Abstract
Background: Studies suggest that preterm delivery is a risk factor for early language delays, but knowledge is scarce about the persistence of the delays and whether the association is of a linear kind. To resolve this, effects of confounding risk factors that are both shared within a family and pregnancy specific need to be distinguished from effects of preterm delivery. Our study examines the association between early gestational age and language outcomes, using a sibling-control design.
Methods: The sample comprises 22 499 siblings from the Norwegian Mother and Child Birth Cohort Study, recruited between 1999 and 2008. Mothers rated child language comprehension and production at 18 and 36 months. Analyses compared siblings discordant on gestational age group (early preterm, delivery at week 22–33; late preterm, 34–36; early term, 37–38; full term, >38) and type of onset of delivery (spontaneous; provider-initiated), and compared these findings with conventional cohort analyses.
Results: The findings revealed inverse linear relations between the gestational age groups, and persistent but diminishing language delays. Effects of preterm delivery were substantial on both language production and comprehension at 18 months. By 36 months, the effects of preterm delivery were weaker, but still extensive, in particular for language production in provider-initiated births. When comparing sibling-control with cohort analyses, preterm group was less important among spontaneous births, but remained important in provider-initiated births. Familial and pregnancy risk factors partly explained this.
Conclusions: Distinctive factors seem to underlie effects of preterm delivery across spontaneous and provider-initiated births.
Keywords: Preterm delivery, gestational age, spontaneous labour, induced labour, provider-initiated, language delay, sibling design, MoBa cohort study
Key Messages
Preterm birth is associated with delayed language at 18 and 36 months of age, with the delays tapering off at 36 months.
Lower gestational age combined with provider-initiated delivery was associated with the most severe and persistent language delays.
Familial risk factors could not account for the effect of preterm birth on the language outcomes in children from provider-initiated delivery.
The results suggest that different factors might underlie language delays associated with provider-initiated delivery, and future studies could identify what these factors might be.
Introduction
Preterm delivery (i.e. birth occurring before gestational week 37) is a major global health problem, and early preterm delivery (gestational weeks 22–33) is particularly associated with neonatal mortality and short- and long-term morbidity. 1,2 The number of preterm deliveries has increased. 3 This is mostly driven by greater numbers of late preterm deliveries (gestational weeks 34–36), related to increases in obstetric interventions. 4,5 In addition, an increasing number of deliveries now occur early term (gestational weeks 37–38). 6,7
Because preterm delivery poses a risk for early developmental delays, the increasing numbers of births before term are bound to have a huge impact on public health. 8 Although most children with language delays are born full term, 9 this adverse developmental outcome is strongly associated with preterm deliveries. 10–13 Moreover, both children born at term with language delays and preterm born children show risks for later problems with socio-emotional functioning, reading and writing, 13–16 making it vital to understand and identify language delay trajectories related to different gestational ages.
Because children born early term display a risk for language delays too, although to a lesser extent than for preterm deliveries, 7,8,16–19 an inverse linear relation between gestational age and the severity of the language delays might be assumed. Younger gestational age could also be related to delays in both language comprehension and production which, in children with specific language impairments, involves more impairing and severe delays. 20–23
However, examining associations between gestational length and language delay poses methodological challenges. Rather than gestational age at birth per se, unmeasured risk factors that differ between families (e.g. genetic risks or absence of stimulating environments) may explain the association. 24–25 A design that compares siblings who are discordant with respect to gestational age can take such factors into account. 26–28 In combination with adjustment for observed risk factors that may vary between siblings within families (e.g. hypertension or maternal smoking), 29 this would improve the effect estimates (see Figure 1 ).
Figure 1.
Representation of putative confounders adjusted for in cohort, adjusted cohort, sibling-control and adjusted sibling-control designs.
Most preterm deliveries occur spontaneously by onset of preterm labour or rupture of the membranes, and less than one-third of preterm deliveries are provider-initiated by obstetricians to ward off greater harm to the health of the mother or child (e.g. due to intrauterine growth restriction). 3,30,31 Because provider-initiated preterm deliveries might represent more imperilled pregnancies, 32 this might relate to more serious and persistent language delays and different risk factors, compared with spontaneous preterm deliveries. However, this remains to be examined.
A prospective sibling-control design was used to revisit and expand the examination of three issues: (i) potential inverse linear associations between gestational age at delivery and risk for language delays across spontaneous and provider-initiated deliveries; (ii) the strength and persistence of these associations across ages 18 and 36 months; And (iii) the degree to which these associations are brought about by risk factors stable within families, risk factors variable within families or actual gestational age effects of preterm delivery.
Methods
Participants
Data from birth until 3 years of age was derived from the population-based Norwegian Mother and Child Birth Cohort Study (MoBa) conducted at the Norwegian Institute of Public Health. 33 The cohort includes 114 500 children and 95 200 mothers, recruited from across Norway in the period 1999–2008, with a maternal informed consent rate of 40.6% and concession from the Norwegian Data Protection Authority.
This study utilised data from the Medical Birth Registry of Norway (MBRN) and maternal-reported MoBa questionnaires at the 17th and 30th week of gestation, and child ages of 18 and 36 months (with response rates of 95%, 92%, 77% and 62%, respectively). We used data version 7, quality-assured and released in 2013 [ http://www.fhi.no/moba-en ]. We included all sibling pregnancies for which gestational age data from MBRN, as well as data at child age 18 months ( n = 21 453) or 36 months ( n = 17 899) were available. This comprised 22 499 siblings, from 10 299 sibling pairs, 607 sibling trios and 20 sibling quartets. As twins are concordant for the exposure, we included the first-borns ( n = 220) from multiple births with other older or younger participating siblings. Table 1 shows descriptive sample information.
Table 1.
Descriptive characteristics of sibling sample from the Norwegian Mother and Child Birth Cohort Study ( N = 22 499)
Variables | N (M ± SD) | N missing |
---|---|---|
Child | ||
Gestational age at birth, weeks | (39.9 ± 11.8) | 0 |
Boys | 11 577 | 0 |
Twins | 220 | 0 |
Malformations at birth | 535 | 0 |
SGA | 335 | 16 |
Pregnancy | ||
Unplanned pregnancy | 3599 | 439 |
Preeclampsia | 712 | 0 |
IVF | 370 | 0 |
Gestational diabetes | 294 | 0 |
Recurrent urinary tract infections | 665 | 0 |
Bleeding between weeks 13–28 | 347 | 0 |
Hypertensive state | 1 115 | 0 |
Depressive symptoms (SCL cutoff) | 1036 | 370 |
Smoking during pregnancy | 1264 | 26 |
Alcohol during pregnancy | 6432 | 692 |
Maternal weight status (BMI) | – | 627 |
BMI < 18.5 | 601 | – |
BMI > 18.5 - < 30.0 | 19 385 | – |
BMI > 30.0 | 1886 | – |
Demographics | ||
Motheŕs completed education | – | 1 114 |
University > 4 years | 5955 | – |
University ≤ 4 years | 9929 | – |
High school or lower | 5501 | – |
Marital status during pregnancy | – | 317 |
Married/partner | 21 832 | – |
Divorced/Single | 227 | – |
Other | 123 | – |
Age mother, years | (30.2 ± 4.1) | 0 |
N, sample size; M, median; S, standard deviation; SGA, small for gestational age; IVF, in vitro fertilization; SCL, Hopkins Symptom Checklist-5; BMI, body mass index.
Measures
Delivery
Gestational age at birth was based on ultrasound examinations (or for n = 64 on the date of last menstruation registered in MBRN), and was divided into four gestational age groups: early preterm (gestation weeks 22/0 to 33/6); late preterm (weeks 34/0 to 36/6); early term (weeks 37/0 to 38/6); and full term (> weeks 39/0). We also divided delivery into two onset types: spontaneous and provider-initiated (i.e. induced or primary caesarean delivery on maternal or fetal indications). 3,34 Frequencies and sibling discordance of gestational age group and type of delivery onset are shown in Table 2 . In the sibling-control analyses, the difference between the gestational age average of the sibling group and the individual score (e.g. for a trio we divide it by 3), provided ‘deviations from the family means’ in any family.
Table 2.
Number (%) of children in each delivery group (left side) and members of discordant sibling groups (right side)
Number of children in each group
|
Members of discordant sibling group
|
|||
---|---|---|---|---|
Gestation | Spontaneous | Provider-initiated | Spontaneous | Provider-initiated |
duration (weeks) | n = 18 484 (%) | n = 4015 (%) | of n = 18 484 (%) | of n = 4015 (%) |
< 34 | 127 (0.7) | 101 (2.5) | 110 (0.6) | 92 (2.3) |
34–36 | 469 (2.5) | 266 (6.6) | 381 (2.1) | 232 (5.8) |
37–38 | 2439 (13.2) | 1068 (26.6) | 1783 (9.7) | 770 (19.5) |
> 38 | 15 449 (83.6) | 2580 (64.3) | 2503 (13.5) | 404 (10.1) |
The columns to the left show the total number of children delivered spontaneously and provider-initiated from the different delivery groups. The columns to the right display how many of these children were members of a discordant sibling group (not born in the same group) and subsequently included in the sibling analyses.
Outcomes
Language skills at 18 and 36 months were assessed by maternal-reported items derived from several established scales: the Norwegian version of the Ages and Stages Questionnaires, 35 the Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers, 36 the Non-Verbal Communication Checklist, 37 the Early Screening of Autistic Traits Questionnaire, 38 the Social Communication Questionnaire 39 and a question about the complexity and comprehensibility of child utterances with six response categories. 40 Our language comprehension 41 and language production 42 outcomes are latent variables based on previous Confirmatory Factor Analyses (CFA) work showing very good model fits with high non-overlapping factor loadings (see references 41,42 for description of construction, validation and specific items included). The language comprehension constructs at 18 months (five items) and 36 months (seven items) captured understanding of or appropriate responses to the language utterances of others, and were measurement invariant across time. 41 The language production construct at 18 months was based on one item enquiring whether the child could utter eight or more words, in addition to ‘mama’ and ‘dada’, and at 36 months this construct captured the complexity of child utterances and conversations (seven items). These were thus not the same across time. 42
Confounders
In the adjusted analyses, we only controlled for pregnancy-specific child, mother or pregnancy factors that can vary between pregnancies and that preceded the exposure. Confounders from MBRN included: parity, smoking, alcohol intake, pregnancy-specific hypertensive state, bleeding (weeks 13–28), recurrent urinary tract infections, unplanned pregnancy, gestational diabetes, in vitro fertilization, low (< 18.5) and high (> 30) body mass index (BMI), gender, multiple birth status, serious malformations at birth and small for gestational age [SGA; i.e. 2 standard deviations (SD) difference from the uterine growth curve 43 ]. Mothers also reported on anxiety and depressive symptoms during pregnancy, set above the clinical cut-off on the Hopkins Symptom Checklist-5. 44 Due to high correlations between hypertensive state and preeclampsia ( r = 0.74), and maternal age and parity ( r = 0.91), preeclampsia and maternal age were not included.
Statistical analyses
Four models were examined for both outcomes at 18 and 36 months, using Mplus version 7.2. 45 In all analyses the outcome was modelled as a latent variable using an item response theory approach (i.e. multi-level CFA for categorical (ordinal) data using a logit link function). 46 First, main effect cohort analyses including all siblings were conducted to address possible inverse linear trends in associations between gestational age and the outcomes, and adjusted cohort analyses were examined for comparison purposes. Next, the same associations were examined by first calculating unadjusted (only adjusting for unobserved stable family factors) and then adjusted sibling-control (also adjusting for pregnancy-specific observed factors) models (see Supplementary text and Supplementary Figure S1 , available as Supplementary data at IJE online, for details about the sibling models).
We standardized the beta coefficients for the means by dividing them by the square root of the total (within + between) variance, so the spontaneous term group was the group of reference with a mean of 0 and a variance of 1. The same sibling sample was used for all analyses but, as separate analyses were performed for the outcomes, sample sizes vary. Because we estimated missing data in a multilevel sample using a full-information maximum likelihood approach that does not allow missing data estimation of independent variables, codes for missing data in the variables unplanned pregnancy (2.0%), smoking (0.1%) and alcohol intake during pregnancy (3.1%), depressive symptoms (1.6%), BMI (2.8%) and SGA (0.1%) were included as dummy variables.
Neither participation nor attrition in MoBa is random. 47,48 When examining attrition as a function of preterm birth using conditional logistic regression on the total sample of siblings in MoBa, children delivered early preterm were underrepresented as shown by odds ratios for non-response at 18 and 36 months (2.14, P = .000; 1.90, P = .000), whereas late preterm (0.53, P = .000; 0.57, P = .000) and early term children (0.86, P = .012; 0.91, P = .088) were overrepresented, compared with the term group.
Results
At 18 months (see Table 3 ), the cohort analyses revealed a clear inverse trend showing poorer language comprehension with shorter gestational length, with similar effects for spontaneous and provider-initiated births. Compared with the full term groups, the early preterm groups showed mean levels of language comprehension that were around 1 SD lower, which is a sizeable effect equal to 15 points on an IQ test. The effects were smaller in the late preterm (0.34–0.39 SD) and early term groups (0.14–0.23 SD). The adjusted cohort model reduced the effects only marginally. In the unadjusted sibling-control model, the linear trends for the spontaneous deliveries were sustained overall, but for the early preterm delivery group the effect was substantially smaller than in the cohort analyses. Among provider-initiated births, the inverse linear trend was even more pronounced, with effects for the early preterm, late preterm and early term groups being stronger than among corresponding spontaneous groups. The adjusted sibling-control model shows that the pregnancy-specific covariates accounted for a lesser portion of the effects for all groups, as the inverse linear trends of gestational age were preserved and only slightly diminished. The effects for the provider-initiated births were still larger than in the cohort analyses, and the effect for the provider-initiated early preterm group was still stronger than for the corresponding spontaneous group.
Table 3.
Means (95% confidence interval) of language skills at age 18 months (not adjusted for gestational age)
Language comprehension (
N
= 21 353)
|
||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Cohort unadjusted
a |
Cohort adjusted
b |
Sibling-control unadjusted
c |
Sibling-control adjusted
d |
|||||
Gestation duration (weeks) | Spontaneous | Provider-initiated | Spontaneous | Provider-initiated | Spontaneous | Provider-initiated | Spontaneous | Provider-initiated |
< 34 | −1.06*** | −1.10*** | −0.95*** | −0.97*** | −0.55** | −1.71*** e | −0.42 * | −1.54*** e |
(−1.31, −0.80) | (−1.36, −0.84) | (−1.20, −0.70) | (−1.23, −0.70) | (−0.88, −0.23) | (−2.15, −1.28) | (−0.75, −0.10) | (−1.98, −1.10) | |
34–36 | −0.34*** | −0.39*** | −0.29*** | −0.27** | −0.30*** | −0.72*** e | −0.23** | −0.54*** |
(−0.46, −0.22) | (−0.57, −0.22) | (−0.41, −0.17) | (−0.45, −0.08) | (−0.46, −0.13) | (−0.99, −0.45) | (−0.40, −0.07) | (−0.81, −0.27) | |
37–38 | −0.14*** | −0.23*** | −0.15*** | −0.16** | −0.12 * | −0.30*** e | −0.14*** | −0.19 * |
(−0.21, −0.08) | (−0.32, −0.15) | (−021, −0.09) | (−0.26, −0.07) | (−0.20, −0.04) | (−0.45, −0.15) | (−0.22, −0.06) | (−0.34, −0.05) | |
> 38 | 0.00 | −0.08 * | 0.00 | −0.03 | 0.00 | −0.09*** | 0.00 | −0.06 * |
Reference | (−0.14, −0.02) | Reference | (−0.08, 0.02) | Reference | (−0.14, −0.04) | Reference | (−0.11, −0.01) | |
Language production ( N = 21 231) |
||||||||
Cohort unadjusted a | Cohort adjusted b | Sibling-control unadjusted c | Sibling-control adjusted d | |||||
| ||||||||
Gestation duration (weeks) | Spontaneous | Provider-initiated | Spontaneous | Provider-initiated | Spontaneous | Provider-initiated | Spontaneous | Provider-initiated |
| ||||||||
< 34 | −0.47*** | −0.59*** | −0.40*** | −0.57*** | −0.30 * | −1.15*** e | −0.26 | −1.17*** e |
(−0.66, −0.28) | (−0.83, −0.36) | (−0.59, −0.21) | (−0.80, −0.34) | (−0.57, −0.02) | (−1.60, −0.69) | (−0.53, 0.01) | (−1.62, −0.71) | |
34–36 | −0.18** | −0.22** | −0.15** | −0.13 | −0.18 * | −0.53*** e | −0.18 * | −0.44*** |
(−0.29, −0.07) | (−0.36, −0.07) | (−0.26, −0.04) | (−0.28, 0.02) | (−0.33, −0.02) | (−0.77, −028) | (−0.33, −0.02) | (−0.68, −0.20) | |
37–38 | −0.08** | −0.19*** e | −0.09** | −0.14** | −0.06 | −0.33*** e | −0.09 * | −0.20** |
(−0.13, −0.03) | (−0.26, −0.12) | (−0.14, −0.04) | (−0.22, −0.06) | (−0.14, 0.01) | (−0.47, −0.19) | (−0.16, −0.02) | (−0.34, −0.06) | |
> 38 | 0.00 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.02 | 0.00 | −0.03 | 0.00 | −0.01 |
Reference | (−0.05, 0.06) | Reference | (−0.02, 0.07) | Reference | (−0.07, 0.02) | Reference | (−0.05, 0.04) |
a Analyses with the same sibling sample used in a conventional cohort design.
b Analyses with the same sibling sample used in a conventional cohort design, controlling for pregnancy-specific confounders (parity, smoking or alcohol intake, hypertensive state, bleeding in weeks 13–28, recurrent urinary tract infections, unplanned pregnancy, gestational diabetes, in vitro fertilization, low (< 18.5) and high (> 30) BMI (middle group is reference), maternal anxiety and depressive symptoms, child gender, multiple births status, serious malformations at birth and small for gestational age).
c Analyses using the sibling-control design, controlling for unobserved familial risks, but not for measured confounders.
d Analyses using the sibling-control design, controlling for unobserved familial risks, and for pregnancy-specific confounders (parity, smoking or alcohol intake, hypertensive state, bleedingin weeks 13−28, recurrent urinary tract infections, unplanned pregnancy, gestational diabetes, in vitro fertilization, low (<18.5) and high (> 30) BMI (middle group is reference), maternal anxiety and depressive symptoms, child gender, multiple births status, serious malformations at birth and small for gestational age).
e Significant mean level difference between equivalent spontaneous and provider-initiated delivery groups at the ≤ 0.05 level.
* P = < 0.05, ** P = < 0.01, *** P =< 0.001.
We also found an inverse linear trend between shorter gestational length and poorer language production with the cohort model at 18 months, for both spontaneous and provider-initiated delivery, with a stronger association in the provider-initiated early term than in the equivalent spontaneous group. In the adjusted cohort model, all effects were marginally smaller, and less substantial for the provider-initiated late preterm group. In the unadjusted sibling-control analysis, the language production delays across the spontaneous groups decreased compared with the cohort results. In contrast, the effects in the provider-initiated groups doubled in size, and became different from the means of all equivalent spontaneous groups. In the adjusted sibling-control model, only the mean level difference across the two early preterm groups was sustained.
The cohort results in Table 4 shows that the language delays are less pronounced at 36 months. There were no effects of preterm group on language comprehension for spontaneous deliveries. For provider-initiated deliveries, there was an inverse linear tendency: the early preterm group and late preterm group both showed poorer language comprehension than the full term group, the former being different from the equivalent spontaneous group. These effects were not sustained in the sibling-control analyses, and the minor effect of spontaneous early term delivery in the unadjusted sibling-control model disappeared in the adjusted model.
Table 4.
Means (95% confidence interval) of language skills at age 36 months (not adjusted for gestational age)
Language comprehension ( N = 17 871) |
||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Cohort unadjusted
a |
Cohort adjusted
b |
Sibling-ontrol unadjusted
c |
Sibling-Control Adjusted
d |
|||||
Gestation duration (weeks) | Spontaneous | Provider-initiated | Spontaneous | Provider-initiated | Spontaneous | Provider-initiated | Spontaneous | Provider-initiated |
< 34 | 0.14 | −0.53** e | 0.17 | −0.45* e | 0.45 | −0.59 e | 0.52 | −0.51 e |
(−0.36, 0.64) | (−0.90, −0.16) | (−0.33, 0.67) | (−0.83, −0.06) | (−0.25, 1.15) | (−1.24, 0.05) | (−0.16, 1.21) | (−1.20, 0.18) | |
34–36 | −0.21 | −0.36** | −0.17 | −0.27 | 0.03 | −0.27 | 0.08 | −0.18 |
(−0.44, 0.02) | (−0.61, −0.10) | (−0.40, 0.05) | (−0.54, 0.00) | (−0.28, 0.34) | (−0.78, 0.24) | (−0.24, 0.40) | (−0.70, 0.34) | |
37–38 | −0.06 | −0.12 | −0.06 | −0.10 | −0.17* | −0.06 | −0.16 | −0.01 |
(−0.17, 0.04) | (−0.28, 0.03) | (−0.16, 0.05) | (−0.28, 0.07) | (−0.33, 0.00) | (−0.36, 0.25) | (−0.32, 0.00) | (−0.32, 0.30) | |
> 38 | 0.00 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.02 | 0.00 | −0.06 | 0.00 | −0.06 |
Reference | (−0.09, 0.12) | Reference | (−0.07, 0.11) | Reference | (−0.15, 0.03) | Reference | (−0.14, 0.03) | |
| ||||||||
Language production ( N = 17 896) |
||||||||
Cohort unadjusted a | Cohort adjusted b | Sibling-control unadjusted c | Sibling-control adjusted d | |||||
| ||||||||
Gestational length (weeks) | Spontaneous | Provider-initiated | Spontaneous | Provider-initiated | Spontaneous | Provider-initiated | Spontaneous | Provider-initiated |
| ||||||||
<34 | −0.36** | −0.74*** e | −0.30* | −0.63*** e | 0.00 | −0.84** e | 0.03 | −0.76** e |
(−0.59, −0.13) | (−1.00, −0.47) | (−0.53, −0.06) | (−0.89, −0.36) | (−0.33, 0.33) | (−1.35, −0.34) | (−0.29, 0.36) | (−1.29, −0.24) | |
34–36 | −0.23** | −0.36*** | −0.21** | −0.23** | −0.15 | −0.45** | −0.09 | −0.30* |
(−0.38, −0.09) | (−0.52, −0.20) | (−0.34, −0.07) | (−0.39, −0.07) | (−0.35, 0.05) | (−0.74, -0.16) | (−0.28, 0.11) | (−0.59, −0.01) | |
37–38 | −0.08* | −0.13* | −0.09** | −0.06 | −0.09* | −0.15 | −0.11* | −0.09 |
(−0.15, −0.02) | (−0.22, −0.03) | (−0.15, −0.03) | (−0.17, 0.05) | (−0.18, 0.00) | (−0.33, 0.02) | (−0.19, −0.02) | (−0.26, 0.08) | |
> 38 | 0.00 | −0.08* | 0.00 | −0.02 | 0.00 | −0.08** | 0.00 | −0.06* |
Reference | (−0.14, −0.01) | Reference | (−0.07, 0.03) | Reference | (−0.13, -0.03) | Reference | (−0.11, −0.01) |
a Analyses with the same sibling sample used in a conventional cohort design.
b Analyses with the same sibling sample used in a conventional cohort design, controlling for pregnancy-specific confounders (parity, smoking or alcohol intake, hypertensive state, bleeding in weeks 13–28, recurrent urinary tract infections, unplanned pregnancy, gestational diabetes, in vitro fertilization, low (< 18.5) and high (> 30) BMI (middle group is reference), maternal anxiety and depressive symptoms, child gender, multiple births status, serious malformations at birth and small for gestational age).
c Analyses using the sibling-control design, controlling for unobserved familial risks, but not for measured confounders.
d Analyses using the sibling-control design, controlling for unobserved familial risks, and for pregnancy-specific confounders (parity, smoking or alcohol intake, hypertensive state, bleeding in weeks 13–28, recurrent urinary tract infections, unplanned pregnancy, gestational diabetes, in vitro fertilization, low (< 18.5) and high (> 30) BMI (middle group is reference), maternal anxiety and depressive symptoms, child gender, multiple births status, serious malformations at birth and small for gestational age).
e Significant mean level difference between equivalent spontaneous and provider-initiated delivery groups at the ≤ 0.05 level.
* P =< 0.05, ** P =< 0.01, *** P =< 0.001.
For language production, lower gestational age was still related to poorer levels in the unadjusted cohort results, with strongest effects in children from provider-initiated delivery, and with the early preterm group effect being different from the same spontaneous group. The confounder adjustments slightly reduced these effects. When comparing siblings across unadjusted and adjusted models, the language production delays in the spontaneous groups only remained for the early term group. In contrast, the language production delays in children from provider-initiated delivery were the same in the cohort and sibling models, except for the early term group where there was no longer strong evidence of an effect. Tests of linear trends are described in Supplementary text , available as Supplementary data at IJE online.
Discussion
As expected, 8,10,11 preterm delivery was a major risk factor for early language delays. The cohort results indicate the level of delays the children struggle with every day: at 18 months, both language comprehension and production delays are profoundly affected by gestation duration, with shorter gestations predicting poorer language skills. By 36 months, the delays are less pronounced. Given that comprehension delays reflect more severe impairments, 20–23 overall language delays seem to be less extensive at 36 months. Nevertheless, the delays are still substantial for some children, 12 particularly regarding language production, and for those who experience provider-initiated deliveries.
The sibling-control models enabled us to ascertain the effects adjusted for unmeasured familial risk factors possibly related to both preterm delivery and language delay. First, we found that among the spontaneous births, some of the apparent inverse linear effects of gestational age on language comprehension and production delays at 18 months, as well as almost all effects at 36 months, were accounted for by unobserved familial risk factors. 26–28 Pregnancy-specific risk factors did not account for a substantial share of the effects, 28 and the remaining effects at 18 months indicated that gestational age was a major explanatory factor for these delays, whereas the 36 months delays were mainly explained by familial risk factors.
In contrast, we found that for the provider-initiated deliveries, familial and pregnancy-specific risk factors did not account for the effects of gestational age on language comprehension and production at either 18 or 36 months. In fact, the sibling-control analyses rather strengthened the notion that preterm delivery is a profound risk factor at 18 months, suggesting that pregnancies ending in provider-initiated preterm deliveries involve additional risks compared with spontaneous preterm births. Moreover, the earlier the provider-initiated delivery, the more implications this has for the children. The reasons for the differential effects are not evident. As we adjusted for typical conditions that obstetricians base their decisions to deliver on, such as hypertension or SGA, 30,31 other unmeasured pregnancy-specific risk factors could be important. Alternatively, gestational age could have added implications for children from provider-initiated delivery.
Several limitations should be mentioned. Even if the outcomes’ construct validity and reliability were established, 41,42 clinical data would have been useful in addition to maternal reports. Moreover, although we did not have access to gestational age-adjusted outcomes, the different impacts across the spontaneous and provider-initiated groups and the large overall effects would not likely be explained by 2 months of language development. Relatedly, the lack of time-invariant language production measures did not permit longitudinal analyses or statistical comparison of effect sizes over time. Importantly, for comparison reasons, the adjusted cohort analyses only included pregnancy-specific confounders, and we need to be careful about generalizing to women with only one child. Furthermore, MoBa mothers show socio-demographic advantages compared with the average Norwegian woman giving birth in the same period, 47,48 possibly explaining the low SGA prevalence. The inclusion of missing data as dummy variables could also potentially introduce bias. Finally, our sample is affected by attrition. If mothers lost to follow-up have a higher rate of both preterm deliveries and children with language delays, this would lead to additional bias. We included covariates not shared by siblings and modelled random measurement error in our outcome. However, we acknowledge that estimates from sibling designs can be biased if covariates more familial than the exposure are included and random measurement error is not accounted for. 49
In sum, this study emphasizes the importance of sibling-control designs when studying effects of preterm deliveries. First, we could ascertain robust inverse linear associations between gestational age at delivery and early language delays. Second, we found that these associations were particularly strong for provider-initiated deliveries. Third, even if language delays diminish between 18 and 36 months, the delays persist for the early preterm children, particularly those from provider-initiated delivery. Finally, we find that different factors probably underlie the language delays of preterm children delivered provider-initiated compared with spontaneous delivery. Indeed, this underlines the need for examining spontaneous and provider-initiated preterm births separately, to address issues of aetiology, 32 and future studies could aim to identify such factors, for example by comparing similarity in full and half siblings.
Funding
This work was supported by the Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Division of Mental Health. The Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study is supported by the Norwegian Ministry of Health, the Ministry of Education and Research, the National Institutes of Health/National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences [contract no NO1-ES-75558] and the National Institutes of Health/National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke [grant numbers 1 UO1 NS 047537-01, 2 UO1 NS 047537-06A1].
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank the editor and referees, the MoBa team and participating families.
Conflict of interest: The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.
References
- 1. Moster D, Lie RT, Markestad T . Long-term medical and social consequences of preterm birth . N Engl J Med 2008. ; 359 : 262 – 73 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Saigal S, Doyle LW . An overview of mortality and sequelae of preterm birth from infancy to adulthood . Lancet 2008. ; 371 : 261 – 69 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Blencowe H, Cousens S, Oestergaard MZ, et al. . National, regional, and worldwide estimates of preterm birth rates in the year 2010 with time trends since 1990 for selected countries: a systematic analysis and implications . Lancet 2012. ; 379 : 2162 – 72 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Davidoff MJ, Dias T, Damus K, et al. . Changes in the gestational age distribution among US singleton births: impact on rates of late preterm birth, 1992 to 2002 . Semin Perinatol 2006. ; 30 : 8 – 15 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Gyamfi-Bannerman C . The scope of the problem: the epidemiology of late preterm and early-term birth . Semin Perinatol 2011. ; 35 : 246 – 48 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Engle WA, Kominiarek MA . Late preterm infants, early term infants, and timing of elective deliveries . Clin Perinatol 2008. ; 35 : 325 – 41 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Stene-Larsen K, Brandlistuen RE, Lang AM, Landolt MA, Latal B, Vollrath ME . Communication Impairments in Early Term and Late Preterm Children: A Prospective Cohort Study following Children to Age 36 Months . J Pediatr 2014. ; 165 : 1123 – 28 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Boyle EM, Poulsen G, Field DJ, et al. . Effects of gestational age at birth on health outcomes at 3 and 5 years of age: population based cohort study . BMJ 2012. ; 344 : e896 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Zubrick SR, Taylor CL, Rice ML, Slegers DW . Late language emergence at 24 months: An epidemiological study of prevalence, predictors, and covariates . J Speech Lang Hear Res 2007. ; 50 : 1562 – 92 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Menyuk P, Liebergott J, Schultz M . Early Language Development in Full-term and in Premature Infants . Hillsdale, UK: : Lawrence Erlbaum; , 1995. . [Google Scholar]
- 11. Ribeiro L, Zachrisson H, Schjolberg S, Aase H, Rohrer-Baumgartner N, Magnus P . Attention problems and language development in preterm low-birth-weight children: Cross-lagged relations from 18 to 36 months . BMC Pediatr 2011. ; 11 : 59 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Guarini A, Sansavini A, Fabbri C, Alessandroni R, Faldella G, Karmiloff-Smith A . Reconsidering the impact of preterm birth on language outcome . Early Hum Dev 2009. ; 85 : 639 – 45 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Wolke D, Samara M, Bracewell M, Marlow N . Specific language difficulties and school achievment in children born at 25 weeks gestation or less . J Pediatr 2008. ; 152 : 256 – 62 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Benasich AA, Curtiss S, Tallal P . Language, Learning, and Behavioral Disturbances in Childhood: A Longitudinal Perspective . J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1993. ; 32 : 585 – 94 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Beitchman JH, Wilson B, Johnson CJ, et al. . Fourteen-year follow-up of speech/language-impaired and control children: psychiatric outcome . J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2001. ; 40 : 75 – 82 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. de Jong M, Verhoeven M, van Baar AL . School outcome, cognitive functioning, and behaviour problems in moderate and late preterm children and adults: A review . Semin Fetal Neonatal Med 2012. ; 17 : 163 – 69 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Quigley MA, Poulsen G, Boyle E, et al. . Early term and late preterm birth are associated with poorer school performance at age 5 years: a cohort study . Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2012. ; 97:F167 – 73 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. McGowan JE, Alderdice FA, Holmes VA, Johnston L . Early Childhood Development of Late-Preterm Infants: A Systematic Review . Pediatrics 2011. ; 127 : 1111 – 24 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Dong Y, Chen S, Yu J . A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Long-Term Development of Early Term Infants . Neonatology 2012. ; 102 : 212 – 21 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Thal DJ, Katich J . Predicaments in early identification of specific language impairment. Does the early bird always catch the worm ? In: Cole KN, Dale PS, Thal DJ. (eds). Assessment of Communication and Language . Baltimore, MD: : Paul H. Brookes; , 1996. . [Google Scholar]
- 21. Leonard LB . Children with Specific Language Impairment . Cambridge, MA: : MIT Press; , 2014. . [Google Scholar]
- 22. Bishop DV, Holt G, Line E, McDonald D, McDonald S, Watt H . Parental phonological memory contributes to prediction of outcome of late talkers from 20 months to 4 years: a longitudinal study of precursors of specific language impairment . J Neurodev Dis 2012. ; 4 : 3 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Ellis Weismer S . Typical talkers, late talkers, and children with specific language impairment: a language endowment spectrum ? In: Paul R. (ed). Language Disorders from a Developmental Perspective: Essays in Honor of Robin S. Chapman . Mahwah, NJ: : Erlbaum; , 2007. . [Google Scholar]
- 24. Kovas Y, Hayiou-Thomas ME, Oliver B, Dale PS, Bishop DVM, Plomin R . Genetic influences in different aspects of language development: The etiology of language skills in 4.5-year-old twins . Child Dev 2005. ; 76 : 632 – 51 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Zambrana IM, Pons F, Eadie P, Ystrom E . Trajectories of language delay from age 3 to 5: persistence, recovery and late onset . Int J Lang Commun Disord 2014. ; 49 : 304 – 16 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Brandlistuen RE, Ystrom E, Nulman I, Koren G, Nordeng H . Prenatal paracetamol exposure and child neurodevelopment: a sibling-controlled cohort study . Int J Epidemiol 2013. ; 42 : 1702 – 13 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. D’Onofrio BM, Class QA, Rickert ME, Larsson H, Långström N, Lichtenstein P . Preterm birth and mortality and morbidity: A population-based quasi-experimental study . JAMA Psychiatry 2013. ; 70 : 1231 – 40 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Susser E, Eide MG, Begg M . Invited Commentary: The Use of Sibship Studies to Detect Familial Confounding . Am J Epidemiol 2010. ; 172 : 537 – 39 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Hauge LJ, Aarø LE, Torgersen L, Vollrath ME . Smoking During Consecutive Pregnancies Among Primiparous Women in the Population-Based Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study . Nicotine Tob Res 2013. ; 15 : 428 – 34 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Morken N-H, Magnus P, Jacobsson B . Subgroups of preterm delivery in the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study . Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand 2008. ; 87 : 1374 – 77 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Morken N-H, Källen K, Jacobsson B . Outcomes of preterm children according to type of delivery onset: a nationwide population-based study . Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 2007. ; 21 : 458 – 64 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Savitz DA, Dole N, Herring AH, et al. . Should spontaneous and medically indicated preterm births be separated for studying aetiology? Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 2005. ; 19 : 97 – 105 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Magnus P, Irgens L, Haug K, et al. . Cohort Profile: The Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study (MoBa) . Int J Epidemiol 2006. ; 35 : 1146 – 50 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Englund-Ögge L, Brantsæter AL, Sengpiel V, et al. . Maternal dietary patterns and preterm delivery: results from large prospective cohort study . BMJ 2014. ; 38 : g1446 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Janson H, Squires J . Parent-completed developmental screening in a Norwegian population sample: a comparison with US normative data . Acta Paediatr 2004. ; 93 : 1525 – 29 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Robins D, Fein D, Barton M, Green J . The Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers: An Initial Study Investigating the Early Detection of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders . J Autism Dev Disord 2001. ; 31 : 131 – 44 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Schjolberg S. A population based study on early detection of autism spectrum disorders. Poster presented at the First International Conference on The Social Brain . Goteborg: Sweden; March 2003 . [Google Scholar]
- 38. Swinkels SHN, Dietz C, van Daalen E, Kerkhof IHGM, van Engeland H, Buitelaar JK . Screening for Autistic Spectrum in Children Aged 14 to 15Â Months. I: The Development of the Early Screening of Autistic Traits Questionnaire (ESAT) . J Autism Dev Disord 2006. ; 36 : 723 – 32 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Rutter M, Bailey A, Lord C . The Social Communication Questionnaire: Manual . Los Angeles, CA: : Western Psychological Services; , 2003. . [Google Scholar]
- 40. Dale PS, Price TS, Bishop DVM, Plomin R . Outcomes of Early Language Delay: I. Predicting Persistent and Transient Language Difficulties at 3 and 4 Years . J Speech Lang Hear Res 2003. ; 46 : 544 – 60 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Zambrana IM, Ystrom E, Pons F . Impact of gender, maternal education, and birth order on the development of language comprehension: a longitudinal study from 18 to 36 months of age . J Dev Behav Pediatr 2012. ; 33 : 146 – 55 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Zambrana IM, Ystrom E, Schjølberg S, Pons F . Action imitation at 1½ years is better than pointing gesture in predicting late development of language production at 3 years of age . Child Dev 2013. ; 84 : 560 – 73 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Maršál K, Persson PH, Larsen T, Lilja H, Selbing A, Sultan B . Intrauterine growth curves based on ultrasonically estimated foetal weights . Acta Paediatr 1996. ; 85 : 843 – 48 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Strand BH, Dalgard OS, Tambs K, Rognerud M . Measuring the mental health status of the Norwegian population: A comparison of the instruments SCL-25, SCL-10, SCL-5 and MHI-5 (SF-36) . Nord J Psychiatry 2003. ; 57 : 113 – 18 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Muthén LK, Muthén BO . Mplus User’s Guide . 7th edn . Los Angeles, CA: : Muthén & Muthén; , 1998–2012. . [Google Scholar]
- 46. Muthén B, Asparouhov T . Beyond multilevel regression modeling: Multilevel analysis in a general latent variable framework . In: Hox J, Roberts K. (eds). Handbook of Advanced Multilevel Analysis . New York, NY: : Routledge; , 2010. . [Google Scholar]
- 47. Nilsen R, Vollset S, Gjessing H, et al. . Self-selection and bias in a large prospective pregnancy cohort in Norway . Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 2009. ; 23 : 597 – 608 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Nilsen RM, Surén P, Gunnes N, et al. . Analysis of Self-selection Bias in a Population-based Cohort Study of Autism Spectrum Disorders . Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 2013. ; 27 : 553 – 63 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Frisell T, Öberg S, Kuja-Halkola R, Sjölander A . Sibling comparison designs: bias from non-shared confounders and measurement error . Epidemiology 2012. ; 23 : 713 – 20 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.