Abstract
The poor health status of the Western honey bee, Apis mellifera, compared to its Eastern counterpart, Apis cerana, is remarkable. This has been attributed to lower pathogen prevalence in A. cerana colonies and to their ability to survive infestations with the ectoparasitic mite, Varroa destructor. These properties have been linked to an enhanced removal of dead or unhealthy immature bees by adult workers in this species. Although such hygienic behavior is known to contribute to honey bee colony health, comparative data of A. mellifera and A. cerana in performing this task are scarce. Here, we compare for the first time the removal of freeze-killed brood in one population of each species and over two seasons in China. Our results show that A. cerana was significantly faster than A. mellifera at both opening cell caps and removing freeze-killed brood. The fast detection and removal of diseased brood is likely to limit the proliferation of pathogenic agents. Given our results can be generalized to the species level, a rapid hygienic response could contribute to the better health of A. cerana. Promoting the fast detection and removal of worker brood through adapted breeding programs could further improve the social immunity of A. mellifera colonies and contribute to a better health status of the Western honey bee worldwide.
Introduction
Due to major and widespread losses of colonies [1], the health status of A. mellifera, the most widespread managed pollinator of crops and wild flora [2–3] has triggered much attention in the beekeeping as well as in the scientific communities [1,4]. Meanwhile, its Eastern counterpart, A. cerana, harbors far fewer parasites and pathogens [5–12], suggesting that it benefits from an overall higher resilience to biotic threats, and this despite its exposure to heterospecific pathogens (e.g. deformed wing virus) [12–13].
The reasons for this striking difference have been attributed to the collective defense against parasites and pathogens, which is universally expressed in eusocial insects [14–15] and more strongly so in A. cerana compared to A. mellifera [16–18]. Hygienic behavior constitutes such a defense system and reinforces the social immunity of a honey bee colony [19]. When performing hygienic brood removal, adult bees detect the cells in which diseased or dead brood occur, uncap their wax seal and extirpate the affected individuals together with the pathogenic agents from the cells and the nest, thus preventing further spread of the disease within the colony. Hygienic removal can target brood infested with V. destructor mites [16,20–23] or infected by bacteria (Paenibacillus larvae) [24–25], fungi (Ascosphaera apis) [26–28] or viruses (e.g. deformed wing virus) [29–30]. However, most of this knowledge is derived from A. mellifera and comparative data with A. cerana are scarce [20].
Hygienic behavior is a heritable genetic trait, which is commonly taken into account in A. mellifera breeding programs in order to improve the vitality of the stocks [31–32]. Such programs have been running for several years and the hygienic abilities and disease resistance of breeding A. mellifera colonies have largely been strengthened [33–34]. The assay consisting in monitoring the removal of freeze-killed brood from a comb section [35] has been acknowledged as the most conservative and reliable screening procedure to quantify the hygienic behavior of a colony [36]. We here use this method to provide the first comparison of the hygienic ability of A. mellifera and A. cerana over 72 hours. Given the better general health status of A. cerana, we hypothesized that A. cerana has a faster and higher hygienic response towards diseased brood than A. mellifera. Colonies of both species kept at the same location in Southeast China were tested simultaneously in spring and in autumn to take potential environmental and seasonal variations of hygienic behavior into account [37–38]. The results of these experiments yielded a comparative overview of the differences in freeze-killed brood removal ability between Western and Eastern honey bee colonies. We discuss whether their respective hygienic performances can contribute to the differences in health status reported in these two honey bee species.
Materials and Methods
Experimental colonies
We used four A. mellifera and four A. cerana full-sized colonies kept in Langstroth hives. The A. mellifera colonies were derived from unselected and heterogeneous stocks of European origin imported to China more than a century ago [39]. The A. cerana colonies were of indigenous origin (A. c. cerana). Since A. mellifera colonies are more populous and their workers larger in size, the hives contained 6–8 frames, whereas the smaller A. cerana colonies occupied 3–4 frames (~12,000–16,000 workers in A. mellifera colonies [40] and ~9,000–12,000 in A. cerana colonies). Given the intrusive nature of repeated evaluations of hygienic behavior, we also compared the hygienic abilities of colonies installed in observation hives. These allow the measure of hygienic removal at a higher temporal resolution without interfering with the behavior of workers. For these undisturbed measurements, five additional colonies of each species were installed in four-frame observation hives (two levels of two contiguous frames). One week before the experiment, four frames fully covered with workers (~8,000–9,000 in A. mellifera colonies [40] and ~9,000–12,000 in A. cerana colonies) and the queen were collected from each colony and placed into an observation hive. All of the colonies were located in a single apiary on Zhejiang University campus (Hangzhou, China). During the experiment, colonies were healthy and had ample honey and pollen stores.
Freeze-killed brood assays
The hygienic ability of each colony was assessed with the standard freeze-killed brood method [35], which requires freezing capped worker brood to death with liquid nitrogen (N2) and monitoring their removal by workers. In order to freeze-kill the brood in each colony, a circular comb section with a high number of purple-eyed pupae was delimited with a tapered polyvinyl chloride plumbing tube (Ø = 75 mm, length = 100 mm) by twisting it down into the comb until it reached the midrib. We then poured 300 ml of liquid N2 into the tube—first slowly to 5 mm in height and more rapidly once the cells had started to freeze. To avoid damage, the tube was left to thaw for 20 min before it was gently removed from the comb. Finally, the treated frames were returned into their original colonies.
Data recording and statistical analyses
In the experiment with full-sized colonies in Langstroth hives, the status of frozen cells was recorded twice on the first day and three times a day at three-hour intervals on the following days. For each recording event, the combs were taken out of their hives and the workers on their surface were gently brushed away. The areas of frozen brood were photographed before returning the comb into its original hive. The status of each cell (capped, uncapped or brood removed) was later determined based on the photographs. The experiment was performed once in late spring and repeated in mid-autumn 2013. In the experiment with observation hives, the frames were left in the hives and the status of each frozen brood cell (capped or uncapped) was recorded through the glass sides at two-hour intervals. This was done three times on the first day and four times a day during the subsequent days. These ten colonies were observed during the same period as the eight full-sized colonies in autumn 2013. Within the frozen areas, an occupied cell that was partially or completely uncapped, irrespective of the presence of brood, was considered as targeted by hygienic behavior. In both hive types, counts were performed until all of the frozen brood cells were uncapped, or terminated after three days if a colony failed to uncap all brood.
Each frozen cell was treated as an individual in a survival analysis. Kaplan-Meier plots and log-rank (Mantel-Cox) tests were used to describe and compare the hygienic abilities between species and seasons over time. We assessed the difference in hygienic task intensity experimentally induced in both species by statistically comparing the number of capped cells in the frozen area with a Student’s t-test for independent samples. The same test was used to compare arcsine-transformed percentage of brood uncapped at the first time-check and at 24 hours and thus assess the speed of hygienic detection of dead brood in these species. All statistical analyses were performed using the program SPSS (version 20.0).
Results
Frozen comb sections contained 122 ± 12.0 and 118 ± 20.8 capped cells in A. mellifera and A. cerana, respectively. The number of freeze-killed cells showed no significant difference between the two honey bee species (Student’s t-test, p = 0.60).
All full-sized A. mellifera and A. cerana colonies uncapped all of the freeze-killed brood within 72 h. However, the log-rank test showed that both cell uncapping and brood removal of A. cerana workers was significantly higher than that of A. mellifera in spring (uncapping: χ2 = 752.65, p < 0.001, Fig 1a; removal: χ2 = 116.40, p < 0.001, Fig 1b) and in autumn (uncapping: χ2 = 2005.32, p < 0.001, Fig 1a; removal: χ2 = 619.54, p < 0.001, Fig 1b). Within the first three hours, A. cerana uncapped over twice as much dead brood as A. mellifera (Student’s t-test, p = 0.048 in spring, p = 0.006 in autumn; Fig 2). At 24 hours, A. cerana had uncapped significantly more brood (Student’s t-test, p = 0.010 in spring, p = 0.021 in autumn), but had not removed more dead pupae (Student’s t-test, p = 0.258 in spring, p = 0.287 in autumn). Both A. cerana and A. mellifera, showed significantly higher brood uncapping and removal in autumn than in spring (log-rank test, uncapping: A. cerana, χ2 = 797.22, p < 0.001, A. mellifera, χ2 = 26.90, p < 0.001, Fig 1a; removal: A. cerana, χ2 = 312.96, p < 0.001, A. mellifera, χ2 = 66.02, p < 0.001, Fig 1b).
In the observation hives, although one of the five A. cerana colonies did not complete the task by uncapping only 88.2% of the frozen brood cells over three days, this species exerted a significantly more intense hygienic behavior than A. mellifera colonies (log-rank test, χ2 = 641.20, p < 0.001; Fig 1c). A. cerana also uncapped significantly more freeze-killed brood cells during the first two hours compared to A. mellifera (Student’s t-test, p = 0.004; Fig 2).
Discussion
The data clearly show that Eastern honey bees, A. cerana, are consistently faster than Western honey bees, A. mellifera and thus probably more efficient in hygienic brood detection (as shown by their uncapping of cells containing dead brood) and removal. This higher efficiency was irrespective of season and of hive type and was expressed within the first few hours of exposure to freeze-killed brood. Both species performed hygienic behavior to a higher degree in autumn compared to spring.
In this study, we monitored hygienic behavior in a single population of introduced A. mellifera and of endemic A. cerana. A. mellifera was imported in China around 1896 [39], well before breeding for hygienic behavior started in the North America or in Europe [41] and selection for hygienic behavior has not been practiced since this time in the sampled population. The domesticated A. cerana population used in our experiments originated from indigenous wild A. cerana Fabricius colonies of the mainland population. For both species, the queens have been naturally and locally mated in the populations sampled. This indicates that our sample is representative for non-bred Chinese A. cerana and A. mellifera populations of the southeast region of mainland China. In line with this fact, the brood removal after 24h measured in the A. mellifera population studied of 38.9 ± 12.2% and 59.2 ± 28.2% in spring and in autumn, respectively, corresponded to that reported for colonies of intermediate hygienic abilities (5 to 65%) [36]. Hygienic behavior in honey bee colonies can be highly variable among and within populations and subspecies, due to differences in habitat, genetic lineage and geography [23,33,38,42]. Repeating our experiment in other regions where both species co-occur is thus necessary to generalize the results.
When comparing freeze-killed brood hygiene between the two experimental seasons, we found that the colonies of both honey bee species detected and removed dead brood faster in autumn than they did in spring (Fig 1a). Autumn is the season at which A. mellifera colonies are at the greatest risk of viral infections in Europe [43]. Indeed, it is during this season that the prevalence of deformed wing virus and acute bee paralysis virus, two harmful viruses closely associated with V. destructor infestation, is peaking in A. mellifera [44]. Severe mite infestations, which are very likely to promote virus infections [45], are also observed in autumn in China [46]. Similarly, sacbrood virus, the most common pathogen of A. cerana, is more prevalent in autumn and late winter [46]. Hence, hygienic behavior could be reinforced in both honey bee species by means of natural selection, when pathogenic pressures are at their highest.
A. cerana colonies detected and removed dead brood significantly faster compared to A. mellifera (Fig 1), indicating a more efficient hygienic behavior. This advantage remained independently of hive type and season, suggesting that it is an intrinsic characteristic of the Eastern honey bee. Larger colonies might be more efficient in hygienic behavior, because more workers can be allocated to this task. However, it is the less populous A. cerana that showed faster hygienic behavior, suggesting that colony size is less relevant in this regard. A fast hygienic response is likely to have an adaptive value since a rapid brood removal should limit the transmission of fast replicating pathogens (e.g. viruses and bacteria) within the colony [47–52]. The sooner diseased brood is removed (i.e. the fewer viruses or bacteria can replicate), the lower are the chances for hygienic adult workers to become contaminated per os with pathogenic amounts of microorganisms [53]. At 24 hours, the difference in degree of hygienic behavior expressed by full colonies of both species decreased, suggesting that hygienic removal in the first few hours after brood damage are important for effective hygiene. Evaluating hygienic capacities of colonies used in selection programs in the first hours after brood killing might thus improve their success. Testing A. mellifera colonies highly selected for hygienic behavior and disease resistance [33–34,54] for their brood removal capacity as soon as a few hours after brood killing could also test the proposed link between rapidity of diseased brood removal and good colony health.
The trigger of hygienic behavior relies on the stimulus intensity emitted by unhealthy brood and on the olfactory sensitivity of workers [52,55–57]. Thus, workers that initiate the hygienic behavior by detecting and uncapping the cells show a greater olfactory sensitivity compared to those that complete the process by removing the brood [57]. The difference in efficiency observed between A. cerana and A. mellifera may result from the superior olfactory sensitivity reported for A. cerana workers [58–59]. Alternatively, but not mutually exclusive, a higher susceptibility of brood to antagonists in A. cerana [60] might contribute to this difference by producing signals leading to their removal earlier and in greater quantity. Further studies are required to identify the proximate mechanisms underlying the faster hygienic response of A. cerana and the possible contribution of this trait to the better health of honey bees. Confirming this link would provide an opportunity for further improving the success of existing breeding programs for hygienic behavior by integrating fast detection and removal of diseased brood ahead of the 24 or 48 h currently used as standards.
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to Mr. Zhendong Jiang for building the observation hives. We thank Yanzheng Zhang, Shuai Wang, Xu Yingying and Xiasen Jiang for their assistance with observations. We also thank two reviewers for their constructive comments.
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the paper.
Funding Statement
This work was supported by Modern Agro-industry Technology Research System (No. CARS-45) and Science and Technology Department of Zhejiang Province, China (No. 2012C12906-19) to F.H., by the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities to H.Z., by a Swiss National Science Foundation grant (No. 31003A_147363) to V.D. and P.N. and by the Vinetum Foundation to P.N. All of the funding bodies had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Neumann P, Carreck N. Honey bee colony losses. J Apic Res. 2010;49: 1–6. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Potts SG, Biesmeijer JC, Kremen C, Neumann P, Schweiger O, Kunin WE. Global pollinator declines: trends, impacts and drivers. Trends Ecol Evol. 2010;25: 345–353. 10.1016/j.tree.2010.01.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Breeze TD, Vaissière BE, Bommarco R, Petanidou T, Seraphides N, Kozák L, et al. Agricultural policies exacerbate honeybee pollination service supply-demand mismatches across Europe. PLoS ONE. 2014;9: e82996 10.1371/journal.pone.0082996 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Moritz RFA, de Miranda J, Fries I, Le Conte Y, Neumann P, Paxton RJ. Research strategies to improve honeybee health in Europe. Apidologie. 2010;41: 227–242. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Anderson DJ. Mites and pathogens of the European honey bee Apis mellifera and the Eastern hive bee Apis cerana in Papua New Guinea. Auckland: DSIR, Entomology Division; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Chen YW, Wang CH, An J, Ho KK. Susceptibility of the Asian honey bee, Apis cerana, to American foulbrood, Paenibacillus larvae. J Apic Res. 2000;39: 169–175. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Ellis JD, Munn PA. The worldwide health status of honey bees. Bee World. 2005;86: 88–101. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Ai H, Yan X, Han R. Occurrence and prevalence of seven bee viruses in Apis mellifera and Apis cerana apiaries in China. J Invertebr Pathol. 2012;109: 160–164. 10.1016/j.jip.2011.10.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Li J, Qin H, Wu J, Sadd BM, Wang X, Evans JD, et al. The prevalence of parasites and pathogens in Asian honeybees Apis cerana in China. PLoS ONE. 2012;7: e47955 10.1371/journal.pone.0047955 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Zhao HX, Zeng XN, Liang Q, Zhang XF, Huang WZ, Chen HS, et al. Study of the obp5 gene in Apis mellifera ligustica and Apis cerana cerana. Genet Mol Res. 2015;14: 6482–94. 10.4238/2015.June.12.1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Chantawannakul P, de Guzman LI, Li J, Williams GR. Parasites, pathogens, and pests of honeybees in Asia. Apidologie. 2016;47; 301–324. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Yañez O, Zheng HQ, Su XL, Hu FL, Neumann P, Dietemann V. Potential for virus transfer between the honey bees Apis mellifera and Apis cerana. J Apic Res. 2016;54: 179–191. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Wilfert L, Long G, Leggett HC, Schmid-Hempel P, Butlin R, Martin SJM, et al. Deformed wing virus is a recent global epidemic in honeybees driven by Varroa mites. Science. 2016;351: 594–597. 10.1126/science.aac9976 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Cremer S, Armitage SA, Schmid-Hempel P. Social immunity. Curr Biol. 2007;17: R693–R702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Meunier J. Social immunity and the evolution of group living in insects. Phil Trans R Soc B. 2015;370: 20140102 10.1098/rstb.2014.0102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Peng YS, Fang Y, Xu S, Ge L. The resistance mechanism of the Asian honey bee, Apis cerana Fabr., to an ectoparasitic mite, Varroa jacobsoni Oudemans. J Invertebr Pathol. 1987;49: 54–60. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Peng YS, Fang Y, Xu S, Ge L, Nasr ME. Response of foster Asian honey bee (Apis cerana Fabr.) colonies to the brood of European honey bee (Apis mellifera L.) infested with parasitic mite Varroa jacobsoni Oudemanns. J Invertebr Pathol. 1987;49: 259–264. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Rosenkranz P, Aumeier P, Ziegelmann B. Biology and control of Varroa destructor. J Invertebr Pathol. 2010;103 Suppl 1: S96–S119. 10.1016/j.jip.2009.07.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Wilson-Rich N, Spivak M, Fefferman NH, Starks PT. Genetic, individual, and group facilitation of disease resistance in insect societies. Ann Rev Entomol. 2009;54: 405–423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Rath W, Drescher W. Response of Apis cerana Fabr. towards brood infested with Varroa jacobsoni Oud. and infestation rate of colonies in Thailand. Apidologie. 1990;21: 311–321. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Boecking O, Drescher W. The removal response of Apis mellifera L. colonies to brood in wax and plastic cells after artificial and natural infestation with Varroa jacobsoni Oud. and to freeze-killed brood. Exp Appl Acarol. 1992;16: 321–329. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Rosenkranz P, Tewarson NC, Singh A, Engels W. Differential hygienic behaviour towards Varroa jacobsoni in capped worker brood of Apis cerana depends on alien scent adhering to the mites. J Apic Res. 1993;32: 89–93. [Google Scholar]
- 23.de Guzman LI, Rinderer TE, Frake AM, Kirrane MJ. Brood removal influences fall of Varroa destructor in honey bee colonies. J Apic Res. 2016;54: 216–225. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Rothenbuhler WC. Behavior genetics of nest cleaning in honey bees. IV. Responses of F1 and backcross generations to disease-killed brood. Am Zool. 1964;4: 111–123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Spivak M, Reuter GS. Resistance to American foulbrood disease by honey bee colonies Apis mellifera bred for hygienic behavior. Apidologie. 2001;32: 555–565. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Gilliam M, Taber S III, Richardson GV. Hygienic behavior of honey bees in relation to chalkbrood disease. Apidologie. 1983;14: 29–39. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Gilliam M, Taber S, Lorenz BJ, Prest DB. Factors affecting development of chalkbrood disease in colonies of honey bees, Apis mellifera, fed pollen contaminated with Ascosphaera apis. J Invertebr Pathol. 1988;52: 314–325. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Palacio MA, Rodriguez E, Goncalves L, Bedascarrasbure E, Spivak M. Hygienic behaviors of honey bees in response to brood experimentally pin-killed or infected with Ascosphaera apis. Apidologie. 2010;41: 602–612. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Toufailia HMA, Amiri E, Scandian L, Kryger P, Ratnieks FLW. Towards integrated control of varroa: effect of variation in hygienic behaviour among honey bee colonies on mite population increase and deformed wing virus incidence. J Apic Res. 2015;53: 555–562. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Mondet F, Alaux C, Severac D, Rohmer M, Mercer AR, Le Conte Y. Antennae hold a key to Varroa-sensitive hygiene behaviour in honey bees. Sci Rep. 2015;5: 10454 10.1038/srep10454 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Milne CP. Estimates of the heritabilities of and genetic correlation between two components of honey bee (Hymenoptera: Apidae) hygienic behavior: uncapping and removing. Ann Entomol Soc Am. 1985;78: 841–844. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Harbo JR, Harris JW. Heritability in honey bees (Hymenoptera: Apidae) of characteristics associated with resistance to Varroa jacobsoni (Mesostigmata: Varroidae). J Econ Entomol. 1999;92: 261–265. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Spivak M, Reuter GS. Performance of hygienic honey bee colonies in a commercial apiary. Apidologie. 1998;29: 291–302. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Danka RG, Harris JW, Villa JD, Dodds GE. Varying congruence of hygienic responses to Varroa destructor and freeze-killed brood among different types of honeybees. Apidologie, 2013;44: 447–457. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Büchler R, Andonov S, Bienefeld K, Costa C, Hatjina F, Kezic N, et al. Standard methods for rearing and selection of Apis mellifera queens. In: Dietemann V; Ellis J D; Neumann P (Eds) The COLOSS BEEBOOK, Volume I: standard methods for Apis mellifera research. J Apic Res. 2013;52: 1–30. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Spivak M, Downey DL. Field assays for hygienic behavior in honey bees (Hymenoptera: Apidae). J Econ Entomol. 1998;91: 64–70. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Xonis C, Thrasyvoulou A, El Taj HF. Variability of hygienic behavior in bee Apis mellifera macedonica. Bulg J Agric Sci. 2015;21: 674–679. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Rasolofoarivao H, Delatte H, Raveloson Ravaomanarivo LH, Reynaud B, Clémencet J. Assessing hygienic behavior of Apis mellifera unicolor (Hymenoptera: Apidae), the endemic honey bee from Madagascar. Genet Mol Res. 2015;14: 5879–5889. 10.4238/2015.June.1.5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Yang GH. Harm of introducing the western honeybee Apis mellifera L. to the Chinese honeybee Apis cerana F. and its ecological impact. Acta Entomol Sin. 2005;48: 401–406. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Burgett M, Burikam I. Number of adult honey bees (Hymenoptera: Apidae) occupying a comb: a standard for estimating colony populations. J Econ Entomol. 1985;78: 1154–1156. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Spivak M, Gilliam M. Hygienic behaviour of honey bees and its application for control of brood diseases and varroa Part II. Studies on hygienic behaviour since the Rothenbuhler era. Bee World. 1998;79: 169–186. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Athreya SVR, Reddy MS. Variation of hygienic behaviour (nest cleaning behaviour) in honey bee, Apis cerana indica F. in different eco habitats of South India. Curr Biot. 2013;7: 101–104. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Sumpter DJ, Martin SJ. The dynamics of virus epidemics in Varroa-infested honey bee colonies. J Anim Ecol. 2004;73: 51–63. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Tentcheva D, Gauthier L, Zappulla N, Dainat B, Cousserans F, Colin ME, et al. Prevalence and seasonal variations of six bee viruses in Apis mellifera L. and Varroa destructor mite populations in France. Appl Environ Microbiol. 2004;70: 7185–7191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Neumann P, Yañez O, Fries I, de Miranda JR. Varroa invasion and virus adaptation. Trends Parasitol. 2012;28: 353–354. 10.1016/j.pt.2012.06.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Liang Q, Chen DF. Bee protection. Beijing: China Agriculture Press; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Highfield AC, El Nagar A, Mackinder LC, Noël LM, Hall MJ, Martin SJ, et al. Deformed wing virus implicated in overwintering honeybee colony losses. Appl Environ Microbiol. 2009;75: 7212–7220. 10.1128/AEM.02227-09 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Soroker V, Hetzroni A, Yakobson B, David D, David A, Voet H, et al. Evaluation of colony losses in Israel in relation to the incidence of pathogens and pests. Apidologie. 2011;42: 192–199. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Topolska G, Gajda A, Pohorecka K, Bober A, Kasprzak S, Skubida M, et al. Winter colony losses in Poland. J Apic Res. 2010;49: 126–128. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Pajuelo AG, Torres C, Bermejo FJO. Colony losses: a double blind trial on the influence of supplementary protein nutrition and preventative treatment with fumagillin against Nosema ceranae. J Apic Res. 2008;47: 84–86. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Briggs CJ, Godfray HCJ. The dynamics of insect-pathogen interactions in seasonal environments. Theor Popul Biol. 1996;50: 149–177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Mondet F, Kim SH, de Miranda JR, Beslay D, Le Conte Y, Mercer AR. Specific cues associated with honey bee social defence against Varroa destructor infested brood. Sci Rep. 2016;6: 25444 10.1038/srep25444 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Chen Y, Evans J, Feldlaufer M. Horizontal and vertical transmission of viruses in the honey bee, Apis mellifera. J Invertebr Pathol. 2006;92: 152–159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Strange JP, Calderone NW. Evaluation of apicultural characteristics of first-year colonies initiated from packaged honey bees (Hymenoptera: Apidae). J Econ Entomol. 2009;102: 485–492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Masterman R, Smith BH, Spivak M. Brood odor discrimination abilities in hygienic honey bees (Apis mellifera L.) using proboscis extension reflex conditioning. J Insect Behav. 2000;13: 87–101. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Spivak M, Masterman R, Ross R, Mesce KA. Hygienic behavior in the honey bee (Apis mellifera L.) and the modulatory role of octopamine. J Neurobiol. 2003;55: 341–354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Gramacho KP, Spivak M. Differences in olfactory sensitivity and behavioral responses among honey bees bred for hygienic behavior. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2003;54: 472–479. [Google Scholar]
- 58.Woltedji D, Song F, Zhang L, Gala A, Han B, Feng M, et al. Western honeybee drones and workers (Apis mellifera ligustica) have different olfactory mechanisms than Eastern honeybees (Apis cerana cerana). J Proteome Res. 2012;11: 4526–4540. 10.1021/pr300298w [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Jung JW, Park KW, Oh H, Kwon HW. Structural and functional differences in the antennal olfactory system of worker honey bees of Apis mellifera and Apis cerana. J Asia-Pac Entomol. 2014;17: 639–646. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Page P, Lin Z, Buawangpong N, Zheng H, Hu F, Neumann P, et al. Social apoptosis in honey bee superorganisms. Sci Rep. 2016;6: 27210 10.1038/srep27210 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are within the paper.