Abstract
Recent comparative studies have indicated the existence of a common cranial evolutionary allometric (CREA) pattern in mammals and birds, in which smaller species have relatively smaller faces and bigger braincases than larger species. In these studies, cranial allometry was tested using a multivariate regression between shape (described using landmarks coordinates) and size (i.e. centroid size), after accounting for phylogenetic relatedness. Alternatively, cranial allometry can be determined by comparing the sizes of two anatomical parts using a bivariate regression analysis. In this analysis, a slope higher or lower than 1 indicates the existence of positive or negative allometry, respectively. Thus, in those species that support the CREA ‘rule’, positive allometry is expected for the association between face size and braincase size, which would indicate that larger species have disproportionally larger faces. In the present study, I applied these two approaches to explore cranial allometry in 83 Galliformes (Aves, Galloanserae), ranging in body weight from 30 g to 2.5 kg. The multivariate regression between shape and centroid size revealed the existence of a significant allometric pattern resembling CREA, whereas the second analysis revealed a negative allometry for beak size and braincase size (i.e. contrary to the CREA ‘rule’, larger galliform species have disproportionally shorter beaks than smaller galliform species). This study suggests that the presence of CREA may be overestimated when using cranium size as the standard measurement.
Keywords: Allometry, brain size, constraint, geometric morphometrics
Introduction
It has been recently reported that there exists a common allometric pattern across mammalian taxa, in which small species possess proportionally shorter faces and wider braincases than large species. This trend has been called the cranial evolutionary allometric (CREA) ‘rule’ (Cardini & Polly 2013; Cardini et al. 2015). Previously described in four groups of placental mammals with divergent morphologies and lifestyles (antelopes, bats, mongooses, and squirrels) (Cardini & Polly 2013), this trend has also been found in marsupials (Cardini et al. 2015). Taking in to account both the long divergence time and the existence of relevant differences in craniofacial ossification between placentals mammals and marsupials (e.g. Smith 1997), the finding suggests the existence of a new ‘rule’ for cranial evolution.
Skull allometries have been generally interpreted as adjustments necessary to compensate the physical constraints predicted by the square-cube law (e.g. Emerson & Bramble 1993), originally formulated by Galileo Galilei in the Two New Sciences in 1638. In his book, Galilei already applied this law to explain the disproportionately thick bones of large animals. According to the square-cube law, when the length of an object is scaled up by a factor k, its area will be increased by k2 and its volume by k3. In biological terms, this means that if organisms grow isometrically (i.e. maintaining their shape), both their length and area dimensions would gradually become smaller in proportion to their body mass (i.e. volume). Regarding the skull, maintenance of isometric growth would lead to large species with, for example, relatively small oral gapes, small tooth areas, thinner bones, small corneas or less muscle force, which could decrease their feeding performance and/or other organismal abilities. The physical constraints imposed by the square-cube law could be overcome by growing allometrically, i.e. by changing shape with size increase and growing, for example, disproportionately larger jaws or thicker muscles.
Slater and van Valkenburgh (2009) explicitly tested the effect of facial allometry on skull performance in felids. Using finite element analysis, the authors showed that the skulls of larger felids possess both a lower biting force and a decrease in work efficiency relative to the skull of small felids. This means that a disproportionally large face does not improve the feeding performance in felids, but it could still be advantageous for these ambush predators – felids capture their prey with a single bite – to have a disproportionally larger oral gape that facilitates the capture of large, energetically valuable preys (Slater & Van Valkenburgh 2009). The reduction in skull strength caused by a longer face seems to have been compensated by an allometry in bone thickness: large felids have relatively thicker skull bones than small felids (Slater & Van Valkenburgh 2009; Chamoli & Wroe 2011). These studies indicate that: 1) allometric growth does not necessarily increase performance, 2) the disadvantage due to one allometric trait can be compensated by other traits, 3) costs and benefits of growing allometrically would depend on the species' behaviour (Bock & Wahlert 1965), e.g. a disproportionately large mouth may be advantageous for an ambush predator, but not for species displaying other feeding behaviours (e.g. Slater et al. 2009).
In contrast to mammals, geometric morphometric studies in birds are scarce. Nonetheless, available studies have shown that birds also display the CREA ‘rule’ (Bright et al 2016). In these CREA studies, cranium shape was quantified by geometric morphometric methods. Accordingly, species comparisons were conducted after the landmark configurations were set to the same orientation, position, and scale (i.e. after removing arbitrary shape differences). The scaling step was accomplished by using cranium size (i.e. centroid size) as the standard measurement. However, as stressed by Radinsky (1983; 1984), face and braincase proportion studies should be performed using a standard that is not a composite of these two regions, as scaling by cranium size induces covariation between the face and the braincase, even when they grow independently of each other. Thus, studies of cranial proportions based on cranium size as the standard measurement may lead to spurious results (see Fig. 1).
Cranial allometry has traditionally been studied by computing the slope of a regression line in order to describe the relationship between the sizes of two anatomical parts (for a review see e.g. Klingenberg 1998; Warton et al. 2006). For the length-length relationship, a slope of 1 indicates isometry (i.e. the proportions of the two regions are similar in both small and large species). A slope higher or lower than 1 indicates the existence of positive or negative allometry, respectively. A positive allometry for face size relative to braincase size means that larger species have disproportionally larger faces (i.e. the CREA ‘rule’), whereas a negative allometry indicates that the face is relatively shorter in larger species. Thus, species displaying the CREA ‘rule’ should possess a positive allometry for face and braincase size. In this study, I tested this hypothesis in Galliformes by applying 3D geometric morphometric methods to quantify the cranium shape, and explored cranial allometry using two regressions: 1) a multivariate regression between shape and centroid size, and 2) a major axis regression of beak and braincase centroid size, after accounting for phylogenetic relatedness. These analyses led to contradictory results, suggesting that the presence of CREA in Galliformes might only be an artificial pattern induced when cranium size is used as the standard measurement.
Material and Methods
A total of 83 adult crania representing all living families of the order Galliformes were sampled. These crania are part of the osteological collection at the Natural History Museum at Tring (UK). A set of 26 landmark coordinates were obtained using a MicroScribe 3D digitizer (Fig. 2). Shape data was extracted from landmarks coordinates using Generalized Procrustes Analysis (GPA). Shape analyses were performed on the symmetric component derived from the GPA (Klingenberg et al. 2002).
The www.birdtree.org platform was used for generating a subset of 1000 equally plausible phylogenetic trees for the 83 galliform species included in this study, using both Hackett and Ericson phylogenetic backbones (9993 OTUs) (Jetz et al. 2012). The phylogenetic information contained in each tree set was summarized by computing a single 50% majority-rule consensus tree (Rubolini et al. 2015). A set of phylogenetic independent contrasts (PICs) of shape and size (i.e. centroid size, CS) data was computed for each consensus tree (Klingenberg & Gidaszewski 2010). A Procrustes ANOVA using shape contrasts as the dependent variable, and the phylogenetic tree from which the contrasts were computed as an independent, discrete factor, was performed to test the effect of using different phylogenetic hypotheses to obtain the shape data.
Evolutionary allometry was studied using: 1) a multivariate regression of shape contrasts onto size contrasts (log-transformed centroid size, log-CS), 2) a major axis (MA) regression of upper beak size against braincase size (i.e. PICs centroid size). The statistical significance of the multivariate regression was assessed by a permutation approach (10,000 iterations). The allometric pattern was considered significant when the probability of randomly obtaining a regression vector that accounted for a higher percentage of shape variance was lower than 0.05 (Klingenberg & Marugán-Lobón 2013). The significance of the bivariate regression was tested against the null hypothesis (no correlation between the variables). The observed slope was considered equal to 1 (i.e. isometry) if the fitted and residual scores for the sample, computed from a regression with a slope of 1, were uncorrelated.
Generalized Procrustes Analysis, PICs and multivariate regressions were computed using MorphoJ v.1.06d (Klingenberg 2011). Procrustes ANOVA was performed in R (v. 3.2.3) (R Core Team 2016) using procD.lm in GEOMORPH (v. 3.0.1). Major axis analysis was performed in R using ma and slope test in SMATR (v. 3.4.3) (Warton et al. 2012. Consensus trees were obtained using Mesquite v.2.75 (Maddison & Maddison 2011) and three-dimensional visualizations of the results were generated in Landmark editor v.3.0.0.6 (Wiley et al. 2005).
Results
The Procrustes ANOVA showed non-significant differences between shape data obtained from Hackett or Ericson phylogenies (F = 0.0015; df = 163; P = 1); only the results obtained from the former will be discussed here. The multivariate regression of shape contrasts onto log-CS contrasts showed the existence of a significant allometric pattern that explained 12.3 % of the variation in shape (P < 0.001). This pattern coincided with the one described in mammals and other bird taxa where small species showed relatively shorter faces and wider braincases than larger species (Cardini & Polly 2013; Cardini et al. 2015 ref) (Fig. 3a-c).
Contrary to expectations, the pattern depicted by CREA was not observed using a MA regression of beak size and braincase size (i.e. centroid size contrasts). The MA analysis showed that there was a significant linear relationship between beak and braincase size (r2 = 0.87; P < 0.001). However, the observed slope (β = 0.63; C.I.: 0.58-0.68) deviated significantly from 1 (N = 82; F = 124.73; P = 0), indicating the existence of negative allometry between beak and braincase size (i.e. larger galliform species have disproportionally shorter beaks) (Fig. 3d).
Discussion
This study suggests that the common approaches for the study of cranial allometry may lead to conflicting results if cranium size is used as the standard measurement. Although the existence of a common developmental constraint between the face and the braincase, as suggested by the CREA ‘rule’, requires further empirical work, the existence of a strong negative allometry of brain mass—and therefore, braincase size—relative to body mass in amniotes is well documented (e.g. Martin, 1981; Armstrong, 1983). This observation led some researchers to suggest that brain size could be related primary to metabolism rather than to body mass (e.g. Martin 1981; Armstrong 1983; Bennett & Harvey 1985). It has been generally accepted that the material and energy exchange that takes place during metabolism would be physically constrained by the surface area of either the whole organism or its internal transport networks, in which case it is generally expected that the metabolism scales as an exponent of 2/3 or 3/4 relative to body mass, respectively (for a review see Glazier 2005). These scaling exponents corresponded to those estimated for brain mass and body mass (e.g. Martin, 1981; Armstrong, 1983), supporting the idea that the observed negative allometry between these two traits could be explained in terms of the square-cube law, as a consequence of the existence of a universal, physical constraint on metabolism.
Early studies focused on exploring the scaling relationships between brain mass and basal metabolic rate (BMR) reported a high and significant relationship, as would be expected if these two traits were causally related (Armstrong 1983; Hofman 1983; Bennett & Harvey 1985). However, these early analyses did not account for the confounding effects of either body mass or phylogenetic relationships. More recently, it has been shown that when these effects are corrected, BMR explains a very low amount of brain mass variation (Isler & Van Schaik 2006; Isler & van Schaik 2006). These results suggest that metabolism could not explain the strong negative allometry of brain mass – and therefore, braincase size – relative to body mass in amniotes.
However, the strong allometry of braincase size relative to body mass could still be explained in terms of the square-cube law if cranial proportions at early developmental stages are taken into account. It has been shown that juveniles of some amniote species display relatively larger braincases and shorter faces than adults (Mitteroecker et al. 2004; Bhullar et al. 2012). Probably a disproportionately large braincase is more accentuated in neonates. Taking this in to account, isometric growth during the postnatal stage – note that, according to the square-cube law, it is expected brain size grows with an exponent of 1 relative to body mass – would probably lead to a head too big to be supported by the neck at later developmental stages, i.e. head proportions at early developmental stages may be physically non-viable at adulthood in some amniote species. Therefore, the negative allometry of braincase size relative to body mass could be understood as an adjustment to the physical constraint (sensuMaynard Smith et al. 1985) imposed by the square-cube law not on metabolism, but on head size.
Acknowledgments
I am grateful to C. Klingenberg for his advice and to R. Prys-Jones and J. Cooper for kindly allowing me access to the skeletal collection at the Natural History Museum at Tring (UK). I want to thank N. Young for guidance to Bright et al paper, and to R. Marcucio for his support during these last years. I acknowledge support from the European Commission (Marie Curie fellowship PIOF-GA-2008-219676) and the National Institute of Dental and Craniofacial Research (Grant numbers: R01-DE018234, R01-DE019638 to R. Marcucio and B. Hallgrímsson)
References
- Armstrong E. Relative brain size and metabolism in mammals. Science. 1983;220:1302–1304. doi: 10.1126/science.6407108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bennett PM, Harvey PH. Brain size, development and metabolism in birds and mammals. J Zool. 1985;207:491–509. [Google Scholar]
- Bhullar BA, Marugán-Lobón J, Racimo F, Bever GS, Rowe TB, Norell MA, Abzhanov A. Birds have paedomorphic dinosaur skulls. Nature. 2012;487:223–226. doi: 10.1038/nature11146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bock WJ, Wahlert G. Adaptation and the form-function complex. Evolution. 1965;19:269–299. [Google Scholar]
- Bright JA, Marugán-Lobón J, Cobb SN, Rayfield EJ. The shapes of bird beaks are highly controlled by nondietary factors. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2016;113(19):5352–5357. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1602683113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardini A, Polly D, Dawson R, Milne N. Why the long face? Kangaroos and wallabies follow the same “rule” of cranial evolutionary allometry (CREA) as placentals. Evol Biol. 2015;42:169–176. [Google Scholar]
- Cardini A, Polly PD. Larger mammals have longer faces because of size-related constraints on skull form. Nat Commun. 2013;4:2458. doi: 10.1038/ncomms3458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chamoli U, Wroe S. Allometry in the distribution of material properties and geometry of the felid skull: Why larger species may need to change and how they may achieve it. J Theor Biol. 2011;283:217–226. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2011.05.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Emerson SB, Bramble DM. Sclaing, allometry, and skull design. In: Hanken J, Hall BK, editors. The Skull Volume 3: Functional and Evolutionary Mechanisms. Chicago University Press; Chicago: 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Glazier DS. Beyond the “3/4-power law”: variation in the intra-and interspecific scaling of metabolic rate in animals. Biol Rev. 2005;80:611–662. doi: 10.1017/S1464793105006834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hofman MA. Energy metabolism, brain size and longevity in mammals. Q Rev Biol. 1983:495–512. doi: 10.1086/413544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Isler K, van Schaik C. Costs of encephalization: the energy trade-off hypothesis tested on birds. J Hum Evol. 2006;51:228–243. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2006.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Isler K, Van Schaik CP. Metabolic costs of brain size evolution. Biol Lett. 2006;2:557–560. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2006.0538. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jetz W, Thomas GH, Joy JB, Hartmann K, Mooers AO. The global diversity of birds in space and time. Nature. 2012;491:444–448. doi: 10.1038/nature11631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klingenberg CP. MorphoJ: an integrated software package for geometrics morphometrics. Mol Ecol Resour. 2011;11:353–357. doi: 10.1111/j.1755-0998.2010.02924.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klingenberg CP. Heterochrony and allometry: the analysis of evolutionary change in ontogeny. Biol Rev. 1998;73:79–123. doi: 10.1017/s000632319800512x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klingenberg CP, Barluenga M, Meyer A. Shape analysis of symmetric structures: quantifying variation among individuals and asymmetry. Evolution. 2002;56:1909–1920. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2002.tb00117.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klingenberg CP, Gidaszewski NA. Testing and quantifying phylogenetic signals and homoplasy in morphometric data. Syst Biol. 2010;59:245–261. doi: 10.1093/sysbio/syp106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klingenberg CP, Marugán-Lobón J. Evolutionary Covariation in Geometric Morphometric Data: Analyzing Integration, Modularity and Allometry in a Phylogenetic Context. Syst Biol. 2013;62:591–610. doi: 10.1093/sysbio/syt025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maddison WP, Maddison DR. Mesquite: a modular system for evoluitonary analysis. 2011 Version 2.75. [Google Scholar]
- Martin RD. Relative brain size and basal metabolic rate in terrestrial vertebrates. Nature. 1981;293:57–60. doi: 10.1038/293057a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maynard Smith J, Burian R, Kauffman S, Alberch P, Campbell J, Goodwin B, Lande R, Raup D, Wolpert L. Developmental Constraints and Evolution: A Perspective from the Mountain Lake Conference on Development and Evolution. Q Rev Biol. 1985;60:265–287. [Google Scholar]
- Mitteroecker P, Gunz P, Bernhard M, Schaefer K, Bookstein FL. Comparison of cranial ontogenetic trajectories among great apes and humans. J Hum Evol. 2004;46:679–698. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2004.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. 2016 Version 3.2.2. URL http://.R-project.org.
- Radinsky L. Allometry and reorganization in horse skull proportions. Science. 1983;221:1189–1191. doi: 10.1126/science.221.4616.1189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radinsky LB. Basicranial axis length v. skull length in analysis of carnivore skull shape. Biol J Linn Soc. 1984;22:31–41. [Google Scholar]
- Rubolini D, Liker A, Garamszegi LZ, MØLLER AP, SAINO N. Using the BirdTree. org website to obtain robust phylogenies for avian comparative studies: A primer. Curr Zool. 2015;61:959–965. doi: 10.1093/czoolo/61.6.959. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slater GJ, Dumont ER, Van Valkenburgh B. Implications of predatory specialization for cranial form and function in canids. J Zool. 2009;278:181–188. [Google Scholar]
- Slater GJ, Van Valkenburgh B. Allometry and performance: the evolution of skull form and function in felids. J Evolution Biol. 2009;22:2278–2287. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2009.01845.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith KK. Comparative patterns of craniofacial development in eutherian and metatherian mammals. Evolution. 1997:1663–1678. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1997.tb01489.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Warton DI, Duursma RA, Falster DS, Taskinen S. smatr 3-an R package for estimation and inference about allometric lines. Methods Ecol Evol. 2012;3:257–259. [Google Scholar]
- Warton DI, Wright IJ, Falster DS, Westoby M. Bivariate line-fitting methods for allometry. Biol Rev. 2006;81:259–291. doi: 10.1017/S1464793106007007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiley David F, Amenta Nina, Alcantara Dan A, Ghosh Deboshmita, Kil Yong J, Delson Eric, Harcourt-Smith Will, Rohlf F James, John K, St, Hamann Bernd. Evolutionary morphing. Proceedings of the IEEE; Minneapolis. 2005. pp. 431–438. [Google Scholar]