal.pone.0029482 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Hahn T, Marquand A, Ehlis A-C, Dresler T, Kittel-Schneider S, Jarczok TA, et al. Integrating neurobiological markers of depression. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2011;68(4). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Habes I, Krall SC, Johnston SJ, Yuen KS, Healy D, Goebel R, et al. Pattern classification of valence in depression. Neuroimage Clin. 2013;2:675–83. Epub 2013/11/02. 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.05.001 S2213-1582(13)00057-0 [pii]. . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Klasen M, Kreifelts B, Chen Y-H, Seubert J, Mathiak K. Neural processing of emotion in multimodal settings. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2014;8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Garrido-Vasquez P, Jessen S, Kotz SA. Perception of emotion in psychiatric disorders: on the possible role of task, dynamics, and multimodality. Soc Neurosci. 2011;6(5–6):515–36. Epub 2011/10/04. 10.1080/17470919.2011.620771 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Weisberg J, Milleville SC, Kenworthy L, Wallace GL, Gotts SJ, Beauchamp MS, et al. Social Perception in Autism Spectrum Disorders: Impaired Category Selectivity for Dynamic but not Static Images in Ventral Temporal Cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2012. Epub 2012/09/29. bhs276 [pii] 10.1093/cercor/bhs276 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.De Jong J, Hodiamont P, De Gelder B. Modality-specific attention and multisensory integration of emotions in schizophrenia: Reduced regulatory effects. Schizophrenia research. 2010;122(1):136–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.de Gelder B, Vroomen J, de Jong SJ, Masthoff ED, Trompenaars FJ, Hodiamont P. Multisensory integration of emotional faces and voices in schizophrenics. Schizophrenia research. 2005;72(2):195–203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Barrett LF, Mesquita B, Gendron M. Context in Emotion Perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2011;20(5):286–90. 10.1177/0963721411422522 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Hasson U, Malach R, Heeger DJ. Reliability of cortical activity during natural stimulation. Trends Cogn Sci. 2010;14(1):40–8. 10.1016/j.tics.2009.10.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Maurage P, Campanella S. Experimental and clinical usefulness of crossmodal paradigms in psychiatry: an illustration from emotional processing in alcohol-dependence. 2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 22.Kragel PA, LaBar KS. Multivariate neural biomarkers of emotional states are categorically distinct. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2015;10(11):1437–48. 10.1093/scan/nsv032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Satpute AB, Wilson-Mendelhall CD, Kleckner IR, Barrett LF. Emotional experience. Brain mapping: An encyclopedic reference. 2015;3:65–72. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Kotz SA, Kalberlah C, Bahlmann J, Friederici AD, Haynes JD. Predicting vocal emotion expressions from the human brain. Human Brain Mapping. 2013;34(8):1971–81. 10.1002/hbm.22041 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Shinkareva SV, Wang J, Kim J, Facciani MJ, Baucom LB, Wedell DH. Representations of modality‐specific affective processing for visual and auditory stimuli derived from functional magnetic resonance imaging data. Human brain mapping. 2014;35(7):3558–68. 10.1002/hbm.22421 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Royet JP, Plailly J, Delon-Martin C, Kareken DA, Segebarth C. fMRI of emotional responses to odors: influence of hedonic valence and judgment, handedness, and gender. Neuroimage. 2003;20(2):713–28. 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00388-4 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Daselaar SM, Rice HJ, Greenberg DL, Cabeza R, LaBar KS, Rubin DC. The spatiotemporal dynamics of autobiographical memory: neural correlates of recall, emotional intensity, and reliving. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18(1):217–29. 10.1093/cercor/bhm048 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kreifelts B, Ethofer T, Grodd W, Erb M, Wildgruber D. Audiovisual integration of emotional signals in voice and face: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2007;37(4):1445–56. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.06.020 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Zupan B, Neumann D. Affect recognition in traumatic brain injury: responses to unimodal and multimodal media. J Head Trauma Rehabil. 2014;29(4):E1–E12. 10.1097/HTR.0b013e31829dded6 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Brosnan M, Johnson H, Grawmeyer B, Chapman E, Benton L. Emotion recognition in animated compared to human stimuli in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45(6):1785–96. 10.1007/s10803-014-2338-9 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Magnee MJ, Oranje B, van Engeland H, Kahn RS, Kemner C. Cross-sensory gating in schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorder: EEG evidence for impaired brain connectivity? Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(7):1728–32. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.012 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Gerrards-Hesse A, Spies K, Hesse FW. Experimental inductions of emotional states and their effectiveness: a review. British journal of psychology. 1994;85:55–78. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Ambadar Z, Schooler JW, Cohn JF. Deciphering the enigmatic face: the importance of facial dynamics in interpreting subtle facial expressions. Psychol Sci. 2005;16(5):403–10. 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01548.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Sato W, Kochiyama T, Yoshikawa S, Naito E, Matsumura M. Enhanced neural activity in response to dynamic facial expressions of emotion: an fMRI study. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2004;20(1):81–91. 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.01.008 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.de Gelder B, Vroomen J, Pourtois G, Weiskrantz L. Non-conscious recognition of affect in the absence of striate cortex. Neuroreport. 1999;10(18):3759–63. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Kilts CD, Egan GFG, D.A., Ely TD, Hoffman JM. Dissociable neural pathways are involved in the recognition of emotion in static and dynamic facial expressions. NeuroImage. 2003;18:156–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Atkinson AP, Dittrich WH, Gemmell AJ, Young AW. Emotion perception from dynamic and static body expressions in point-light and full-light displays. Perception. 2004;33(6):717–46. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Gross JJ, Levenson RW. Emotion elicitation using films. Cognition & Emotion. 1995;9(1):87–108. 10.1080/02699939508408966 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Rottenberg J, Ray RD, Gross JJ. Emotion elicitation using films In: Coan JA, Allen JJB, editors. The handbook of emotion elicitation and assessment. London: Oxford University Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Hubert W, de Jong-Meyer R. Psychophysiological response patterns to positive and negative film stimuli. Biol Psychol. 1990;31(1):73–93. Epub 1991/08/01. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Hubert W, de Jong-Meyer R. Autonomic, neuroendocrine, and subjective responses to emotion-inducing film stimuli. Int J Psychophysiol. 1991;11(2):131–40. Epub 1991/08/01. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.de Gelder B, Bertelson P. Multisensory integration, perception and ecological validity. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences. 2003;7:460–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Bertelson P, de Gelder B. The psychology of multimodal perception In: Spense C, Driver J, editors. Crossmodal space and crossmodal attention. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2004. p. 141–77. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Lakens D, Fockenberg DA, Lemmens KPH, Ham J, Midden CJH. The evaluation of affective pictures depends on their brightness. Cognition and Emotion. 2013. 10.1080/02699931.2013.781501 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Gabrielsson A, Lindström E. The influence of musical structure on emotional expression In: Juslin PN, Sloboda J, editors. Music and emotion: Theory and research Series in affective science. New York: Oxford University Press; 2001. p. 223–48. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Juslin PN, Laukka P. Expression, perception, and induction of musical emotions: A review and a questionnaire study of everyday listening. Journal of New Music Research. 2004;33(3):217–38. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Coutinho E, Cangelosi A. Musical emotions: predicting second-by-second subjective feelings of emotion from low-level psychoacoustic features and physiological measurements. Emotion. 2011;11(4):921–37. 10.1037/a0024700 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Yuen KSL, Johnston SJ, De Martino F, Sorger B, Formisano E, Linden DEJ, et al. Pattern classification predicts individuals’ responses to affective stimuli. Translational Neuroscience. 2012;3(3):278–87. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Carroll JD, Chang JJ. Analysis of individual differences in multidimensional scaling via an N-way generalization of 'Eckard-Young' decomposition. Psychometrika. 1970;35:283–320. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Russell JA, Bullock M. Multidimensional scaling of emotional facial expressions: similarity from preschoolers to adults. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1985;48(5):1290. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Haller S, Wetzel SG, Radue EW, Bilecen D. Mapping continuous neuronal activation without an ON–OFF paradigm: initial results of BOLD ceiling fMRI. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;24:2672–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Fox PT, Raichle ME. Stimulus rate determines regional brain blood flow in striate cortex. Annals of neurology. 1985;17(3):303–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Kay KN, Rokem A, Winawer J, Dougherty RF, Wandell BA. GLMdenoise: a fast, automated technique for denoising task-based fMRI data. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2013;7:Article 247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Erez J, Cusack R, Kendall W, Barense MD. Conjunctive coding of complex object features. Cerebral Cortex. 2015:bhv081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Henriksson L, Khaligh-Razavi S-M, Kay K, Kriegeskorte N. Visual representations are dominated by intrinsic fluctuations correlated between areas. NeuroImage. 2015;114:275–86. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.04.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Pereira F, Mitchell T, Botvinick M. Machine learning classifiers and fMRI: a tutorial overview. Neuroimage. 2009;45:S199–S209. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.11.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Shinkareva SV, Wang J, Kim J, Facciani MJ, Baucom LB, Wedell DH. Representations of modality-specific affective processing for visual and auditory stimuli derived from fMRI data. Human Brain Mapping. 2014;35(7):3558–68. 10.1002/hbm.22421 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Bishop CM. Pattern recognition and machine learning. New York: Springer; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 59.Kriegeskorte N, Goebel R, Bandettini P. Information-based functional brain mapping. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103(10):3863–8. . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Kim J, Schultz J, Rohe T, Wallraven C, Lee SW, Bulthoff HH. Abstract representations of associated emotions in the human brain. J Neurosci. 2015;35(14):5655–63. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4059-14.2015 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Stelzer J, Chen Y, Turner R. Statistical inference and multiple testing correction in classification-based multi-voxel pattern analysis (MVPA): random permutations and cluster size control. Neuroimage. 2013;65:69–82. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.063 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Etzel JA, Zacks JM, Braver TS. Searchlight analysis: promise, pitfalls, and potential. NeuroImage. 2013;78:261–9. Epub 2013/04/06. S1053-8119(13)00291-7 [pii] 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.041 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Abdi H, Williams LJ, Valentin D, Bennani-Dosse M. STATIS and DISTATIS: optimum multitable principal component analysis and three way metric multidimensional scaling. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Computational Statistics. 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 64.Shinkareva SV, Malave VL, Mason RA, Mitchell TM, Just MA. Commonality of neural representations of words and pictures. Neuroimage. 2011;54:2418–25. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Maddock RJ, Buonocore MH. Activation of left posterior cingulate gyrus by the auditory presentation of threat-related words: an fMRI study. Psychiatry Res. 1997;75(1):1–14. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Maddock RJ, Garrett AS, Buonocore MH. Posterior cingulate cortex activation by emotional words: fMRI evidence from a valence decision task. Hum Brain Mapp. 2003;18(1):30–41. 10.1002/hbm.10075 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Fossati P, Hevenor SJ, Graham SJ, Grady C, Keightley ML, Craik F, et al. In search of the emotional self: an fMRI study using positive and negative emotional words. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(11):1938–45. Epub 2003/11/05. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Klasen M, Kenworthy CA, Mathiak KA, Kircher T, Mathiak K. Supramodal representation of emotions. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31(38):13635–43. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2833-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Park JY, Gu BM, Kang DH, Shin YW, Choi CH, Lee JM, et al. Integration of cross-modal emotional information in the human brain: an fMRI study. Cortex. 2010;46(2):161–9. 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.06.008 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Peelen MV, Atkinson AP, Vuilleumier P. Supramodal Representations of Perceived Emotions in the Human Brain. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:10127–34. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2161-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Pourtois G, de Gelder B, Vroomen J, Rossion B, Crommelinck M. The time-course of intermodal binding between seeing and hearing affective information. Neuroreport. 2000;11(6):1329–33. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Robins DL, Hunyadi E, Schultz RT. Superior temporal activation in response to dynamic audio-visual emotional cues. Brain Cogn. 2009;69(2):269–78. 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.08.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Beauchamp MS, Lee KE, Argall BD, Martin A. Integration of auditory and visual information about objects in superior temporal sulcus. Neuron. 2004;41(5):809–23. Epub 2004/03/09. S0896627304000704 [pii]. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Phan KL, Wager T, Taylor SF, Liberzon I. Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. NeuroImage. 2002;16(2):331–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Sabatinelli D, Fortune EE, Li Q, Siddiqui A, Krafft C, Oliver WT, et al. Emotional perception: meta-analyses of face and natural scene processing. NeuroImage. 2011;54(3):2524–33. Epub 2010/10/19. S1053-8119(10)01303-0 [pii] 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.011 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Lewis PA, Critchley HD, Rotshtein P, Dolan RJ. Neural correlates of processing valence and arousal in affective words. Cerebral cortex. 2007;17:742–8. 10.1093/cercor/bhk024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Anderson AK, Christoff K, Stappen I, Panitz D, Ghahremani DG, Glover G, et al. Dissociated neural representations of intensity and valence in human olfaction. Nature Neuroscience. 2003;6(2):196–202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Chikazoe J, Lee DH, Kriegeskorte N, Anderson A. Population coding of affect across stimuli, modalities and individuals. Nature Neuroscience. 2014;17:1114–22. 10.1038/nn.3749 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Lindquist KA, Satpute AB, Wager TD, Weber J, Barrett LF. The Brain Basis of Positive and Negative Affect: Evidence from a Meta-Analysis of the Human Neuroimaging Literature. Cereb Cortex. 2015. 10.1093/cercor/bhv001 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Ethofer T, Van De Ville D, Scherer K, Vuilleumier P. Decoding of emotional information in voice-sensitive cortices. Current Biology. 2009;19(12):1028–33. 10.1016/j.cub.2009.04.054 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Harry B, Williams MA, Davis C, Kim J. Emotional expressions evoke a differential response in the fusiform face area. 2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 82.Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Fitzsimmons JR, Cuthbert BN, Scott JD, Moulder B, et al. Emotional arousal and activation of the visual cortex: an fMRI analysis. Psychophysiology. 1998;35:199–210. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Mourao-Miranda J, Volchan E, Moll J, de Oliveira-Souza R, Oliveira L, Bramati I, et al. Contributions of stimulus valence and arousal to visual activation during emotional perception. Neuroimage. 2003;20(4):1955–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN. International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida, 2008 Contract No.: A-8.
- 85.Bradley M, Lang PJ. The International affective digitized sounds (IADS)[: stimuli, instruction manual and affective ratings: NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention; 1999.
- 86.Viinikainen M, Jääskeläinen IP, Alexandrov Y, Balk MH, Autti T, Sams M. Nonlinear relationship between emotional valence and brain activity: evidence of separate negative and positive valence dimensions. Human brain mapping. 2010;31(7):1030–40. 10.1002/hbm.20915 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Viinikainen M, Kätsyri J, Sams M. Representation of Perceived Sound Valence in the Human Brain. Human Brain Mapping. 2012;33:2295–305. 10.1002/hbm.21362 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
fMRI data are available via OpenfMRI database (ds000205). Behavioral data are summarized in S1 Table.